- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Brain Research Institute, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany
Introduction: A fundamental property of the neocortex is its columnar organization in many species. Generally, neurons of the same column share stimulus preferences and have strong anatomical connections across layers. These features suggest that neurons within a column operate as one unified network. Other features, like the different patterns of input and output connections of neurons located in separate layers and systematic differences in feature tuning, hint at a more segregated and possibly flexible functional organization of neurons within a column.
Methods: To distinguish between these views of columnar processing, we conducted laminar recordings in macaques’ area V1 while they performed a demanding attention task. We identified three separate regions with strong gamma oscillatory activity, located in the supragranular, granular, and infragranular laminar domains, based on the current source density (CSD).
Results and Discussion: Their characteristics differed significantly in their dominant gamma frequency and attention-dependent modulation of their gramma power and gamma frequency. In line, spiking activity in the supragranular, infragranular, and upper part of the granular domain exhibited strong phase coherence with the CSD signals of their domain but showed much weaker coherence with the CSD signals of other domains.
Conclusion: These results indicate that columnar processing involves a certain degree of independence between neurons in the three laminar domains, consistent with the assumption of multiple, separate intracolumnar ensembles. Such a functional organization offers various possibilities for dynamic network configuration, indicating that neurons in a column are not restricted to operate as one unified network. Thus, the findings open interesting new possibilities for future concepts and investigations on flexible, dynamic cortical ensemble formation and selective information processing.
Introduction
The primate visual cortex consists of a complex network of neurons organized into multiple distinct areas that make up the visual processing pathways. Structurally, the cortex comprises six vertically stratified layers. Horizontally, it is organized into an array of vertical columns that include neurons of all six layers (Mountcastle, 1997; Rockland and Ichinohe, 2004; Barbas et al., 2022). Multiple lines of evidence suggest a functional role for these columnar circuits. Notably, neurons within the same column share similar preferences for specific stimulus features (Kaschube et al., 2010; Alonso, 2016). In the early visual cortex, neurons within a column have largely overlapping spatial receptive fields (RF) (Mountcastle, 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962, 1963). Furthermore, in areas V1 and V2, neurons within a column share similar preferences for orientation, color, luminance, and ocular dominance (Poggio et al., 1975; Schiller et al., 1976; Livingstone and Hubel, 1984; Lennie et al., 1990; Peterhans and Von der Heydt, 1993; Garg et al., 2019).
This shared feature selectivity among neurons within the same column has been consistently observed across various areas of the visual cortex (Tootell et al., 1993; Niebur and Koch, 1994; Blasdel and Campbell, 2001; Vanduffel et al., 2002; Gattass et al., 2005; Ahmed et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013; Franken and Reynolds, 2021; Westerberg et al., 2021). Anatomically, the different types of neurons in the cortical layers exhibit characteristic patterns of strong vertical connections within columns, which have been proposed to implement a canonical microcircuit (Douglas and Martin, 2004). Based on these and related findings, the cortical column is widely recognized as a cohesive, functionally integrated local neuronal ensemble that operates as a single, unified entity (Mountcastle, 1957; Douglas et al., 1989; Douglas and Martin, 2007; Jones and Rakic, 2010; Capone et al., 2016; Hosoya, 2019).
However, despite important neuroanatomical and neurophysiological properties suggesting a unified mode of operation within a cortical column, further evidence suggests a potential for more segregated modes of operation. In such a scenario, neurons in different layers or groups of layers may not always operate in a fully integrated manner across the entire column but contribute to separate ensembles within a column. In line, neurons located in different layers of the same column can differ systematically in their feature tuning, for example, for orientation (Martinez et al., 2002; Ringach et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2020), stimulus size (Bijanzadeh et al., 2018), and other features (DiCarlo and Johnson, 2000; Hirsch et al., 2002; Hirsch and Martinez, 2006). The assumption of distinct functional roles for neurons in the column’s different layers is also supported by their different pattern of input and output connections (Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Rockland, 2019) and theoretical considerations on their function, e.g., in grouping and attention (Grossberg, 2001; Yazdanbakhsh and Grossberg, 2004; Hirsch and Martinez, 2006; Shushruth et al., 2012). Thus, whether neurons within a cortical column operate as a single, unified ensemble or in multiple, more segregated ensembles is open. Furthermore, in the latter case, the question would arise whether the configuration and interactions of such intra-columnar ensembles are fixed or might change on psychophysical timescales, possibly depending on the processing demands imposed by varying stimuli or behavioral tasks.
At the larger anatomical scale of cortical areas, experimental (Bosman et al., 2012; Gregoriou et al., 2012; Grothe et al., 2012; Drebitz et al., 2018) and theoretical studies (Aertsen et al., 1989; Segev and Rall, 1998; Borgers and Kopell, 2008; Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2010; Briggs et al., 2013; Harnack et al., 2015) indicate that selective synchronization of oscillatory neuronal activity within the γ-band (30–100 Hz) provides a mechanism to configure neuronal ensembles dynamically and flexibly according to changing computational requirements. Changing γ-band synchronization between neurons thereby serves to modulate the functional connectivity within local neuronal populations (Aertsen et al., 1989; Azouz and Gray, 2003; Doesburg et al., 2007; Battaglia et al., 2012; Engel et al., 2013; Palmigiano et al., 2017; Drebitz et al., 2018) and allows for selective information routing and processing in neuronal systems despite of fixed anatomical connections (Fries, 2015; Kreiter, 2020).
Here, we apply this conceptual framework to the single cortical column to better understand its mode of operation and to improve the understanding of the internal dynamics of columnar processing and the information flow across columns. Assuming that a column operates as a single, unified entity, the concept predicts that neurons throughout the column synchronize to the same γ-rhythm and show uniform properties, regarding, for example, the γ-peak frequency, attention-dependent modulations of this frequency, or attention-dependent modulation of the oscillation’s amplitude. In contrast, if neurons in a column operate (at least temporarily) in functionally distinct ensembles, the neurons of each local ensemble should contribute to their ensemble’s temporal dynamics but not to a column-wide shared γ-rhythm. This ensemble-specific γ-oscillatory activity could differ in spectral characteristics and functionally relevant properties from those located in different columnar regions.
We tested these predictions using linear multi-contact probes to record neuronal activity simultaneously across the layers of cortical columns in area V1 of two macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta). The animals performed a demanding attention task that required tracking the continuously changing shape of a previously cued stimulus. Subsequently, we computed the time-resolved current source density (CSD) to characterize the oscillatory γ-band activity (30–90 Hz) across layers.
We consistently found three distinct “hotspots” with strong γ-oscillatory activity during stimulus processing. They were separated from each other, with one located in the granular, one in the supragranular, and one in the infragranular layers of the column. The γ-oscillatory CSD signals at these three hotspots synchronized strongly with the spiking activity across the laminar domain in which they resided but less so with the spiking activity of other domains. Comparing the γ-band oscillations’ properties between the three regions showed marked differences. The dominant γ-frequencies and the strength of attention-dependent modulations of the γ-power differed significantly between the γ-oscillations at the three locations. Thus, our results indicate the presence of three different γ-oscillatory networks in the three major laminar domains of V1 columns, supporting the view that neurons within a cortical column can flexibly operate in multiple, at least partially segregated, ensembles.
Materials and methods
Surgical procedures
Two adult male macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta) were implanted with titanium head posts and recording chambers above areas V1/V2 under aseptic conditions. All procedures were approved by the local authorities (Senator für Gesundheit, Bremen, Germany) and followed the German Animal Welfare Act (TierSchG) and the European Council directive 2010/63/EU for laboratory animal care and use.
Data acquisition and experimental setup
During recording sessions, monkeys sat in a custom-made primate chair in front of a 20-inch CRT monitor (monkey I: 93 cm, monkey II: 95 cm) with a display resolution of 1,280 × 1,024 pixels and a refresh rate of 100 Hz (ELSA Ecomo 750). Eye position was monitored by video-oculography (IScan Inc., Woburn, MA, United States). See Drebitz et al. (2018), for details of the experimental setup.
Semi-chronic recordings in cortical area V1 were performed with linear multi-contact probes. Each probe had 16 or 32 electrodes with a center-to-center spacing of 100 μm and a pointy tip (ATLAS Neuroengineering bvba, Leuven, Belgium). The iridium oxide-coated electrodes had an impedance of 0.25 MΩ at 1 kHz and a diameter of 15*15 μm. A small custom-made microdrive was fixed to a grid in the recording chamber to hold and move the probe in a direction perpendicular to the cortical surface. After each recording session, the probe was retracted until the tip was located just below or in the dura mater. It was repositioned within the cortex at the start of each subsequent session. We conducted up to five sessions before removing the microdrive from the recording chamber, replacing the linear probe if necessary, and placing it in a new position in the chamber. At the initial probe insertion, neuronal signals were monitored to detect the penetration of the dura and the entering of the individual electrodes into the cortex to control for accurate depth placement. Using an automatic visual stimulation and data analysis procedure, we simultaneously mapped the receptive fields (RF) of all recording sites along the probe based on their evoked entire spiking activity and the induced γ-band power in the LFP in response to a moving bar (see Drebitz et al., 2019 for details). Given the semi-chronic recording approach, the receptive field (RF) locations remained consistent across successive sessions for which the same probe was used at the same place. Nevertheless, prior to the recordings in these successive sessions, we confirmed the previously observed RF locations by manually mapping the RFs using a bar stimulus.
Neuronal signals were amplified 20,000-fold for monkey I and 16,000-fold for monkey II with a wideband preamplifier (MPA32I) and a programmable gain amplifier (PGA 64, 1–5,000 Hz; Preamplifier and PGA, Multi-Channel Systems GmbH, Reutlingen, Germany) and subsequently digitized with a 12-bit ADC at 25 kHz sampling rate. The signals of all recording sites were referenced against the titanium recording chamber (27 mm diameter) that was in contact with bone and intracranial tissue.
