Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neural Circuits, 15 January 2019
This article is part of the Research Topic Computational Methodologies in Brain Imaging and Connectivity: EEG and MEG Applications View all 11 articles

Dominant Patterns of Information Flow in the Propagation of the Neuromagnetic Somatosensory Steady-State Response

  • 1Department of Biomedical Physiology and Kinesiology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • 2Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Institute, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • 3Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • 5Department of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 6Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Methods of functional connectivity are applied ubiquitously in studies involving non-invasive whole-brain signals, but may be not optimal for exploring the propagation of the steady-state responses, which are strong oscillatory patterns of neurodynamics evoked by periodic stimulation. In our study, we explore a functional network underlying the somatosensory steady-state response using methods of effective connectivity. Human magnetoencephalographic (MEG) data were collected in 10 young healthy adults during 23-Hz vibro-tactile stimulation of the right hand index finger. The whole-brain dynamics of MEG source activity was reconstructed with a linearly-constrained minimum variance beamformer. We applied information-theoretic tools to quantify asymmetries in information flows between primary somatosensory area SI and the rest of the brain. Our analysis identified a pattern of coupling, leading from area SI to a source in the secondary somato-sensory area SII, thalamus, and motor cortex all contralateral to stimuli as well as to a source in the cerebellum ipsilateral to the stimuli. Our results support previously reported empirical evidence collected both in in vitro and in vivo, indicating critical areas of activation of the somatosensory system at the level of systems neuroscience.

Introduction

Electrophysiological steady-state responses (SSR) entrained to the frequency and phase of periodic stimuli provide a non-invasive method to test the integrity of sensory pathways. Many studies characterized the shapes of individual SSR and their dominant frequencies, but mapping of the SSR as the propagation of a neural signal within the wider network underlying somatosensory function remains largely unexplored. A combination of steady-state responses and statistical tools explicitly describing information transfers could provide a data-informed approach for studying the integrity of sensory pathways at the level of systems neuroscience.

In general, the steady-state response arises from strong oscillatory dynamics of neural activity evoked by periodic stimulation, in contrast to temporal patterns of the transient changes in signal dynamics caused by the onsets of stimuli, such as event-related potentials (ERPs). In our study, we will focus on the somatosensory steady-state response (SSSR), which is elicited by periodic tactile stimulation, and provides effective means to investigate oscillatory brain sensory networks (Nangini et al., 2006). The SSSR is typically observed at the frequencies of the driving stimulus, possibly with higher harmonics, reaching a resonance-like maximum in amplitude of around 20Hz (Snyder, 1992). The steady-state response is believed to be a result of the interplay between the on-going activity and afferent input, reflecting the intrinsic dynamic properties of the sensory networks at the scale of neuronal populations (Engel et al., 2001).

Previous studies have extensively linked the somatosensory steady-state response with primary somatosensory area SI using the model of a single equivalent current dipole under the assumption that response to tactile stimuli can be localized only to one specific location (Baumgartner et al., 1993; Hari et al., 1993). Multiple dipole modeling of electric brain sources is possible but requires more complex assumptions about the number of underlying sources, their locations and possible orientations of the dipoles (Hoshiyama et al., 1997). In contrast, model-free beamformer techniques can be used in an attempt to reconstruct whole-head volumetric maps of neural dynamics (Robinson and Rose, 1992; Van Veen et al., 1997). In both cases, dipole modeling or beamforming, not much research has been performed to study the propagation of the steady-state response throughout the wider network underlying human somatosensory function.

Typical studies on steady-state responses characterize source activation based on similarity between source dynamics (functional connectivity), which is ultimately driven by their similarity with stimuli (Bardouille and Ross, 2008; Schlee et al., 2008). In an extreme case, two perfect sine waves can be considered as highly correlated, but from the point of information-theoretic analysis, such a model assumes no information flow between them. In contrast to functional connectivity, effective connectivity (an influence which one signal exerts over another signal) essentially quantifies information transfer, thus being an attempt to explicitly describe information processing in a network (Vicente et al., 2011). Typical directionality measures are asymmetric, and can be applied in both directions, estimating an amount of information transferred from one source to another, and vice versa. Asymmetry in informant transfer would then indicate the dominant directionality in propagation of information in the network.

An analysis of dominant information flow with respect to SI would provide more insight in reconstructing the propagation of somatosensory response, inferring the directionality of communications within the network of distributed neuronal ensembles. In this study, we establish a fully data-driven approach using information-theoretic tools to describe the effective connectivity between neuromagnetic sources activated in response to the mechanic pressure periodically applied to a finger. We explore the exchange of information between the primary somatosensory area SI and the rest of the brain. We identify several distinctive sources which are localized contralaterally to stimuli in the secondary somatosensory area SII, thalamus, motor cortex, and ipsilaterally in the cerebellum. Specifically, we show that more information is transferred from SI to all those sources, than in the reverse direction.

