Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Netw. Physiol., 16 May 2023
Sec. Systems Interactions and Organ Networks
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain-Heart Interaction: Recent Insights in Methods View all 4 articles

Depression as a cardiovascular disorder: central-autonomic network, brain-heart axis, and vagal perspectives of low mood

  • NeuroCardiovascular Intelligence Lab, Bioengineering and Robotics Research Center E Piaggio & Department of Information Engineering, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy

If depressive symptoms are not caused by the physiological effects of a substance or other medical or neurological conditions, they are generally classified as mental disorders that target the central nervous system. However, recent evidence suggests that peripheral neural dynamics on cardiovascular control play a causal role in regulating and processing emotions. In this perspective, we explore the dynamics of the Central-Autonomic Network (CAN) and related brain-heart interplay (BHI), highlighting their psychophysiological correlates and clinical symptoms of depression. Thus, we suggest that depression may arise from dysregulated cardiac vagal and sympathovagal dynamics that lead to CAN and BHI dysfunctions. Therefore, treatments for depression should target the nervous system as a whole, with particular emphasis on regulating vagal and BHI dynamics.

1 Introduction

The World Health Organization (WHO) reports that “depression is a leading cause of disability worldwide” and the condition affects at least 322 million people. Even more surprising is that 10 million people with depression have thought about suicide and 3 million have actual suicide plans. Those who actually seek help are diagnosed through the administration of questionnaires and subjective interviews. To date no specific physiological or biochemical markers are considered in current clinical practice, and no physiological measurement objectively distinguishes among the different subtypes of depression. Economically-speaking, the cost of lost productivity due to depression in the EU has been estimated at over €70 billion per year (World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe, 2018; Jaffe et al., 2019; Johnston et al., 2019; Greenberg et al., 2021).

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition (DSM-5 American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force, 2013), if depressive symptoms are not attributable to the physiological effects of a substance or other medical or neurological conditions, the depression and related subtypes are to be considered mental disorders, i.e., disorders somehow targeting the central nervous system (CNS) exclusively.

But why do we assume that these changes result from intrinsic functioning of the brain only? Why is it that more than 60% of patients with major depression may show no response to first-line antidepressant treatment mainly targeting brain neurotransmitters (Johnston et al., 2019)? Why do recent meta-analyses show inconsistency between studies that only focus on brain dynamics and depression (e.g., Müller et al., 2017)? These questions suggest that previous literature on depression has mainly focused on brain dynamics, while giving inadequate attention to body dynamics.

The brain and heart have long been studied and treated individually at the cortical/subcortical or neuro-peripheral level, including cardiovascular, blood pressure and respiration dynamics (as part of non-invasive monitoring) using specific techniques focused on specific system dynamics. However, the physiological system is complex. In other words, because of the numerous interactions of many subcomponents, the system as a whole exhibits characteristics that the individual components cannot act on. The CNS and autonomic nervous system (ANS) control all body organs simultaneously through anatomical and biochemical/functional connections that have a lasting impact on health and especially disease.

In piecing together segregated parts of research and in following the development of a comprehensive vision on whole-nervous system mental health and related science, here we pose the following fundamental hypotheses:

- In the absence of medical or neurological illnesses, brain changes in depression are not only the cause but also the consequence of a mood disorder

- The nervous system as a whole mediates emotional processing and regulation

- Abnormal vagal activity propels emotion dysregulation in depression

Scientific evidence supporting these hypotheses leverage upon the definition of the Central-Autonomic Network (CAN), which comprises brain regions involved in autonomic control (Benarroch, 1993; Beissner et al., 2013; Valenza et al., 2019; Valenza et al., 2020), as well as Brain-Heart Interplay (BHI), which comprises the functional links between CNS and ANS through electrical, biochemical, and physical communications. In this perspective, depression is envisioned as a manifestation of a dysregulation of cardiac vagal and sympathovagal dynamics sustaining CAN dysfunctions, which in turn reflects on functional BHI. Consequently, a “functional-vagal theory” of depression is stated, suggesting that depression treatment should also target brain-heart dynamics and, especially, act at a nervous-system-wise level.

1.1 Central and autonomic correlates of emotion regulation and emotional processing at a glance

Emotion regulation refers to the process by which individuals modify, maintain, or control their emotions, including their experience, expression, and physiological responses (Gross, 2015). It involves a range of strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, and attentional deployment, that are used to regulate emotional experiences and their impact on behavior and cognition. Emotion regulation can be adaptive, promoting psychological wellbeing and social functioning, or maladaptive, contributing to psychopathology and interpersonal difficulties. On the other hand, emotional processing refers to the cognitive and affective operations involved in the appraisal, interpretation, and response to emotional stimuli (Hamann and Canli, 2004). It includes perceptual, attentional, and memory processes, as well as higher-order cognitive processes, such as cognitive reappraisal and problem-solving. Emotional processing can be influenced by individual differences in emotion regulation and emotion dysregulation, as well as by cultural and social factors.

The vagus nerve, a complex network of nerve fibers that originates in the brainstem and extends throughout the body, is known to be involved in a wide range of physiological processes, including emotion regulation, processing, and communication (Porges, 2007; Rottenberg, 2007). Different vagal control systems seem to be phylogenetically ordered and behaviorally linked to social communication, mobilization and immobilization (Porges, 2007). Activation of the ANS during encoding or retrieval of emotional information may modulate the neural processes mediating mood congruent memory (Critchley et al., 2003; 2005). Neural control of the heartbeat as measured through Heart Rate Variability (HRV) is involved in processing of emotional information (for example, Valenza and Scilingo, 2014; Damasio et al., 2000; Panksepe, 2000; Lewis et al., 2010), and regulation of brain activity dynamics (Craig, 2003; Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2012; Beissner et al., 2013).

