Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Neural Circuits, 19 October 2023
This article is part of the Research Topic Global Excellence in Neural Circuits: Central and South America View all 5 articles

Autogenous cerebral processes: an invitation to look at the brain from inside out

Pedro E. Maldonado,,
&#x;Pedro E. Maldonado1,2,3*†Miguel Concha-Miranda&#x;Miguel Concha-Miranda4†Miriam Schwalm&#x;Miriam Schwalm5†
  • 1Departamento de Neurociencia, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 2Biomedical Neuroscience Institute (BNI), Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 3National Center for Artificial Intelligence (CENIA), Santiago, Chile
  • 4Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 5Department of Biological Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States

While external stimulation can reliably trigger neuronal activity, cerebral processes can operate independently from the environment. In this study, we conceptualize autogenous cerebral processes (ACPs) as intrinsic operations of the brain that exist on multiple scales and can influence or shape stimulus responses, behavior, homeostasis, and the physiological state of an organism. We further propose that the field should consider exploring to what extent perception, arousal, behavior, or movement, as well as other cognitive functions previously investigated mainly regarding their stimulus–response dynamics, are ACP-driven.

Introduction

Most neuroscience efforts have focused on understanding the sensory and motor processes necessary to maintain homeostasis. The associated findings have been structured around a conceptual framework in which brain activity primarily results from interactions between the organism and its environment. In this traditional view, neuronal responses in the form of particular firing patterns result from an external stimulus triggering the respective sensory system in the brain. This activity sets off a chain of physiological events through several neuronal circuits, resulting in sensory perception or motor action. Brain function has been extensively studied under this empiricist approach (Yuste, 2015), focusing on neuronal activity as a consequence of external stimulation (Raichle, 2010). However, neuronal activity investigated under this paradigm has continuously revealed intriguing features, such as considerable response variability upon repetitive presentations of the same stimulus. Until fairly recently, this variability was generally referred to either as “noise” or “spontaneous activity” (Stevens and Zador, 1998; Faisal et al., 2008). Central to the “noise” interpretation was the notion that neuronal response variability is primarily a consequence of stochastic processes occurring on different observational scales, from the molecular to the network level (Perkel et al., 1967; Calvin and Stevens, 1968). Further investigation revealed that neuronal variability (Softky and Koch, 1993; Yarom and Hounsgaard, 2011; Uddin, 2020) also exists in the absence of external input (Hartmann et al., 2015) and that it displays properties substantially different from noise (Arieli et al., 1996; Sadaghiani et al., 2010; Uddin, 2020). Two examples of intrinsically emerging activity that were observed and distinguished from background noise in neuronal recordings early on are thalamocortical slow wave events (Steriade et al., 1991) and hippocampal sharp wave ripples (Csicsvari et al., 2000), both related to homeostasis (Fultz et al., 2019) and behavior (Michon et al., 2019).

Using different versions of the classical stimulus–response paradigm has indeed revealed many core principles of neuroscience. It has significantly advanced our knowledge of how neurons in the different loci of the brain respond to sensory stimuli, how motor actions are planned and executed, how this activity is organized, and how it contributes to higher cognitive processes such as perception or decision-making. Due to this legacy, the stimulus–response dyad has justifiably been kept as a standard experimental paradigm in today’s neuroscientific toolkit and still largely dominates our current conceptual framework of brain function (see also Marom, 2020 for a different viewpoint on why this might be problematic). However, focusing solely on stimulus–response-based activity strongly contrasts with our daily experience as active agents exploring and interacting with our surroundings (Maturana, 1970; Varela et al., 1991; Ahissar and Assa, 2016; Haggard, 2017). Naturalistic experimental approaches (König and Luksch, 1998) have revisited the investigation of intrinsic brain processes, especially during task-independent activity (Raichle, 2010, 2015), and concepts of “spontaneous” or “ongoing activity” both assume an active and independent role of the brain (Friston, 2010; Clark, 2013). The theoretical frameworks resulting from these approaches consider the importance of intrinsically initiated neuronal processes and have provided insights into their spatiotemporal representation (Yuste et al., 2005; Deco and Kringelbach, 2017). The study of Maturana and Varela in particular contributed significantly to the concept of the brain as a closed-loop system. They emphasized that the brain operates not as an isolated entity but as an inseparable part of a dynamic feedback loop with its environment (Maturana, 1970; Maturana and Varela, 1987). Maturana and Varela’s contributions challenged traditional views of the brain as a passive recipient of sensory input, paving the way for a more holistic understanding of cognition as an active, self-organizing process deeply intertwined with the outside world.

