- 1Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Tübingen Hearing Research Centre, Molecular Physiology of Hearing, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- 2Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNS), Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
- 3Bayer Health Care Pharmaceuticals, Global Drug Discovery Pharma Research Centre Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany
- 4Institute of Pharmacy, Pharmacology, Toxicology and Clinical Pharmacy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
The complex mechanism by which stress can affect sensory processes such as hearing is still poorly understood. In a previous study, the mineralocorticoid (MR) and/or glucocorticoid receptor (GR) were deleted in frontal brain regions but not cochlear regions using a CaMKIIα-based tamoxifen-inducible CreERT2/loxP approach. These mice exhibit either a diminished (MRTMXcKO) or disinhibited (GRTMXcKO) auditory nerve activity. In the present study, we observed that mice differentially were (MRTMXcKO) or were not (GRTMXcKO) able to compensate for altered auditory nerve activity in the central auditory pathway. As previous findings demonstrated a link between central auditory compensation and memory-dependent adaptation processes, we analyzed hippocampal paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) and long-term potentiation (LTP). To determine which molecular mechanisms may impact differences in synaptic plasticity, we analyzed Arc/Arg3.1, known to control AMPA receptor trafficking, as well as regulators of tissue perfusion and energy consumption (NO-GC and GC-A). We observed that the changes in PPF of MRTMXcKOs mirrored the changes in their auditory nerve activity, whereas changes in the LTP of MRTMXcKOs and GRTMXcKOs mirrored instead the changes in their central compensation capacity. Enhanced GR expression levels in MRTMXcKOs suggest that MRs typically suppress GR expression. We observed that hippocampal LTP, GC-A mRNA expression levels, and ABR wave IV/I ratio were all enhanced in animals with elevated GR (MRTMXcKOs) but were all lower or not mobilized in animals with impaired GR expression levels (GRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs). This suggests that GC-A may link LTP and auditory neural gain through GR-dependent processes. In addition, enhanced NO-GC expression levels in MR, GR, and MRGRTMXcKOs suggest that both receptors suppress NO-GC; on the other hand, elevated Arc/Arg3.1 levels in MRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs but not GRTMXcKOs suggest that MR suppresses Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels. Conclusively, MR through GR inhibition may define the threshold for hemodynamic responses for LTP and auditory neural gain associated with GC-A.
Highlights
• Baseline MR suppresses GR levels.
• Enhanced GR suppresses presynaptic hippocampal excitability.
• Baseline MR and GR suppress hippocampal NO-GC.
• Baseline MR inhibits Arc/Arg3.1.
• Enhanced GR increases GC-A, LTP, and auditory neural gain.
Introduction
Hearing dysfunction in response to age, acoustic trauma, or posttraumatic stress has been linked with different stress responses possibly influencing cognitive functions (Meltser and Canlon, 2011; Canlon et al., 2013; Basner et al., 2014; Jafari et al., 2019; Mazurek et al., 2019; Wang and Puel, 2020; Nadhimi and Llano, 2021). During stress, the naturally occurring glucocorticoid hormones (corticosterone in rodents and cortisol in humans) activate two different receptors. Besides aldosterone, the mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) shows high affinity to glucocorticoids, while the glucocorticoid receptor (GR) gradually becomes occupied by stress-induced high glucocorticoid concentrations (see for a review de Kloet et al., 2018). Both MR and GR actions need to be in balance for maintenance of homeostasis and health (see for a review de Kloet et al., 2018). The expression of MR dominates in glutamatergic neurons of the hippocampus, while GR is more ubiquitously expressed in the CNS (Reul and de Kloet, 1985; Chao et al., 1989; de Kloet et al., 2005; McEwen et al., 2016). To better understand the thus far elusive relationship between balanced stress receptor activity, hearing, and cognition, we previously employed a Cre/loxP-based approach in which a tamoxifen (TMX)-inducible Cre is expressed in the forebrain and hippocampus via the CaMKIIα promoter (Dragatsis and Zeitlin, 2000; Wang et al., 2013). The resulting MRGRCaMKIIαCreERT2 double knockout (MRGRTMXcKO) lacked MR and GR expression in frontal brain and in the hippocampal region, while cochlear MR and GR expression was unchanged (Marchetta et al., 2022). Remarkably, the examination of MRCaMKIIαCreERT2 single knockout (MRTMXcKO), GRCaMKIIαCreERT2 single knockout (GRTMXcKO), and MRGRTMXcKO mice unraveled unfavorable effects of central MR deletion and favorable effects of central GR deletion on peripheral auditory nerve processing (Marchetta et al., 2022), suggesting a top-down effect of central MR and GR activities. We reconsidered that the coordinated function of central MR and GR are predominantly predicted to influence the extinction (MR) and consolidation (GR) of long-term potentiation (LTP; see for a review de Kloet et al., 2018). Any adaptive central auditory responses following sound enrichment or acoustic trauma were found to be accompanied by increased hippocampal LTP levels (Matt et al., 2018; Likhtik and Johansen, 2019; Knipper et al., 2020; Marchetta et al., 2020b; Knipper et al., 2022; Manohar et al., 2022; Savitska et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2022). The association between adaptive central auditory responses following sound enrichment or auditory trauma and LTP changes is suggested to be triggered through corticothalamic feedforward and feedback circuits (Antunes and Malmierca, 2021; Knipper et al., 2022). This encouraged us to investigate the influence of changed peripheral auditory processing in MRTMXcKO and GRTMXcKO (Marchetta et al., 2022) on central auditory responses of these animals in relation to short- and long-term hippocampal plasticity responses. Since the stress-induced drop in central adaptive auditory responses is restorable by inhibition of 3′,5′-cyclic guanosine monophosphate (cGMP) hydrolyzing phosphodiesterase 9A (Savitska et al., 2022), we additionally asked to what extent cGMP-producing nitric oxide (NO)-sensitive (NO-GC), encoded by the mammalian Gucy1a1, Gucy1a2, and Gucy1b1 genes, and membrane-bound (GC-A) guanylyl cyclase, encoded by the mammalian Npr1 gene, may correlate with changes in auditory or hippocampal circuits acutely induced by MR and/or GR deletion. This is of particular interest because cGMP plays an important role in AMPA and NMDA receptor signaling, facilitating synaptic plasticity and memory formation (Giesen et al., 2022), specifically including functions of NO-GC (Koesling et al., 2016; Nelissen et al., 2021, 2022) and possibly also GC-A (Kroker et al., 2012). As AMPA receptor surface diffusion during LTP was shown to be changed by GR activation (Groc et al., 2008), the mRNA of activity-regulated cytoskeletal protein (Arc, also known as Arg3.1) that controls AMPA receptor trafficking during LTP/long-term depression (LTD; Guzowski et al., 1999; Plath et al., 2006; Kuipers et al., 2016) was analyzed.
Based on the herein presented findings, we conclude that balanced basal MR and GR activities play a critical role in setting the threshold for presynaptic reactivities in the hippocampus. Basal MR expression may suppress GR expression levels, which enables higher levels of LTP. This dynamic setting of MR and GR activity appears to keep the threshold for presynaptic excitability (PPF through GR), the NO-GC level (through MR and GR), and neuronal Arc/Arg3.1 level (through MR) low. The elevation of GR levels may lead to elevated hippocampal GC-A and Arc/Arg3.1, elevated LTP, and elevated central auditory neural gain. We conclusively suggest that GR-induced changes in GC-A activity are involved in auditory neural gain, and thus may provide a means for altering corticofugal top-down feedback.
Materials and methods
Animals
Animal care, use, and experimental protocols correspond to national and institutional guidelines and were reviewed and approved by University of Tübingen, Veterinary Care Unit, and the Animal Care and Ethics Committee of the regional board of the Federal State Government of Baden-Württemberg, Germany. All experiments were performed according to the European Union Directive 2010/63/EU for the protection of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes. In-house bred mice were kept in a specified pathogen free facility at 25°C on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle with average noise levels of around 50–60 dB SPL. The weight of the animals was recorded on each experimental day.
In the present study, three TMX-inducible conditional knockout mouse lines were studied, in which MR and/or GR were deleted mainly in the forebrain. MRGRTMXcKO, MRTMXcKO, GRTMXcKO and corresponding control animals were generated as previously described (Marchetta et al., 2022). In brief, homozygous floxed MR, GR (Berger et al., 2006; Erdmann et al., 2007), or MRGR lines, in which exon 3 of Mr and/or Gr is flanked by loxP sites, were crossed with a CaMKIIα CreERT2 line (kindly provided by Prof. Günther Schütz). After confirmation of normal hearing function, all mice received an intraperitoneal injection of 1 mg TMX in 100 μL TMX-solution (Sigma-Aldrich, T-5648, Munich) twice a day for five consecutive days at the lowest age of approximately eight weeks. 50 mg TMX were dissolved in 500 μL Ethanol abs. (Merck, Darmstadt) and 4.5 mL sunflower oil (Sigma-Aldrich, S-5007). After the last injection, animals were allowed to recover in their home cages for four weeks before experiments started. For the respective transgenic mouse line, homozygous floxed Cre-negative littermates, which also received TMX injections, were used as controls. For all lines, mice of both sexes ranging between 1.8 months (beginning of the experiment) and 7.9 months (end of the experiment) were used. The genetic status of all mouse lines was confirmed by genotyping using gene-specific PCR protocols.