Visual task paradigm
During V1 recordings, the animals performed a demanding shape-tracking task requiring the animals to attend to the continuously changing shape of a target stimulus and respond to the reappearance of its initial shape while fixating a central fixation point (FP) (Drebitz et al., 2018) in the presence of distractor stimuli. The appearance of this shape at one of the distractor stimuli had to be ignored. The stimulus configuration for the task encompassed up to four stimuli, isoeccentrically arranged around the FP (Figure 1A). One of the four stimuli was centered on the overlapping RFs of the neurons recorded in the V1 column. A second stimulus resided close to but outside of the V1 RFs. The other two stimuli were placed at point mirrored locations in the opposite visual hemifield with respect to the central FP (Figure 1A). For estimating neuronal responses without a nearby stimulus and to control that the nearby stimulus is located outside the RFs, in half of the trials either the stimulus in the RF or the stimulus nearby were not shown. Before the trial onset, a spatial cue was presented in the form of an annulus with a diameter of 1°, a line width of 0.25°, and a luminance of 3.8 cd/m2. This cue indicated the specific location where the behaviorally relevant stimulus would appear during the subsequent trial. The two stimulus locations within or nearby the recorded V1 RFs were cued with 30% probability, respectively. The two stimulus locations in the opposite hemifield were cued with 20% probability, resulting in three task conditions. Attention is focused either on the stimulus within the RFs of the recorded V1 site (‘attend in’ condition), on the stimulus close to the recorded V1 site (‘attend nearby’ condition), or at a stimulus in the opposite hemifield (“attend away” condition, see Figure 1B).
Figure 1. Task description and layer discrimination. (A) Stimulus configuration of the three task conditions relevant for this study. Stimuli were shown at four locations on screen, one of them in the receptive fields (RFs) of the neurons recorded within the V1 column (indicated by a green dashed circle, not present on screen. Attention was cued by a spatial cue prior to the stimulus onset to one of the four locations (indicated in red). Attention could be focused on the recorded V1 site (“attend in”), the distractor close to the recorded V1 site (“attend nearby”), and at one of the two stimuli in the opposite hemifield (“attend away”). (B) Morphing sequence of a cued target stimulus with two morphing cycles (MCs). After cue presentation, animals initiated the trial by pressing a lever and holding fixation to the central fixation point (FP). Then, the cue disappeared, and after 1,050 ms with only the FP shown (Baseline period, Base.), all stimuli were presented on screen. After a static presentation for 520 ms, they started to continuously morph into other complex shapes within 1,000 ms. The animals had to signal the reappearance of the initial shape at the cued stimulus location by releasing the lever within a response window of 510 ms (550 ms monkey I). (C) Estimation of the laminar position of electrodes. Left panel: Depth profile of the CSD calculated from the average LFP in V1 in response to stimulus onset for one exemplary session of monkey I. White dashed lines mark the borders between the three major laminar domains: supragranular (SG), granular (G), and infragranular (IG) layers. Right panel: Average Pearson correlation coefficient between the CSD signals below 15 Hz during the baseline period of all combinations of two electrodes. Data taken from the same example session as in the left panel. Large and similar correlation values occurred between pairs of electrodes located within the same laminar domain.
The cue appeared with the FP (square, 0.15°x0.15°, 2.45 Cd/m2) on the dark monitor (background luminance: 0.03 Cd/m2) and vanished after the monkeys initiated the trial by pressing a lever. After a baseline period of 1,050 ms, the complex-shaped stimuli (~1°–1.5° diameter, line width 0.25°) appeared, and following a static period (Figure 1B), they started to change their shape, by morphing continuously into other complex shapes. One morph cycle (MC) lasted one second, i.e., the time for morphing from one shape into the next. When the initial shape of the cued target stimulus reappeared at the end of MCs 2, 3, or 4, monkeys had to release the lever within a response window (Figure 1B, red rectangle) of 550 ms (510 ms), starting 380 (310) ms before the MC ends and terminating 170 (200) ms after its end for monkey I (monkey II). Shapes for each stimulus were drawn randomly from a set of 12 shapes for monkey I, two of which could become the target shape, and from a set of 8 shapes for monkey II, all of which could become the target shape. To heighten task difficulty, the stimulus luminance changed randomly with each frame of the screen. For this, a random, integer gray pixel value was selected before each frame update [0 255] corresponding to luminance changes from 0.02 to 45.4 Cd/m2, on average 3.8 Cd/m2 (for details: Grothe et al., 2018; Lisitsyn et al., 2020). From trial initiation until lever release, monkeys had to hold their gaze within a window of 1° diameter centered on the fixation square. If they did so and responded within the response window to the reappearance of the target shape at the cued stimulus location, they received a water reward. Otherwise, the trial ended without reward.
Data analysis
All analyses described below were performed with custom MATLAB scripts running on MATLAB v. 2020a (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States). The violin plots were created using an open-source MATLAB script available at https://github.com/bastibe/Violinplot-Matlab (Bechtold, 2016).
Data selection
We recorded neuronal data in 14 sessions for monkey I and 18 sessions for monkey II. If not stated otherwise, data from MCs 2 and 3 of correctly performed trials was analyzed. If an MC terminated with the reappearance of the initial shape of the target stimulus, only data up to 150 ms before the animal released the lever were used.
For the power analysis of CSD and LFP data across the cortical depth and spectrolaminar profiles of phase coherence (PhC) between two CSD signals, all available electrodes of each session for each monkey were used.
Subsequent analyses required the signals from the electrode representing the hotspot of γ-band activity within each of the three major domains of the column, i.e., the supragranular, the granular, and the infragranular layers. For this purpose, we first computed in all 32 sessions the 1/f corrected (see Methods: Spectral analysis) power spectrum of the CSD signals in MCs 2 and 3 for each of the five electrode positions in the granular layer, the five adjacent electrode positions above in supragranular layers, and the five electrode positions below in infragranular layers across all correctly performed trials on the attend-in condition with four stimuli present. Then, we computed the mean spectrum across sessions for each of these 15 electrode positions of each monkey. Taking the mean spectrum across these spectra for the 15 electrode positions allowed defining the borders of the frequency window for γ-power calculation as the width of the PSD peak in the γ-band at half height of its maximum value (42.5–130.5 Hz in both animals). Finally, we computed the mean γ-power within these borders from each electrode’s power spectrum in each session. In the resulting depth profile of CSD γ-power of each recording session, we searched in each of the three domains for a discrete peak flanked at both sides by electrode positions of the same domain with lower γ-power. In the few sections without such a discrete peak, we searched for a peak hidden in the flank of another larger peak in neighboring regions, as indicated by a flattening in such a flank, but without a local maximum. Therefore, we calculated the gradient of the γ-power between the section’s electrodes. In the case of a local minimum of the gradient at a rising flank or a local maximum of the gradient in a falling flank, we selected the first or second electrode from the electrode pair with the gradient’s extremum, respectively. If both approaches failed, no electrode was selected for the investigated domain of the column and session.
To analyze phase coherence between CSD and ESA signals, we used the CSD signals from the hotspot electrodes of the three (or fewer) domains. To avoid analyzing phase coherence based on recording sites with no or only weak responses to the stimulus, we excluded electrodes with a CSD γ-power during MCs 2 and 3 less than 2.5 times the CSD γ-power during the baseline period. As a source for ESA signals, we selected two-thirds of all electrodes in each session with the strongest ESA γ-power in MCs two and three for the ‘attend in’ condition.
For analyzing attention-dependent modulations of the power and dominant frequency in the γ-band, we used the 26 sessions with a behavioral performance of 65% or better (disregarding trials terminated by breaking fixation).
Estimation of current source density
To determine the time-resolved CSD signals for each trial, we first extracted the local field potentials (LFP) by filtering the recorded broadband neuronal signals with a low-pass FIR filter (pass: < 160 Hz, stop: 300 Hz, suppression: 80 dB) in forward and backward direction. The resulting LFPs were subsequently downsampled to 1,000 Hz and used for calculating the CSD signal with the spline inverse CSD method (iCSD), as described by Pettersen et al. (2006). This approach supposes that the recorded LFPs result from current sources within the cortical column that are evenly distributed in cylindrical discs stacked on top of each other and that the CSD varies smoothly in z-direction along the column’s center axis (perpendicular to the cortical layers) as described by a set of cubic polynomials. For the present analyses, we assumed a disc radius of 500 μm and an isotropic medium with a conductivity of 0.4 S/m (Logothetis et al., 2007). The depth profile of the CSD was calculated and spatially smoothed with a Gaussian filter (SD 200 μm, in a finite window of 1,000 μm) for each point in time using the open-access software provided by and available at Pettersen (2005). This provided for each electrode and trial a CSD signal as a function of time. For the discrimination of layers based on source and sink locations, we averaged the LFP within the first 250 ms starting after stimulus onset (see Figure 1B) across the trials of all conditions with a stimulus in the RF before computing the CSD using the spline iCSD method.
Layer identification and electrode alignment
To identify the position of the individual electrodes of a probe with respect to the cortical layers and align the electrodes between recording sessions according to their cortical position, we combined two procedures:
First, we used the spatiotemporal pattern of the CSD calculated with the spline iCSD method from the average field potential response to stimulus onset as described above and identified the border between layer III and layer IV based on the inversion of the CSD’s polarity from the source closest to the cortical surface to the initial sink below (see Figure 1D, left panel for an example). Second, we calculated a correlation matrix containing for each pair of electrodes the average Pearson correlation between their CSD signals during the baseline period (Figure 1B; see also Senzai et al., 2019). For this purpose, in each trial, the time-resolved CSD signals of all electrodes were low-pass filtered with an FIR filter pass: ≤ 15 Hz, stop: 20 Hz, suppression: 35 dB) in forward and backward directions within the time window from 100 ms after the start of the baseline period until 100 ms before its end. Subsequently, the Pearson correlation coefficient between these low-frequency CSD signals was calculated for each trial and pair of electrodes. Finally, the median value of each electrode pair’s correlation coefficients across trials provided an entry into the matrix of median correlation coefficients (see Figure 1D, right panel for an example). The correlation matrix usually delivered a sharp decrease in correlation around the border of layers III/IV and helped to refine the results of the first procedure. Recording sites above the border between layers III and IV were categorized as supragranular. Recording sites located within 0.5 mm below this border were categorized as granular, and sites further below as infragranular (Maier et al., 2010; Van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Ferro et al., 2021).