Building upon these foundations, we propose the concept of autogenous cerebral processes (ACPs) as intrinsic brain operations initiated independently of the environment. ACPs can shape stimulus–response properties and influence a wide range of behavior, homeostasis, and physiological states of an organism, while external input plays only a minor role in their emergence. Our definition of ACPs includes phenomena that under natural conditions occur on a continuum of two extremes: they can either be internally or externally driven and emerge from a combination of internal and external drivers at a given time (Figure 1A). A particular ACP arises when internal drivers are either the only or dominant source (Figure 1B). External drivers comprise inputs outside of the physical boundaries of the brain, such as sensory stimuli or induced changes in homeostasis (e.g., a decrease in oxygen or glucose levels). Internal drivers emerge from within the brain during corollary discharge or top-down control of attention. Since internal drivers can be modulated by external input and vice versa, ACPs are restricted to a particular time window. Consequently, past external influences—although they may have been shaped by internal drivers—are irrelevant to the current ACP. For example, past sensory input can determine the contents of a dream but will not be part of the contingent drivers that initiate the dream. This definition directly implies that different ACPs may be of different durations. Brief internally triggered processes, such as the mentioned corollary discharge, may last only a few milliseconds. Other ACPs, such as the act of dreaming, may have long-lasting inner drivers, extending over several minutes to hours. The central principle to consider is that at a given time, ACPs can occur with little or no sensory–motor interactions. This definition nevertheless implies that ACPs can be modulated by peripheral stimulation or environmental coupling as the internal drivers of ACPs can be subjected to external influences.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. ACPs are intrinsic operations of the brain, initiated independently of the environment. (A) We define internal operations as brain processes with little or no influence from external inputs. In natural conditions, the degree to which external and internal drivers influence a brain process can vary. As the figure suggests, different cerebral processes move along the extremes of purely externally or internally driven processes. ACPs would be located on the line’s left (green) side. (B) Cerebral processes are initiated independently of the environment when external drivers are not contingent on the development of the process. External drivers may have shaped the system’s state but do not participate in developing the current ACP dynamics.

As such, ACPs underlie perception, arousal, motivated behavior, or movement, while most have been studied primarily regarding their response to external input. However, identifying ACPs is challenging because (i) most experimental paradigms so far are centered on the researchers’ ability to control and trigger a change in neural activity; (ii) different ACPs can be measured by the same proxy (e.g., locomotion or pupil dilation, which in turn have often been assigned to various underlying mechanisms or states, such as arousal, attention, or perception, depending on the scope of the study); and (iii) even when ACPs are successfully captured, their role in explaining behavior has so far likely been underestimated.

To illustrate the concept of ACPs, we here consider the initiation of a motor response. It has been shown that visually guided or object-guided motor action engages brain circuits partially different from the ones associated with internally generated movements (Vaillancourt et al., 2003; Haggard, 2017). It has also been demonstrated that Parkinson’s disease patients show better performance during sensory-guided movements than during internally initiated movements (Majsak et al., 1998). This phenomenon is known as “paradoxical movement” (Glickstein and Stein, 1991) and has been explained as a result of different involvement of basal ganglia circuits in both types of actions (Siegert et al., 2002). A similar distinction has been suggested for externally and internally generated verbal responses, showing different brain activation patterns (Tremblay and Gracco, 2006). These observations suggest that a given behavioral output can result from externally or internally driven mechanisms that recruit different but partially overlapping brain circuits. We propose that a similar organization can be assumed for various phenomena, a perspective highlighting the importance of internally generated processes that our current experimental paradigms may unintentionally conceal.

Understanding the relevance of ACPs on different observational scales

Concepts that are similar to the definition of ACPs have been explored at the single-cell level (Steriade et al., 2001; Li et al., 2009), but the idea of characterizing internally generated activity has been more traditionally pursued in the context of whole-brain connectivity networks (Raichle, 2010, 2015; Schroeder et al., 2010). However, the absence of a theoretical framework and corresponding terminology hinders our ability to identify and characterize ACPs and their functions. By distinguishing ACPs from classical stimulus-driven processes, our understanding of internal and external drivers of neuronal activity and their interactions might significantly improve. Current methodologies bias neuroscience toward studying behavior and neuronal activity elicited by peripheral stimulation or as a result of long-term steady states. This bias may lead us to overlook changes in brain states with little or no influence from external events, and behavior due to such changes might remain unidentified. We may reveal new insights about their function and origin by applying experimental paradigms that consider internally driven processes. To advance our ability to explain the brain’s essential dynamics, we must introduce a framework incorporating ACPs as distinct and autonomously functioning sources of neuronal dynamics. Previously, some ACPs have been referred to as spontaneous behavior (Stringer et al., 2019), uninstructed movements (Musall et al., 2019), activeness (Gallero-Salas et al., 2021), internal states (Calhoun et al., 2019), or spontaneous activity (Fox et al., 2006). These descriptions refer to observable phenomena that cannot be attributed exclusively to external events. Instead, it is likely possible to explain behavioral outcomes and changes in neural activity with greater precision and accuracy when including these internal constructs (Musall et al., 2019). However, the need to include internal brain dynamics to be able to interpret neuronal signals more precisely contrasts with the lack of a unified theoretical framework describing them. Consequently, we suggest characterizing some of those processes as “inside-out” (Buzsáki, 2019), which may lay the groundwork for building a systematic taxonomy of ACP.

Examples of presumptive ACPs

The following non-exhaustive list of phenomena illustrates what could be characterized within the proposed ACP framework.