Hearing measurements
Mice were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of a mixture of Fentanyl (0.05 mg/kg bodyweight (BW), Fentanyl-Hameln, Hameln Pharma plus, Hameln, Germany), Midazolam (5.0 mg/kg BW, Midazolam-hameln®; Hameln Pharma plus), Medetomidin (0.5 mg/kg BW, Sedator®; Albrecht, Aulendorf, Germany) and atropine sulfate (0.2 mg/kg BW, B. Braun, Melsungen, Germany) diluted with water ad. inj. (Ampuwa, Fresenius KABI, Bad Homburg, Germany) to an injection volume of 10 mL/kg BW. Additional doses of anesthetics were administered if needed. The anesthesia was antagonized after the measurements by a subcutaneously administered mixture of Naloxon (1.2 mg/kg BW, Naloxon-hameln®; Hameln Pharma plus), Flumazenil (0.55 mg/kg BW, Flumazenil®; Fresenius KABI), and Atipazemol (2.5 mg/kg BW, Antisedan®; VETOQUINOL GmbH, Ravensburg, Germany) diluted with water ad. inj. (Ampuwa) to an injection volume of 10 mL/kg BW.
The anesthetized mice lay on a pre-warmed resting pad (37°C) in a soundproof chamber (IAC 400-A, Industrial Acoustics Company GmbH, Niederkrüchten, Germany). Auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) in anesthetized mice were evoked by short-duration sound stimuli with the same stimulus parameters for all groups. The ABRs represent the summed activity of neurons in distinct anatomical structures along the ascending auditory pathway recorded from subcutaneous cranial electrodes. A microphone (Bruel & Kjaer 4191, Naerum, Denmark) was used to calibrate and record the acoustic stimuli. ABR thresholds were elicited with click stimuli (100 μs duration with an FFT mean of 5.4 kHz). The stimulus level was increased stepwise from 10 to 100 dB SPL in 5 dB steps. Stimuli were generated with an I-O-card (PCI-6052E, PCI-6251, or PCIe-6259, National Instruments, Austin, Texas, United States) in an IBM compatible computer. The SPL of the stimuli was modulated by custom-made amplifier and attenuator systems (Wulf Elektronik, Frankfurt, Germany). The measured signals were band-pass filtered from 200 Hz to 5 kHz (F1, 6-pole Butterworth hardware Filter, Wulf Elektronik) and amplified by 100,000. The analog/digital (A/D) rate was 20 kHz. Each stimulus had a recording interval of 16 ms and was directly repeated and averaged up to 512 times.
Field excitatory postsynaptic potential recordings in hippocampal slices
Extracellular field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) recordings were performed according to standard methods as previously described (Matt et al., 2011; Chenaux et al., 2016). In brief, 400 μm thick hippocampal brain slices were coronally sectioned on a vibratome (Leica VT 1000S, Wetzlar, Germany) in ice-cold dissection buffer (mM): 127 NaCl, 1.9 KCl, 1.2 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, 2 MgSO4, and 1.1 CaCl2, constantly saturated with 5% CO2 and 95% O2 (pH 7.4). Slices were incubated in carbogenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (in mM: 127 NaCl, 1.9 KCl, 1.2 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, 1 MgSO4, 2.2 CaCl2; pH 7.4) for 1 h at 30°C and afterwards stored at room temperature. Recordings were performed in a submerged-type recording chamber (Warner Instruments, Holliston, MA, United States). Stimulation (TM53CCINS, WPI, Sarasota, FL, United States) and recording (artificial cerebrospinal fluid-filled glass pipettes, 2–3 MΩ) electrodes were positioned in the stratum radiatum to record Schaffer collateral fEPSPs. Signals were amplified with an Axopatch 200B (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA, United States), digitized at 5 kHz with an ITC-16 (HEKA, Reutlingen, Germany) and recorded using WinWCP from the Strathclyde Electrophysiology Suite. Stimuli (100 μs) were delivered through the stimulus isolator (WPI). For each individual slice the strength of the stimulation (typically between 30 and125 μA) was chosen to evoke 40%–60% of the maximal response, defined by initial fEPSP slope. Only slices that showed stable fiber volley (FV) and fEPSP were used for further recording. The same stimulus intensity was applied during baseline recording (0.067 Hz, 20–30 min) and during induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) using 100 stimuli during 1 s (100 Hz). The baseline was determined by averaging fEPSP initial slopes from the period before the tetanic stimulation (at least 15 min of stable recording). The level of LTP was determined by averaging fEPSP slopes from the period between 50 and 60 min after the high-frequency stimulation (HFS). Before the tetanic stimulation, each slice was used to record paired-pulse facilitation [PPF, 10–20–50–100–200–500 ms interstimulus interval (ISI) at the same stimulation strength as LTP recordings]. The paired-pulse ratio of EPSP2/EPSP1 slope and amplitude at each ISI were defined per slice and mean values per group were plotted. EPSP1 was calculated as an average of EPSP1s from all ISIs for each single slice.
Four traces were averaged for each single data point analyzed.
Riboprobe synthesis
To amplify Arc/Arg3.1, we used the following primers: for: 5′-CGA AGT GTC CAA GCA GGT G-3′; and rev: 5′-TGA TGG CAT AGG GGC TAA CA-3′. To amplify NO-GC, we used the following primers: for: 5′-ATC CTC TTC AGC GGC ATT GTG-3′ and rev: 5′-TGC ATT GGT TCC TTC TTG CCC-3′. To amplify GC-A, we used the following primers: for: 5′-TGT GAA ACG TGT GAA CCG GA-3′ and rev: 5′-AGG CGG ATC GTT GAA AGG G-3′. To amplify GR, we used the following primers: for: 5′-TCC CCC TGG TAG AGA CGA AG-3′ and rev: 5′-GGC TGG TCG ACC TAT TGA GG-3′. To amplify MR, we used the following primers: for: 5′-GAG ATG AGG CTT CTG GGT GT-3′ and rev: 5′-CAG GAT CAT GGA CGG GGA TG-3′. These fragments were cloned into the pCR II Topo vector (Invitrogen, Karlsruhe, Germany) and their nucleotide sequences were verified by an automated sequencer. Plasmids were isolated using QIAprep Spin Miniprep Kit from Qiagen (Hilden, Germany). Complementary strands for sense and antisense riboprobes were transcribed from either Sp6 or T7 RNA polymerases and labeled using rNTP mix containing digoxigenin labeled uridine triphosphates. All restriction enzymes, RNA polymerases and digoxigenin-labeled rNTP were purchased from Roche Diagnostics (Mannheim, Germany).
Co-localization of mRNA and protein in brain sections
A separate subset of mice from those used for in vitro electrophysiology measurements was deeply anesthetized with CO2 and then sacrificed by decapitation. Brain tissue was prepared and sectioned with a vibratome at 60 μm, as previously described (Singer et al., 2013a, 2016). mRNA and protein were co-localized on free-floating brain sections as previously described (Singer et al., 2013a). In brief, following prehybridization for 1 h at 37°C, sections were incubated overnight with NO-GC, GC-A, Arc/Arg3.1, MR, or GR riboprobes at 56°C, incubated with anti-digoxigenin antibody conjugated to alkaline phosphatase (anti-Dig-AP, Roche, 11093274910), and developed as previously described (Singer et al., 2013a). For protein detection, streptavidin–biotin was blocked according to the manufacturer’s instructions (Streptavidin–Biotin Blocking Kit, Vector Laboratories, Newark, CA, United States) after blocking endogenous peroxidase. Sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with the primary antibody against parvalbumin (Abcam, Berlin, Germany, anti-rabbit, 1:500, ab11427) as a marker for inhibitory neurons, followed by incubation with the secondary antibody (biotinylated goat anti-rabbit, Vector Laboratories, BA-1000) and chromogenic detection (AEC, 3-amino-9-ethylcarbazole, Vector Laboratories, SK-4200). For microscopy (BX61 microscope, Olympus, Hamburg, Germany) evaluation photographs of the hippocampus and auditory cortex were taken at a bregma position between −1.58 and −2.18 with a bright-field camera (DP 71, Olympus) for detection of mRNA and protein, without adjusting the picture frame or the plane of focus.
Identification of GRE binding sites in NO-GC and GC-A upstream regions
To identify potential glucocorticoid-responsive elements (GRE) binding sites in NO-GC and GC-A upstream regions, Benchling was used to import and annotate the following genes: Gucy1a1, Gucy1a2, Gucy1b1, and Npr1. Subsequently, known sequences for GRE binding sites were aligned (see Supplementary Methods). These sequences were based on JASPAR and sequences previously identified by Meijsing et al. (2009), Polman et al. (2013), and van Weert et al. (2017).
Quantification and statistical analysis
All statistical information and n-numbers can be found in the results section and in Supplementary Table S1. Data was tested for a normal distribution (the Shapiro–Wilk normality test, α = 0.05). Differences of the means were compared for statistical significance either by unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test (parametric)/Mann–Whitney U-test (non-parametric), repeated measurement (RM) 2-way ANOVA, 2-way ANOVA (parametric) with α = 0.05 and correction for type 1 error using Sidak’s and Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons tests.
In figures, significance and a trend for significance is indicated by asterisks [(*) p < 0.08, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001]. n.s. denotes non-significant results (p ≥ 0.08). The p-values of the 2-way ANOVAs refer to the main effect of the genotype.
ABR analysis
For each individual ear, the peak input–output function for amplitude of the click-ABR measurements were averaged for intensities between 0 and 40 dB relative to threshold (re thr) and analyzed as previously described (Chumak et al., 2016).
Two peak classes were selected: (1) early peaks (at 1.2–1.8 ms, wave I), interpreted as the sum of the first stimulus-related action potential within the auditory nerve, and (2) delayed peaks (at 4.1–4.9 ms, wave IV), the response from the auditory midbrain.