At a nervous-system-wise level, the brain and the ANS concurrently regulate all peripheral systems and modulate emotional regulation and processing, as well as mood states (Craig, 2003; Lane et al., 2009; Lane and Wager, 2009; Thayer et al., 2012; Beissner et al., 2013; Candia-Rivera et al., 2022). Functionally, emotions have been linked to the brain’s predictions about the state of the body. The predictions are constantly compared to ascending bodily or interoceptive signals, transmitted, for example, by the vagus nerve (Candia-Rivera et al., 2022; Hsueh et al., 2023). If the resulting prediction error, that is, the difference between the predicted and the actual state, is too big, subjective (e.g., affective) consequences ensue in order to re-balance disturbances of homeostasis, for example, by adaptive behaviour (Seth, 2013). Such interoceptive predictions, which have also been related to depression (Barrett and Simmons, 2015), are reflected in the interaction between the CNS and the ANS (Candia-Rivera et al., 2022; Hsueh et al., 2023).

1.2 Depression, subtypes, and somatic symptoms

Depression is a common disorder characterized by persistent feelings of sadness, loss of interest or pleasure in activities, and other symptoms that impair daily functioning (e.g., feelings of guilt or low self-worth, disturbed sleep or appetite, feelings of tiredness, poor concentration). Depression is associated with a range of cognitive, affective, and behavioral deficits, including difficulties in emotion regulation and emotion processing. Individuals with depression often experience emotion dysregulation, characterized by heightened negative affect, rumination, and reduced positive affect. Indeed, emotion dysregulation, refers to difficulties in regulating emotions effectively. Emotion dysregulation can manifest as intense, unstable, or inappropriate emotional responses, or as difficulties in modifying emotional responses to fit the situation (Abravanel and Sinha, 2015).

In 2008, WHO globally ranked major depression as the third cause of burden of disease and projected that the disease will rank first by 2030 (Malhi and Mann, 2018), unmasking social and economic costs of depressed state (Olesen et al., 2012). Moreover, despite the progress in both pharmacological and psychological therapies, clinicians involved in the management of depression are often faced with treatment resistance, highlighting the necessity to develop alternative therapeutic options. Recent research also suggests that depression symptoms cycle with daily rhythms and hormonal changes (Mendoza, 2019), but there is currently no technology to measure objective biomarkers in daily life. Current diagnosis relies on “structured” interviews based on a patient’s subjective description of symptoms and subsequent interpretation of these by a physician. Diagnosis of major depression results from a positive response to 5 out of 9 listed symptoms, many of which are total opposites of each other (DSM-5). Having a wide variation of symptoms and behaviour, severity, onset and course, depression is a very heterogeneous disorder (Lux and Kendler, 2010; Goldberg, 2011; Lieblich et al., 2015; Drysdale et al., 2017; Feczko et al., 2019). While behavioural correlates of depression are reported in DSM-5 2013, clinically-reliable physiological and biochemical markers for an objective diagnosis are unknown despite encouraging research findings (Rottenberg, 2007; Corrigan et al., 2010; Gaebler et al., 2013; Valenza and Scilingo, 2014; Garcia et al., 2016; Drysdale et al., 2017; Gentili et al., 2017; Brown et al., 2018; Caldwell and Steffen, 2018; Hartmann et al., 2019; Catrambone et al., 2021).

Although several studies have described different subtypes of depression (van Loo et al., 2012; Kessing and Bukh, 2017; Rantala et al., 2018; Beijers et al., 2019), the etiology and etiological factors involved have rarely been studied (Kessing and Bukh, 2017). Clinically, subtypes of depression are important for predicting prognosis and treatment outcome and can be identified by polarity, symptoms, onset (due to a particular event, season or age), recurrence and severity (Thase, 2013). In terms of bipolarity, a distinction is made between unipolar depression and bipolar depression, where bipolar depression is characterized by mood changes, including (hypo) mania and/or mixed episodes. The DSM-5 American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force, 2013 distinguishes four symptom profiles: depression, atypical, anxiety, and psychotic depression. Major depression is characterized by three or more symptoms, including anhedonia or lack of a mood response to positive events, psychomotor inhibition or agitation, weight loss, excessive guilt, and trouble sleeping early in the morning. Major depression affects about 25%–30% of people with depression. People with depression usually do not respond to placebo treatment and may not benefit from psychotherapy and social interventions. Atypical depression features symptoms such as overeating, weight gain, and excessive sleeping, as well as sensitivity to phobias, anxiety, chronic pain, and rejection. Atypical patients generally have a higher proportion of younger women and higher rates of suicide attempts. Anxious depression is associated with abnormal anxiety, fear of tension or loss of control, while psychotic depression presents with delusions or hallucinations with a high relapse rate and frequent hospitalizations in 15%–20% of patients. In terms of onset, early-onset depression (ages 18–30) is more likely to present with personality disorders and neuroticism than late-onset depression (ages 31–70). DSM-5 differentiates mild, moderate, and severe depression based on severity, which predicts long-term risk of relapse and suicide and guides treatment options to some extent (e.g., mild depression cannot be treated with antidepressants). There is no chronic evolution of depressive episodes). Major depression involves an inadequate response to multiple treatments, is complicated by psychotic symptoms, and/or is associated with severe psychotic comorbidities or psychosocial factors (Clarkin et al., 2019). It affects approximately 30% of patients with major depression and there is no consensus on the biological basis (Rush et al., 2006; Fekadu et al., 2009; Trevino et al., 2014). Based on the latent class analysis model, three subtypes of treatment-resistant depression were identified. Major depression (frequency: 66%), moderate depression with anxiety (9%) and mild depression with anxiety/somatization (25%) (Liao et al., 2019).