Perception

To fully understand perception, it is necessary to consider that changes in activity in the sensory organs are frequently a result of self-initiated actions whenever experimental conditions allow for free behavior. This phenomenon has been termed “active sensing,” in contrast to the earlier studied “passive sensing,” the perceptual process resulting from the response to a sudden, unpredicted stimulus (Bajcsy, 1988; König and Luksch, 1998; Bajcsy et al., 2018). For example, in the visual system, active sensing occurs when retinal activity changes due to saccadic movements (Ito et al., 2017). Active perception or active sensing (König and Luksch, 1998)—the original expression was borrowed from the field of machine learning (Bajcsy, 1988)—can be considered an ACP as the associated intrinsic motor activity leads to an active exploration of the environment. For example, the perception of touch requires active movements of the hand (Gibson, 1962; Lederman and Klatzky, 1987) or whisker movements in the case of somatosensory perception in rodents (von Heimendahl et al., 2007; Prescott et al., 2011). This relationship between motor actions (e.g., eye or hand movements) and sensory perception (visual or somatosensory) has been described (Blakemore et al., 2000; Melloni et al., 2009; Wurtz, 2015, 2018) and was shown to be similar for other sensory modalities such as olfaction (Wachowiak, 2011) and audition (Morillon et al., 2015). The principle of motor action modulating visual perception or modulating sensory activity per se seems to be a generalized ACP that occurs in humans and other animals, such as rodents, monkeys, fish, and bats (Frens and Van Opstal, 1998; Ulanovsky and Moss, 2008; Stamper et al., 2012; Hofmann et al., 2013; Krieger and Groh, 2015; Pomberger et al., 2020; Umeda et al., 2022). Another well-studied mechanism of a perceptual ACP is corollary discharge. The term was initially proposed by Sperry (1950) and referred to the “anticipatory adjustment in the visual center specific for each movement concerning its direction and speed.” Corollary discharge occurs during the initiation of movement, making it a part of active sensing and an ACP, as it can be intrinsically triggered (Poulet and Hedwig, 2002; Schneider et al., 2014). For example, during eye movements, the superior colliculus can modulate activity in visual regions such as the frontal eye field (Sommer and Wurtz, 2006) or in area MT (Berman and Wurtz, 2010). In rats, the secondary motor cortex may modulate secondary visual areas during eye movements (Itokazu et al., 2018). The same regions have been shown to inhibit the auditory cortex during action execution (Schneider et al., 2014), especially for sounds that result from the actions performed (Schneider et al., 2018).

Arousal

Another putative ACP, arousal, has primarily been studied as a reaction to external stimuli (Ajasse et al., 2018). It has also been examined in the context of internal, higher-order cognitive processes such as visual awareness (Naber et al., 2011), attention (Binda and Murray, 2015; Ajasse et al., 2018), visual memory (Montefusco-Siegmund et al., 2022), emotional perception (Vasquez-Rosati et al., 2017), and working memory (Zokaei et al., 2019), among others (Mathôt, 2018). Low arousal-related inherent signal dynamics are spontaneous and systematic activity during rest periods. These resting-state or default-mode networks were initially used to describe typical functional connectivity patterns between brain regions (Biswal et al., 1995; Raichle, 2011). Although we now understand that spontaneous brain activity fluctuations critically contribute to brain function (Fultz et al., 2019; Uddin, 2020), this activity, often referred to as the ‘global signal,’ is still conventionally treated as noise and typically subjected to artifact removal in resting-state data analysis (Power et al., 2017). Neuromodulation, as a mechanism of regulating arousal, can be considered an ACP as it can intrinsically trigger spontaneous changes or shifts in the functional state (Eggermann et al., 2014; Reimer et al., 2016). More specifically, neuromodulatory transmitters act on target cells in local or large-scale neural networks by affecting synaptic and membrane properties over ion channels, directly influencing conductance, or gaining control of output firing rates (Buzsáki, 2019). The release of neuromodulators such as acetylcholine (ACh), noradrenaline (NE), serotonin, dopamine, and GABA and glutamate upon the external stimulation or animal exploration of their environment has been well studied (Reisine et al., 1982; Sirinathsinghji and Heavens, 1989; Constantinople and Bruno, 2011; Picciotto et al., 2012; Duncan et al., 2014; Eggermann et al., 2014; Hai et al., 2016; Li and Jasanoff, 2020; Breton-Provencher et al., 2021; Mohan et al., 2023). Nevertheless, it has been repeatedly shown that they are also released depending on intrinsic mechanisms, such as “internal clocks” (Kafka et al., 1983; Meck, 1996; Kiehn et al., 2023) and during sleep onset or while waking up (Siegel J, 2004; Siegel J. M, 2004; Lopez-Rodriguez et al., 2007; John et al., 2008; Dash et al., 2009; Berridge et al., 2012; Oh et al., 2019). Notably, these neuromodulation-dependent state changes can be further modulated by the onset of locomotion or other behavioral changes (Poulet and Petersen, 2008; Niell and Stryker, 2010; Bennett et al., 2013; Vinck et al., 2015).

Motivated behavior

Behavior is a crucial element in maintaining homeostasis and implicates interactions with the environment to seek food, shelter, or interaction with other organisms, regulated by intrinsic reward and metabolic cost. Behavior expresses internal brain dynamics and, as such, is a putative ACP. More specifically, one theoretical perspective proposes that the intrinsic activity of a distributed network, comprising areas such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and cingulate, can influence the expression of motivated behavioral drives (Raichle and Gusnard, 2005). Some well-characterized motivated behaviors are thermostasis (Tan and Knight, 2018), food intake (Raynor et al., 2015), and sleep (Vyazovskiy, 2015). One intriguing example of self-rewarded behavior is the act of play. Playful behavior is extensively present across mammals (Iwaniuk et al., 2001), involving either locomotor, sexual, or social behaviors (Burghardt, 2005; Brooks and Burghardt, 2023). Play involves a distributed network comprising cortical and subcortical regions (Siviy and Panksepp, 2011), and an essential characteristic is that it is spontaneous and voluntary (Burghardt, 2005; Brooks and Burghardt, 2023). Rats spontaneously engage in rough-and-tumble play (Panksepp et al., 1984), consistent with its self-rewarding aspect (Calcagnetti and Schechter, 1992). As a strongly self-driven behavior, play is an example of an ACP embodied in a neural network distributed over large areas of the brain. This internally generated process may also arise from state changes in the medial prefrontal cortex and other areas controlling motivated behavior (Raichle and Gusnard, 2005; Reinhold et al., 2019).