Materials and Methods

Experiment

MEG data were collected at a sample rate of 1,250 Hz with a bandwidth of 0–300 Hz using a 151-channel whole head first-order gradiometer system (CTF, Coquitlam, BC, Canada). Subjects (n = 10) were seated upright with their head resting in the helmet shaped scanner. Head localization coils were placed on the nasion, and left and right pre-auricular points for co-registration of MEG data with subject-specific anatomical MR images. A small elastic air bladder was fitted to the right index finger pad. Vibrotactile stimulation was applied to the finger by pressurizing the bladder with 23 Hz trains of 10 ms compressed air pulses of 3 s in duration. The inter-train interval was randomly distributed between 3 and 5 s. A white noise masking sound was binaurally presented at 90 dB SPL via insert phones to mask sounds possibly associated with the stimulator. MEG and the driving signal for the stimulator were collected simultaneously for 10 min. Subjects watched a subtitled silent movie and were asked to stay alert during the MEG recording. No responses were required. Informed and written consent was obtained from each subject before participating in the study, which had been approved by the Research Ethics Board at Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care. More details on the data acquisition can be found in Bardouille and Ross (2008).

Asymmetry in Information Transfer

In this section we describe a non-linear method to quantify the transfer of information between two processes. This method explicitly assumes that sensory processing may be considered the result of generation and transformation of cooperative modes of neural activity (Bressler, 1995, 2002; McIntosh, 1999). Specifically, the principles emphasize the integrative capacity of the brain in terms of ensembles of coupled neural systems interacting in a non-linear way (Nunez, 1995; Jirsa and McIntosh, 2007). Typically, in a non-linear analysis of EEG or MEG, it is assumed that individual time series, xt and yt, represent the manifestation of underlying multi-dimensional non-linear dynamics (Stam, 2005). To estimate the exchange of information between two systems, we reconstruct, from time series of observations, the dynamics in the multi-dimensional state space of the underlying model. It can be done with time delay embedding

xt=(xt,xt-τx,,xt-τx(dx-1))T    (1)
yt=(yt,yt-τy,,yt-τy(dy-1))T    (2)

wherein each time series, xt and yt, is converted to a sequence of vectors in a multi-dimensional space. Here dx and dy are embedding dimensions, and τx and τy are embedding delays measured in multiples of the sampling interval. Note that the ultimate goal is not to reconstruct an orbit in the state space that is closest to the true one. However, Takens' embedding theorem states that if the embedding dimension m is sufficiently high, the macrocharacteristics of a dynamic system such as the entropy or information content of a signal can be approximated (Takens, 1981).

Information transfer from system xt to yt can be quantified using information-theoretic tools. Intuitively, yt is a nonlinear cause of xt, denoted as YX, if past and present values of yt contain information about future values of xt, provided that information about past and present of xt itself is excluded (Palus et al., 2001). The transfer of information from Y to X is quantified as the conditional mutual information I(xt + k, yt | xt) between xt + k and yt given xt. Palus and Vejmelka (2007) showed that under certain conditions, I(xt + k, yt | xt) is equivalent to the measure of transfer entropy T(YX) proposed by Schreiber (2000). The transfer of information T(YX) or I(xt + k, yt | xt) can be expressed in terms of individual H(·) and joint entropies H(·, ·) and H(·, ·, ·) as following:

Tk(YX)I(xt+k,yt|xt)=H(xt+k,xt)+H(yt,xt)                                                            -H(xt+k,yt,xt)-H(xt+k)    (3)

where the index k is used to designate dependence of the transfer information on the future lag k, which is measured in units of data points. In a similar way we define the transfer of information Tk(XY) from X to Y:

Tk(XY)I(yt+k,xt|yt)    (4)

In general, it holds that I(yt + k, xt | yt) ≠ I(xt + k, yt| xt). The difference between the two measures, Tk(XY) and Tk(YX), characterizes an asymmetry in the predictive power between two signals, and thus indicate the directionality of the dominant information transfer between the systems underlying the observed signals. Specifically, we define the asymmetry in information transfer Dk(YX) as:

Dk(YX)=Tk(YX)-Tk(XY)    (5)

Note that this statistic is anti-symmetrical, e.i. Dk(YX) = −Dk(XY), and produces zero values between identical signals.

Estimation of Information Transfer

Until now we described the theoretical aspects of constructing a measure of the information transfer (3) and (4). A crucial issue is how to estimate them from finite noisy time series. Here we give a short description of the pipeline which was tested in previous studies using linear and linear models (Chávez et al., 2003; Vakorin et al., 2009, 2012, 2013).