Estimation of entire spiking activity
To investigate the synchronization between spiking activity and the CSD signals, we estimated the entire spiking activity (ESA), which is a continuous signal and which does not reject spikes with small amplitude by thresholding (for a comparison of MUA and ESA, see Drebitz et al., 2019). Since ESA not only encompasses spikes surpassing a set threshold but includes the full range of population spiking activity, it preserves information that might otherwise be discarded, improving spectral frequency analysis of correlated activity. ESA is particularly beneficial for data with a low signal-to-noise ratio, where spike amplitudes and background noise are difficult to distinguish (Brosch et al., 1997; Drebitz et al., 2019).
We obtained the ESA by first filtering the neuronal data with a high-pass FIR filter (pass: > 400 Hz, stop: 300 Hz, suppression: 80 dB) in forward and backward directions to obtain the high-frequency components of the signal. The high-passed signal was full-wave-rectified and low-pass filtered with the same FIR filter as used for obtaining the LFP in forward and backward directions. Subsequently, the ESA signal was downsampled to 1,000 Hz.
Spectral analysis
The spectral decomposition of CSD, LFP, and ESA signals was performed by convolving them with complex Morlet’s wavelets, Ψ(t,f0) = A exp.(−t2/2σt2) exp.(2iπf0t), with σf = 1/πσt and (Kronland-Martinet et al., 1987; Torrence and Compo, 1998; Taylor et al., 2005). The wavelets were normalized such that the total energy was 1, requiring the normalization factor A to be . The center frequencies f0 of the daughter wavelets were set between 5 and 160 Hz based on a scheme of Torrence and Compo (1998). The time-frequency resolved power spectral density was then calculated by taking the square of the absolute value of the result of the convolution and dividing it by the Nyquist frequency of 500 Hz (Taylor et al., 2005; Drebitz et al., 2018). If not stated otherwise, we corrected for the bias of the PSD by multiplying the PSD values with the central frequency of the corresponding wavelet. Time-frequency resolved power spectral density was calculated separately for each session and attentional condition. Power spectra were calculated by taking the mean of all power values for the same frequency across time within MCs 2 and 3.
Estimation of the dominant γ-frequency
The dominant γ-frequency for the CSD signal of the hotspot electrode (see Data selection) of a given columnar domain was calculated for each trial based on the average γ-oscillatory cycle period. Only γ-oscillations with more than average amplitude during MCs 2 and 3 were considered. We first filtered the CSD signals with a broad bandpass filter (FIR-filter, passband: 35 – 120 Hz, stop frequencies: 25 Hz and 140 Hz, suppression: 20 dB) in forward and backward directions. The band-limited CSD signals were Hilbert transformed to calculate instantaneous phase and amplitude of the γ-oscillations (Le Van Quyen et al., 2001; Rosenblum et al., 2001). The time course of the amplitude was smoothed with a Gaussian filter (). In time spans of at least 40 ms duration in which the amplitude continuously exceeds the median amplitude value during MCs 2 and 3 of the trial, we determined the points in time when the γ-oscillations’ troughs occurred, i.e., when the instantaneous phase crosses . Since this happened usually between the phase values sampled at discrete times, we determined the crossing of using linear interpolation. The period lengths of the γ-oscillatory cycles were calculated as the time difference between successive troughs. Few periods incompatible with frequencies within the filter’s pass band (i.e., >1/35 s or < 1/120 s) were discarded. For each session and each attentional condition, we determined the median γ-cycle period based on all periods calculated as described above. We calculated the dominant frequency as the reciprocal of the median γ cycle period.
Estimation of phase coherence
We quantified the phase coherence (PhC) at time and frequency using the phase locking value (PLV), as shown in Equation 1:
The phase values and at times and frequency were obtained from the convolution of CSD- and ESA-signals with complex Morlet’s wavelets (see Spectral Decomposition).
To allow for the comparison of measurements with different numbers of trials, we corrected the PhC values for their trial-number-dependent bias by subtracting the expected value () as described in Equation 2. The EV is the level of PhC expected for N trials with random phase relations (Grothe et al., 2012), which is given by:
Equation 1 describes the PhC calculation for a CSD-ESA signal pair. The calculation for two CSD signals follows the same procedure, with the second CSD signal replacing the ESA signal.
To describe the PhC between the CSD signal of a hotspot and ESA or CSD signals across the entire column, we calculated spectrolaminar PhC profiles using ESA data from all channels surviving the inclusion criteria (see Data selection). For the spectrolaminar PhC profiles based on CSD signals across the column, we excluded electrode combinations involving the same signal (i.e., the electrode representing the respective domain’s hotspot) as well as signals from the two adjacent electrodes from further analysis. Average PhC depth profiles for each domain’s hotspot were calculated across the PhC depth profiles of all recording sessions of both animals aligned according to their depth by taking the median for the PhC with corresponding depth and frequency. For the average PhC depth profiles based on CSD signals across the column, channels at a given depth with data from less than two-thirds of the total recording sessions (typically around the hotspot taken as reference) were omitted. To quantify attentional modulation of PhC between the hotspots in the three laminar domains, we calculated the average γ-PhC (42.5–130.5 Hz) during MCs two and three for the ‘attend in and ‘attend nearby’ conditions separately for each recording session and laminar domain pair in both animals. For each pair, the first CSD signal was taken from the hotspot electrode within the respective domain (for more details, see “Data Selection”). The second electrode, located in a different laminar domain, was selected based on the highest γ-PhC (average γ-band PhC during ‘attend in’ conditions, MCs 2/3) with the hotspot electrode.
Statistics
Before pooling data across animals, we ensured that the data from both animals was not statistically different (p > 0.05). Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were conducted for each analysis involving pooled data to verify that the datasets were comparable.
The statistical procedures to determine significance are denoted alongside each comparison in brackets. These procedures included Kruskal-Wallis tests for assessing significant differences between multiple independent sample populations (at p < 0.05), initially conducted on unpaired data to address the multiple comparisons issue due to comparisons between three hotspots. Subsequently, Dunn’s tests were applied to identify significantly distinct sample groups (p < 0.05). For the groups where Dunn’s test indicated significance, we conducted a one-sided post-hoc Wilcoxon rank-sum test, providing the pertinent statistical parameters.
For the statistical evaluation of paired data, we initially conducted a Friedman’s test to determine whether there were statistically significant differences among the related groups. Subsequently, we employed post-hoc Wilcoxon-signed rank tests (two-sided). To control for the type I errors due to multiple comparisons, we applied the Tukey–Kramer, or Bonferroni corrections to adjust the p-values.
Results
To investigate the laminar pattern and characteristics of γ-oscillatory activity within the cortical column in area V1, we recorded neuronal activity using linear multi-contact probes while monkeys performed a demanding shape-tracking task (Figures 1A,B). It required attending one of up to four stimuli with complex shapes (Figure 1A) that change continuously from one shape to another within one second. After two to four of these morphing cycles (MC), monkeys had to respond to the reoccurrence of the initial shape of the behaviorally relevant target stimulus (Figure 1B). One of the stimuli was placed within the RFs of the neurons recorded within a V1 column and was either the target or a non-relevant distractor stimulus (Figure 1A).
Three distinct hotspots of γ-band activity within V1 columns
To examine the vertical profile of oscillatory neuronal activity in the γ-band within V1 columns, we calculated the time-resolved CSD along a linear multi-contact probe and its power spectrum during MCs 2 and 3. The depth profiles of these power spectra for the individual sessions of both monkeys indicate the presence of several well-separated hotspots of high power in the γ-band along the recorded columns’ vertical axis (see Figure 2A, left column for examples). The observations from individual sessions were confirmed after averaging the aligned and normalized power depth profiles of each monkey’s recording sessions. We observed a distinct peak within each of the three columnar domains (supragranular, granular, and infragranular) in 30 out of 32 recording sessions from both animals. However, in two sessions, either a discernible peak in the granular or infragranular domain was absent. On average, across all sessions, we found three distinct peaks of γ-power for each animal, one in each major laminar domain of the column, i.e., in the supragranular, the granular, and the infragranular layers. The laminar profile of the maximal γ-power across all electrodes confirms the presence of these three peaks in both monkeys (Figure 2B, right column, red curves). For comparison, we show the corresponding depth profiles of power spectra calculated from the LFP signals. Inspection of the profiles obtained for the individual sessions (see Figure 2A, right column for examples) shows, on the one hand, that part of the peaks observed in the corresponding CSD profiles occur at similar locations and frequencies in the LFP profiles. On the other hand, the regions with enhanced γ-power are spatially much more extended than the more localized CSD peaks. In line, the average depth profile of LFP spectra (Figure 2B, middle column) and the laminar profile of its maximal γ-power at each electrode (Figure 2B, right column, blue curve) show enhanced γ-power across extended regions along the column, with little evidence for three separate hotspots.