Network dynamics

The idea that the brain’s electrical activity is a continuous and ongoing process was first proposed by Hans Berger at the beginning of the last century (Berger, 1929). Further studies confirmed that even without overt behavior, the brain continues to display coherent activity (Sokoloff, 1977). Arieli et al. (1995) found that the spontaneous firing of single neurons was highly correlated with optical signals and measurements of local field potentials obtained from the cat’s primary visual cortex. Although this activity was spontaneous, it appeared to be highly structured. This idea was reinforced when the same authors reported that ongoing activity, without any given sensory input, dynamically switched cortical states closely corresponding to orientation maps (Kenet et al., 2003). The ACP most studied over the last decade is the already-mentioned default-mode network (Raichle, 2015). This network was defined as a pattern of long-range functional connections between different brain regions that emerged during simple visual fixation or even when participants had their eyes closed. The functional connectivity maps revealed under these conditions differ from the activation observed for attention-demanding, non–self-referential tasks. It has been shown that local neuronal activity (Deco and Kringelbach, 2017) or even the bursting of a single neuron (Li et al., 2009) can influence changes in the global functional brain state through hierarchical processing. Deco and Kringelbach (2017) further argue that intrinsic ignition can describe a brain area’s local capabilities to transmit its activity to other regions in a given functional state. As such, it could occur independently of any sensory process. Thus, intrinsic ignition describes a brain-wide integration of neuronal activity elicited from the propagation of both feed-forward and recurrent activity, making it a definitive example of an ACP.

Discussion

Essential aspects of brain activity can only be understood by considering intrinsic processes and neuroscience, yet a conceptual framework is needed to comprehensively analyze them. The prevalence of the classical stimulus–response paradigm has led to a reductionist distinction between sensory input and noise, neglecting phenomena that we here summarize under the concept of ACPs. The challenge of unifying disparate evidence within a field of research is ubiquitous in neuroscience. For example, as memory research has grown in complexity, different classifications have helped to bring together seemingly divergent mechanisms under a more comprehensive framework (Johnson, 1983; Squire et al., 1993; Tulving and Craik, 2000). Similar efforts have been made for other cognitive functions by introducing the concepts of top-down and bottom-up-driven perception (Kinchla and Wolfe, 1979; Gregory, 1980; Gilbert and Sigman, 2007; Gilbert and Li, 2013) and, more recently, the distinction between external and internal attention (Chun et al., 2011). Part of what we have learned from these developments is that new conceptual categories can guide scientific efforts toward experimental data that still need a detailed neurobiological explanation. In the context of ACPs, we propose that even though most of the phenomena involving ACPs can be categorized under one of the current theoretical frameworks, i.e., either as a top-down or “internal” process, some of their characteristics are not fully conceptually defined. Despite its enormous flexibility, the bottom-up vs. top-down scheme falls short of incorporating all possible ACPs, mainly because some do not necessarily follow the anatomical architecture proposed for top-down processes. ACPs may not be exclusively embodied in feedback or lateral connections nor do they follow a clear anatomical hierarchy. For example, it is known that the superior colliculus can alter visual processing (Sommer and Wurtz, 2006) or that midbrain areas modulate each other in sensorimotor loops (Mukherjee et al., 2018). In these examples, the characteristic circularity of sensorimotor loops makes it challenging to fit a hierarchy that distinguishes bottom-up from top-down processes. The same difficulty may arise when we study the interaction between higher-order areas, such as the cortex and hippocampus (Siapas et al., 2005), and specific areas in the frontal cortex (Cho et al., 2020). The top-down framework also does not sufficiently emphasize the intrinsic nature of ACPs, since most top-down processes are understood in the context of their bottom-up counterparts. This definition leaves out ACPs such as spontaneous activity, circadian rhythm, dreaming, and, to some extent, voluntary behavior.

Coda

In this study, we propose to identify a set of brain processes that can occur without the influence of an external drive. We have reviewed some well-known examples, usually studied under stimulus–response paradigms, and suggest that they can also be characterized as autogenous processes (ACPs). Examples of ACPs can be found in the literature under various terms: spontaneous behavior, uninstructed movements, activeness, internal states, active sensing, spontaneous, task-independent, or intrinsic activity. Cognitively complex animals, including humans, behave in a distinctly non-robotic fashion, and several of their high-level behavioral capacities relate to ACPs. Natural and artificial intelligence differences likely originate from ACPs, enriching natural cognition. Thus, complex behaviors such as play and creativity cannot be reduced to stimulus–response contingencies.