There are three points to note. First, in estimating the measure (5), to reduce the variance of estimation error and to increase the robustness of the results, Tk(YX) and Tk(XY) can be substituted by their averaged estimates, < Tk(YX) >k and < Tk(XY) >k, respectively, where < … >k denotes averaging over a selected range of future lags k = 1, …, kmax (Palus et al., 2001). Thus, the measure D(YX) can be expressed as

D(YX)=<Tk(YX)>k-<Tk(XY)>k    (6)

The second issue is estimation of the entropies themselves. The straightforward approach is to divide the state-space into bins, i = 1, 2, 3, …, of some size δ and calculate the entropy of the multidimensional dynamics through constructing a multidimensional histogram, estimating probabilities of being in the bin i, pi(x, δ). This study took another approach, as proposed by Prichard and Theiler (1995) who estimated individual and joint entropies H(x) through the corresponding correlation integral Cq(x, r).

Correlation integral is interpreted as the likelihood that the distance between two randomly chosen points or embedding vectors, xi and xj, representing the multi-dimensional dynamics of x at times i and j, is smaller than some characteristic scale length r. The entropy can be estimated through correlation integral as

H(x)=-i(bins)pi(x,δ)log2pi(x,δ)-log2Cq(x,r)    (7)

where the correlation integral Cq(x, r) is a function of the scale parameter r, which in general can be related to the bin size δ. The parameter q is the integral order.

In turn, the Cq(x, r) is defined as

Cq(x,r)=1N(N-1)q-1×j=1N[ijΘ(r-xi-xj)]    (8)

where N is the number of data points (embedding vectors), Θ is the Heaviside function, and ∥·∥ stands for the maximum norm distance between two embedding vectors xi and xj. For a given vector xi, the function ijΘ(r-xi-xj) represents the number of the points j such that the distance between the d-dimensional vectors xi and xj is less than r. In this study, we used the second order (q = 2) correlation integral, following Pompe (1993) who proposed to call this a measure of generalized mutual information.

The third point is related to the issue of choosing the optimal embedding parameters, τ and d. This is a crucial and, in general, non-trivial step. There are many competing approaches proposed in the literature, and all of them are heuristic and somewhat mutually exclusive. The task can be simplified in case of frequency priors when we are interested in interactions for a specific frequency band (Montez et al., 2006). The idea is that the embedding lag τ should be short enough to capture the fastest oscillations cut off by the highest frequency fH, while the lowest frequency fL determines the embedding window d × (τ − 1) of a delay vector. The embedding window should be long enough to allow for the slowest processes. This leads to the following estimators:

τ=fsϵfH
d=ϵfHfL+1    (9)

where fs is the sampling frequency, and ϵ = 2…3 is a constant factor related to the Nyquist sampling theorem, indicating that a process must be sampled at least twice the highest frequency of its fluctuations.

Workflow of the Analysis

First, MEG data were down-sampled to a 250Hz sampling rate, and separated into trials of five second durations including 1 s pre-stimulus and 1 s post-stimulus intervals. The stimuli were of 3 s durations. For each MEG sensor and each trial, the baseline correction was achieved by subtracting the mean signal strength across the pre-stimulus interval, and a band pass filter was applied between 16 and 28 Hz. Based on the prior knowledge about the SSSR, we isolated the steady-state MEG data as the time interval from 500ms after stimulus onset to the end of the stimulus train, [0.5s 3.0s]. The first half second was discarded to avoid possible transitory effects.

Source activity was reconstructed with synthetic aperture magnetometry (SAM) (Robinson and Vrba, 1999). The nasion and left/right pre-auricular points were identified on the subjects' anatomical MRI to co-register the MEG data. Isolating the scalp in the MRI generated a realistic head model for source estimation. Based on this head model and the steady-state MEG data, a weighting coefficient set was determined at each node on a 5 × 5 × 5 mm grid encompassing the whole brain using a data-driven linearly constrained minimum variance beamformer (Robinson and Rose, 1992; Van Veen et al., 1997). The coefficient sets, in linear combination with the MEG data, estimated the source activity at each grid node to generate whole brain estimates of neuronal activity over time. For more details on the preprocessing procedures and source reconstruction, see Bardouille and Ross (2008). The methods for estimating asymmetries in information transfer were applied to the reconstructed steady-state source activity.

Further, the steady-state time series for each voxel were normalized to have the mean of zero and the variance of one. Then, the delay vectors was constructed with the embedding dimension of 5 and the lag of 4 time points according to the criteria (9), assuming that fL = 16Hz and fH = 28 Hz. In a previous study (Vakorin et al., 2010), for each subject, we identified a source localized to the primary somatosensory area SI, contralateral to stimuli. This source was characterized as having higher signal regularity among other sources (MEG virtual channels), based on the analysis of sample entropy (Vakorin et al., 2010). Net information transfer as defined in (6) was calculated between area SI and all other virtual channels, for all the single trials, with subsequent averaging across trials.

The volumetric maps of the net information transfer with respect to area SI were created and co-registered with the anatomical MRI identifying the head localization coil locations in the MR image. Each subject's MRI was transformed into Talairach space, and we applied the same transformation to the maps of information exchange on a subject-by-subject basis. Then, for each virtual channel, one-sample t-test was apply to compare the mean score to zero across subjects, and the volumetric maps of t-test values and corresponding p-values were created. AFNI software (Cox, 1996) was used to visualize the volumetric data, with the t-test map superimposed on the subject-averaged anatomical MRI. For each cluster of virtual channels, we computed the mean effect size, averaged across all virtual channels in the given cluster. Effect size was calculated as Hedges' g statistic (Hedges, 1981), which was proposed to correct a bias for Cohen's d statistic for small sample size (n < 20).