Figure 2. Cortical depth profile of CSD- and LFP-power in area V1. (A) The upper left panel displays the power (1/frequency corrected, maximum set to 1) of CSD signals averaged across MCs 2/3 for trials with attention directed toward the RFs of recorded neurons (‘attend in’). The panel represents the average of an exemplary recording session of monkey I (same as in Figure 1D). For the right panel, all conventions are equal, but power values are based on the LFP signal. The lower left panel displays the CSD power during the ‘attend in’ condition for an exemplary recording site of monkey II. The lower right panel displays the same but for the LFP power. All other conventions are the same as for Monkey I. (B) The upper left and middle panels show the same (CSD left, LFP right) as in (A) (upper panels) for the average across all recording sessions. The lower left and middle panels display the same for Monkey II. The rightmost panels show the average γ-power of all recording sites within the γ-band. The red graphs represent the maxima of γ-CSD power, and the blue graphs represent the maxima of γ-LFP power. The vertical dashed lines (white and black) indicate the border between supragranular and granular layers (upper) and granular and infragranular layers (lower), respectively.
γ-Frequency differs between γ-power hotspots in V1 columns
After identifying the three hotspots of γ-oscillatory activity in V1 columns, we asked whether they reflect three local groups of neurons operating during stimulus processing as a single, unified entity or, to some extent, functionally distinct ensembles. Therefore, we compared characteristics of neuronal γ-rhythmic activity between the three hotspots of V1 columns that could provide information on the mode of operation of the columnar networks. The precise frequency of the γ-oscillatory activity is one indicator that characterizes the dynamics of oscillatory networks. Identical γ-frequencies among the three hotspots’ CSD signals would be expected if they all reflect a shared population rhythm across the entire column, while separate local γ-oscillatory networks are less likely to have identical γ-frequencies. We computed the time-resolved CSD signal for all trials in a session. Subsequently, we calculated the dominant γ-frequency for each of the three attentional conditions and each of the three hotspots of each session of both monkeys, using trials with four stimuli shown (see methods).
The results (Figure 3A, left panel) show that the γ-frequencies at the three different hotspots differed significantly (Kruskal-Wallis test, = 19.63, p = 5.47116*10−5, nsup = 78, ngra = 75, ninf = 75). After multi-comparison correction, the observed differences between the γ-frequency at the supragranular and granular hotspots and between the granular and the infragranular hotspots were significant (post hoc Dunn’s test, α < 0.05). The median γ-peak frequency of 70.7 Hz at the granular hotspot was about 4 Hz higher than at the supragranular hotspot with 67.0 Hz and about 5 Hz higher than at the infragranular hotspot with 65.3 Hz (Wilcoxon rank sum tests, Z = 2.958, p = 0.0031 and Z = 4.1534, p = 3.2758*10−5, respectively). No significant difference was found between the γ-frequency at the supragranular and infragranular hotspot.
Figure 3. Comparison of spectral characteristics of the CSD signals γ-oscillations at the hotspot of the supragranular-, granular, and infragranular domain of both animals in V1. (A) The left panel shows the distributions of dominant γ-frequencies across recording sessions. Highlighted in red, blue, and brown are the distributions for the supragranular, granular, and infragranular domains, respectively. The right panel shows the distribution of the dominant γ-frequency of the supragranular CSD signals for the three attentional conditions. (B) Distribution of the change in γ-power for the ‘attend in’ (left) and ‘attend nearby’ (right) conditions compared to the ‘attend away’ condition. ** indicate significance at p < 0.01, and * at p < 0.05.
The dominant γ-frequencies at the hotspots of the three domains exhibit considerable variability between different recording sessions (Figure 3A). However, significant positive correlations were observed between the dominant γ-frequencies of supragranular and granular hotspots (r = 0.3264, p = 0.0051, Bonferroni-corrected p = 0.0154, n = 72) and between those of infragranular and granular hotspots, although the latter did not survive the multicomparison correction (r = 0.2712, p = 0.0212, Bonferroni-corrected p = 0.0636, n = 72). These results suggest that if γ-band activity in the granular domain oscillates at a comparably high frequency, the supragranular and infragranular domains in the same column also tend to oscillate at comparably high frequencies. No significant correlation was found between the dominant γ-frequencies of supragranular and infragranular hotspots, even before the multicomparison correction (r = 0.1487, p = 0.2125, n = 72). All comparisons were performed using Pearson’s correlation.
Effects of attention differ between γ-power hotspots in V1 columns
If neurons within V1 columns operate to some extent in functionally distinct ensembles, they might be differentially affected by the direction of selective attention. Therefore, we investigated whether γ-frequency and γ-power differed between the three different attentional conditions (“attend in,” “attend nearby,” and “attend away,” see Figure 1 for illustration). Again, we used the trial-based, time-resolved CSD signal. However, now we separately analyzed the three attentional conditions in each session of both animals and determined the dominant γ-frequency for each hotspot and attentional condition. We found an effect of attention for the supragranular hotspot (Friedman test, = 11.62, p = 0.003, n = 26). Attending the stimulus in the RF results in a median γ-frequency of 68.2 Hz, which is significantly higher than the 66.3 Hz when attending the closely neighboring stimulus or 66.0 Hz when attending stimuli in the opposite hemifield (Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, Z = 2.4255, p = 0.0459 and Z = 2.9589, p = 0.0093, respectively; p-values are Tukey–Kramer corrected). In contrast, no significant effect of attention on γ-frequency was observed for the granular and infragranular hotspots. Thus, the results indicate that attention differently affects the frequency of the γ-oscillatory activity at the three hotspots. Its major effect is a small frequency increase of about 2 Hz at the supragranular hotspot when attention is directed to the stimulus within the RF.
In addition to the different effects of attention on γ-frequency at the three hotspots, their γ-power was also affected differently by attention. Again, we used the electrode centered on each hotspot. The relative increase of γ-power when attending the stimulus within the RF compared to attending stimuli in the opposite hemifield differed significantly between the three hotspots (Figure 3B, left panel; Kruskal-Wallis test, = 6.1191, p = 0.0469, n = 23). In this condition, the median attention-dependent increase of γ-power was 38.1 % at the supragranular hotspot, 28.5 % at the granular hotspot, and 16.0 % at the infragranular hotspot. The difference between the attentional modulations at the supragranular and infragranular hotspots was statistically significant (post hoc Dunn’s test, α < 0.05 and Wilcoxon rank sum test, Z = 2.2848, p = 0.0223).
Similarly, directing attention to a stimulus near the RF as compared to the opposite hemifield also had differential effects on the γ-power of the three hotspots (Figure 3B right panel; Kruskal-Wallis test, = 22.8595, p = 1.0867*10−5). In this condition, the relative changes as compared to the ‘attend away’ condition were generally smaller, with 12.8 % at the supragranular hotspot, 7.5 % at the granular hotspot, and − 4.1 % at the infragranular hotspot. Again, the difference between the attentional modulations at the supragranular and infragranular hotspots was statistically significant (post hoc Dunn’s test, α < 0.05 and Wilcoxon rank sum test, Z = 3.698, p = 2.6245*10−4).
Taken together, the three distinct hotspots of strong γ-oscillatory activity in cortical columns in V1 exhibit different characteristics not only in terms of their dominant γ-frequency but also in terms of the strength by which attention modulates their γ-frequency as well as their γ-power. This suggests that separate networks within these three major laminar domains engage in distinct γ-oscillations, which differ in frequency and the effect of attention on their spectral properties.
Intra-columnar synchronization indicates separate γ-oscillatory networks
Suppose V1 columns comprise three distinct networks, each exhibiting its own γ-oscillatory dynamics. In that case, we expected that the activity of neurons that belong to the network would show stronger coupling to its γ-rhythm compared to the γ-rhythms of the other two networks. If, on the other hand, neurons across the entire column function as a unified ensemble with a single common γ-rhythm, then the activity of neurons throughout the column should exhibit similar coupling to this shared rhythm, regardless of where it is measured.
To test these predictions, we calculated the phase coherence (PhC) of multi-unit activity of each electrode with the three population rhythms. Subsequently, we investigated the columnar PhC patterns. As a measure of multi-unit activity, we used the entire spiking activity (ESA), which is a continuous signal and more sensitive to low amplitude spikes than measures requiring thresholding (Drebitz et al., 2019, 2020; Ahmadi et al., 2021a,b). The ESA signals vary considerably in strength across cortical depth but are similar in spectral composition (see Figure 4C for an exemplary ESA-power depth profile).
Figure 4. Depth profiles of phase coherence (PhC) of supragranular, granular and infragranular CSD with the entire spiking activity (ESA) at each electrode of both animals. (A) The left heatmap shows the average PhC depth profile between the CSD signal of the γ-power hotspot in the supragranular domain (red title) with the ESA across all electrodes of all layers. The middle and rightmost heatmaps show the same for the CSD signals of the granular (blue title) and infragranular (brown title) hotspots, respectively. The vertical dashed lines indicate the borders between supragranular and granular layers and between granular and infragranular layers. The rightmost panel shows the average γ-PhC (28–78 Hz) across depth for each of the three CSD signals with ESA across depth, as the ratio between each PhC value and the sum of the PhC values at the same depth. The lines are colored in the same hue as the corresponding heatmap’s title. (B) The left panel shows the distributions of the sessions’ average γ-PhC between ESA and the CSD signal of one of the three hotspots (depicted on the abscissa) across the supragranular electrodes. The white dot depicts the median γ-PhCs and the upper and lower edges of the black vertical line the 25th and 75th percentile, respectively. The middle and right panels show the corresponding combinations of granular and infragranular ESA signals with the CSD signal of γ-oscillatory activity at each of the three hotspot electrodes. (C) The heatmap depicts the average ESA power across a column for all ‘attend in’-conditions of an exemplary recording session of monkey II. * Indicates significance between 0.01 < p < 0.05, ** indicates significance at p < 0.01.
The spectral patterns of PhC across cortical depth reveal that the CSD signal of each hotspot was maximally coherent with ESA from the same laminar domain with a peak in the γ-band (Figure 4A, left three plots). Consequently, the set of ESA signals of a domain showed maximum γ-PhC with the CSD signal of the same domain and weaker γ-PhC with the CSD signals of the other two domains (Figure 4B). All but two of these differences were statistically significant (Table 1).