Wave IV/I ratio was calculated by dividing the ABR wave IV amplitude by ABR wave I amplitude for individual animals at all intensities higher than 5 dB (re thr). For each individual ear, stimulus levels of 10–30 dB, 35–55 dB, and 60–80 dB re thr were averaged to yield three repeated measurements for fibers of different sensitivity and spontaneous rate [high-spontaneous rate (SR) low-threshold fibers, middle-SR, and low-SR high-threshold fibers; Bharadwaj et al., 2014]. Inter-peak latency growth functions were calculated by subtracting the ABR wave I latency from the ABR wave IV amplitudes for individual animals for increasing stimulus levels with reference to the ABR thresholds (from higher than 5 dB to a maximum of 80 dB re thr) and grouped into stimulus level ranges as described (10–30 dB, 35–55 dB, and 60–80 dB). For statistical analyses, single ears are used as sample size.
fEPSP recordings in hippocampal slices
Data was analyzed and processed using Clampfit 10 (Molecular Devices) and Excel (Microsoft). The data presented per experimental group/condition contained (in addition to mean ± SEM) single dots showing the fEPSP slope values for each individual brain slice. The n indicates the number of slices and animals (slices/animals) used in the analysis. Recordings which did not show stable baseline or shifting ±2% from the average in the baseline recording were not included in the statistical analysis. For statistical analyses, single slices are used as sample size.
Co-localization of mRNA and protein and immunohistochemistry in brain sections
Brain sections were quantified by integrating density values of color pixels for each single specimen using ImageJ software (NIH, Bethesda, MD; United States). Quantification of the mRNA from the double method staining was performed by artificially separating the image into three defined color “channels,” which were selected as the average signal from all groups of the background, the protein staining, and the mRNA staining. A detection threshold was then defined in the mRNA color “channel” which was consistent across all groups and genotypes. The integrated density was then calculated for each image and all images are averaged for one animal. The density values of all specimens stained within the same experiment were then normalized to the group mean (i.e., all hippocampal brain sections stained in the same experiment gave an average value of 1.0). This correction allowed for compensation of the high intertrial variation of staining intensity. All sections from one mouse were then averaged and entered the statistical evaluation as n = 1. When no quantifiable staining could be measured, the group was excluded from analysis.
Results
Acute deletion of MR in adult mice leads to elevated GR expression levels
Global and conditional MR deletion affected the expression profiles of GR in central neurons (Berger et al., 2006; Erdmann et al., 2007). We questioned if comparable effects are observed after TMX-induced acute deletion in MR and GRTMXcKOs. Brain sections of MR and GRTMXcKOs were exposed to GR- or MR-specific riboprobes. Data was evaluated quantitively as previously described (Singer et al., 2013a; Eckert et al., 2021). As exemplarily shown in Figure 1A, MRTMXcKOs had a significantly higher expression of GR mRNA in the hippocampus in comparison to their WT controls [Figure 1A, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(6) = 3.530, p = 0.0124, n = 4 mice each]. In contrast, conditional ablation of GR did not significantly affect the MR mRNA abundance in the hippocampus in comparison to WT controls [Figure 1B, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(8) = 1.993, p = 0.0814, n = 5 mice each]. The finding confirms that the differential GR expression which was observed in global and conditional MR/GR double- and single-KOs (Berger et al., 2006; Erdmann et al., 2007) is also observed in TMX-induced MRTMXcKOs. This proves the validity of the CaMKIIα-based TMX-inducible CreERT2/loxP approach for central deletion of MR and GR.
Figure 1. GR and MR mRNA expression level (blue) in the CA1 region of the hippocampus of MR and GRTMXcKO mice. (A) MRTMXcKO (green) mice showed significantly higher GR mRNA expression levels in comparison to their WT controls (gray). (B) GRTMXcKO (red) mice showed no difference in MR mRNA expression levels in comparison to their WT controls (gray). Mean ± SEM. ns = p > 0.08, * = p < 0.05.
This also means that the MRTMXcKO phenotype should not only be interpreted with respect to the central MR deletion but also from the perspective of increased GR expression levels.
Differential impact of central MR and/or GR deletion on central auditory neural gain, PPF, and LTP
We next analyzed the central auditory responses (ABR wave IV) relative to the altered cochlear auditory processing (ABR wave I) of MRTMXcKOs, GRTMXcKOs, and MRGRTMXcKOs mice (Figure 2, left). Raw ABR wave amplitude and latency values were consistent with previously-reported data for all genotypes (data not shown; Marchetta et al., 2022). ABR wave IV/I ratio (input/output function) analysis for stimulus ranges corresponding to auditory fibers of different sensitivity and response range [10–30 dB, 35–55 dB, and 60–80 dB re thr for high-spontaneous rate (SR), middle-SR, and low-SR auditory fibers, respectively (Bharadwaj et al., 2014)] revealed that the reduced auditory nerve response of MRTMXcKOs could be centrally compensated through neural gain [higher ABR wave IV/I ratio; Figure 2A, left, repeated measurement (RM) 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 49) = 7.652, p = 0.008, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, MR WT: n = 30 ears from 16 mice, MRTMXcKO: n = 21 ears from 14 mice]. Accordingly, the inter-peak latency of MRTMXcKO mice reached similar values in comparison to their WT controls (Supplementary Figure S2A). On the other hand, the TMX-mediated deletion of central GR resulted in slightly reduced ABR wave IV/I ratio indicating less neural gain, although this effect was not statistically significant (Figure 2B, left, RM 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 28) = 3.284, p = 0.0807, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, GR WT: n = 14 ears from 8 mice, GRTMXcKO: n = 16 ears from 8 mice), despite the enhanced/disinhibited ABR wave amplitudes in these conditional mutants (Marchetta et al., 2022). The inter-peak latency of GRTMXcKO mice was significantly shorter in comparison to their WT controls, particularly above 30 dB (Supplementary Figure S2B). Finally, the conditional deletion of both MR and GR in the MRGRTMXcKOs, resulted in an unchanged ABR wave IV/I ratio (Figure 2C, left, RM 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 42) = 0.1546, p = 0.6962, MRGR WT: n = 19 ears from 11 mice, MRGRTMXcKO: n = 25 ears from 14 mice). We interpret this as an intermediate response resulting from counterbalancing effects of the individual cKOs. In addition, the inter-peak latency of MRGRTMXcKO mice was equally long as for their WT controls (Supplementary Figure S2C). As we assume that in our mouse model MR and GR are not deleted in the ascending auditory pathway (cochlea, brainstem), the alterations observed in auditory neural gain are likely a top-down effect resulting from the forebrain deletion of MR and/or GR, as also described to be the case for blood pressure changes resulting from MR overexpression under the CamKIIα promoter (Lai et al., 2007).
Figure 2. ABR wave IV/I amplitude ratio and LTP of MRTMXcKO, GRTMXcKO, and MRGRTMXcKO in comparison to their WT controls. (A) MRTMXcKOs (green) had a significantly higher wave IV/I ratio in comparison to their WT controls (gray). The MRTMXcKOs had significantly higher LTP in comparison to their WT controls. (B) GRTMXcKOs (red) had a slightly, non-significantly lower wave IV/I ratio compared to their WT controls (gray). GRTMXcKOs had a trend toward lower LTP in comparison to their WT controls. (C) MRGRTMXcKOs (blue) did not show a significant difference of wave IV/I ratio in comparison to their WT controls (gray). Contrary to this, MRGRTMXcKOs had significantly lower LTP in comparison to their WT controls. Mean ± SEM. ns = p > 0.08, (*) = p < 0.08, * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01.
The predicted influence of stress and hippocampal LTP on central auditory adaptive responses (Singer et al., 2013b; Jafari et al., 2018; Matt et al., 2018; Savitska et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2022) motivated us to study hippocampal LTP on acute coronal brain slices in the individual and compound MR and GRTMXcKO models (Figure 2, right). LTP was induced by tetanic stimulation (1 s, 100 Hz) of CA3 Schaffer’s collateral axons, and fEPSPs were recorded from the dendrites of CA1 pyramidal cells that form synaptic contacts with CA3 neurons (Matt et al., 2018). LTP, determined by averaging fEPSP slopes from the period between 50 and 60 min after the high-frequency stimulation (HFS), was significantly higher after 50–60 min with changes seen from ~35 min onwards in MRTMXcKOs (Figure 2A, right, green; 162.16% ± 8.25%, n = 9 slices from 5 animals) in comparison to WT controls [Figure 2A, right, gray; 125.44% ± 7.90%, n = 7 slices from 4 animals; Mann–Whitney U-test, U(117.8, 167.9) = 6, p = 0.0093], while the GRTMXcKOs had a trend toward lower LTP in comparison to WT controls [Figure 2B, right, GRTMXcKO, red, 128.96% ± 8.98%, n = 9 slices from 6 animals, WT, gray, 165.52% ± 18.27%, n = 7 slices from 5 animals; Mann–Whitney U-test, U(151.0, 124.0) = 12, p = 0.0721]. On the other hand, MRGRTMXcKOs exhibited significantly lower LTP in comparison to their respective WT controls [Figure 2C, right, MRGRTMXcKO, blue, 122.88% ± 8.12%, n = 13 slices from 7 animals, WT, gray, 157.30% ± 13.18%, n = 12 slices from 6 animals; Mann–Whitney U-test, U(160.7, 121.3) = 45, p = 0.0076].