Results

Previously, having applied an analysis of signal regularity quantified as inverted signal complexity (sample entropy), we localized activation of a source in the somatosensory area SI contralateral to stimuli (Vakorin et al., 2010). Specifically, Figure 1 shows the volumetric subject-averaged activation map for a local peak in area SI. The activation map was thresholded at the level of 90% of its maximum, and was superimposed on the subject-averaged anatomical MRI. The color bar in Figure 1 indicates the magnitude distribution of the measure of signal regularity averaged across subjects. Higher values coded in red colors are associated with an increase in signal regularity of the steady-state response at a given source location. This was a result obtained in our previous study. In the current study, we used the source associated with the subject-specific peak in regularity in area SI, as the seed for an analysis of directionality of coupling from SI to the rest of the brain.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Axial (A), coronal (B), and saggital (C) views of volumetric regularity (inverted complexity) map of the somatosensory steady-state response, shown for a peak located in the primary somatosensory area SI. The map is thresholded at the level of 90% of the maximum regularity value of 0.026. Higher values coded with red colors are associated with higher signal regularity of the steady-state response at a given location, compared to other sources.

For each subject, we generated volumetric maps of the net information transfer with respect to a source in area SI (the seed), with the rest of the brain being the target. For almost all virtual channels, the hypothesis on the normality of the distribution of net information transfer across subjects could not be rejected. The robustness of the asymmetry in information transfer across subjects was then tested with t-tests. The distribution of t-test values across all the virtual channels is shown in Figure 2. Note that positive values for the t-statistic are associated with the dominant transfer of information from the primary somatosensory area SI, whereas the negative values indicate that net information is coming into area SI. As can be seen from Figure 2, the distribution is skewed toward the positive values, indicating that, on average, area SI is considered a generator of information in the propagation of the somatosensory steady-state response.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Distribution of the t-test values across the virtual channels (voxels). The channels from the upper 0.1%-tail, which is defined by 0.999-quantile = 4.77, represent two brain areas, shown in Figure 3.

Next, we identified the upper 0.1%-tail of this distribution defined by the 0.999-quantile equal to 4.77. Then, we used a clustering procedure 3Dclus from AFNI software to find clusters of active voxels, removing clusters below the size limit of 100–200 voxels (the results were robust with respect to the cluster size limit). The two clusters were localized in the secondary somatosensory area SII (Hedges' g = 1.4 ± 0.3) and thalamus (Hedges' g = 1.3 ± 0.2). Specified in Talairach coordinate system, the peak located in area SII was found to be around x = –53 mm [L], y = –9 mm [P], and z = 27 mm [S], whereas the coordinates of the thalamic source were around x = –6 mm [L], y = –4 mm [P], and z = –4 mm [I]. Figure 3 shows the axial, coronal, and sagittal views of the volumetric map of net information transfer with respect to area SI for these two locations: sub-panels in the left are associated with area SII (Figures 3A,C,E), where sub-panels in the right column illustrate activation in thalamus (Figures 3B,D,G). The map was thresholded at the 0.999-quantile, and was superimposed on the subject-averaged anatomical MRI. Note that red color represents the net transfer of information directed from SI to SII and thalamus.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Volumetric map of net information transfer with respect to the primary somatosensory area SI. The map is thresholded at the 0.999-quantile (0.1% tail in Figure 2), and is superimposed on subject-averaged anatomical MRI. Two clusters of virtual channels are identified: secondary somatosensory area SII (left column, A,C,E), and a thalamic source (right column, B,D,F). The directionality of dominant information transfer is from SI to SII and thalamus.

With a decreased threshold for the t-test statistics associated with a 0.5% tail, similar procedure identified four clusters. Specifically, in addition to sources in area SII and thalamus contralateral to stimuli, we found a source in the motor cortex (Figures 4A,C,E) around x = –44 mm [L], y = –3 mm [P], and z = 54 mm [S] contralaterally, and in the cerebellum ipsilaterally (Figures 4B,D,F) around x = 23 mm [R], y = –53 mm [P], and z = -17 mm [I]. Effect size for both these sources was estimated to be around Hedges' g = 1.3 ± 0.1.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Volumetric map of net information transfer with respect to the primary somatosensory area SI, similar to Figure 3, but with a decreased threshold associated with the 0.5% tail of the distribution in Figure 2. The directionality map is superimposed on subject-averaged anatomical MRI. With the decreased threshold, two more clusters of virtual channels are identified: one in the motor cortex (A,C,E), and one in the cerebellum (B,D,F). The directionality of dominant information transfer is from SI to motor cortex and cerebellum.