According to National Institutes of Health and Caregiving guidelines, treatment of depression should include psychological education, low- or high-intensity psychosocial interventions, electroconvulsive therapy, and antidepressant treatment for severe depression. Although first-line pharmacological treatment for major depression usually consists of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor or a serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor as monotherapy, up to 50%–60% of patients with major depression may not respond to the initial treatment and may require alternative therapies or combination treatments (Johnston et al., 2019). Furthermore, there is no evidence that serotonin transporter genotype alone or its interaction with stressful life events is associated with an increased risk of depression (Risch et al., 2009). Lifestyle habits, including diet, exercise and sleep, have been shown to play an important role (Lopresti et al., 2013; Kaseva et al., 2016). Although mainly targeting the brain, clinical procedures in the diagnosis and treatment of depression are already rooted in somatic interventions. The classification of depression subtypes is based on corporeal changes, such as weight loss/gain, and types of movements (retardation/agitation). Specific antidepressant therapies are also chosen based on their effects on somatic symptoms including constipation, nausea, and fatigue, e.g., a patient with nausea will be treated with mirtazapine rather than another serotoninergic drug (NICE National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2018). In the treatment of depressive symptoms, the so-called “implicit memory,” present in terms of somatic and affective states without awareness of connections with past experiences, is fundamental for trauma-focus psychotherapeutic treatment (Hase et al., 2017).

1.3 Vagal activity correlates of depression and depressive symptoms

Dysfunctional cardiac vagal activity has been increasingly linked to depression, substantiating the notion that a compromised ANS dynamics can contribute to mood disorders. Reduced cardiac vagal control, as measured through HRV series (Rajendra Acharya et al., 2006) is associated with depressive symptoms (Rottenberg, 2007; Kemp et al., 2010; Paniccia et al., 2017; Brown et al., 2018; Caldwell and Steffen, 2018; Hartmann et al., 2019). Reduced vagal activity in major depression was not only associated with depression severity but also predicted the persistence of depressive symptoms (Rottenberg, 2007). In contrast, patients with depression may exhibit increased vagal activity compared to healthy controls when experiencing heightened emotional reactivity in response to emotionally arousing stimuli (Garcia et al., 2016); the observed inconsistency may be attributed to the significant variability in the psychopathology of depressive symptoms among individuals, as well as the influence of different medication types and dosages on vagal activity (Rottenberg, 2007).

Depression may also lead to somatic diseases such as stroke, diabetes, and obesity, which are related to dysfunctions in metabolism, immunity, inflammation, and autonomic regulation (Penninnx et al., 2013). Moreover, dysfunctional vagal activity in depression may often result in heart disease (e.g., Rovai et al., 2015). A meta-analysis showed that depression increased the risk of all strokes by 34%–63% (Dong et al., 2012) and increased the risk by 30%–90% of the population with coronary artery disease (CAD) (Nicholson et al., 2006). Furthermore, the presence of depressive symptoms predicts a worse prognosis for CHD (Van Melle et al., 2004). Conversely, patients with CHD are more likely to develop depressive symptoms or overall major depressive disorder (Konrad et al., 2016). Thus, targeting depressive symptoms not only improves mood but also has a positive effect on CVD outcome (e.g., Angermann et al., 2016).

In line with the aforementioned evidence, vagus nerve stimulation has recently been approved by the EU regulatory body for treatment-resistant depression, supported by promising evidence (e.g., Kumar et al., 2019). In fact, non-invasive stimulation of the vagus nerve improves emotional regulation, confirming that the vagus nerve is causally involved in emotional processing (Colzato et al., 2017; Candia-Rivera et al., 2022; Hsueh et al., 2023).

Evidence on dysfunctional CAN and related BHI dynamics has been reported for depression and emotion dysregulation (Taggart et al., 2011; Terhaar et al., 2012; Gaebler et al., 2013; Gaebler et al., 2016; Catrambone et al., 2019).

In summary, depression is linked to dysfunctional vagal activity, which could potentially contribute to the development of somatic diseases. On one hand, depression might be related to parasympathetic-dominant hypoarousal, leading to diminished emotional expression, sensations of emptiness, helplessness, and hopelessness, excessive drowsiness, cognitive impairments, and weakened defensive responses. On the other hand, depression may be connected to decreased vagal activity levels (Chambers and Allen, 2002), although the findings are not entirely consistent (Rottenberg, 2007).

2 A functional-vagal theory and central-autonomic network perspectives

The aforementioned literature proves that there is a close association between emotional regulation and processing, ANS and CNS control over the body’s internal state. Nonetheless, it seems that contemporary psychiatry and clinical psychology largely assume that the functional and dynamic interactions within the human nervous system during emotional processing, regulation, and dysregulation are primarily driven by brain-based processes rather than by bottom-up processes, such as depression and its various subtypes. To this extent, it is worth mentioning the scientific debate on the nature of emotions that has lasted over a century (James, 1880). Recent research shows that neural control over heartbeat dynamics creates and initiates emotional responses (Candia-Rivera et al., 2022; Hsueh et al., 2023). This scientific finding undermines the “classical” emotion theories that suggest emotions are solely functional states of the brain. Consequently, the current grand challenge is to demonstrate the significant causal involvement of dynamical vagal and sympathovagal activities in depression and its subtypes, which may be reflected in changes in the brain.

So why are vagal activity levels lower when a patient has parasympathetic-dominant hypoarousal symptoms?

To explain the autonomic correlates observed in depression, it is here assumed that depression is a disease involving the parasympathetic nervous system, therefore involving dysregulation of the dynamical CAN through vagal control and sympathovagal interplay. Accordingly, let us assume that time-varying vagal activity results from the complex interaction between body- and brain-related dynamics, and pathological ANS dysfunctions are associated with significant CAN changes and emotional dysregulation. According to the Fourier theory, every time series can be represented as a linear combination (i.e., sums and differences) of sinusoidal functions that have specific amplitude and frequency. Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Functional BHI, HRV series, and vagal activity in exemplary healthy and depressed conditions. Vagal activity is illustrated in the Fourier domain, so each sinusoid represents a component of vagal oscillations. The mean of each sinusoid is linked to the so-called “vagal tone,” which may be reduced in depression. The amplitude of each sinusoid is thought to be linked to vagally-dominant hypoarousing symptoms in depression.