To investigate to what extent the differences in central compensation or LTP are associated with differences in the presynaptic state of Schaffer’s collaterals in the hippocampus, we studied paired-pulse facilitation (PPF), an indication of presynaptic activity underlying short-term plasticity. PPF, a transient increase in the probability of glutamate release (Zucker and Regehr, 2002), was studied in MRTMXcKO, GRTMXcKO, and MRGRTMXcKO brain slices as described in methods and previous studies (Satake et al., 2012).
The PPF was investigated in each brain slice prior to LTP induction using varying ISIs of 10, 20, 50, 100, 200, and 500 ms and the same stimulation strength that was also used for LTP recordings (Figure 3). The MRTMXcKO mice had a significantly lower PPF, seen in its slope ratio, in comparison to their WT controls [Figure 3A, green vs. gray circles, 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 78) = 14.48, p = 0.0003, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, MRTMXcKO: n = 8 slices from 5 animals, WT: n = 7 slices from 4 animals]. In contrast, in GRTMXcKO mice, the PPF slope ratio was not significantly different from their respective controls [Figure 3B, red vs. gray circles, 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 78) = 2.172, p = 0.1446, GRTMXcKO: n = 9 slices from 6 animals, WT: n = 7 slices from 5 animals]. In MRGRTMXcKO mice also, no difference in PPF slope ratio was observed in comparison to their WT controls [Figure 3C, blue vs. gray circles, 2-way ANOVA, F(1, 138) = 2.217, p = 0.1388, MRGRTMXcKO: n = 13 slices from 7 animals, WT: n = 12 slices from 6 animals].
Figure 3. PPF as an indicator of short-term plasticity. (A) The analysis of PPF in MRTMXcKOs (green) showed a significantly lower paired-pulse ratio of the EPSP2/EPSP1 slope in comparison to WT controls (gray) with a significant post-hoc test at 50 ms ISI. (B) GRTMXcKOs (red) showed no difference in paired-pulse ratio of the EPSP2/EPSP1 slope in comparison to WT controls (gray). (C) MRGRTMXcKOs (blue) showed no difference in paired-pulse ratio of the EPSP2/EPSP1 slope in comparison to WT controls (gray). Mean ± SEM. ns = p > 0.08, *** = p < 0.001.
We further examined the fEPSP slopes and fiber volley (FV) amplitudes of individual cKOs, in order to ensure that the effects observed in the LTP and PPF were not due to a change in the basal synaptic transmission properties. fEPSP slope or FV amplitudes in MRTMXcKOs (Supplementary Figure S3A), GRTMXcKOs (Supplementary Figure S3B), and MRGRTMXcKOs (Supplementary Figure S3C) were not different from their respective WT controls. Further, the increase in fEPSP slope remained proportional to FV amplitudes and did not differ between the different genotypes (Supplementary Figure S3A–C, bottom). Overall, the findings suggest that basal synaptic transmission properties were unchanged by cKO of MR and/or GR.
In conclusion, the reduced PPF in MRTMXcKO implies that MR coordinates the release probability in the presynapse of hippocampal neurons, possibly through elevation of GR expression levels. We further conclude that the enhanced LTP and ABR wave IV/I ratio in MRTMXcKOs are linked to the reduced suppression of GR expression levels in these mice. In that sense, GR expression levels regulate LTP and auditory neural gain. The lack of effect in GRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs is consistent with this notion.
Deletion of MR and/or GR in the hippocampus differentially affects NO-GC, GC-A, and Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA levels
To better understand the differential impact of acute deletion of MR and GR function on hippocampal LTP and auditory processing, we tested for altered cGMP generator expression profiles, previously hypothesized to provide the missing link between auditory processing and LTP. The mRNA expression profiles of a crucial NO-GC subunit and membrane-bound GC-A were analyzed in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Two isoforms of NO-GC exist: the more widely expressed NO-GC1 and NO-GC2. Both isoforms build a heterodimer complex with the beta-1 subunit (Koesling et al., 2016), the changes of which parallel that of the alpha subunit (Mergia et al., 2006; Koesling et al., 2016). Therefore, we targeted NO-GC β1 mRNA for analysis of expression profiles. The recently observed potentiating influence of altered cGMP generator activity on LTP and synaptic AMPA receptor transport activity (Nelissen et al., 2021, 2022) moreover motivated us to correlate levels of cGMP generators with changes of cytoplasmic Arc/Arg3.1, the mRNA of which is targeted to dendrites during LTP/LTD changes in response to neuronal activity (Korb and Finkbeiner, 2011; Goel et al., 2019). In situ hybridizations of NO-GC β1, GC-A, and Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA were analyzed in brain sections and quantified in the CA1 region of the hippocampus according to previously established protocols (Matt et al., 2018; Eckert et al., 2021; Figure 4). MRTMXcKOs exhibited significantly higher levels of NO-GC, Arc/Arg3.1, and GC-A mRNA in comparison to their WT controls [Figure 4A, NO-GC left, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(10) = 3.651, p = 0.0045, n = 6 mice each; Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA middle, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(6) = 53.63, p < 0.0001, n = 4 mice each; GC-A mRNA right, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(8) = 6.874, p = 0.0001, n = 5 mice each]. GRTMXcKOs also had significantly higher levels of NO-GC mRNA [Figure 4B, left, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(8) = 3.924, p = 0.0044, n = 5 mice each] but strikingly showed no significant differences in levels of Arc/Arg3.1 and GC-A mRNA in comparison to their WT controls [Figure 4B, Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA middle, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(6) = 0.4093, p = 0.6965, n = 4 mice each; GC-A mRNA right, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(8) = 0.3671, p = 0.7231, n = 5 mice each]. MRGRTMXcKO displayed significantly higher levels of NO-GC mRNA and Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA, as also observed in MRTMXcKOs [Figure 4C, NO-GC, left, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(4) = 11.17, p = 0.0004, n = 3 mice each; Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA middle, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(4) = 2.883, p = 0.0449, n = 3 mice each]. On the other hand, the GC-A expression levels in the MRGRTMXcKO were significantly reduced [Figure 4C, right, unpaired two-tailed student’s t-test, t(4) = 5.941, p = 0.0040, n = 3 mice each].
Figure 4. NO-GC, Arc/Arg3.1, and GC-A expression in the CA1 region of the hippocampus in MR, GR, and MRGRTMXcKO mice. (A) MRTMXcKO (green) mice showed significantly higher levels of NO-GC mRNA expression, Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA expression, and GC-A mRNA expression in comparison to their WT controls (gray). (B) GRTMXcKO (red) mice showed significantly higher NO-GC mRNA expression levels, equal Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA expression levels, and equal GC-A mRNA expression levels in comparison to their WT controls (gray). (C) MRGRTMXcKO (blue) mice showed significantly higher levels of NO-GC mRNA expression levels and Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA expression, but significantly lower GC-A mRNA expression levels in comparison to their WT controls (gray). Mean. ns = p > 0.08, * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001.
We additionally tested for altered cGMP generator expression levels in the auditory cortex of MRTMXcKO and GRTMXcKO mice in neurons of all cortical layers (layer I–VI). We found that GC-A expression levels in the auditory cortex for both MRTMXcKO and GRTMXcKO mice mirror the findings in the hippocampus (Supplementary Figure S4), hypothalamus, and amygdala (data not shown). NO-GC expression levels in MRTMXcKO and GRTMXcKO mice were also both significantly higher in the auditory cortex in comparison to their respective WT controls (Supplementary Figure S4), as also observed in the hippocampus; however, NO-GC levels throughout the rest of the brain were more variable (data not shown).
In order to test whether MR or GR directly influence NO-GC or GC-A via their classical transcriptional cis-activation GR element binding motifs, a search was carried out for MR/GR-specific binding motifs (GRE sequences) in the genes encoding NO-GC (i.e., Gucy1a1, Gucy1a2, Gucy1b1) and GC-A (Npr1). No relevant GRE sequences could be detected in Gucy1b1 or Npr1. For Gucy1a1 and Gucy1a2, each gene was found to contain a conserved GRE sequence (Supplementary Figures S5, S6). Specifically, for Gucy1a1, this GRE sequence (5′-AGGAACACCATGTTCTG-3′) was found to span across the start codon of the transcribed part of the gene. This also included an Atoh sequence (5′-CAGAAGG-3′) 25 bp upstream of the GRE sequence. For Gucy1a2, the GRE sequence was a palindrome (5′-AGAACAAACTGTTCT-3′) located near multiple regulatory regions in the intron between exons 3 and 4.
Altogether, we here demonstrate that (i) decreased PPF in MRTMXcKOs may be linked to elevated GR expression levels (Figure 5A). (ii) Enhanced NO-GC expression levels in MR, GR, and MRGRTMXcKOs, imply that both receptors’ activity is required to effectively suppress NO-GC in hippocampal neurons under physiological conditions, which may be supported by GRE elements responding to both MR and GR (Figure 5B). (iii) Increased Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels in MRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs but not in GRTMXcKOs (Figure 5C) do not correlate to LTP as simply. The changes of Arc/Arg3.1 in MRGRTMXcKOs rather point to basal MR activity but not GR activity as being inhibitory to Arc/Arg3.1 levels (Figure 5C). Further, the gradient of Arc/Arg3.1 changes in MRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs suggests that elevated GR expression may stimulate Arc/Arg3.1 (Figure 6B). (iv) MRTMXcKOs exhibited an increased GR expression, suggesting that under physiological conditions MR suppresses GR expression. When this suppression is removed in MRTMXcKOs, animals exhibit elevated LTP, GC-A, and ABR wave IV/I ratio. This is not seen in GRTMXcKOs, suggesting that GR may contribute to the empowerment of central neural gain during auditory adaption by controlling the energization through GC-A (Figure 5D).