Discussion

Typically, measures for quantifying similarities between the signals are symmetric. Basically, almost all measures of functional connectivity belong to this class. In contrast, asymmetry in the time courses between brain areas may indicate another aspect of coordinated activity. Specifically, a significant difference in the amount of information sent in one direction and in the other, may be indicative of elevated communication between nodes in a functional network. Information generated by a dynamic system (which can be quantified as signal complexity of individual nodes) and information transfer between them would be considered two complementary sides of the interplay between specialization and integration processes (Mišić et al., 2011; Vakorin et al., 2012). An analysis of net information transfer thus would provide more insight in reconstructing the sequence and directionality of interactions occurring in the network of distributed neuronal ensembles, which may be hidden for measures of functional connectivity.

The data used in our study have been differently analyzed in two previous studies (Bardouille and Ross, 2008; Vakorin et al., 2010). Bardouille and Ross (2008) applied a measure called inter-trial coherence (ITC) (Stapells et al., 1987) to identify brain areas that activate synchronously during the steady-state response with consistent phase relations to the vibrotactile stimulation of the right index finger. Although they were able to identify a handful of sensorimotor areas, only area SI activated contralaterally to the stimulus was consistently expressed across all the subjects.

Using the same data, Vakorin et al. (2010) further studied the activation of the somatosensory steady-state response (SSSR) with entropy-based statistics constructed from a combination of information theory and non-linear dynamics. Instead of estimating the consistency of the frequency-specific phase dynamics across trials, this study explored the signal complexity (or regularity) of individual trials, using sample-entropy, a measure that is closely related to the mean rate of information generated by a non-linear system underlying the observed time series (Richman and Moorman, 2000; Vakorin and McIntosh, 2012). In contrast to ITC, this analysis identified activity not only in area SI contralateral to stimuli, but also bilaterally in the PPC, as regions with increased signal regularity, consistently expressed across the subjects.

In the same data, a seed analysis with respect to a source with a higher signal regularity, located in in SI, a measure of pattern synchronization called cross-sample entropy was used to generate synchrony maps between SI and the rest of the brain (Vakorin et al., 2010). Cross-sample entropy is similar to sample entropy but defined for a pair of time series. The analysis confirmed the synchronized dynamics of neuromagnetic activity between SI and PPC, robustly expressed across subjects. As no regions of interest were defined a priori, the map of pattern synchronizations between activated regions emerged automatically on the noisy background. The patterns contained in PPC regions were found to be well coordinated with those in SI, not necessarily being phase locked.

In spite of some progress made in understanding the SSSR, the question on the different stages of the development of the SSSR requires a further exploration. For example, in studies on the auditory steady-state response (ASSR) as a result to a periodically acoustic signal, it has been demonstrated that the development of the ASSR to amplitude-modulated sound occurs in cortical regions at slower modulation rates (<60 Hz) and in brainstem at faster modulation rates (>60 Hz) (Dimitrijevic and Ross, 2008). Despite analogous functional implications about the spatial characteristics of the SSSR, a better understanding of the somatosensory processing is expected.

Establishing a fully data-driven pipeline, this study explored the transmission of information between neuromagnetic sources in a functional network activated by vibrotactile stimulation of the right index finger. First, whole-brain neural activity representing the somatosensory steady-state response was reconstructed with a beamformer technique. Then, the information exchange was quantified between the primary somatosensory area SI and the rest of the brain. The volumetric map of the asymmetry in information transfer with respect to SI was created. We identified several brain areas wherein interactions with area SI were consistent across subjects. Specifically, the sources were localized contralaterally to the stimuli in the secondary somatosensory area SII, the thalamus, the motor cortex, and ipsilaterally to the stimuli in the cerebellum.

Thalamus sends and receives signals to and from cortical as well as to and from sub-cortical areas within the local and large-scale thalamo-cortical circuitry (Ribary et al., 2014). Five out of about 50 nuclei of the thalamus act as relays, receiving inputs from sensory peripheral receptors and sending the information to the primary sensory cortex for sophisticated processing(Ward, 2013). The relay nuclei which innervate the primary sensory projection areas, can be called the first-order relay nuclei. The other 45 higher-order thalamic nuclei participate in complex cortical and sub-cortical networks, relaying information between cortical and subcortical areas without primary sensory inputs (Sherman, 2012).

MEG has a limited spatial resolution and is not able to distinguish between first-order and higher-order thalamic nuclei. Directed coupling between area SI and thalamus, as found in our study, represent an averaged information transfer between these two brain regions. The imbalance in functional connectivity observed as SI → thalamus, would be consistent with a variance in the number and efficacy of the cortico-thalamic and thalamo-cortical projections, assuming similarities between the visual and somatosensory systems. It is estimated that there are 10-100 cortico-geniculate axons for every geniculo-cortical axon (Sherman and Koch, 1986). In cats, it was estimated that about 60% of the synaptic terminals in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) are cortical in origin, where only about 10% come from retina (Montero, 1991).