We borrowed inspiration from Sejnowski (2014), who surveyed current methods of neuroscience and arranged them by spatiotemporal scales, to playfully create an initial conceptual schematic of ACPs and their spatial and temporal resolution (Figure 2). By doing so, we obtained an initial framework to characterize what will eventually reveal the fundamental contributions of ACPs to higher-order cognitive processes and behavior. This may facilitate the study of their commonalities, physiological mechanisms, and potentially distinct functional and structural architectures. As potential new experimental avenues, we suggest the following short-term approaches: (i) changes in experimental design: include free behavior as active sensing or natural behavior (i.e., Markowitz et al., 2023); (ii) including internal processes as explanatory variables, i.e., explaining neural variability or changes in brain dynamics as a result of a process initiated by the organism itself (movement, internally driven changes in a functional state, arousal, or perception); (iii) carefully examine brain dynamics during quiescence states; (iv) explore intrinsic states with the help of deep learning approaches; and (v) identify “spontaneous” but recurring patterns of brain activity that occur in the absence of external stimulation but are highly relevant for brain function (Livne et al., 2022). Following these experimental approaches could reveal the relevance of internal states for explaining cerebral dynamics and behavior beyond the known effects of external manipulation.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Autogenous cerebral processes: conceptual illustration of how these processes can be identified regarding their putative spatial and temporal resolution.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

PM: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MC-M: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MS: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Center for Artificial Intelligence, CENIA FB210017, Fondecyt N° 1190318 to PM and BNI, Project ACE 210007.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Michael Brecht for comments on an earlier manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor AN-P declared a shared affiliation with the author PM at the time of review.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahissar, E., and Assa, E. (2016). Perception as a closed-loop convergence process. Elife 5:e12830. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajasse, S., Benosman, R. B., and Lorenceau, J. (2018). Effects of pupillary responses to luminance and attention on visual-spatial discrimination. J. Vis. 18:6. doi: 10.1167/18.11.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arieli, A., Shoham, D., Hildesheim, R., and Grinvald, A. (1995). Coherent spatiotemporal patterns of ongoing activity revealed by real-time optical imaging coupled with single-unit recording in the cat visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 2072–2093. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.5.2072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arieli, A., Sterkin, A., Grinvald, A., and Aertsen, A. (1996). Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 273, 1868–1871. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajcsy, R. (1988). Active perception. Proc. IEEE 76, 966–1005. doi: 10.1109/5.5968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajcsy, R., Aloimonos, Y., and Tsotsos, J. K. (2018). Revisiting active perception. Auton Robots. 42, 177–196. doi: 10.1007/s10514-017-9615-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, C., Arroyo, S., and Hestrin, S. (2013). Subthreshold mechanisms underlying state-dependent modulation of visual responses. Neuron 80, 350–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, H. (1929). Über das Elektroenkephalogramm des Menschen. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 87, 527–570. doi: 10.1007/BF01797193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, R. A., and Wurtz, R. H. (2010). Functional identification of a pulvinar path from superior colliculus to cortical area mt. J. Neurosci. 30, 6342–6354. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6176-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, C. W., Schmeichel, B. E., and España, R. A. (2012). Noradrenergic modulation of wakefulness/arousal. Sleep Med. Rev. 16, 187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binda, P., and Murray, S. O. (2015). Spatial attention increases the pupillary response to light changes. J. Vis. 15:1. doi: 10.1167/15.2.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S. J., Smith, J., Steel, R., Johnstone, C. E., and Frith, C. D. (2000). The perception of self-produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self-monitoring. Psychol. Med. 30, 1131–1139. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799002676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breton-Provencher, V., Drummond, G. T., and Sur, M. (2021). Locus coeruleus norepinephrine in learned behavior: anatomical modularity and spatiotemporal integration in targets. Front. Neural Circ. 15:638007. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.638007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, H. J. B., and Burghardt, G. M. (2023). A review of interspecific social play among nonhuman animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 151:105232. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burghardt, GM. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2019). The brain from inside out. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Calcagnetti, D. J., and Schechter, M. D. (1992). Place conditioning reveals the rewarding aspect of social interaction in juvenile rats. Physiol. Behav. 51, 667–672. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90101-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calhoun, A. J., Pillow, J. W., and Murthy, M. (2019). Unsupervised identification of the internal states that shape natural behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 2040–2049. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0533-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvin, W. H., and Stevens, C. F. (1968). Synaptic noise and other sources of randomness in motoneuron interspike intervals. J. Neurophysiol. 31, 574–587. doi: 10.1152/jn.1968.31.4.574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, I., Song, H. J., Kim, H., and Sul, S. (2020). Older adults consider others' intentions less but allocentric outcomes more than young adults during an ultimatum game. Psychol. Aging 35, 974–980. doi: 10.1037/pag0000577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 73–101. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 181–204. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinople, C. M., and Bruno, R. M. (2011). Effects and mechanisms of wakefulness on local cortical networks. Neuron 69, 1061–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csicsvari, J., Hirase, H., Mamiya, A., and Buzsáki, G. (2000). Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events. Neuron 28, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00135-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dash, M. B., Douglas, C. L., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2009). Long-term homeostasis of extracellular glutamate in the rat cerebral cortex across sleep and waking states. J. Neurosci. 29, 620–629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5486-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deco, G., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2017). Hierarchy of information processing in the brain: a novel 'intrinsic ignition' framework. Neuron 94, 961–968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, N. W., Wiebking, C., and Northoff, G. (2014). Associations of regional GABA and glutamate with intrinsic and extrinsic neural activity in humans—a review of multimodal imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 36–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggermann, E., Kremer, Y., Crochet, S., and Petersen, C. C. H. (2014). Cholinergic signals in mouse barrel cortex during active whisker sensing. Cell Rep. 9, 1654–1660. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Zacks, J. M., and Raichle, M. E. (2006). Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 23–25. doi: 10.1038/nn1616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frens, M. A., and Van Opstal, A. J. (1998). Visual-auditory interactions modulate saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res. Bull. 46, 211–224. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00007-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fultz, N. E., Bonmassar, G., Setsompop, K., Stickgold, R. A., Rosen, B. R., Polimeni, J. R., et al. (2019). Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep. Science 366, 628–631. doi: 10.1126/science.aax5440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallero-Salas, Y., Han, S., Sych, Y., Voigt, F. F., Laurenczy, B., Gilad, A., et al. (2021). Sensory and behavioral components of neocortical signal flow in discrimination tasks with short-term memory. Neuron 109, 135–148.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.10.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychol. Rev. 69, 477–491. doi: 10.1037/h0046962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, C. D., and Li, W. (2013). Top-down influences on visual processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 350–363. doi: 10.1038/nrn3476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, C. D., and Sigman, M. (2007). Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54, 677–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glickstein, M., and Stein, J. (1991). Paradoxical movement in Parkinson’s disease. Trends Neurosci. 14, 480–482. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90055-Y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 290, 181–197.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 196–207. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hai, A., Cai, L. X., Lee, T., Lelyveld, V. S., and Jasanoff, A. (2016). Molecular fMRI of serotonin transport. Neuron 92, 754–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, C., Lazar, A., Nessler, B., and Triesch, J. (2015). Where’s the noise? Key features of spontaneous activity and neural variability arise through learning in a deterministic network. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1004640. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, V., Sanguinetti-Scheck, J. I., Künzel, S., Geurten, B., Gómez-Sena, L., and Engelmann, J. (2013). Sensory flow shaped by active sensing: sensorimotor strategies in electric fish. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 2487–2500. doi: 10.1242/jeb.082420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, J., Yamane, Y., Suzuki, M., Maldonado, P., Fujita, I., Tamura, H., et al. (2017). Switch from ambient to focal processing mode explains the dynamics of free viewing eye movements. Sci. Rep. 7:1082. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01076-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itokazu, T., Hasegawa, M., Kimura, R., Osaki, H., Albrecht, U. R., Sohya, K., et al. (2018). Streamlined sensory motor communication through cortical reciprocal connectivity in a visually guided eye movement task. Nat. Commun. 9:338. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02501-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N., Nelson, J. E., and Pellis, S. M. (2001). Do big-brained animals play more? Comparative analyses of play and relative brain size in mammals. J. Comp. Psychol. 115, 29–41. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, J., Ramanathan, L., and Siegel, J. M. (2008). Rapid changes in glutamate levels in the posterior hypothalamus across sleep-wake states in freely behaving rats. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 295, R2041–R2049. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.90541.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. K. (1983). A multiple-entry, modular memory system. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 17, 81–123. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60097-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kafka, M. S., Wirz-Justice, A., Naber, D., Moore, R. Y., and Benedito, M. A. (1983). Circadian rhythms in rat brain neurotransmitter receptors. Fed. Proc. 42, 2796–2801.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kenet, T., Bibitchkov, D., Tsodyks, M., Grinvald, A., and Arieli, A. (2003). Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature 425, 954–956. doi: 10.1038/nature02078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiehn, J. T., Faltraco, F., Palm, D., Thome, J., and Oster, H. (2023). Circadian clocks in the regulation of neurotransmitter systems. Pharmacopsychiatry 56, 108–117. doi: 10.1055/a-1027-7055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinchla, R. A., and Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: “top-down,” “bottom-up,” or “middle-out”. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 225–231. doi: 10.3758/BF03202991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