Figure 5. A summary of the hypothesized effects of MR and GR in a physiological state and stress state (GR↑). (A) When at a normal level, MR inhibits GR (green dashed line, upper half). However, neither MR nor GR affect PPF when at physiological levels. At elevated GR conditions (lower half), PPF is low. (B) Both stress receptors exhibit an inhibitory effect on NO-GC expression levels, though the inhibitory effect of MR (green solid line) is stronger than that of GR (red dashed line). (C) MR inhibits Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels (green dashed line, upper half); however, when GR is elevated, Arc/Arg3.1 is stimulated (lower half). (D) Basal MR stimulates GC-A expression levels and LTP (green dashed line, upper half). Elevated GR expression also leads to elevated GC-A and to elevated LTP (lower half).
Figure 6. An abstract scheme of the pathways that can be concluded from the acute deletion of MR (green). (A) MR deletion leads to elevated GR expression levels. (B) As a result of elevated GR, an increase in GC-A and Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels in the hippocampus can be observed. (C) This change in expression pattern may be functionally related to higher hippocampal LTP. (D) Elevated GC-A and Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels and elevated LTP potentially provide a means for top-down mechanisms allowing for auditory neural gain.
Discussion
By investigating TMX-induced conditional single or combined deletion of MR and GR in forebrain regions, we show for the first time that central MR and GR activity must be balanced in order to set thresholds for presynaptic activity and central auditory compensation (auditory neural gain). Changes in MR and GR activities result in significant alterations of cGMP generator expression levels in hippocampal neurons. In detail, we suggest (i) baseline hippocampal MR expression levels suppress GR expression. (ii) MR-induced control of GR may set a threshold for presynaptic plasticity. (iii) Basal MR may set a threshold for AMPA receptor trafficking by Arc/Arg3.1, while elevated GR levels may enhance Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels. (iv) Elevated GR may enhance LTP and GC-A expression levels and thereby contribute to LTP-dependent neural auditory gain.
Basal MR activities suppress GR expression
Until now, insights into the physiological function of stress receptors in the adult brain have been limited because global GR KOs (Cole et al., 1995) or early Cre-mediated deletion of GR (Erdmann et al., 2008) is lethal in mice. Also, a global lack of MR induces early postnatal death linked with bodily dehydration (exsiccosis) due to massive renal sodium and water loss (Berger et al., 1998). Early Cre-induced conditional MR deletion is expected to allow long-term adaptation processes to compensate for the absence of MR (Erdmann et al., 2008). Some of these limitations were avoided by the acute TMX-induced deletion of MR and GR studied here. Although in Marchetta et al., 2022, a TMX-induced deletion of MR and GR protein was only shown for the MRGRTMXcKO and not for the single MRTMXcKO and GRTMXcKO, the observed higher GR expression levels in MRTMXcKOs, also previously reported in global or conditional MRTMXcKO mice (Berger et al., 2006; Erdmann et al., 2007), may point to successful targeting of individual receptor expression. We however cannot entirely exclude that the receptor protein may not have been completely deleted (knocked out), at the time of the experiments. Nevertheless, it can be assumed that an upregulation of GR in MRTMXcKOs reflects the previously-described balancing effect of MR on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which stems from central neurons (see for a review de Kloet et al., 2018). Thus, elevated corticosterone levels were reported in conditional GR but not MR mutants (Erdmann et al., 2007), and consistently higher corticosterone levels were measured in GRTMXcKOs but not in MRTMXcKOs (Marchetta et al., 2022). In line with this, normal corticosterone levels—occurring when HPA axis feedback regulation is balanced—are the result of physiologically lower GR levels when baseline MR activities prevent excessive GR expression (Harris et al., 2013), as also concluded from the findings of the present study. This tight control of the HPA axis was recently demonstrated in heterozygous GR mutants that had an enhanced HPA axis activity in response to restraint stress (Harris et al., 2013). Interestingly, the HPA axis overshoot in heterozygous GR mutants in response to stress was counterbalanced through an overexpression of MR in these heterozygous GR mutants (Harris et al., 2013). Apparently, MR exerts a tonic inhibitory influence on HPA axis activity, and thereby determines the threshold of reactivity during stress (see for a review de Kloet et al., 2018).
In conclusion, we suggest that the predicted tonic inhibitory influence of MR on the HPA axis is severely disturbed upon the acute MR or GR deletion.
GR may suppress plasticity in presynaptic hippocampal activity
The TMX-induced deletion of MR in hippocampal regions of MRTMXcKOs resulted in reduced PPF (Figures 3, 5A). Because PPF results from a prior accumulation of residual Ca2+ at the synaptic terminal and a lingering effect of Ca2+ on the exocytotic Ca2+ sensor of releasable vesicles during the second stimulus (as reviewed in Thomson, 2000; Zucker and Regehr, 2002), we expect the probability of vesicular release to be transiently decreased in MRTMXcKOs. This is likely due to increased GR expression in these mutants, as can be concluded from the absence of PPF effects in GRTMXcKO and MRGRTMXcKO (Figure 5A). We thus assume that baseline MR or GR activity alone do not strongly affect presynaptic excitability, but rather that increased GR activity, as occurs in the MRTMXcKOs, suppresses PPF (Figures 1A, 3A, 5A). This finding may contrast findings in which corticosterone application in hippocampal CA1 neurons resulted in a brief increase in miniature excitatory postsynaptic current frequency, a feature described to occur through MR activities (Sarabdjitsingh et al., 2016; de Kloet et al., 2018; Joels, 2018). Further studies are required to clarify this controversy. It is noteworthy to consider the findings in our previous study, which implied a reduced activity at the inner hair cell synapse in the MRTMXcKO (Marchetta et al., 2022). The peripheral depression of activity in MRTMXcKOs is comparable to the here-observed more central changes of reduced release probability in the presynapse of hippocampal neurons (Figure 3; Marchetta et al., 2022). Overall, it gives credence to the hypothesis that balanced central MR and GR activities set the threshold for sensory activity centrally and peripherally by targeting presynaptic mechanisms, possibly through tuning reactivity of top-down auditory feedback loops, which would need to be examined in future studies.
Baseline MR and GR activity keeps NO-GC expression levels low in neurons
It was previously suggested that the complementary functions of MR and GR on presynaptic excitability in hippocampal brain neurons may be the result of distinct transcriptional networks activated by the glucocorticoid receptors (Reul and de Kloet, 1985; de Kloet et al., 2000; Obradovic et al., 2004; Mifsud and Reul, 2016; McCann et al., 2021).
Considering a possible MR/GR driven transcriptional control of pre- or post-synaptic activity by NO-GC, we searched for respective GREs that bind to MR and GR (Shimba and Ikuta, 2020) in the upstream regions of NO-GC subtypes (Shimba and Ikuta, 2020). GREs constitute a palindromic consensus sequence AGAACAnnnTGTTCT (Polman et al., 2013); however, several other GRE-like sequence motifs have been identified (Meijsing et al., 2009; van Weert et al., 2017). We found a GRE in the start codon region of Gucy1a1 but not in the β1 subunit. In addition to a GRE sequence in Gucy1a1, the detection of a 25 bp Atoh sequence moreover suggests that this GRE binding motif is potentially used as a MR-specific recognition site, since GRE sequences that coincide with an Atoh consensus sequence within 400 bp of the GRE have been defined to be MR-specific (van Weert et al., 2017). Additionally, a GRE motif was found in Gucy1a2 in an intron between exons 3 and 4, apparently completely outside of known regulatory regions, questioning the significance of this potential GRE motif. Regarding the observation of elevated Gucy1b1 NO-GC expression levels both in single MRTMXcKOs and GRTMXcKOs as well as in double MRGRTMXcKOs, we must question how a potential GRE in the start codon region of Gucy1a1, but not β1 subunit may be interpreted in the context of the findings in the present study. While it is necessary to confirm expression changes of Gucy1a1 in MRGRTMXcKOs in future studies, it is important to consider that changes in β1 subunits went along with changes in α1 expression (Mergia et al., 2006; Koesling et al., 2016). Further, expression levels of NO-GC1 and NO-GC2 in the brain mirror each other (Koesling et al., 2016). Thus, we hypothesize that under physiological conditions, NO-GC1 (α1, β1) or NO-GC2 (α2, β1) is suppressed by MR and GR (Figure 5B). The elevation of NO-GC expression in MRGRTMXcKOs in comparison to MRTMXcKOs, which exhibit elevated GR expression levels. This may suggest that the inhibitory effect of MR on NO-GC may outweigh that of GR. Using NO-GC1 and NO-GC2 KO mice, the differential localization of NO-GC1 in hippocampal presynapses and NO-GC2 in postsynapses was shown to contribute to LTP through facilitation of presynaptic (NO-GC1) and postsynaptic (NO-GC2) excitability of hippocampal neurons (see for a review Koesling et al., 2016). Although more detailed studies are needed to validate the functionality of the GRE binding motifs, the data nevertheless suggest one possible pathway of how MR and GR could influence NO-GC1 (Figure 5B). This is in line with the observation that steroid hormone receptors exert positive or negative effects on the expression of target genes (Beato and Klug, 2000). Thereby, our findings suggest that baseline MR and GR activity could control the NO-GC abundance, which may be relevant for sensing NO released from eNOS- and nNOS-producing hippocampal cells to influence plasticity events (Son et al., 1996; Hopper and Garthwaite, 2006).