Depression is often characterized by a similar or reduced vagal tone and increased emotional reactivity compared to healthy individuals, coupled with parasympathetic-dominant hypoarousal symptomatology. In any case, depression may be associated with pathological vagal dynamics. In order to transition from pathological vagal dynamics to healthy ones, the amplitude of the sinusoid, i.e., the emotional reactivity (Garcia et al., 2016), must be reduced, and the mean of the sinusoid, i.e., the vagal tone, must be increased. This approach is hereby referred to as the “functional vagal theory of depression” While this theory may be useful in understanding the underlying pathophysiology of depression and its relationship to neurocardiovascular dysfunctions, the non-specificity of brain and neurocardiovascular dynamics (Saul and Valenza, 2021), along with the nonlinear and complex nature of the cardiovascular system, makes its direct translational clinical application challenging.

Indeed, although numerous physiological conditions and pathologies are associated with HRV-derived and brain-signal-derived biomarkers that correlate with either the severity of the pathology or the physiological state, these correlations may not be particularly specific, especially when experimental conditions and methodological approaches vary. Consequently, it is possible that a healthy individual in a particular neuro-autonomic state may exhibit similar dynamics to a subject with pathology in a different psycho-physiological state. The nonlinearity of the cardiovascular system poses challenges since an increase in vagal activity could potentially result in an increase in heart rate, as the effect of vagal stimulation on heart rate strongly depends on the level of sympathetic stimulation occurring concurrently (Sunagawa et al., 1998). This phenomenon, known as accentuated antagonism, is partly attributed to the inhibitory effect of the vagally released ACh on the release of NorEpinephrine from nearby sympathetic nerve endings. Additionally, the complexity of the cardiovascular system, resulting from the many interactions of numerous sub-components, means that the system as a whole exhibits properties that the individual components acting alone cannot demonstrate. The system’s nonlinearity, coupled with the multiple feedback mechanisms of sympathetic and vagal activity on cardiovascular control, makes the system extremely sensitive to input, even infinitesimal changes (Sunagawa et al., 1998). Therefore, we do not explicitly endorse the use of the mean of the sinusoid, i.e., the so-called vagal tone, to detect depression. Rather, it is the author’s opinion that brain-heart related biomarkers are more promising in targeting specific oscillations and spatial information over the scalp or brain region associated with afferent or efferent peripheral activity (Pfurtscheller et al., 2017; Catrambone et al., 2021; Pfurtscheller et al., 2021; Pfurtscheller et al., 2022; Pfurtscheller et al., 2023).

Prospectively, the diagnosis and treatment of depression and its subtypes should move from a symptomatic- and brain-centred view exclusively to a functional and whole-body framework where vagally-mediated CAN dynamics play a crucial and causal role. In other words, modern medicine for this highly disabling mood disorder should reverse the current diagnostic and treatment framework for depression by moving from a symptomatic-only and brain-dominated view to a new functional-vagal and whole-body conception. To this extent, science needs to achieve the following goals:

Gain Fundamental Knowledge on SympathoVagal activity & Functional Brain-Body interplay. There is the need to develop effective biomarkers of dynamical sympathovagal activity and associated CAN/brain-heart interplay to characterize physiological sympathovagal activity and functional BHI in healthy conditions. These biomarkers may be defined from generic multivariate signal processing methods and subsequent feature extraction in the time, frequency, and/or nonlinear and complexity domains; Characterise brain-heart-mediated emotional responses. There is the need to characterise a dynamical emotional profile in the healthy through time-varying autonomic- and CAN-related metrics. Gender, age, and other socio-demographic factors should be considered because they are known to affect heartbeat dynamics and, therefore, CAN-related and BHI-related metrics; Characterise brain-heart-mediated biomarkers specific of emotional dysregulation and depression. An impaired dynamical emotional profile of depression should then be characterised throughout treatment by mapping and comparing associated autonomic and CAN and BHI patterns to a healthy profile. Experimental paradigms including standardised emotional elicitation as well as personalised recall scripts combined with non-invasive brain stimulation should be employed to stimulate the emergence of dysfunctional emotion regulation. The desired outcome may be achieved through identifying personalized autonomic and BHI-related features that are distinct to each individual and contribute to their depressive symptomatology. By utilizing these features, the pace and intensity of brain and vagal stimulations can be adjusted accordingly, enabling concurrent progress towards remission and recovery.

Taking a functional-vagal perspective and a broader view of the nervous system into account can help clinicians better understand the clinical symptoms of depression that drive current practice and allow for objective neurophysiological assessment. Instead of changing the current clinical paradigm, this approach can complement the existing diagnostic framework and provide new diagnostic criteria that take into account the essential role of aetiology and pathophysiology in diagnostic decision-making. By adopting quantitative assessments based on specific BHI-related disease biomarkers, modern psychiatry can align itself with other medical specialties, including cardiology and neurology. This approach will enable psychiatry to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of the biological underpinnings of mood disorders, bringing it closer to the diagnostic and treatment practices of these related fields. This holistic view may contribute to the update of the so-called Research Domain Criteria (Morris and Cuthbert, 2012), which proposes to study five psychopathological domains including negative and positive valence, cognitive systems, social processes, and arousal/regulatory systems to better understand mood disorders.

It is important to note that a comprehensive investigation of dysfunctional neural dynamics in depression should also consider sympathetic dynamics. In terms of symptomatology, sympathetically dominated hyperarousal is marked by emotional hyperactivity, reactivity, impulsivity, anxiety, and anger, accompanied by parasympathetic hyposensitivity. Evidence concerning the correlation between sympathetic activity and depression is relatively scarce compared to vagally-related measures, likely due to challenges in quantitatively assessing sympathetic activity (Valenza et al., 2018). Nevertheless, it is posited that dysfunctional sympathetic dynamics may contribute to the increased risk of cardiac complications observed in depression (Nicholson et al., 2006; Barton et al., 2007; Dong et al., 2012). Indeed, exposure to risk or trauma has been found to stimulate the ANS, resulting in sympathetic and/or parasympathetic hyperalertness, leading to a pathological mood state in which an individual is unable to experience positive emotional states (Corrigan et al., 2010). When uncontrolled ANS (i.e., sympathetic-dominant hyperarousal and/or parasympathetic-dominant hypoarousal) cannot control heightened emotional or depressive states, patients often report being unable to cope with emotional and physiological arousal (Ogden, 2006; Corrigan et al., 2010). The “window of tollerance” model of autonomic arousal states that there is a “window” of healthy autonomic tolerance when intense emotions and a state of calm or relaxation can be integrated and integrated throughout the body. The periaqueductal grey area, which is also part of the CAN, is thought to be involved in these mechanisms (Corrigan et al., 2010; Beissner et al., 2013). Identifying CNS-ANS or brain-heart markers of depression—ideally under naturalistic conditions—can help maintain or re-establish a healthy “Window of Tolerance,” for example, through personalised concurrent brain-body stimulation.