König, P., and Luksch, H. (1998). Active sensing -closing multiple loops. Z Naturforsch C J Biosci. 53, 542–549. doi: 10.1515/znc-1998-7-808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krieger, P., and Groh, A. (2015). Sensorimotor integration in the whisker system. New York: Springer.

Google Scholar

Lederman, S. J., and Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 342–368. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90008-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, N., and Jasanoff, A. (2020). Local and global consequences of reward-evoked striatal dopamine release. Nature 580, 239–244. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2158-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. Y., Poo, M. M., and Dan, Y. (2009). Burst spiking of a single cortical neuron modifies global brain state. Science 324, 643–646. doi: 10.1126/science.1169957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livne, T., Kim, D., Metcalf, N. V., Zhang, L., Pini, L., Shulman, G. L., et al. (2022). Spontaneous activity patterns in human motor cortex replay evoked activity patterns for hand movements. Sci. Rep. 12:16867. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-20866-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Rodriguez, F., Medina-Ceja, L., Wilson, C. L., Jhung, D., and Morales-Villagran, A. (2007). Changes in extracellular glutamate levels in rat orbitofrontal cortex during sleep and wakefulness. Arch. Med. Res. 38, 52–55. doi: 10.1016/j.arcmed.2006.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majsak, M. J., Kaminski, T., Gentile, A. M., and Flanagan, J. R. (1998). The reaching movements of patients with Parkinson’s disease under self-determined maximal speed and visually cued conditions. Brain 121, 755–766. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.4.755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, J. E., Gillis, W. F., Jay, M., Wood, J., Harris, R. W., Cieszkowski, R., et al. (2023). Spontaneous behaviour is structured by reinforcement without explicit reward. Nature 614, 108–117. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-05611-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marom, S. (2020). Dialogue across chasm: are psychology and neurophysiology incompatible? Neuron 107, 600–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathôt, S. (2018). Pupillometry: psychology, physiology, and function. J. Cogn. 1:16. doi: 10.5334/joc.18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maturana, HR. (1970). Biological computer laboratory research report BCL 9.0. Urbana IL: University of Illinois.