In conclusion, baseline MR and GR activity may keep NO-GC expression levels in neurons low. The target of a specific GRE element found in upstream regions of Gucy1a1 NO-GC1, the dominant NO-GC isoform in hippocampal presynapses, may need further specification.
Baseline MR may keep Arc/Arg3.1 levels low in neurons
TMX-induced deletion of MR in frontal brain regions resulted in increased Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels in the hippocampus (Figure 4A). As Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels in the hippocampus are elevated in MRGRTMXcKOs but not GRTMXcKOs (Figure 4C), we hypothesize that basal activity of MR but not GR keeps Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels in hippocampal neurons low (Figure 5C). While we hypothesize that basal levels of GR have no effect on Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels, higher levels of GR (as seen in MRTMXcKOs) enhance it (Figure 6B).
Arc/Arg3.1 expression changes have been shown to influence the strength of individual synapses, during both LTP (Rodriguez et al., 2005) and LTD (Guzowski et al., 1999; Plath et al., 2006; Tzingounis and Nicoll, 2006; Park et al., 2008; Waung et al., 2008; Bramham et al., 2010; Yilmaz-Rastoder et al., 2011; Wall and Correa, 2018; Zhang and Bramham, 2021). Moreover it is important to consider that neuronal stimuli induce the rapid transcription of the Arc/Arg3.1 gene (within 5 min; Ramirez-Amaya et al., 2005) and translocation of its mRNA from the nucleus to the cytoplasm (within 30 min; Guzowski et al., 1999). From the cytoplasm, Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA incorporates into a large ribonucleoprotein complex that is actively transported along the dendrite (Dynes and Steward, 2007). From the pre-existing Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA pool in dendrites, a selected pool is translated rapidly by acute neuronal activity, changing surface expression of AMPA receptors (Hedde et al., 2021). The present study may indicate that baseline MR levels influence the level of Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA in the cytoplasm of pyramidal neurons. In addition, when the control of GR expression by MR is lost (as in MRTMXcKOs), Arc/Arg3.1 expression levels are enhanced more than in MRGRTMXcKOs. This indicates that elevated GR levels may stimulate the level of Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA in the cytoplasm of pyramidal neurons. This makes sense considering that previous studies demonstrated that a selective activation of glucocorticoid receptors promotes lateral diffusion and enhanced surface expression of AMPA receptors in CA1 neurons (Karst and Joels, 2005; Groc et al., 2008; Martin et al., 2009), a feature linked with LTP (Groc et al., 2008). It is therefore feasible that MR—both at basal levels and through control of GR—sets the reactivity for Arc/Arg3.1 mobilization in dendrites and its rapid translation in spines through defining the cytoplasmatic Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA pool.
Elevated GR activity relates to elevated LTP, elevated GC-A, and elevated auditory neural gain
A strong association has been suggested between critical auditory nerve activity and central auditory neural gain, measured via enhanced ABR wave IV/I ratio, and plasticity changes in LTP, assessed both through in vitro electrophysiology (Marchetta et al., 2020b; Savitska et al., 2022) and through a hippocampus-dependent learning test in animal models (Matt et al., 2018). It has been speculated that a critical input of auditory activity is necessary for the recruitment of a reinforcement process that interacts between the auditory thalamus, prefrontal cortex (PFC), and the hippocampus. Thus, the dorsal aspect of the MGB, which receives input from the primary auditory cortex and projects to higher-order auditory regions (Antunes and Malmierca, 2021; Mease and Gonzalez, 2021), is likely part of the topographically complex connectivity pattern projecting between the medio-dorsal thalamus, the medial PFC, and the hippocampal formation (Bueno-Junior and Leite, 2018). These corticofugal projections from the PFC influence auditory processing at lower levels of the cortical sensory hierarchy and often include activation of mesolimbic areas, such as the basolateral amygdala, the activation of which is involved in top-down feedback reinforcement processes (Asilador and Llano, 2020; Suga, 2020).
The medio-dorsal thalamus/PFC/hippocampal connectivity (see Figure 6) is not only sensitive to stress responses (for review, see Jett et al., 2017), but is also part of the extra-hypothalamic pathways involved in stress-control, emotional states, attention, and vigilance influenced by glucocorticoids (de Kloet et al., 2000, 2019; Wingenfeld and Otte, 2019; Antunes and Malmierca, 2021; Knipper et al., 2022). Interestingly, the TMX-induced deletion of MR in frontal brain regions resulted in enhanced ABR wave IV/I ratio (neural gain) and LTP. If this response were due to increased GR expression resulting from the loss of MR, an acute deletion of GR should result in opposite changes of ABR wave IV/I ratio and LTP. In line, when GR is deleted (as in GRTMXcKOs and MRGRTMXcKOs), animals do not have higher wave IV/I ratio and their LTP is unchanged or lower, respectively, indicating that GR elevation – but not baseline GR levels – contributes to elevated neural gain and LTP. Interestingly, the higher wave IV/I ratio and LTP in MRTMXcKO occurred with an elevation of GC-A expression levels. Also, lower LTP occurred with lower GC-A, as can be seen from MRGRTMXcKOs (Figure 5D). Comparable changes in GC-A were detected not only in the hippocampus but also in the auditory cortex (Supplementary Figure S4) and amygdala. Differences in GC-A activity may thus provide a means for altering corticofugal top-down feedback facilitation processes (Asilador and Llano, 2020; Suga, 2020) and thereby contribute to auditory neural gain.
Oitzl and de Kloet were the first to demonstrate that MRs and GRs mediate the storage of spatial information in a coordinated manner (Oitzl and de Kloet, 1992). Their study suggested that MR promotes the extinction of information, while GR is essential for consolidation. Supporting this, memory storage was impaired when progesterone and the glucocorticoid antagonist mifepristone was given immediately after the learning trial (Oitzl and de Kloet, 1992), while the MR antagonist spironolactone under the same condition did not affect consolidation but rather retrieval (Oitzl and de Kloet, 1992). Meanwhile, memory storage was shown to be impaired in numerous studies, e.g., when GRs are deleted in the amygdala or hippocampus or when GR antagonists are administered in the hippocampus immediately after learning, prior to consolidation (see for a review de Kloet et al., 1999; Rodrigues et al., 2009; Luksys and Sandi, 2011; Roozendaal and McGaugh, 2011; Schwabe et al., 2012; de Kloet et al., 2018).
Very recently, the treatment of WT mice with a phosphodiesterase 9A inhibitor restored a stress-induced drop of temporal auditory processing and hippocampal LTP (Savitska et al., 2022). This finding can now possibly be linked to elevated auditory neural gain, LTP, and GC-A expression levels. Increased GC-A expression levels here corresponded with elevated GR expression levels when the inhibition of GR was removed in MRTMXcKOs, while expression levels were not elevated in GRTMXcKOs (Figure 4). Indeed, the memory-enhancing phosphodiesterase 9A inhibitor (Kroker et al., 2012) has been suggested to regulate a pool of cGMP that is independent of nNOS (Harms et al., 2019). Other findings suggest that the phosphodiesterase 9A in the brain even preferentially regulates nuclear- and membrane-proximal pools of cGMP, which include the GC-A generated cGMP pools (Patel et al., 2018). Within this framework, the present study links for the first time the recognized function of the GR stress receptors in LTP (de Kloet et al., 2018; Joels, 2018) with the perfusion and blood supply driven metabolism-promoting function of the transmembrane GC-A. GC-A is activated through the atrial natriuretic peptide and brain natriuretic peptide (Potter, 2011) and has meanwhile emerged as a key regulator of energy consumption and metabolism counteracting vasoconstriction by inducing vasodilation (Kuhn, 2016). This role of GC-A has also been linked with neurogenesis (Muller et al., 2009) and angiogenesis (Kuhn et al., 2009), two processes which are essential for proper memory-dependent processes (Anacker and Hen, 2017). A critical role of GC-A for central auditory processing has been suggested through the use of global GC-A KO mice, which are unable to maintain proper central auditory processing following auditory trauma (Marchetta et al., 2020a).
Conclusion
Our data suggest that under physiological conditions, MR suppresses GR expression to keep the threshold for memory consolidation high (Figure 6A). The threshold of LTP is implemented via MR-induced control of postsynaptic Arc/Arg3.1 levels for optimized AMPA-receptor trafficking (Figure 6B), via MR- and GR-induced control of NO-GC (Figure 5B) and neuronal GC-A levels (Figure 6B) for optimized energy supply during hemodynamic responses.
Hemodynamic responses take place within a glutamatergic neural feedforward signaling, that includes a neuronal-derived nitric oxide (NO) release from the glutamatergic synapses as well as endothelial-derived NO from blood vessels (Sohal et al., 2009; Czeh et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2017; Csabai et al., 2018; Czeh et al., 2018; Han et al., 2019). This is followed by a metabolic feedback signal in the smooth muscle cells of parenchymal arterioles, which results in vasodilation (for a review see Attwell et al., 2010; Kisler et al., 2017; Ledo et al., 2021). cGMP signaling plays an integral role for the LTP results of the present study (Sanderson and Sher, 2013; Bradley and Steinert, 2016; Dorner-Ciossek et al., 2017; Borovac et al., 2018). We conclude that GR inhibition by MR may define the threshold for hemodynamic responses through the control of GC-A levels (Figure 6B). Through lowering GR expression levels via MR, the threshold for metabolically-demanding neuronal responses and vasodilation (possibly requiring GC-A) would be kept high. In future studies, it would be of particular interest to investigate how the suppression of NO-GC by MR and GR and the elevation of GC-A by increased GR levels contribute to LTP and related auditory neural gain (Figure 6).