Table 1. Statistical comparison of the strength of γ-PhC between the major laminar domain’s ESA signals and the three CSD signals from γ-power hotspots.
Since ESA and its power in the γ-band varied strongly with cortical depth, the depth profile of PhC is correspondingly biased. Therefore, we calculated the mean PhC between ESA and each of the three CSD signals at each depth and averaged values within the γ-band. We normalized these three PhC-values for each recording site by dividing them by the sum of the three values (Figure 4A, right panel).
The results reveal that ESA signals throughout the supragranular domain synchronize mostly with the γ-rhythm of the supragranular hotspot and in much smaller proportions with the γ-rhythm of the other two hotspots (Figure 4A, right panel, red curve). At the border to the granular domain, the ESA’s PhC with the supragranular hotspot steps down to a lower level, which becomes even lower for ESA in the infragranular domain. A similar pattern occurs for the γ-rhythm at the infragranular hotspot. ESA signals throughout the infragranular domain synchronize mostly in similarly high proportions with this γ-rhythm (brown curve) and in much smaller proportions with the γ-rhythms of the other two hotspots. Above the border between infragranular and granular layers, the PhC of ESA with the infragranular γ-rhythm quickly declines. The fairly similar proportions of PhC by which the spiking activity of neurons throughout both laminar domains follow predominantly the γ-rhythm measured at the hotspot of the same domain suggests that the neurons across each domain belong to a unified neuronal ensemble and follow a domain-specific, common γ-rhythm.
The depth profile of ESA’s relative PhC with the γ-rhythm of the granular hotspot has a maximum close to the upper border of the granular domain, from where it gradually declines to the lower border and stays smaller throughout the infragranular domain (Figure 4A, right panel, blue curve). Above the upper border, ESA’s relative PhC declines quickly. Although the granular hotspot’s PhC is driven predominantly by spiking within the granular layer, this proportion is very similar to the PhC proportion associated with the supragranular hotspot’s γ-rhythm. This pattern indicates that the γ-oscillatory network underlying hotspot of γ-power in the upper part of the granular domain might not extend homogeneously throughout the entire granular domain but is mainly located in its upper part.
In summary, the pattern of the PhC between the spiking activity across the column and the three hotspots of γ-power indicates three separate networks in the major laminar domains that engage in γ-oscillatory activity. Typically, the spiking activity of such a network’s neurons locks predominantly to their own network’s rhythm and less to the rhythms of the other networks.
Attention modulates the intra-columnar synchronization within the γ-band
After demonstrating that the γ-CSD signals of a hotspot mainly represent the same domain’s γ-oscillatory activity and that the power of these local γ-oscillatory activities can be modulated differently by attention, we examined whether attention also affects the strength of interlaminar γ-synchronization. Figure 5A shows the depth profiles of PhC between the CSD signal of each domain’s hotspot and the CSD signals of all other electrodes for the two conditions where attention was either directed to the stimulus within the recorded neurons’ RF (‘attend in’ left column) or to the stimulus close to, but outside, the RFs (‘attend nearby’ right column). Similar to comparisons between the CSD signals of the hotspot of each domain and ESA signals across depth (Figure 4), we observed strong γ-PhC between the granular and supragranular domains (Figure 5A, upper and middle rows) and much weaker PhC between the infragranular hotspot’s γ-activity and that of the other two domains (lower row). Furthermore, we found that PhC in the γ-band increased with attention between all three domains (Figure 5B).
Figure 5. Attention-dependent modulation of phase coherence between γ-oscillatory hotspots and other sites across the column (CSD-CSD PhC). (A) The heat maps show the depth profile of the average PhC between the supragranular (first row), the granular (second row), and the infragranular (third row) hotspots’ CSD signal with the CSD signals of electrodes across all layers. Coherence values of hotspot channels with themselves are omitted, resulting in the gray regions where the hotspots’ CSD signals were taken (see methods). In the left column, depth profiles show the average PhC pattern for the ‘attend in’ condition, with attention focused on the stimulus within the RF of the recorded neurons. The right column shows the average PhC pattern when attention is located on the stimulus close to but outside the RF (‘attend nearby’). (B) The three depth profiles show the average differences between the PhC-depth profiles of the ‘attend in’ and ‘attend nearby’ conditions shown in (A). (C) The scatter plots show for each recording site of both animals the average γ-PhC (42.5–130.5 Hz) during the “attend in” condition plotted against the average γ-PhC during the “attend nearby” condition for all possible combinations of the three laminar domains. The titles indicate the domain pair for which γ-PhC is estimated, with the first CSD signal indicating the hotspot location and the second indicating the location of the electrode with the highest γ-PhC with the hotspot electrode is used.
The comparison of γ-PhC between the CSD signal of the hotspot of one domain and the electrode in a different domain showing the highest γ-PhC with the hotspot across all recording sites of both animals revealed significant differences between all compared pairs (Figure 5C). The median γ-PhC between the supragranular and infragranular pair increased with attention by 0.014 (Figure 5C upper plot; γ-PhCattend in = 0.121, γ-PhCattend nearby = 0.107, n = 32, p = 4.2204*10−05, z = 4.0951), by 0.025 between the granular and supragranular pair (middle plot; γ-PhCattend in = 0.283, γ-PhCattend nearby = 0.258, n = 31, p = 0.0019598, z = 3.0963), and by 0.02 between the infragranular and granular pair (lower plot; γ-PhCattend in = 0.09, γ-PhCattend nearby = 0.07, n = 31, p = 6.9469*10−05, z = 3.9781; all comparisons Wilcoxon signed-rank tests). This additive effect of attention on γ-PhC between laminar domains did not differ significantly between the different pairs (Kruskal-Wallis test, = 0.5, p = 0.7803, nsup/infra = 32, ngra/supra = 31, ninf/gran = 31).
Discussion
Our study shows that V1 columns contain three distinct regions characterized by robust γ-oscillatory activity. These regions are localized in the supragranular, granular, and infragranular layers. The γ-oscillatory activities at these three locations exhibit notable differences in their characteristics. This concerns the dominant γ-frequency, which is significantly higher at the granular layer’s hotspot than at the supragranular and infragranular layers’ hotspots. Furthermore, attention increased the γ-frequency at the hotspot in the supragranular layers, while we observed no significant differences at the other two hotspots. Attention also changed the γ-power differentially across the three sites, with the strongest increase observed in supragranular layers and much smaller increases observed in infragranular layers. We also found that the spiking activity in the three laminar domains was more coherent with the CSD signal of the hotspot in the same domain but much less with the CSD signals of other domains, and in addition, that the strength of PhC between all three hotspots was increased by attention.
Observing three distinct hotspots of high γ-power in CSD signals along the depth profile of V1 columns suggests that γ-oscillatory activity originates in three separate groups of neurons within a column. In good agreement, several previous studies showed a non-uniform distribution of γ-oscillatory activity in LFP across the cortical layers in area V1 (Maier et al., 2010; Spaak et al., 2012; Xing et al., 2012; Van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Lowet et al., 2017; Han et al., 2021; Kienitz et al., 2021) and in extrastriate visual areas (Buffalo et al., 2011; Scheeringa and Fries, 2019; Gieselmann and Thiele, 2022) of cats, macaques and in the human cortex (Csercsa et al., 2010). Mendoza-Halliday and colleagues even observed a canonical pattern of laminar oscillatory activity across multiple cortical areas in macaques and other primates. They reported a gradual increase in LFP γ-power from deep to superficial layers, consistently peaking in the supragranular layers (Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2024).
However, all these LFP-based studies reported less than three peaks along the cortical depth. They typically showed smoother spatial patterns that vary considerably between studies: Peaks in V1 LFP γ-band power have been reported for example in supragranular and infragranular layers (Van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Lowet et al., 2017; Gieselmann and Thiele, 2022), or only in granular (Maier et al., 2010) or supragranular layers (Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2024). In addition, the laminar location of the LFP’s γ-power peak has been shown to shift between transient and sustained stimulus periods (Xing et al., 2012).
This variability in the laminar distribution of LFP γ-power likely reflects differences across studies in factors such as the stimuli used, the attentional demands of the behavioral task, and the recording and analysis procedures. In addition, when we compare the LFP- and CSD-based depth profiles in the present study, we find that the limited spatial resolution of LFP signals prevents the detection of all three γ-oscillators within a cortical column.
The presence of three hotspots of γ-oscillatory activity in the CSD raises the question of whether the three corresponding groups of neurons operate in a functionally fully integrated mode in one ensemble, forming one γ-oscillatory network throughout the column or whether they instead reflect three separate, even though flexibly interacting ensembles, each creating a γ-oscillatory network on its own. Several properties of the γ-oscillatory activity originating at the three hotspots suggest the latter possibility.
First, the dominant γ-frequency at the granular hotspot is significantly higher than the γ-frequencies of the supra- and infragranular hotspots. The corresponding neurons should follow the same rhythm if they belong to the same coherently oscillating ensemble. Even if this rhythm varies in instantaneous frequency, the average frequency should have been the same throughout the ensemble. Instead, the observed differences in γ-frequency suggest that the neurons from which the CSD signals at the three hotspots originate belong to different networks, each with its individual γ-oscillatory dynamics.
A second important difference between the neurons of these three networks concerns the effects of attention. Attending the stimulus processed by neurons throughout the column enhanced the γ-frequency of the supragranular network significantly but not of the two other γ-oscillatory networks. This finding may also help to explain discrepancies between previous studies in V1, which in part observed attention-dependent frequency changes in the γ-band of comparable effect sizes (Bosman et al., 2012; Das and Ray, 2018), while others report no significant changes after accounting for multi-comparisons (Ray and Maunsell, 2010). Differences in the frequencies of recordings with conventional microelectrodes in different layers and the spatial averaging of LFP signals might contribute to these divergent outcomes. Ferro et al. (2021) reported a significant γ-frequency increase in V1 when pooling γ-frequency estimates based on LFP recordings from linear electrode arrays across all layers, but only slight differences in spectral signatures of attention between layers.