As for anatomical connections between SI and SII, it was often found, at least in the mouse, when the cortical areas are mutually interconnected, they are also connected through a cortico-thalamo-cortical pathway arranged in parallel. The prevailing belief was originally based on an assumption that information is transmitted through direct cortico-cortical pathways (Kaas, 1987). For instance, in the visual system, a corollary of such a postulate would be that the information sent from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the primary visual cortex, remains primarily in the cortex when it is transferred up the higher levels of the cortical hierarchy (Olshausen et al., 1993). However, other studies suggest that after the first transmission of sensory information to the cortex from the periphery, much of information exchange between cortical areas is performed by the higher-order thalamic relays through the cortico-thalamic-cortical routes (Sherman, 2001; Guillery and Sherman, 2002; Sherman and Guillery, 2002). For example, using optical imaging, Theyel et al. (2009) studied the cortical hierarchy in slices of the mouse brain, involving the thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory areas. Their results indicate that direct cortico-cortical projections from SI are not necessary to activate SII. On the contrary, it was a cortico-thalamic-cortical circuit starting in area SI that strongly activated area SII.

Note that in our study, we report only the dominant part of bi-directional communication between the cortical areas. We found that more information was transferred from area SI to area SII, than vice versa. Physiologically, it may seem to be a trivial result, as it is consistent with both hypotheses on information processing in cortical hierarchy. Methodologically, however, the analysis was performed in a fully data-driven manner. It is interesting to report that the pathway SI → SII was identified among all the possible connections between an activated source in area SI and all other sources defined by the nodes of the grid used to reconstruct the whole-head neuromagnetic activity.

We also identified connections leading from area SI to the motor cortex and the cerebellum. A number of studies has shown that in the rat SI can project to important motor control centers (Petrof et al., 2015) as well as to cerebellum (Bower et al., 1981). Both areas showed activation in response to electrically stimulated SI (Brown and Bower, 2002). Specifically, SI projected to the cerebellar granule cell patches either contralaterally or ipsilaterally, depending on the laterality of the peripheral projections of those patches. It was proposed that the cerebellum can be responsible for coordinating the acquisition of sensory data, which is further process by the nervous system, at least in the rats (Bower and Woolston, 1983; Morissette and Bower, 1996). In the human brain, a number of functional MRI (fMRI) studies reported ipsilateral activation of the cerebellar sources in reaction to tactile stimuli. For example, passive movement with the tactile system activated the primary supplementary motor and pre-motor cortex, as well as SI and SII contralaterally and the cerebellum ipsilaterally (Yang et al., 2011). Similar results were found in another fMRI study involving stroking and touching tasks (Zaman et al., 2001).

In general, our results thus demonstrate that exploring the asymmetries in information transfer in the propagation of a steady-state response with information-theoretic tools is a powerful method for describing information processing in a data-driven manner, and is able to reveal coordinated activity hidden from conventional analysis of functional connectivity. For central processing of sensory information, our results were found to be consistent with previously reported empirical evidence on coupling between primary somato-sensory area SI and secondary area SII, thalamus, motor cortex, and cerebellum.

Limitations of the Study

In our study, we quantify only net transfer of information, not considering information flows in both directions separately. The rational for this is that in the models based on coupled oscillators, unidirectional coupling may lead to spurious detection of a bidirectional coupling (Bezruchko and Smirnov, 2010). In other words, in general, we cannot distinguish two situations: a unidirectional coupling or bidirectionally coupled systems with a weak coupling in one direction and a strong coupling in the other. Also note that we quantify asymmetry in information transfer, meaning that equal information flows in both directions will cancel each other. This would potentially explain why we do not observe activation in some areas like posterior parietal cortex (PPC) with effective connectivity. Specifically, in our previous study (Vakorin et al., 2010), we identified a connection between SI and bilateral PPC, using a measure called cross-sample entropy, which represents a non-linear method for estimating functional connectivity. This would imply that neurodynamics at SI and PPC are essentially similar, and therefore net information transfer between them is close to zero. In the current study, we quantify a dis-balance in information transfer, reporting connections with a high asymmetry.

We limited our analysis to the canonical beta frequency band, which was associated with the stimulus frequency. We did not divide the whole beta band into sub-bands. A number of studies have indicated that the directionality of coupling for spectrally resolved statistics can be sensitive to phase delays (or advances) between the signals in the presence of strong phase synchronization (Vakorin et al., 2013). We used a canonical beta frequency band, which on the one hand, can be associated with the stimulus frequency, and on the other hand, is relatively wide to avoid spurious effects related to phase synchronization.

We also acknowledge that our study may have been limited by a relatively small sample size (n = 10), although, as described in section Results, effect size for activation clusters was strong, based on Hedgesg statistic (Hedges, 1981).