Considering the swiftly growing body of scientific evidence highlighting dysfunctional BHI dynamics in a range of pathological conditions (Taggart et al., 2011; Tahsili-Fahadan et al., 2017; Joyner, 2019; Riching et al., 2019; Riganello et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2020; Méloux et al., 2020; Costagliola et al., 2021; Nowroozpoor et al., 2021; Tumati et al., 2021; Vaccarino et al., 2021; Bekała et al., 2022; Grassi et al., 2022; Kumral et al., 2022; Lazaridi et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2022; Seligowski et al., 2022; Wang and Peng, 2022; Xue et al., 2022), it is possible that cardiologists will run EEG/psychometric/emotional assessments before their standard clinical evaluation. Likewise, psychiatrists and clinical psychologists may need to conduct EEG/MRI scans and 24-h cardiac holter monitoring to consider depression as a neurocardiovascular disorder.”

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

GV wrote the manuscript. The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

Work partially supported by the European Commission Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under Grant 101017727″EXPERIENCE” and by the Italian Ministry of Education and Research (MIUR) in the Framework of the FoReLab Project (Departments of Excellence).

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author GV declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abravanel, B. T., and Sinha, R. (2015). Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology. J. psychiatric Res. 61, 89–96. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angermann, N., Gelbrich, G., Störk, S., Gunold, H., Edelmann, F., Wachter, R., et al. (2016). Effect of escitalopram on all-cause mortality and hospitalization in patients with heart failure and depression: The MOOD-HF randomized clinical trial. Jama 315, 2683–2693. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.7635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L., Karmakar, C., Gray, R., Jindal, R., Lim, T., and Bryant, C. (2018). Heart rate variability alterations in late life depression: A meta-analysis. J. Affect Disord. 235, 456–466. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16 (7), 419–429. doi:10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, D. A., Dawood, T., Lambert, E. A., Esler, M. D., Haikerwal, D., Brenchley, C., et al. (2007). Sympathetic activity in major depressive disorder: Identifying those at increased cardiac risk? J. Hypertens. 25 (10), 2117–2124. doi:10.1097/HJH.0b013e32829baae7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beijers, L., Wardenaar, K. J., van Loo, H. M., and Schoevers, R. A. (2019). Data-driven biological subtypes of depression: Systematic review of biological approaches to depression subtyping. Mol. psychiatry 24 (6), 888–900. doi:10.1038/s41380-019-0385-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beissner, F., Meissner, K., Bär, K. J., and Napadow, V. (2013). The autonomic brain: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis for central processing of autonomic function. J. Neurosci. 33, 10 503–510 511. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekała, A., Płotek, W., Siwicka-Gieroba, D., Sołek-Pastuszka, J., Bohatyrewicz, R., Biernawska, J., et al. (2022). Melatonin and the brain–heart crosstalk in neurocritically ill patients—from molecular action to clinical practice. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 23 (13), 7094. doi:10.3390/ijms23137094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (1993). The central autonomic network: Functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clin. Proc. 68, 988–1001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, Y. T., and Steffen, P. R. (2018). Adding HRV biofeedback to psychotherapy increases heart rate variability and improves the treatment of major depressive disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 131, 96–101. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Candia-Rivera, D., Catrambone, V., Thayer, J. F., Gentili, C., and Valenza, G. (2022). Cardiac sympathetic-vagal activity initiates a functional brain–body response to emotional arousal, PNAS 119 (21), 21195991199–e2119599212. doi:10.1073/pnas.2119599119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catrambone, V., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Gentili, C., and Valenza, G. (2021). Intensification of functional neural control on heartbeat dynamics in subclinical depression. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 221–310. doi:10.1038/s41398-021-01336-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catrambone, V., Greco, A., Vanello, N., Scilingo, E. P., and Valenza, G. (2019). Time-resolved directional brain–heart interplay measurement through synthetic data generation models. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 47, 1479–1489. doi:10.1007/s10439-019-02251-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, A. S., and Allen, J. J. (2002). Vagal tone as an indicator of treatment response in major depression. Psychophysiology 39 (6), 861–864. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3960861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarkin, J. F., Petrini, M., and Diamond, D. (2019). Complex depression: The treatment of major depression and severe personality pathology. J. Clin. Psychol. 75 (5), 824–833. doi:10.1002/jclp.22759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colzato, L. S., Sellaro, R., and Beste, C. (2017). Darwin revisited: The vagus nerve is a causal element in controlling recognition of other’s emotions. Cortex 92, 95–102.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, F. M., Fisher, J. J., and Nutt, D. J. (2010). Autonomic dysregulation and the Window of Tolerance model of the effects of complex emotional trauma. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 17–25. doi:10.1177/0269881109354930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costagliola, G., Orsini, A., Coll, M., Brugada, R., Parisi, P., and Striano, P. (2021). The brain–heart interaction in epilepsy: Implications for diagnosis, therapy, and SUDEP prevention. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurology 8 (7), 1557–1568. doi:10.1002/acn3.51382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). Pain mechanisms: Labeled lines versus convergence in central processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 26, 1–30. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., and Josephs, O. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 126, 2139. doi:10.1093/brain/awg216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J. Comp. neurology 493 (1), 154–166. doi:10.1002/cne.20749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, N., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3 (10), 1049–1056. doi:10.1038/79871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, J. Y., Zhang, Y. H., Tong, J., and Qin, L. Q. (2012). Depression and risk of stroke: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Stroke 43, 32–37. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.630871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drysdale, M., Grosenick, L., Downar, J., Dunlop, K., Mansouri, F., Meng, Y., et al. (2017). Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nat. Med. 23 (1), 28–38. doi:10.1038/nm.4246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DSM-5 American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™. United States: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Feczko, E., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Marr, M., Graham, A. M., Nigg, J. T., and Fair, D. A. (2019). The heterogeneity problem: Approaches to identify psychiatric subtypes. Trends cognitive Sci. 23, 584–601. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fekadu, A., Wooderson, S. C., Markopoulo, K., Donaldson, C., Papadopoulos, A., and Cleare, A. J. (2009). What happens to patients with treatment-resistant depression? A systematic review of medium to long term outcome studies. J. Affect. Disord. 116, 4–11. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaebler, M., Daniels, J. K., Lamke, J. P., Fydrich, T., and Walter, H. (2013). Heart rate variability and its neural correlates during emotional face processing in social anxiety disorder. Biol. Psychol. 94 (2), 319–330. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, R. G., Valenza, G., Tomaz, C. A., and Barbieri, R. (2016). Relationship between cardiac vagal activity and mood congruent memory bias in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 190, 19–25. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentili, C., Benvenuti, S. M., Palomba, D., Greco, A., Scilingo, E. P., and Valenza, G. (2017). Assessing mood symptoms through heartbeat dynamics: An HRV study on cardiosurgical patients. J. Psychiatric Res. 95, 179–188. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, D. (2011). The heterogeneity of "major depression". heterogeneity “major depression” World Psychiatry10 10 (3), 226–228. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00061.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grassi, G., Mancia, G., and Esler, M. (2022). Central and peripheral sympathetic activation in heart failure. Cardiovasc. Res. 118 (8), 1857–1871. doi:10.1093/cvr/cvab222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, P. E., Fournier, A. A., Sisitsky, T., Simes, M., Berman, R., Koenigsberg, S. H., et al. (2021). The economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2010 and 2018). Pharmacoeconomics 39 (6), 653–665. doi:10.1007/s40273-021-01019-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 26 (1), 1–26. doi:10.1080/1047840x.2014.940781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, S., and Canli, T. (2004). Individual differences in emotion processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14 (2), 233–238. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, R., Schmidt, F. M., Sander, C., and Hegerl, U. (2019). Heart rate variability as indicator of clinical state in depression. Front. psychiatry 9, 735. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hase, M., Balmaceda, U. M., Ostacoli, L., Liebermann, P., and Hofmann, A. (2017). The AIP model of EMDR therapy and pathogenic memories. Front. Psychol. 8, 1578. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsueh, B., Chen, R., Jo, Y., Tang, D., Raffiee, M., Kim, Y. S., et al. (2023). Cardiogenic control of affective behavioural state. Nature 615, 292–299. doi:10.1038/s41586-023-05748-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaffe, D. H., Rive, B., and Denee, T. R. (2019). The humanistic and economic burden of treatment-resistant depression in Europe: A cross-sectional study. BMC psychiatry 19, 247–311. doi:10.1186/s12888-019-2222-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1880). The feeling of effort. Boston, MA: The Society.