Besides the dominant γ-frequency of the CSD signals, selective attention also modulated the γ-power differently in the three networks. The attention-dependent increase ranged from 16 % at the infragranular hotspot to 38.1 % at the supragranular hotspot. Interestingly, prior research reported either an inconsistent or negligible impact of attention on V1 γ-power (Buffalo et al., 2011; Bosman et al., 2012; Ferro et al., 2021), observed even a suppressive effect (Chalk et al., 2010; Herrero et al., 2013), or showed comparatively small increases of γ-power in macaques (Rohenkohl et al., 2018) and humans (Magazzini and Singh, 2018). The particularly strong effects in the present study might partly reflect our task’s very high cognitive demands: continuously tracking the dynamic transformations of the target stimulus’s shape until its initial shape reappears in the presence of a closely spaced distractor, and while the luminance of all stimuli changed randomly and quickly. In contrast, previous tasks required detecting changes in color or orientation of a grating stimulus or the sudden appearance of a bright spot on a darker bar. At the same time, the stronger effect might be due to the high spatial resolution of CSD signals, which reduces the diminishing of power modulations by spatial averaging with less modulated or unmodulated signals.
However, the pronounced differences observed for the attention-dependent modulation of γ-frequency and γ-power between the three hotspots of a V1 column fit the expectation for separate γ-oscillatory networks with distinct functional roles that are, therefore, separately and differently influenced by selective attention.
Direct evidence for three networks, each generating its γ-rhythm, comes from the PhC analysis between the highly local entire spiking activity (ESA) acquired throughout the entire column depth and each CSD signal from the three hotspots of γ-power. While the raw PhC between, e.g., the supragranular hotspot’s CSD signal and the ESA at different electrodes varies abruptly, strongly depending on the strength and spectral characteristics of the different ESA signals, the proportions between an ESA signal’s PhCs with the three different CSD signals reveal the spatial organization of the three γ-oscillatory networks. The comparatively high and similar proportion of PhC between spiking activity throughout supra- and infragranular layers with the corresponding hotspots in these laminar domains indicates that neurons throughout each domain engage in a local γ-rhythm. The same holds for the γ-rhythm of the granular hotspot that predominantly reflects the rhythm of neurons’ spiking activity in the granular layers’ upper part. The γ-oscillatory CSD signals at the three hotspots reflect these population rhythms. They are specific for the three different networks, as indicated by the differences in their γ-frequency and the weaker PhC between neurons’ spiking activity and the population rhythms of the other networks compared to their own network’s rhythm.
These results indicate a notable degree of independence between the dynamics of the different γ-oscillatory networks, a trait commonly associated with weakly coupled γ-oscillatory networks (Schuster and Wagner, 1990; Hoppensteadt and Izhikevich, 1998; Kopell and Ermentrout, 2002; Palmigiano et al., 2017). Typically, such dynamics emerge in networks with stronger connections in local groups of neurons that engage in oscillatory dynamics and weaker connections between such local oscillatory networks (Kopell and Ermentrout, 1986; Wang, 1995; Palmigiano et al., 2017). The intracolumnar connectivity in V1 resembles such an architecture. Taking the supragranular layers as an example, most of their neurons’ synaptic inputs (~55 %) originate from intralaminar connections. In contrast, less than half of this percentage (~20 %) arises from granular layers, with a mere ~10 % originating from infragranular layers. The remaining input is traced back to other cortical areas (Dantzker and Callaway, 2000; Binzegger et al., 2009; Lindén et al., 2011; Potjans and Diesmann, 2014). Thus, the known connectivity of the intracolumnar microcircuitry shows critical properties expected if the γ-oscillatory networks in the different laminar domains behave like weakly coupled oscillatory networks.
In support of separate oscillatory networks within different layers, optogenetic stimulation of individual layers in mouse V1 showed that rhythm-generating circuits exist in all major domains of the cortex and can entrain γ-oscillatory activity with distinct γ-peak frequencies (Adesnik, 2018).
The observed differences in γ-peak frequencies across layers, whether due to optical stimulation (Adesnik, 2018) or, as in our case, during sensory stimulation—might be attributed to variations in the connectivity, physiological properties, and the composition of neurons within each laminar network (Veit et al., 2017; Naka et al., 2019; Kooijmans et al., 2020).
It seems intriguing that local columnar networks within V1 exhibit weakly coupled oscillatory behavior even though they are thought to transmit and process stimulus information sequentially from granular to supragranular and infragranular layers without obvious necessity for selection and signal gating. However, this disposition toward weak coupling yields valuable degrees of independence that can enhance information processing efficiency:
(1) A gradual modulation of phase coherence and phase difference between, e.g., the granular and the supragranular networks’ γ-oscillations could implement an adjustable gain factor for the input signal processed in a V1 column before it is passed to subsequent processing stages. Such adjustable gain factors are required, e.g., by divisive normalization models for flexibly weighting different input signals (Carandini and Heeger, 2012; Kreiter, 2020). Furthermore, such a flexible gain modulation within a cortical column may provide another possibility to modulate signal- and information routing at the columnar level and is supported by the observation of attention-dependent changes of Granger causality between columnar networks (Ferro et al., 2021). Our finding of an attention-dependent increase in coupling strength between laminar oscillators supports this idea. Although our task design, with one stimulus in the RF and a second stimulus nearby but outside the RF, does not impose strong demands of input selection or horizontal information integration onto the V1 columnar processing network, we still observe a consistent, albeit small, increase in coupling strength in the γ-band between all laminar domains. This suggests that the changing relevance of the stimulus imposes different demands on the network, which are met by small adjustments in coupling strength across the columnar γ-oscillators. In line, it has been shown that the coupling strength in the γ-band between two networks changes much stronger with attention if the receiving network receives multiple competing inputs and a selection of inputs for processing is required (Bosman et al., 2012; Grothe et al., 2012).
(2) The same basic mechanisms might serve to change the pattern of functional connectivity between the column’s laminar networks and, thereby, the proportions between the weights of intra-columnar inputs and extra-columnar inputs from neighboring columns as well as from bottom-up or top-down inputs originating outside of V1. For example, reduced coupling between granular and supragranular layers could allow for stronger weighting of the supragranular network’s interactions with neighboring columns via horizontal connections. This could promote the local binding of stimulus features for integrating stimulus information from the classical RF and its surround (Vinck and Bosman, 2016; Lowet et al., 2017).
(3) It is known that transmitting signals along a chain of weakly coupled oscillators enhances robustness to perturbations and decreases the likelihood of cascade failures (Cosp et al., 2004; Rode et al., 2019; Csaba and Porod, 2020). The intra-columnar signal pathways across γ-oscillatory networks in different layers could also benefit from this effect.
Future research will be required to understand the possible functional role of the flexible modulation of functional connectivity between different layers in columnar signal- and information processing in the context of different demands due to changing stimulus constellations and task requirements.
We identified three distinct, local γ-oscillatory networks within V1 columns, each in a different laminar domain. Their γ-oscillatory activity revealed notable differences concerning the modulation of their spectral features by selective attention. The phase coherence patterns within and between the networks show characteristics of weakly coupled oscillatory networks. This allows their functional connectivity to change by changing coherence and phase relations between their γ-oscillations. It implies various possible mechanisms that could support cortical information processing at the columnar level. The findings indicate that neurons within a V1 column do not operate as a single, unified ensemble but in multiple γ-oscillatory ensembles with considerable potential for flexible reconfiguration of the columnar functional networks. Because of the anatomical and histological similarities across the cortex, we anticipate that these findings will likely extend to other cortical areas.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal study was approved by the Senator für Gesundheit, Bremen, Germany. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.
Author contributions
ED: Writing – original draft. L-PR: Writing – review & editing. EDG: Writing – review & editing. AK: Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)-331514942.