Limitations of the study
The search for GRE- and MR-specific binding motifs requires experimental validation of the functionality of the binding sites. Moreover, the observation that the in silico prediction tools did not identify GRE motifs in the promoter regions of GC-A or Arc/Arg3.1 does not rule out their existence. Adequate validation of the function of GC-A for LTP-dependent auditory processing and its explicit relation to balanced stress levels is also pending and requires inducible deletion of GC-A under conditions comparable to those shown here for MR/GR deletion.
Data availability statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Ethics statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by University of Tübingen, Veterinary Care Unit and the Animal Care and Ethics Committee of the regional board of the Federal State Government of Baden-Württemberg, Germany. Written informed consent was obtained from the owners for the participation of their animals in this study.
Author contributions
MK and LR conceived the study. DC, MH, PM, JM, and WS performed the experiments and analyzed the data. EN and JP performed data sequencing analysis. MK, LR, RL, PR, JP, and PS wrote the manuscript. MK, WS, and LR supervised the work. MK, LR, RL, PR, PS, EN, and JP reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – DC, PM, MK, RL, and PR are members of the Research Training Group [grant number 335549539/GRK 2381]; FOR 2060 project RU 713/3-2, SPP 1608 RU 316/12-1, KN 316/12-1. JM was supported by the Interdisziplinäres Promotionskolleg Medizin of the University of Tübingen. EN and JP are supported by a restricted research grant from BAYER. Each source of funding provided a budget for personnel and material costs. We acknowledge support by Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen. BAYER was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge support by Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen.
Conflict of interest
PS was employed by the company BAYER.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnmol.2023.1017761/full#supplementary-material
Abbreviations
Arc/Arg3.1, activity regulated cytoskeletal protein; ABR, auditory brainstem response; BW, body weight; cGMP, 3′,5′-cyclic guanosine monophosphate; FV, fiber volley; fEPSP, field excitatory postsynaptic potential; GC, glucocorticoids; GR, glucocorticoid receptor; GRE, glucocorticoid response element; GRTMXcKO, GRCaMKIIαCreERT2 knockout; GC-A, guanylyl cyclase A; HFS, high-frequency stimulation; HPA, hypothalamus-pituitary–adrenal; ISI, interstimulus interval; LTD, long-term depression; LTP, long-term potentiation; MR, mineralocorticoid receptors; NO-GC, nitric oxide (NO)-sensitive (soluble) guanylyl cyclase; MRTMXcKO, MRCaMKIIαCreERT2 knockout; MRGRTMXcKO, MRGRCaMKIIαCreERT2 knockout; PPF, paired-pulse facilitation; SEM, standard error of the mean; TMX, tamoxifen.
References
Anacker, C., and Hen, R. (2017). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility—linking memory and mood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 335–346. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.45
Antunes, F. M., and Malmierca, M. S. (2021). Corticothalamic pathways in auditory processing: recent advances and insights from other sensory systems. Front. Neural Circuits 15:721186. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.721186
Asilador, A., and Llano, D. A. (2020). Top-down inference in the auditory system: potential roles for Corticofugal projections. Front. Neural Circuits 14:615259. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2020.615259
Attwell, D., Buchan, A. M., Charpak, S., Lauritzen, M., Macvicar, B. A., and Newman, E. A. (2010). Glial and neuronal control of brain blood flow. Nature 468, 232–243. doi: 10.1038/nature09613
Basner, M., Babisch, W., Davis, A., Brink, M., Clark, C., Janssen, S., et al. (2014). Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet 383, 1325–1332. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61613-X
Beato, M., and Klug, J. (2000). Steroid hormone receptors: an update. Hum. Reprod. Update 6, 225–236. doi: 10.1093/humupd/6.3.225
Berger, S., Bleich, M., Schmid, W., Cole, T. J., Peters, J., Watanabe, H., et al. (1998). Mineralocorticoid receptor knockout mice: pathophysiology of Na+ metabolism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 9424–9429. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.16.9424
Berger, S., Wolfer, D. P., Selbach, O., Alter, H., Erdmann, G., Reichardt, H. M., et al. (2006). Loss of the limbic mineralocorticoid receptor impairs behavioral plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 195–200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503878102
Bharadwaj, H. M., Verhulst, S., Shaheen, L., Liberman, M. C., and Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2014). Cochlear neuropathy and the coding of supra-threshold sound. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:26. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00026
Borovac, J., Bosch, M., and Okamoto, K. (2018). Regulation of actin dynamics during structural plasticity of dendritic spines: signaling messengers and actin-binding proteins. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 91, 122–130. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2018.07.001
Bradley, S. A., and Steinert, J. R. (2016). Nitric oxide-mediated posttranslational modifications: impacts at the synapse. Oxid. Med. Cell. Longev. 2016, 5681036–5681039. doi: 10.1155/2016/5681036
Bramham, C. R., Alme, M. N., Bittins, M., Kuipers, S. D., Nair, R. R., Pai, B., et al. (2010). The arc of synaptic memory. Exp. Brain Res. 200, 125–140. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1959-2
Bueno-Junior, L. S., and Leite, J. P. (2018). Input convergence, synaptic plasticity and functional coupling across hippocampal-prefrontal-thalamic circuits. Front. Neural Circuits 12:40. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00040
Canlon, B., Theorell, T., and Hasson, D. (2013). Associations between stress and hearing problems in humans. Hear. Res. 295, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.08.015
Chao, H. M., Choo, P. H., and McEwen, B. S. (1989). Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in rat brain. Neuroendocrinology 50, 365–371. doi: 10.1159/000125250
Chen, G., Zhang, Y., Li, X., Zhao, X., Ye, Q., Lin, Y., et al. (2017). Distinct inhibitory circuits orchestrate cortical beta and gamma band oscillations. Neuron 96, 1403–1418.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.033
Chenaux, G., Matt, L., Hill, T. C., Kaur, I., Liu, X.-B., Kirk, L. M., et al. (2016). Loss of SynDIG1 reduces excitatory synapse maturation but not formation in vivo. SynDIG1 regulates excitatory synapse maturation. eNeuro 3, ENEURO.0130–ENEU16.2016. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0130-16.2016
Chumak, T., Ruttiger, L., Lee, S. C., Campanelli, D., Zuccotti, A., Singer, W., et al. (2016). BDNF in lower brain parts modifies auditory fiber activity to gain Fidelity but increases the risk for generation of central noise after injury. Mol. Neurobiol. 53, 5607–5627. doi: 10.1007/s12035-015-9474-x
Cole, T. J., Blendy, J. A., Monaghan, A. P., Krieglstein, K., Schmid, W., Aguzzi, A., et al. (1995). Targeted disruption of the glucocorticoid receptor gene blocks adrenergic chromaffin cell development and severely retards lung maturation. Genes Dev. 9, 1608–1621. doi: 10.1101/gad.9.13.1608
Csabai, D., Wiborg, O., and Czeh, B. (2018). Reduced synapse and axon numbers in the prefrontal cortex of rats subjected to a chronic stress model for depression. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:24. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00024
Czeh, B., Vardya, I., Varga, Z., Febbraro, F., Csabai, D., Martis, L. S., et al. (2018). Long-term stress disrupts the structural and functional integrity of GABAergic neuronal networks in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:148. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00148
Czeh, B., Varga, Z. K., Henningsen, K., Kovacs, G. L., Miseta, A., and Wiborg, O. (2015). Chronic stress reduces the number of GABAergic interneurons in the adult rat hippocampus, dorsal-ventral and region-specific differences. Hippocampus 25, 393–405. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22382
de Kloet, E. R., de Kloet, S. F., de Kloet, C. S., and de Kloet, A. D. (2019). Top-down and bottom-up control of stress-coping. J. Neuroendocrinol. 31:e12675. doi: 10.1111/jne.12675
de Kloet, E. R., Joels, M., and Holsboer, F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 463–475. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683
de Kloet, E. R., Meijer, O. C., de Nicola, A. F., de Rijk, R. H., and Joels, M. (2018). Importance of the brain corticosteroid receptor balance in metaplasticity, cognitive performance and neuro-inflammation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 49, 124–145. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.02.003
de Kloet, E. R., Oitzl, M. S., and Joels, M. (1999). Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends Neurosci. 22, 422–426. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01438-1
de Kloet, E. R., Van Acker, S. A., Sibug, R. M., Oitzl, M. S., Meijer, O. C., Rahmouni, K., et al. (2000). Brain mineralocorticoid receptors and centrally regulated functions. Kidney Int. 57, 1329–1336. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-1755.2000.00971.x
Dorner-Ciossek, C., Kroker, K. S., and Rosenbrock, H. (2017). Role of PDE9 in cognition. Adv. Neurobiol. 17, 231–254. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-58811-7_9
Dragatsis, I., and Zeitlin, S. (2000). CaMKIIalpha-Cre transgene expression and recombination patterns in the mouse brain. Genesis 26, 133–135. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1526-968x(200002)26:2<133::aid-gene10>3.0.co;2-v
Dynes, J. L., and Steward, O. (2007). Dynamics of bidirectional transport of arc mRNA in neuronal dendrites. J. Comp. Neurol. 500, 433–447. doi: 10.1002/cne.21189
Eckert, P., Marchetta, P., Manthey, M. K., Walter, M. H., Jovanovic, S., Savitska, D., et al. (2021). Deletion of BDNF in Pax2 lineage-derived interneuron precursors in the hindbrain hampers the proportion of excitation/inhibition, learning, and behavior. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 14:642679. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2021.642679