Google Scholar

Johnston, K. M., Powell, L. C., Anderson, I. M., Szabo, S., and Cline, S. (2019). The burden of treatment-resistant depression: A systematic review of the economic and quality of life literature. J. Affect. Disord. 242, 195–210. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2018.06.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joyner, M. J. (2019). Depression depresses vasodilation. Circulation Res. 124, 465–466. doi:10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.119.314595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaseva, K., Rosenström, T., Hintsa, T., Pulkki-Råback, L., Tammelin, T., Lipsanen, J., et al. (2016). Trajectories of physical activity predict the onset of depressive symptoms but not their progression: A prospective cohort study. J. Sports Med. 2016, 8947375. doi:10.1155/2016/8947375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., and Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: A review and meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 67 (11), 1067–1074. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessing, L. V., and Bukh, J. D. (2017). The clinical relevance of qualitatively distinct subtypes of depression. World Psychiatry 16 (3), 318–319. doi:10.1002/wps.20461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konrad, R., Jacob, L., Rapp, M. A., and Kostev, K. (2016). Depression risk in patients with coronary heart disease in Germany. World J. Cardiol. 8, 547–552. doi:10.4330/wjc.v8.i9.547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Bunker, M. T., Aaronson, S. T., Conway, C. R., Rothschild, A. J., Mordenti, G., et al. (2019). Durability of symptomatic responses obtained with adjunctive vagus nerve stimulation in treatment-resistant depression. Neuropsychiatric Dis. Treat. 15, 457–468. doi:10.2147/NDT.S196665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumral, D., Al, E., Cesnaite, E., Kornej, J., Sander, C., Hensch, T., et al. (2022). Attenuation of the heartbeat-evoked potential in patients with atrial fibrillation. Clin. Electrophysiol. 8 (10), 1219–1230. doi:10.1016/j.jacep.2022.06.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., McRae, K., and Reiman, E. M. (2009). Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage 44 (1), 213–222. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., and Wager, T. D. (2009). Introduction to a special issue of NeuroImage on brain–body medicine. Neuroimage 47, 781–784. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazaridi, M., Panagiotaropoulou, G., Covanis, P., Karantinos, T., Aggelopoulos, E., Klein, C., et al. (2022). Brain-heart link in schizophrenia: Cognitive inhibitory control deficit in patients is specifically related to parasympathetic dysregulation. Schizophr. Bull. 48 (5), 1155–1163. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbac033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Barrett, L. F. (2010). Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Liao, L., Wu, Z., Mellor, D., Peng, D., Zhang, C., Xu, J., et al. (2019). Subtypes of treatment-resistant depression determined by a latent class analysis in a Chinese clinical population. J. Affect. Disord. 249, 82–89. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2019.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieblich, S. M., Castle, D. J., Pantelis, C., Hopwood, M., Young, A. H., and Everall, I. P. (2015). High heterogeneity and low reliability in the diagnosis of major depression will impair the development of new drugs. BJPsych Open 17 (2), e5–e7. doi:10.1192/bjpo.bp.115.000786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, M., Liu, J., Zhang, L., Xu, W., He, D., Wei, W., et al. (2020). An evidence of brain-heart disorder: Mental stress-induced myocardial ischemia regulated by inflammatory cytokines. Neurological Res. 42 (8), 670–675. doi:10.1080/01616412.2020.1783879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Zhang, X., Wu, Z., Huang, K., Yang, C., and Yang, L. (2022). Brain–heart communication in health and diseases. Brain Res. Bull. 183, 27–37. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2022.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopresti, A. L., Hood, S. D., and Drummond, P. D. (2013). A review of lifestyle factors that contribute to important pathways associated with major depression: Diet, sleep and exercise. J. Affect. Disord. 148 (1), 12–27. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lux, V., and Kendler, K. S. (2010). Deconstructing major depression: A validation study of the DSM-IV symptomatic criteria. Psychol. Med. 40, 1679–1690. doi:10.1017/S0033291709992157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malhi, G. S., and Mann, J. J. (2018). Depression. Lancet 392, 2299–2312. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31948-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Méloux, A., Béjot, Y., Rochette, L., Cottin, Y., and Vergely, C. (2020). Brain-heart interactions during ischemic processes: Clinical and experimental evidences. Stroke 51 (2), 679–686. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.119.027732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendoza, J. (2019). Circadian insights into the biology of depression: Symptoms, treatments and animal models. Behav. Brain Res. 376, 112186. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S. E., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2012). Research domain criteria: Cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 14 (1), 29–37. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.1/smorris