Google Scholar

Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1987). The tree of knowledge: the biological roots of human understanding. Shambhala: Boston

Google Scholar

Meck, W. H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 227–242. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(96)00009-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melloni, L., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Rodriguez, E., and Singer, W. (2009). (micro)saccades, corollary activity and cortical oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 239–245. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michon, F., Sun, J. J., Kim, C. Y., Ciliberti, D., and Kloosterman, F. (2019). Post-learning hippocampal replay selectively reinforces spatial memory for highly rewarded locations. Curr. Biol. 29, 1436–1444.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohan, H., An, X., Xu, X. H., Kondo, H., Zhao, S., Matho, K. S., et al. (2023). Cortical glutamatergic projection neuron types contribute to distinct functional subnetworks. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 481–494. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01244-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montefusco-Siegmund, R., Schwalm, M., Rosales Jubal, E., Devia, C., Egaña, J. I., and Maldonado, P. E. (2022). Alpha eeg activity and pupil diameter coupling during inactive wakefulness in humans. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0060-21.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0060-21.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morillon, B., Hackett, T. A., Kajikawa, Y., and Schroeder, C. E. (2015). Predictive motor control of sensory dynamics in auditory active sensing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 230–238. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, D., Sokoloff, G., and Blumberg, M. S. (2018). Corollary discharge in precerebellar nuclei of sleeping infant rats. Elife 7:e38213. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musall, S., Kaufman, M. T., Juavinett, A. L., Gluf, S., and Churchland, A. K. (2019). Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1677–1686. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naber, M., Frässle, S., and Einhäuser, W. (2011). Perceptual rivalry: reflexes reveal the gradual nature of visual awareness. PLoS One 6:e20910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niell, C. M., and Stryker, M. P. (2010). Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron 65, 472–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, J., Petersen, C., Walsh, C. M., Bittencourt, J. C., Neylan, T. C., and Grinberg, L. T. (2019). The role of co-neurotransmitters in sleep and wake regulation. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 1284–1295. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0291-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Siviy, S., and Normansell, L. (1984). The psychobiology of play: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 465–492. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90005-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkel, D. H., Gerstein, G. L., and Moore, G. P. (1967). Neuronal spike trains and stochastic point processes. II. Simultaneous spike trains. Biophys. J. 7, 419–440. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(67)86597-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picciotto, M. R., Higley, M. J., and Mineur, Y. S. (2012). Acetylcholine as a neuromodulator: cholinergic signaling shapes nervous system function and behavior. Neuron 76, 116–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomberger, T., Löschner, J., and Hage, S. R. (2020). Compensatory mechanisms affect sensorimotor integration during ongoing vocal motor acts in marmoset monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 52, 3531–3544. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulet, J. F., and Hedwig, B. (2002). A corollary discharge maintains auditory sensitivity during sound production. Nature 418, 872–876. doi: 10.1038/nature00919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulet, J. F., and Petersen, C. C. (2008). Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature 454, 881–885. doi: 10.1038/nature07150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Plitt, M., Laumann, T. O., and Martin, A. (2017). Sources and implications of whole-brain fmri signals in humans. Neuroimage 146, 609–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prescott, T. J., Diamond, M. E., and Wing, A. M. (2011). Active touch sensing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 366, 2989–2995. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2010). Two views of brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2011). The restless brain. Brain Connect 1, 3–12. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., and Gusnard, D. A. (2005). Intrinsic brain activity sets the stage for expression of motivated behavior. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 167–176. doi: 10.1002/cne.20752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raynor, H. A., Goff, M. R., Poole, S. A., and Chen, G. (2015). Eating frequency, food intake, and weight: a systematic review of human and animal experimental studies. Front. Nutr. 2:38. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2015.00038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimer, J., McGinley, M. J., Liu, Y., Rodenkirch, C., Wang, Q., McCormick, D. A., et al. (2016). Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat. Commun. 7:13289. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinhold, A. S., Sanguinetti-Scheck, J. I., Hartmann, K., and Brecht, M. (2019). Behavioral and neural correlates of hide-and-seek in rats. Science 365, 1180–1183. doi: 10.1126/science.aax4705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisine, T., Soubrie, P., Artaud, F., and Glowinski, J. (1982). Sensory stimuli differentially affect in vivo nigral and striatal [3h]serotonin release in the cat. Brain Res. 232, 77–87. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90611-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., Hesselmann, G., Friston, K. J., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). The relation of ongoing brain activity, evoked neural responses, and cognition. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:20. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, D. M., Nelson, A., and Mooney, R. (2014). A synaptic and circuit basis for corollary discharge in the auditory cortex. Nature 513, 189–194. doi: 10.1038/nature13724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, D. M., Sundararajan, J., and Mooney, R. (2018). A cortical filter that learns to suppress the acoustic consequences of movement. Nature 561, 391–395. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0520-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, C. E., Wilson, D. A., Radman, T., Scharfman, H., and Lakatos, P. (2010). Dynamics of active sensing and perceptual selection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sejnowski, T. J. (2014). Summary: cognition in 2014. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 79, 237–241. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2014.79.024885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siapas, A. G., Lubenov, E. V., and Wilson, M. A. (2005). Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal theta oscillations. Neuron 46, 141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, J. (2004). Brain mechanisms that control sleep and waking. Naturwissenschaften 91, 355–365. doi: 10.1007/s00114-004-0541-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, J. M. (2004). The neurotransmitters of sleep. J. Clin. Psychiatry 65, 4–7.