Author Contributions

VV, BR, and AM: Concept and design of the study; VV and BR: Data acquisition and analysis; VV, BR, SD, UR, and AM: Drafting the manuscript; VV: Preparing the figures.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by research grants from the J.S. McDonnell Foundation to AM. We thank Maria Tassopoulos-Karachalios for her assistance in preparing this manuscript. We also note that this research was enabled in part by support provided by WestGrid (www.westgrid.ca) and Compute Canada / Calcul Canada (www.computecanada.ca).

References

Bardouille, T., and Ross, B. (2008). MEG imaging of sensorimotor areas using inter-trial coherence in vibrotactile steady-state responses. Neuroimage 42, 323–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner, C., Doppelbauer, A., Sutherling, W., Lindinger, G., Levesque, M., Aull, S., et al. (1993). Somatotopy of human hand somatosensory cortex as studied in scalp EEG. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. Evoked Potent. Sec. 88, 271–279.

PubMed Abstract

Bezruchko, B. P., and Smirnov, D. A. (2010). Extracting Knowledge From Time Series: An Introduction to Nonlinear Empirical Modeling. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Bower, J. M., Beermann, D. H., Gibson, J. M., Shambes, G. M., and Welker, W. (1981). Principles of organization of a cerebro-cerebellar circuit. Brain Behav. Evol. 18, 1–18.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bower, J. M., and Woolston, D. C. (1983). Congruence of spatial organization of tactile projections to granule cell and purkinje cell layers of cerebellar hemispheres of the albino rat: vertical organization of cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 745–766.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. (1995). Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 20, 288–304.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. (2002). Understanding cognition through large-scale cortical networks. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 58 – 61. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, I. E., and Bower, J. M. (2002). The influence of somatosensory cortex on climbing fiber responses in the lateral hemispheres of the rat cerebellum after peripheral tactile stimulation. J. Neurosci. 22, 6819–6829.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chávez, M., Martinerie, J., and Le Van Quyen, M. (2003). Statistical assessment of nonlinear causality: application to epileptic eeg signals. J. Neurosci. Methods 124, 113–28. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00367-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dimitrijevic, A., and Ross, B. (2008). “Chapter Neural generators of the auditory steady-state response,” in The auditory steady-state response. Generation, recording, and clinical application, ed G. Rance (Plural Publishing Inc.), 83–107.

Google Scholar

Engel, A., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guillery, R., and Sherman, S. (2002). Thalamic relay functions and their role in corticocortical communication: generalizations from the visual system. Neuron 33, 163–176. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00582-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., Karhu, J., Hämäläinen, M., Knuutila, J., Salonen, O., Sams, M., et al. (1993). Functional organization of the human first and second somatosensory cortices: a neuromagnetic study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 5, 724–734.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Stat. 6, 107–128.

Google Scholar

Hoshiyama, M., Kakigi, R., Koyama, S., Watanabe, S., and Shimojo, M. (1997). Activity in posterior parietal cortex following somatosensory stimulation in man: magnetoencephalographic study using spatio-temporal source analysis. Brain Topogr 10, 23–30.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jirsa, V., and McIntosh, A. eds (2007). Handbook of Brain Connectivity. Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Kaas, J. (1987). The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications for theories of brain function. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 38, 129–151.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. (1999). Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. Memory 7, 523–48.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mišić, B., Vakorin, V., Paus, T., and McIntosh, A. (2011). Functional embedding predicts the variability of neural activity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero, V. (1991). A quantitative study of synaptic contacts on interneurons and relay cells of the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 257–270.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Montez, T., Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., van Dijk, B., and Stam, C. (2006). Synchronization likelihood with explicit time-frequency priors. Neuroimage 33, 1117–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morissette, J., and Bower, J. M. (1996). Contribution of somatosensory cortex to responses in the rat cerebellar granule cell layer following peripheral tactile stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 109, 240–250.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nangini, C., Ross, B., Tam, F., and Graham, S. (2006). Magnetoencephalographic study of vibrotactile evoked transient and steady-state responses in human somatosensory cortex. Neuroimage 33, 252–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunez, P. (1995). Neocortical Dynamics and Human Brain Rhythms. Oxford University Press.

Olshausen, B., Anderson, C., and Van Essen, D. (1993). A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information. J. Neurosci. 13, 4700–4719.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Palus, M., Komarek, V., Hrncir, Z., and Sterbova, K. (2001). Synchronization as adjustment of infomation rates: Detection from bivariate time series. Phys. Rev. E 63:046211. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.63.046211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palus, M., and Vejmelka, M. (2007). Directionality from coupling between bivariate time series: How to avoid false causalities and missed connections. Phys. Rev. E 75:056211. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.75.056211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrof, I., Viaene, A. N., and Sherman, S. M. (2015). Properties of the primary somatosensory cortex projection to the primary motor cortex in the mouse. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 2400–2407. doi: 10.1152/jn.00949.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pompe, B. (1993). Measuring statistical dependences in a time series. J. Stat. Phys. 73, 587–610.

Google Scholar

Prichard, D., and Theiler, J. (1995). Generralized redundancies for time series analysis. Physica D 84, 476–493.