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, D., Cieslik, E. C., Serbanescu, I., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2017). Altered brain activity in unipolar depression revisited: Meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. JAMA psychiatry 74 (1), 47–55. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NICE National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2018). Depression in adults: Recognition and management. London, UK: NICE.

Google Scholar

Nicholson, A., Kuper, H., and Hemingway, H. (2006). Depression as an aetiologic and prognostic factor in coronary heart disease: A meta-analysis of 6362 events among 146 538 participants in 54 observational studies. Eur. Heart J. 27, 2763–2774. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehl338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowroozpoor, A., Gutterman, D., and Safdar, B. (2021). Is microvascular dysfunction a systemic disorder with common biomarkers found in the heart, brain, and kidneys?a scoping review. Microvasc. Res. 134, 104123. doi:10.1016/j.mvr.2020.104123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, P. (2006). Trauma and the body: A sensorimotor approach to psychotherapy. New York: Norton.

Google Scholar

Olesen, M., Svensson, M., Wittchen, H. U., and Jönsson, B. (2012). The economic cost of brain disorders in Europe. Eur.J. Neurol. 19, 155–162. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2011.03590.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paniccia, M., Paniccia, D., Thomas, S., Taha, T., and Reed, N. (2017). Clinical and non-clinical depression and anxiety in young people: A scoping review on heart rate variability. Auton. Neurosci. 208, 1–14. doi:10.1016/j.autneu.2017.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2000). The cradle of consciousness: A periconscious emotional homunculus?: Commentary by jaak panksepp (bowling green). Neuropsychoanalysis 2 (1), 24–32. doi:10.1080/15294145.2000.10773278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penninx, X., Milaneschi, Y., Lamers, F., and Vogelzangs, N. (2013). Understanding the somatic consequences of depression: Biological mechanisms and the role of depression symptom profile. BMC Med. 11 (1), 129. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Blinowska, K. J., Kaminski, M., Rassler, B., and Klimesch, W. (2022). Processing of fMRI-related anxiety and information flow between brain and body revealed a preponderance of oscillations at 0.15/0.16 Hz. Sci. Rep. 12 (1), 9117. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-13229-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Blinowska, K. J., Kaminski, M., Schwerdtfeger, A. R., Rassler, B., Schwarz, G., et al. (2021). Processing of fMRI-related anxiety and bi-directional information flow between prefrontal cortex and brain stem. Sci. Rep. 11 (1), 22348. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-01710-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Kaminski, M., Blinowska, J., Rassler, B., Schwarz, G., and Klimesch, W. (2023). Respiration-entrained brain oscillations in healthy fMRI participants with high anxiety. Sci. Rep. 13 (1), 2380. doi:10.1038/s41598-023-29482-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Schwerdtfeger, A. R., Seither-Preisler, A., Brunner, C., Aigner, C. S., Brito, J., et al. (2017). Brain–heart communication: Evidence for “central pacemaker” oscillations with a dominant frequency at 0.1 Hz in the cingulum. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128 (1), 183–193. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74 (2), 116–143. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajendra Acharya, U., Paul Joseph, K., Kannathal, N., Lim, C. M., and Suri, J. S. (2006). Heart rate variability: A review. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 44, 1031–1051. doi:10.1007/s11517-006-0119-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantala, M. J., Luoto, S., Krams, I., and Karlsson, H. (2018). Depression subtyping based on evolutionary psychiatry: Proximate mechanisms and ultimate functions. Brain Behav. Immun. 69, 603–617. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2017.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riching, A. S., Major, J. L., Londono, P., and Bagchi, R. A. (2019). The brain–heart axis: alzheimer’s, diabetes, and hypertension. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 3 (1), 21–28. doi:10.1021/acsptsci.9b00091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Larroque, S. K., Di Perri, C., Prada, V., Sannita, W. G., and Laureys, S. (2019). Measures of CNS-autonomic interaction and responsiveness in disorder of consciousness. Front. Neurosci. 13, 530. doi:10.3389/fnins.2019.00530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K. Y., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., et al. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Jama 301 (23), 2462–2471. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rottenberg, J. (2007). Cardiac vagal control in depression: A critical analysis. Biol. Psychol. 74 (2), 200–211. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovai, D., Giannessi, D., and Andreassi, M. G. (2015). Mind injuries after cardiac surgery. J. Cardiovasc. Med. Hagerst. Md.) 16, 844–851. doi:10.2459/JCM.0000000000000133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Wisniewski, S. R., Nierenberg, A. A., Stewart, J. W., Warden, D., et al. (2006). Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: A STAR*D report. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 (11), 1905–1917. doi:10.1176/ajp.2006.163.11.1905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saul, J. P., and Valenza, G. (2021). Heart rate variability and the dawn of complex physiological signal analysis: Methodological and clinical perspectives. Philosophical Trans. R. Soc. A 379 (2212), 20200255. doi:10.1098/rsta.2020.0255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligowski, A. V., Webber, T. K., Marvar, P. J., Ressler, K. J., and Philip, N. S. (2022). Involvement of the brain–heart axis in the link between PTSD and cardiovascular disease. Depress. anxiety 39 (10-11), 663–674. doi:10.1002/da.23271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cognitive Sci. 17 (11), 565–573. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunagawa, K., Kawada, T., and Nakahara, T. (1998). Dynamic nonlinear vago-sympathetic interaction in regulating heart rate. Heart vessels 13, 157–174. doi:10.1007/BF01745040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taggart, P., Boyett, M. R., Logantha, S. J. R., and Lambiase, P. D. (2011). Anger, emotion, and arrhythmias: From brain to heart. Front. Physiol. 2, 67. doi:10.3389/fphys.2011.00067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tahsili-Fahadan, P., and Geocadin, R. G. (2017). Heart–brain axis: Effects of neurologic injury on cardiovascular function. Circulation Res. 120 (3), 559–572. doi:10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.116.308446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terhaar, S., Viola, F. C., Bär, K. J., and Debener, S. (2012). Heartbeat evoked potentials mirror altered body perception in depressed patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123 (10), 1950–1957. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thase, M. E. (2013). The multifactorial presentation of depression in acute care. J. Clin. Psychiatry 74 (2), 3–8. doi:10.4088/JCP.12084su1c.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., and Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 747–756. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevino, K., McClintock, S. M., McDonald Fischer, N., Vora, A., and Husain, M. M. (2014). Defining treatment-resistant depression: A comprehensive review of the literature. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 26 (3), 222–232.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tumati, S., Paulus, M. P., and Northoff, G. (2021). Out-of-step: Brain-heart desynchronization in anxiety disorders. Mol. Psychiatry 26 (6), 1726–1737. doi:10.1038/s41380-021-01029-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaccarino, V., Shah, A. J., Mehta, P. K., Pearce, B., Raggi, P., Bremner, J. D., et al. (2021). Brain-heart connections in stress and cardiovascular disease: Implications for the cardiac patient. Atherosclerosis 328, 74–82. doi:10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2021.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, G., and Scilingo, E. P. (2014). “Autonomic nervous system dynamics for mood and emotional-state recognition: Significant advances in data acquisition, signal processing and classification,” in Series in BioEngineering (Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 1–162.

Google Scholar

Valenza, G., Citi, L., Saul, J. P., and Barbieri, R. (2018). Measures of sympathetic and parasympathetic autonomic outflow from heartbeat dynamics. J. Appl. physiology 125 (1), 19–39. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00842.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, R., Passamonti, L., Duggento, A., Toschi, N., and Barbieri, R. (2020). Uncovering complex central autonomic networks at rest: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on complex cardiovascular oscillations. J. R. Soc. Interface 17, 2019087–2019112. doi:10.1098/rsif.2019.0878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, R., Sclocco, R., Duggento, A., Passamonti, L., Napadow, V., Barbieri, R., et al. (2019). The central autonomic network at rest: Uncovering functional MRI correlates of time-varying autonomic outflow. Neuroimage 197, 383–390. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Loo, H. M., de Jonge, P., Romeijn, J. W., Kessler, R. C., and Schoevers, R. A. (2012). Data-driven subtypes of major depressive disorder: A systematic review. BMC Med. 10, 156. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-10-156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Melle, M., de Jonge, P., Spijkerman, T. A., Tijssen, J. G. P., Ormel, J., van Veldhuisen, D. J., et al. (2004). Prognostic association of depression following myocardial infarction with mortality and cardiovascular events: A meta-analysis. Psychosom. Med. 66, 814–822. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000146294.82810.9c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., and Peng, Y. (2022). Advances in brain-heart syndrome: Attention to cardiac complications after ischemic stroke. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 15, 1053478. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2022.1053478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe (2018). Depression. Avaliable At: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/noncommunicable-diseases/mental-health/areas-of-work/depression.

Google Scholar

Xue, T., Chiao, B., Xu, T., Li, H., Shi, K., Cheng, Y., et al. (2022). The heart-brain axis: A proteomics study of meditation on the cardiovascular system of Tibetan monks. EBioMedicine 80, 104026. doi:10.1016/j.ebiom.2022.104026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: heart rate variabiity (HRV), central autonomic network (CAN), depression, vagal activity, sympathetic activity, brain-heart interplay, brain-heart axis

Citation: Valenza G (2023) Depression as a cardiovascular disorder: central-autonomic network, brain-heart axis, and vagal perspectives of low mood. Front. Netw. Physiol. 3:1125495. doi: 10.3389/fnetp.2023.1125495

Received: 16 December 2022; Accepted: 04 May 2023;
Published: 16 May 2023.

Edited by:

Florian Chouchou, Université de la Réunion, France

Reviewed by:

Laurent M. Arsac, Université de Bordeaux, France
Andy Schumann, University Hospital Jena, Germany

Copyright © 2023 Valenza. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gaetano Valenza, Z2FldGFuby52YWxlbnphQHVuaXBpLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.