Google Scholar

Siegert, R. J., Harper, D. N., Cameron, F. B., and Abernethy, D. (2002). Self-initiated versus externally cued reaction times in Parkinson’s disease. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24, 146–153. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.2.146.991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirinathsinghji, D. J., and Heavens, R. P. (1989). Stimulation of gaba release from the rat neostriatum and globus pallidus in vivo by corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci. Lett. 100, 203–209. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90685-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siviy, S. M., and Panksepp, J. (2011). In search of the neurobiological substrates for social playfulness in mammalian brains. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1821–1830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Softky, W. R., and Koch, C. (1993). The highly irregular firing of cortical cells is inconsistent with temporal integration of random epsps. J. Neurosci. 13, 334–350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-01-00334.1993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokoloff, L. (1977). Relation between physiological function and energy metabolism in the central nervous system. J. Neurochem. 29, 13–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1977.tb03919.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommer, M. A., and Wurtz, R. H. (2006). Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature 444, 374–377. doi: 10.1038/nature05279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 43, 482–489. doi: 10.1037/h0055479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., and Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44, 453–495. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.002321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamper, S. A., Roth, E., Cowan, N. J., and Fortune, E. S. (2012). Active sensing via movement shapes spatiotemporal patterns of sensory feedback. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1567–1574. doi: 10.1242/jeb.068007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Dossi, R. C., and Nuñez, A. (1991, 1991). Network modulation of a slow intrinsic oscillation of cat thalamocortical neurons implicated in sleep delta waves: cortically induced synchronization and brainstem cholinergic suppression. J. Neurosci. 11, 3200–3217. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-10-03200.1991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Timofeev, I., and Grenier, F. (2001). Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1969–1985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C. F., and Zador, A. M. (1998). Input synchrony and the irregular firing of cortical neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 210–217. doi: 10.1038/659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stringer, C., Pachitariu, M., Steinmetz, N., Reddy, C. B., Carandini, M., and Harris, K. D. (2019). Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science 364:255. doi: 10.1126/science.aav7893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, C. L., and Knight, Z. A. (2018). Regulation of body temperature by the nervous system. Neuron 98, 31–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Gracco, V. L. (2006). Contribution of the frontal lobe to externally and internally specified verbal responses: Fmri evidence. Neuroimage 33, 947–957. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E., and Craik, F. I. (2000). The Oxford handbook of memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q. (2020). Bring the noise: reconceptualizing spontaneous neural activity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 734–746. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulanovsky, N., and Moss, C. F. (2008). What the bat’s voice tells the bat's brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 8491–8498. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703550105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umeda, T., Isa, T., and Nishimura, Y. (2022). Temporal dynamics of the sensorimotor convergence underlying voluntary limb movement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 119:e2208353119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2208353119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillancourt, D. E., Thulborn, K. R., and Corcos, D. M. (2003). Neural basis for the processes that underlie visually guided and internally guided force control in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3330–3340. doi: 10.1152/jn.00394.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Vasquez-Rosati, A., Brunetti, E. P., Cordero, C., and Maldonado, P. E. (2017). Pupillary response to negative emotional stimuli is differentially affected in meditation practitioners. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:209. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinck, M., Batista-Brito, R., Knoblich, U., and Cardin, J. A. (2015). Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron 86, 740–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Heimendahl, M., Itskov, P. M., Arabzadeh, E., and Diamond, M. E. (2007). Neuronal activity in rat barrel cortex underlying texture discrimination. PLoS Biol. 5:e305. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V. (2015). Sleep, recovery, and metaregulation: explaining the benefits of sleep. Nat. Sci. Sleep 7, 171–184. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S54036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wachowiak, M. (2011). All in a sniff: olfaction as a model for active sensing. Neuron 71, 962–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurtz, R. H. (2015). Using perturbations to identify the brain circuits underlying active vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140205. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurtz, R. H. (2018). Corollary discharge contributions to perceptual continuity across saccades. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 4, 215–237. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarom, Y., and Hounsgaard, J. (2011). Voltage fluctuations in neurons: signal or noise? Physiol. Rev. 91, 917–929. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00019.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuste, R. (2015). From the neuron doctrine to neural networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 487–497. doi: 10.1038/nrn3962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuste, R., MacLean, J. N., Smith, J., and Lansner, A. (2005). The cortex as a central pattern generator. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 477–483. doi: 10.1038/nrn1686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zokaei, N., Board, A. G., Manohar, S. G., and Nobre, A. C. (2019). Modulation of the pupillary response by the content of visual working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 116, 22802–22810. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1909959116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: intrinsic brain processes, ongoing activity, closed-loop systems, brain dynamics, autogenous

Citation: Maldonado PE, Concha-Miranda M and Schwalm M (2023) Autogenous cerebral processes: an invitation to look at the brain from inside out. Front. Neural Circuits. 17:1253609. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2023.1253609

Received: 05 July 2023; Accepted: 26 September 2023;
Published: 19 October 2023.

Edited by:

Alexia Nunez-Parra, University of Chile, Chile

Reviewed by:

Mario Treviño, University of Guadalajara, Mexico
Diego Restrepo, University of Colorado Denver, United States

Copyright © 2023 Maldonado, Concha-Miranda and Schwalm. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pedro E. Maldonado, pedro@uchile.cl

†These authors have contributed equally to thiswork

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.