Acknowledgments
We thank Iris Grothe for the valuable discussions on the manuscript. We also thank Aleksandra Nadolski, Peter Bujotzek, and Katja Taylor for training and technical assistance. Furthermore, we want to acknowledge Katrin Thoß and Ramazani Hakizimana for their diligent animal care.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Adesnik, H. (2018). Layer-specific excitation/inhibition balances during neuronal synchronization in the visual cortex. J. Physiol. 596, 1639–1657. doi: 10.1113/JP274986
Aertsen, A. M., Gerstein, G. L., Habib, M. K., and Palm, G. (1989). Dynamics of neuronal firing correlation: modulation of “effective connectivity”. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 900–917. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.5.900
Ahmadi, N., Constandinou, T. G., and Bouganis, C.-S. (2021a). Inferring entire spiking activity from local field potentials. Sci. Rep. 11:19045. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-98021-9
Ahmadi, N., Constandinou, T. G., and Bouganis, C.-S. (2021b). Robust and accurate decoding of hand kinematics from entire spiking activity using deep learning. J. Neural Eng. 18:026011. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/abde8a
Ahmed, B., Cordery, P. M., McLelland, D., Bair, W., and Krug, K. (2012). Long-range clustered connections within extrastriate visual area V5/MT of the rhesus macaque. Cereb. Cortex 22, 60–73. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr072
Alonso, J. M. (2016). The geometry of visual cortical maps. Neuron 91, 716–718. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.001
Azouz, R., and Gray, C. M. (2003). Adaptive coincidence detection and dynamic gain control in visual cortical neurons in vivo. Neuron 37, 513–523. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01186-8
Barbas, H., Zikopoulos, B., and John, Y. J. (2022). The inevitable inequality of cortical columns. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 16:921468. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.921468
Battaglia, D., Witt, A., Wolf, F., and Geisel, T. (2012). Dynamic effective connectivity of inter-areal brain circuits. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8:e1002438. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002438
Bijanzadeh, M., Nurminen, L., Merlin, S., Clark, A. M., and Angelucci, A. (2018). Distinct laminar processing of local and global context in primate primary visual cortex. Neuron 100, 259–274.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.020
Binzegger, T., Douglas, R. J., and Martin, K. A. C. (2009). Topology and dynamics of the canonical circuit of cat V1. Neural Netw. 22, 1071–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2009.07.011
Blasdel, G., and Campbell, D. (2001). Functional retinotopy of monkey visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 21, 8286–8301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08286.2001
Borgers, C., and Kopell, N. J. (2008). Gamma oscillations and stimulus selection. Neural Comput. 20, 383–414. doi: 10.1162/neco.2007.07-06-289
Bosman, C., Schoffelen, J.-M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., et al. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron 75, 875–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037
Briggs, F., Mangun, G. R., and Usrey, W. M. (2013). Attention enhances synaptic efficacy and the signal-to-noise ratio in neural circuits. Nature 499, 476–480. doi: 10.1038/nature12276
Brosch, M., Bauer, R., and Eckhorn, R. (1997). Stimulus-dependent modulations of correlated high-frequency oscillations in cat visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 70–76. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.1.70
Buffalo, E. A., Fries, P., Landman, R., Buschman, T. J., and Desimone, R. (2011). Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 11262–11267. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011284108
Capone, F., Paolucci, M., Assenza, F., Brunelli, N., Ricci, L., Florio, L., et al. (2016). Canonical cortical circuits: current evidence and theoretical implications. Neurosci. Neuroecon. 5, 1–8. doi: 10.2147/NAN.S70816
Carandini, M., and Heeger, D. (2012). Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136
Chalk, M., Herrero, J. L., Gieselmann, M. A., Delicato, L. S., Gotthardt, S., and Thiele, A. (2010). Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1. Neuron 66, 114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013
Cosp, J., Madrenas, J., Alarcon, E., Vidal, E., and Villar, G. (2004). Synchronization of nonlinear electronic oscillators for neural computation, IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks. IEEE 15, 1315–1327. doi: 10.1109/TNN.2004.832808
Csaba, G., and Porod, W. (2020). Coupled oscillators for computing: a review and perspective. Appl. Phys. Rev. 7. doi: 10.1063/1.5120412
Csercsa, R., Dombovári, B., Fabó, D., Wittner, L., Erőss, L., Entz, L., et al. (2010). Laminar analysis of slow wave activity in humans. Brain 133, 2814–2829. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq169
Dantzker, J. L., and Callaway, E. M. (2000). Laminar sources of synaptic input to cortical inhibitory interneurons and pyramidal neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 701–707. doi: 10.1038/76656
Das, A., and Ray, S. (2018). Effect of stimulus contrast and visual attention on spike-gamma phase relationship in macaque primary visual cortex. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12:66. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00066
DiCarlo, J. J., and Johnson, K. O. (2000). Spatial and temporal structure of receptive fields in primate somatosensory area 3b: effects of stimulus scanning direction and orientation. J. Neurosci. Soc Neurosc. 20, 495–510. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00495.2000
Doesburg, S. M., Roggeveen, A. B., Kitajo, K., and Ward, L. M. (2007). Large-scale gamma-band phase synchronization and selective attention. Cereb. Cortex 18, 386–396. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm073
Douglas, R. J., and Martin, K. A. C. (2004). Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 419–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144152
Douglas, R. J., and Martin, K. A. C. (2007). Mapping the matrix: the ways of neocortex. Neuron 56, 226–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.017
Douglas, R. J., Martin, K. A. C., and Whitteridge, D. (1989). A canonical microcircuit for neocortex. Neural Comput. 1, 480–488. doi: 10.1162/neco.1989.1.4.480
Drebitz, E., Haag, M., Grothe, I., Mandon, S., and Kreiter, A. K. (2018). Attention configures synchronization within local neuronal networks for processing of the behaviorally relevant stimulus. Front. Neural Circ. 12:71. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00071
Drebitz, E., Rausch, L.-P., and Kreiter, A. K. (2020). A novel approach for removing micro-stimulation artifacts and reconstruction of broadband neuronal signals. J. Neurosci. Methods 332:108549. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2019.108549
Drebitz, E., Schledde, B., Kreiter, A. K., and Wegener, D. (2019). Optimizing the yield of multi-unit activity by including the entire spiking activity. Front. Neurosci. 13:83. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00083
Engel, A. K., Gerloff, C., Hilgetag, C. C., and Nolte, G. (2013). Intrinsic coupling modes: multiscale interactions in ongoing brain activity. Neuron 80, 867–886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.038
Ferro, D., van Kempen, J., Boyd, M., Panzeri, S., and Thiele, A. (2021). Directed information exchange between cortical layers in macaque V1 and V4 and its modulation by selective attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 118:e2022097118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2022097118
Franken, T. P., and Reynolds, J. H. (2021). Columnar processing of border ownership in primate visual cortex. eLife 10:e72573. doi: 10.7554/eLife.72573
Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034
Garg, A. K., Li, P., Rashid, M. S., and Callaway, E. M. (2019). Color and orientation are jointly coded and spatially organized in primate primary visual cortex. Science 364, 1275–1279. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw5868
Gattass, R., Nascimento-Silva, S., Soares, J. G. M., Lima, B., Jansen, A. K., Diogo, A. C. M., et al. (2005). Cortical visual areas in monkeys: location, topography, connections, columns, plasticity and cortical dynamics. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 709–731. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1629
Gieselmann, M. A., and Thiele, A. (2022). Stimulus dependence of directed information exchange between cortical layers in macaque V1. eLife 11:e62949. doi: 10.7554/eLife.62949
Gregoriou, G. G., Gotts, S. J., and Desimone, R. (2012). Cell-type-specific synchronization of neural activity in FEF with V4 during attention. Neuron 73, 581–594. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.019
Grossberg, S. (2001). Linking the laminar circuits of visual cortex to visual perception: development, grouping, and attention. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 513–526. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00030-6
Grothe, I., Neitzel, S. D., Mandon, S., and Kreiter, A. K. (2012). Switching neuronal inputs by differential modulations of gamma-band phase-coherence. J. Neurosci. 32, 16172–16180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0890-12.2012
Grothe, I., Rotermund, D., Neitzel, S. D., Mandon, S., Ernst, U. A., Kreiter, A. K., et al. (2018). Attention selectively gates afferent signal transmission to area V4. J. Neurosci. 38, 3441–3452. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2221-17.2018
Han, C., Wang, T., Yang, Y., Wu, Y., Li, Y., Dai, W., et al. (2021). Multiple gamma rhythms carry distinct spatial frequency information in primary visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 19:e3001466. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001466
Harnack, D., Ernst, U. A., and Pawelzik, K. R. (2015). A model for attentional information routing through coherence predicts biased competition and multistable perception. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 1593–1605. doi: 10.1152/jn.01038.2014
Harris, K. D., and Mrsic-Flogel, T. D. (2013). Cortical connectivity and sensory coding. Nature 503, 51–58. doi: 10.1038/nature12654
Herrero, J. L., Gieselmann, M. A., Sanayei, M., and Thiele, A. (2013). Attention-induced variance and noise correlation reduction in macaque V1 is mediated by NMDA receptors. Neuron 78, 729–739. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.029
Hirsch, J. A., and Martinez, L. M. (2006). Laminar processing in the visual cortical column. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 377–384. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.014
Hirsch, J. A., Martinez, L. M., Alonso, J. M., Desai, K., Pillai, C., and Pierre, C. (2002). Synaptic physiology of the flow of information in the cats visual cortex in vivo. J. Physiol. 540, 335–350. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012777
Hoppensteadt, F. C., and Izhikevich, E. M. (1998). Thalamo-cortical interactions modeled by weakly connected oscillators: could the brain use FM radio principles? Biosystems 48, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/S0303-2647(98)00053-7
Hosoya, T. (2019). The basic repeating modules of the cerebral cortical circuit. Proc. Jpn. Acad. B. 95, 303–311. doi: 10.2183/pjab.95.022
Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cats visual cortex. J. Physiol. 160:106.
Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1963). Shape and arrangement of columns in cats striate cortex. J. Physiol. 165:559.