Google Scholar

Ribary, U., Doesburg, S. M., and Ward, L. M. (2014). “Thalamocortical network dynamics: a framework for typical/atypical cortical oscillations and connectivity,” in Magnetoencephalography, eds S. Supek and C. J. Aine (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag), 429–449.

Google Scholar

Richman, J., and Moorman, J. (2000). Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 278, H2039–49. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.6.H2039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S., and Vrba, J. (1999). “Functional neuroimaging by synthetic aperture magnetometry,” in Recent Advances in Biomagnetism, eds T. Yoshimoto, M. Kotani, S. Kuriki, H. Karibe, and N. Nakasato (Sendai: Tohoku University Press), 302–305.

Google Scholar

Robinson, S. E., and Rose, D. F. (1992). “Current source image estimation by spatially filtered MEG,” in Biomagnetism: Clinical Aspects, eds M. Hoke, S. Erné, Y. Okada, and G. Romani (Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica), 761–765.

Google Scholar

Schlee, W., Weisz, N., Bertrand, O., Hartmann, T., and Elbert, T. (2008). Using auditory steady state responses to outline the functional connectivity in the tinnitus brain. PLoS ONE 3:e3720. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiber, T. (2000). Measuring information transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 85, 461–4. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.85.461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. (2001). Thalamic relay functions. Prog. Brain Res. 134, 51–69. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(01)34005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S., and Guillery, R. (2002). The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 357, 1695–1708. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S., and Koch, C. (1986). The control of retinogeniculate transmission in the mammalian lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 63, 1–20. doi: 10.1007/BF00235642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M. (2012). Thalamocortical interactions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 575–579. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, A. (1992). Steady-state vibration evoked potentials: descriptions of technique and characterization of responses. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 84, 257–68.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Stam, C. (2005). Nonlinear dynamical analysis of EEG and MEG: review of an emerging field. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2266–301. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stapells, D., Makeig, S., and Galambos, R. (1987). Auditory steady-state responses: threshold prediction using phase coherence. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 260–70.

PubMed Abstract

Takens, F. (1981). “Detecting strange attractors in turbulence,” in Dynamical Systems and Turbulence, Lecture Notes in Mathematics, Vol. 898, eds D. A. Rand and L.-S. Young (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer Verlag), 366–381. doi: 10.1007/BFb0091924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theyel, B., Llano, D., and Sherman, S. (2009). The corticothalamocortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 84–88. doi: 10.1038/nn.2449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V., Krakovska, O., and McIntosh, A. (2009). Confounding effects of indirect connections on causality estimation. J. Neurosci. Methods 184, 152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V., and McIntosh, A. (2012). “Mapping the multi-scale information content of complex brain signals,” in Brinciples of Brain Dynamics: Global State Interactions, eds M. I. Rabinovich, K. J. Friston, and P. Varona (Cambridge: The MIT Press), 183–208.

Google Scholar

Vakorin, V., Mišić, B., Krakovska, O., Bezgin, G., and McIntosh, A. (2013). Confounding effects of phase delays on causality estimation. PLoS ONE 8:e5358. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V., Misic, B., Krakovska, O., and McIntosh, A. (2012). Empirical and theoretical aspects of generation and transfer of information in a neuromagnetic source network. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:00096. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V., Ross, B., Krakovska, O., Bardouille, T., Cheyne, D., and McIntosh, A. (2010). Complexity analysis of the neuromagnetic somatosensory steady-state response. NeuroImage 51, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Veen, B., van Drongelen, W., Yuchtman, M., and Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 44, 867–80.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vicente, R., Wibral, R., Lindner, M., and Pipa, G. (2011). Transfer entropya model-free measure of effective connectivity for the neurosciences. 30, 45–67. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0262-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, L. M. (2013). The thalamus: gateway to the mind. Wiley Int. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 609–622. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Wu, J., and He, J. (2011). Programmable tactile pattern presentations operational under mri to investigate neural mechanisms of tactile shape discrimination. J. Neurosci. Methods 201, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaman, A., Moody, W., McGlone, F., and Roberts, N. (2001). An fMRI study of touch. NeuroImage 6:1285. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(01)92599-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: magnetoencephalography (MEG), somatosensory system, steady-state response, causality analysis, complexity analysis

Citation: Vakorin VA, Ross B, Doesburg SM, Ribary U and McIntosh AR (2019) Dominant Patterns of Information Flow in the Propagation of the Neuromagnetic Somatosensory Steady-State Response. Front. Neural Circuits 12:118. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00118

Received: 10 May 2018; Accepted: 17 December 2018;
Published: 15 January 2019.

Edited by:

Edward S. Ruthazer, McGill University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Stefania Della Penna, Università degli Studi G. d'Annunzio Chieti e Pescara, Italy
Nicoletta Berardi, Italian National Research Council, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Vakorin, Ross, Doesburg, Ribary and McIntosh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vasily A. Vakorin, dmFzaWx5X3Zha29yaW5Ac2Z1LmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.