Jones, E. G., and Rakic, P. (2010). Radial columns in cortical architecture: it is the composition that counts. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2261–2264. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq127
Kaschube, M., Schnabel, M., Löwel, S., Coppola, D. M., White, L. E., and Wolf, F. (2010). Universality in the evolution of orientation columns in the visual cortex. Science 330, 1113–1116. doi: 10.1126/science.1194869
Kienitz, R., Cox, M. A., Dougherty, K., Saunders, R. C., Schmiedt, J. T., Leopold, D. A., et al. (2021). Theta, but not gamma oscillations in area V4 depend on input from primary visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 31, 635–642.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.10.091
Kooijmans, R. N., Sierhuis, W., Self, M. W., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2020). A quantitative comparison of inhibitory interneuron size and distribution between mouse and macaque V1, using calcium-binding proteins. Cerebr. Cortex Commun. 1:tgaa068. doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgaa068
Kopell, N., and Ermentrout, G. B. (1986). Symmetry and phaselocking in chains of weakly coupled oscillators. Commun. Pure Appl. Math. 39, 623–660. doi: 10.1002/cpa.3160390504
Kopell, N., and Ermentrout, G. B. (2002). Mechanisms of phase-locking and frequency control in pairs of coupled neural oscillators. Handb. Dynam. Syst. 2, 3–54. doi: 10.1016/S1874-575X(02)80022-4
Kreiter, A. K. (2020). Synchrony, flexible network configuration, and linking neural events to behavior. Curr. Opin. Physio. 16, 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.08.008
Kronland-Martinet, R., Morlet, J., and Grossmann, A. (1987). Analysis of sound patterns through wavelet transforms. Int. J. Pattern Recognit. Artif. Intell. 1, 273–302. doi: 10.1142/S0218001487000205
Le Van Quyen, M., Foucher, J., Lachaux, J., Rodriguez, E., Lutz, A., Martinerie, J., et al. (2001). Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony. J. Neurosci. Methods 111, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(01)00372-7
Lennie, P., Krauskopf, J., and Sclar, G. (1990). Chromatic mechanisms in striate cortex of macaque. J. Neurosci. 10, 649–669. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-02-00649.1990
Li, P., Zhu, S., Chen, M., Han, C., Xu, H., Hu, J., et al. (2013). A motion direction preference map in monkey V4. Neuron 78, 376–388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.024
Lindén, H., Tetzlaff, T., Potjans, T. C., Pettersen, K. H., Grün, S., Diesmann, M., et al. (2011). Modeling the spatial reach of the LFP. Neuron 72, 859–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.006
Lisitsyn, D., Grothe, I., Kreiter, A. K., and Ernst, U. A. (2020). Visual stimulus content in V4 is conveyed by gamma-rhythmic information packages. J. Neurosci. 40, 9650–9662. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0689-20.2020
Livingstone, M. S., and Hubel, D. H. (1984). Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 4, 309–356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-01-00309.1984
Logothetis, N. K., Kayser, C., and Oeltermann, A. (2007). In vivo measurement of cortical impedance spectrum in monkeys: implications for signal propagation. Neuron 55, 809–823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.027
Lowet, E., Roberts, M. J., Peter, A., Gips, B., and De Weerd, P. (2017). A quantitative theory of gamma synchronization in macaque V1. eLife 6:e26642. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26642
Magazzini, L., and Singh, K. D. (2018). Spatial attention modulates visual gamma oscillations across the human ventral stream. Neuroimage 166, 219–229. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.069
Maier, A., Adams, G. K., Aura, C., and Leopold, D. A. (2010). Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:31. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031
Martinez, L. M., Alonso, J. M., Reid, R. C., and Hirsch, J. A. (2002). Laminar processing of stimulus orientation in cat visual cortex. J. Physiol. 540, 321–333. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012776
Mendoza-Halliday, D., Major, A. J., Lee, N., Lichtenfeld, M. J., Carlson, B., Mitchell, B., et al. (2024). A ubiquitous spectrolaminar motif of local field potential power across the primate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 27, 547–560. doi: 10.1038/s41593-023-01554-7
Mountcastle, V. B. (1957). Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cats somatic sensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 20, 408–434. doi: 10.1152/jn.1957.20.4.408
Mountcastle, V. B. (1997). The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain J. Neurol. 120, 701–722. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.4.701
Naka, A., Veit, J., Shababo, B., Chance, R. K., Risso, D., Stafford, D., et al. (2019). Complementary networks of cortical somatostatin interneurons enforce layer specific control. eLife 8:e43696. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43696
Niebur, E., and Koch, C. (1994). A model for the neuronal implementation of selective visual attention based on temporal correlation among neurons. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 141–158. doi: 10.1007/BF00962722
Palmigiano, A., Geisel, T., Wolf, F., and Battaglia, D. (2017). Flexible information routing by transient synchrony. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1014–1022. doi: 10.1038/nn.4569
Peterhans, E., and Von der Heydt, R. (1993). Functional organization of area V2 in the alert macaque. Eur. J. Neurosci. 5, 509–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1993.tb00517.x
Pettersen, K. H. (2005) CSDPlotter. Available at: https://github.com/espenhgn/CSDplotter (Accessed January 22, 2020).
Pettersen, K. H., Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dale, A. M., and Einevoll, G. T. (2006). Current-source density estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward solution: effects of finite extent of neuronal activity and conductivity discontinuities. J. Neurosci. Methods 154, 116–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.12.005
Poggio, G. F., Baker, F. H., Mansfield, R. J. W., Sillito, A., and Grigg, P. (1975). Spatial and chromatic properties of neurons subserving foveal and parafoveal vision in rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 100, 25–59. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90240-1
Potjans, T. C., and Diesmann, M. (2014). The cell-type specific cortical microcircuit: relating structure and activity in a full-scale spiking network model. Cereb. Cortex 24, 785–806. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs358
Ray, S., and Maunsell, J. H. R. (2010). Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation. Neuron 67, 885–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.004
Ringach, D. L., Shapley, R. M., and Hawken, M. J. (2002). Orientation selectivity in macaque V1: diversity and laminar dependence. J. Neurosci. 22, 5639–5651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05639.2002
Rockland, K. S. (2019). What do we know about laminar connectivity? Neuroimage 197, 772–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.032
Rockland, K. S., and Ichinohe, N. (2004). Some thoughts on cortical minicolumns. Exp. Brain Res. 158, 265–277. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2024-9
Rode, J., Totz, J. F., Fengler, E., and Engel, H. (2019). Chimera states on a ring of strongly coupled relaxation oscillators. Front. Appl. Math. Stat. 5:31. doi: 10.3389/fams.2019.00031
Rohenkohl, G., Bosman, C. A., and Fries, P. (2018). Gamma synchronization between V1 and V4 improves behavioral performance. Neuron 100, 953–963.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.019
Rosenblum, M., Pikovsky, A., Kurths, J., Schäfer, C., and Tass, P. A. (2001). Phase synchronization: from theory to data analysis. Handb. Biol. Phys. 4, 279–321. doi: 10.1016/S1383-8121(01)80012-9
Scheeringa, R., and Fries, P. (2019). Cortical layers, rhythms and BOLD signals. Neuroimage 197, 689–698. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.002
Schiller, P. H., Finlay, B. L., and Volman, S. F. (1976). Quantitative studies of single-cell properties in monkey striate cortex. II. Orientation specificity and ocular dominance. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 1320–1333. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.6.1320
Schuster, H. G., and Wagner, P. (1990). A model for neuronal oscillations in the visual cortex: 1. Mean-field theory and derivation of the phase equations. Biol. Cybern. 64, 77–82. doi: 10.1007/BF00203633
Segev, I., and Rall, W. (1998). Excitable dendrites and spines: earlier theoretical insights elucidate recent direct observations. Trends Neurosci. 21, 453–460. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01327-7
Senzai, Y., Fernandez-Ruiz, A., and Buzsáki, G. (2019). Layer-specific physiological features and Interlaminar interactions in the primary visual cortex of the mouse. Neuron 101, 500–513.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.009
Shushruth, S., Mangapathy, P., Ichida, J. M., Bressloff, P. C., Schwabe, L., and Angelucci, A. (2012). Strong recurrent networks compute the orientation tuning of surround modulation in the primate primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 308–321. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-11.2012
Spaak, E., Bonnefond, M., Maier, A., Leopold, D. A., and Jensen, O. (2012). Layer-specific entrainment of gamma-band neural activity by the alpha rhythm in monkey visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 22, 2313–2318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.020
Taylor, K., Mandon, S., Freiwald, W. A., and Kreiter, A. K. (2005). Coherent oscillatory activity in monkey area V4 predicts successful allocation of attention. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1424–1437. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi023
Tiesinga, P. H., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2010). Mechanisms for phase shifting in cortical networks and their role in communication through coherence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:196. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00196
Tootell, R. R. B. H., Born, R. T., and Ash-Bernal, R. (1993). Columnar organization in visual cortex in non-human primates and man. Funct. Organ. Hum. Vis. Cortex 61, 59–73. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-042004-2.50009-5
Torrence, C., and Compo, G. P. (1998). A practical guide to wavelet analysis. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 79, 61–78. doi: 10.1175/1520-0477(1998)079<0061:APGTWA>2.0.CO;2
Van Kerkoerle, T., Self, M. W., Dagnino, B., Gariel-Mathis, M.-A., Poort, J., van der Togt, C., et al. (2014). Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 14332–14341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402773111
Vanduffel, W., Tootell, R. B., Schoups, A. A., and Orban, G. A. (2002). The organization of orientation selectivity throughout macaque visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 12, 647–662. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.6.647
Veit, J., Hakim, R., Jadi, M. P., Sejnowski, T. J., and Adesnik, H. (2017). Cortical gamma band synchronization through somatostatin interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 951–959. doi: 10.1038/nn.4562
Vinck, M., and Bosman, C. A. (2016). More gamma more predictions: gamma-synchronization as a key mechanism for efficient integration of classical receptive field inputs with surround predictions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:35. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00035
Wang, D. (1995). Emergent synchrony in locally coupled neural oscillators. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 6, 941–948. doi: 10.1109/72.392256
Wang, T., Li, Y., Yang, G., Dai, W., Yang, Y., Han, C., et al. (2020). Laminar subnetworks of response suppression in macaque primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 40, 7436–7450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1129-20.2020
Westerberg, J. A., Sigworth, E. A., Schall, J. D., and Maier, A. (2021). Pop-out search instigates beta-gated feature selectivity enhancement across V4 layers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 118:e2103702118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2103702118
Xing, D., Yeh, C. I., Burns, S., and Shapley, R. M. (2012). Laminar analysis of visually evoked activity in the primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 13871–13876. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201478109
Keywords: gamma oscillations, macaque monkey, area V1, laminar recording, attention, current source density, functional organization, dynamic network configuration
Citation: Drebitz E, Rausch L-P, Domingo Gil E and Kreiter AK (2024) Three distinct gamma oscillatory networks within cortical columns in macaque monkeys’ area V1. Front. Neural Circuits. 18:1490638. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2024.1490638
Edited by:
Dario L. Ringach, University of California, Los Angeles, United StatesReviewed by:
Nicholas Hatsopoulos, The University of Chicago, United StatesAndrea Brovelli, UMR7289 Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone (INT), France
Copyright © 2024 Drebitz, Rausch, Domingo Gil and Kreiter. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Eric Drebitz, ZHJlYml0ekBicmFpbi51bmktYnJlbWVuLmRl