Erdmann, G., Schutz, G., and Berger, S. (2007). Inducible gene inactivation in neurons of the adult mouse forebrain. BMC Neurosci. 8:63. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-63
Erdmann, G., Schutz, G., and Berger, S. (2008). Loss of glucocorticoid receptor function in the pituitary results in early postnatal lethality. Endocrinology 149, 3446–3451. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1786
Giesen, J., Mergia, E., Koesling, D., and Russwurm, M. (2022). Hippocampal AMPA- and NMDA-induced cGMP signals are mainly generated by NO-GC2 and are under tight control by PDEs 1 and 2. Eur. J. Neurosci. 55, 18–31. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15564
Goel, A., Cantu, D. A., Guilfoyle, J., Chaudhari, G. R., Newadkar, A., Todisco, B., et al. (2019). Author correction: impaired perceptual learning in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome is mediated by parvalbumin neuron dysfunction and is reversible. Nat. Neurosci. 22:143. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0273-3
Groc, L., Choquet, D., and Chaouloff, F. (2008). The stress hormone corticosterone conditions AMPAR surface trafficking and synaptic potentiation. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 868–870. doi: 10.1038/nn.2150
Guzowski, J. F., McNaughton, B. L., Barnes, C. A., and Worley, P. F. (1999). Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1120–1124. doi: 10.1038/16046
Han, K., Min, J., Lee, M., Kang, B. M., Park, T., Hahn, J., et al. (2019). Neurovascular coupling under chronic stress is modified by altered GABAergic interneuron activity. J. Neurosci. 39, 10081–10095. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1357-19.2019
Harms, J. F., Menniti, F. S., and Schmidt, C. J. (2019). Phosphodiesterase 9A in brain regulates cGMP signaling independent of nitric-oxide. Front. Neurosci. 13:837. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00837
Harris, A. P., Holmes, M. C., de Kloet, E. R., Chapman, K. E., and Seckl, J. R. (2013). Mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor balance in control of HPA axis and behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 648–658. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.08.007
Hedde, P. N., Malacrida, L., Barylko, B., Binns, D. D., Albanesi, J. P., and Jameson, D. M. (2021). Membrane remodeling by arc/Arg3.1. Front. Mol. Biosci. 8:630625. doi: 10.3389/fmolb.2021.630625
Hopper, R. A., and Garthwaite, J. (2006). Tonic and phasic nitric oxide signals in hippocampal long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 26, 11513–11521. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2259-06.2006
Jafari, Z., Kolb, B. E., and Mohajerani, M. H. (2018). Chronic traffic noise stress accelerates brain impairment and cognitive decline in mice. Exp. Neurol. 308, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.06.011
Jafari, Z., Kolb, B. E., and Mohajerani, M. H. (2019). Age-related hearing loss and tinnitus, dementia risk, and auditory amplification outcomes. Ageing Res. Rev. 56:100963. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.100963
Jett, J. D., Bulin, S. E., Hatherall, L. C., McCartney, C. M., and Morilak, D. A. (2017). Deficits in cognitive flexibility induced by chronic unpredictable stress are associated with impaired glutamate neurotransmission in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 346, 284–297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.017
Joels, M. (2018). Corticosteroids and the brain. J. Endocrinol. 238, R121–R130. doi: 10.1530/JOE-18-0226
Karst, H., and Joels, M. (2005). Corticosterone slowly enhances miniature excitatory postsynaptic current amplitude in mice CA1 hippocampal cells. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 3479–3486. doi: 10.1152/jn.00143.2005
Kisler, K., Nelson, A. R., Montagne, A., and Zlokovic, B. V. (2017). Cerebral blood flow regulation and neurovascular dysfunction in Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 419–434. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.48
Knipper, M., Singer, W., Schwabe, K., Hagberg, G. E., Li Hegner, Y., Rüttiger, L., et al. (2022). Disturbed balance of inhibitory signaling links hearing loss and cognition. Front. Neural Circuits 15, 1–25. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.785603
Knipper, M., van Dijk, P., Schulze, H., Mazurek, B., Krauss, P., Scheper, V., et al. (2020). The neural bases of tinnitus: lessons from deafness and Cochlear implants. J. Neurosci. 40, 7190–7202. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1314-19.2020
Koesling, D., Mergia, E., and Russwurm, M. (2016). Physiological functions of NO-sensitive Guanylyl Cyclase isoforms. Curr. Med. Chem. 23, 2653–2665. doi: 10.2174/0929867323666160812145050
Korb, E., and Finkbeiner, S. (2011). Arc in synaptic plasticity: from gene to behavior. Trends Neurosci. 34, 591–598. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.08.007
Kroker, K. S., Rast, G., Giovannini, R., Marti, A., Dorner-Ciossek, C., and Rosenbrock, H. (2012). Inhibition of acetylcholinesterase and phosphodiesterase-9A has differential effects on hippocampal early and late LTP. Neuropharmacology 62, 1964–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.12.021
Kuhn, M. (2016). Molecular physiology of membrane Guanylyl Cyclase receptors. Physiol. Rev. 96, 751–804. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00022.2015
Kuhn, M., Volker, K., Schwarz, K., Carbajo-Lozoya, J., Flogel, U., Jacoby, C., et al. (2009). The natriuretic peptide/guanylyl cyclase--a system functions as a stress-responsive regulator of angiogenesis in mice. J. Clin. Invest. 119, 2019–2030. doi: 10.1172/JCI37430
Kuipers, S. D., Trentani, A., Tiron, A., Mao, X., Kuhl, D., and Bramham, C. R. (2016). BDNF-induced LTP is associated with rapid arc/Arg3.1-dependent enhancement in adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Sci. Rep. 6:21222. doi: 10.1038/srep21222
Lai, M., Horsburgh, K., Bae, S. E., Carter, R. N., Stenvers, D. J., Fowler, J. H., et al. (2007). Forebrain mineralocorticoid receptor overexpression enhances memory, reduces anxiety and attenuates neuronal loss in cerebral ischaemia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1832–1842. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05427.x
Ledo, A., Lourenco, C. F., Cadenas, E., Barbosa, R. M., and Laranjinha, J. (2021). The bioactivity of neuronal-derived nitric oxide in aging and neurodegeneration: switching signaling to degeneration. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 162, 500–513. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2020.11.005
Lee, S., Kang, B. M., Shin, M. K., Min, J., Heo, C., Lee, Y., et al. (2015). Chronic stress decreases cerebrovascular responses during rat Hindlimb electrical stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 9:462. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00462
Likhtik, E., and Johansen, J. P. (2019). Neuromodulation in circuits of aversive emotional learning. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1586–1597. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0503-3
Luksys, G., and Sandi, C. (2011). Neural mechanisms and computations underlying stress effects on learning and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 502–508. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.03.003
Manohar, S., Chen, G. D., Ding, D., Liu, L., Wang, J., Chen, Y. C., et al. (2022). Unexpected consequences of noise-induced hearing loss: impaired hippocampal neurogenesis, memory, and stress. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 16:871223. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.871223
Marchetta, P., Eckert, P., Lukowski, R., Ruth, P., Singer, W., Ruttiger, L., et al. (2022). Loss of central mineralocorticoid or glucocorticoid receptors impacts auditory nerve processing in the cochlea. iScience 25:103981. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.103981
Marchetta, P., Mohrle, D., Eckert, P., Reimann, K., Wolter, S., Tolone, A., et al. (2020a). Guanylyl Cyclase a/cGMP signaling slows hidden, age- and acoustic trauma-induced hearing loss. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:83. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00083
Marchetta, P., Savitska, D., Kubler, A., Asola, G., Manthey, M., Mohrle, D., et al. (2020b). Age-dependent auditory processing deficits after Cochlear Synaptopathy depend on auditory nerve latency and the ability of the brain to recruit LTP/BDNF. Brain Sci. 10. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10100710
Martin, S., Henley, J. M., Holman, D., Zhou, M., Wiegert, O., van Spronsen, M., et al. (2009). Corticosterone alters AMPAR mobility and facilitates bidirectional synaptic plasticity. PLoS One 4:e4714. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004714
Matt, L., Eckert, P., Panford-Walsh, R., Geisler, H. S., Bausch, A. E., Manthey, M., et al. (2018). Visualizing BDNF transcript usage during sound-induced memory linked plasticity. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 11:260. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00260
Matt, L., Michalakis, S., Hofmann, F., Hammelmann, V., Ludwig, A., Biel, M., et al. (2011). HCN2 channels in local inhibitory interneurons constrain LTP in the hippocampal direct perforant path. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 68, 125–137. doi: 10.1007/s00018-010-0446-z
Mazurek, B., Boecking, B., and Brueggemann, P. (2019). Association between stress and tinnitus-new aspects. Otol. Neurotol. 40, e467–e473. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002180
McCann, K. E., Lustberg, D. J., Shaughnessy, E. K., Carstens, K. E., Farris, S., Alexander, G. M., et al. (2021). Novel role for mineralocorticoid receptors in control of a neuronal phenotype. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 350–364. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0598-7
McEwen, B. S., Nasca, C., and Gray, J. D. (2016). Stress effects on neuronal structure: hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 3–23. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.171
Mease, R. A., and Gonzalez, A. J. (2021). Corticothalamic pathways from layer 5: emerging roles in computation and pathology. Front. Neural Circuits 15:730211. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.730211
Meijsing, S. H., Pufall, M. A., So, A. Y., Bates, D. L., Chen, L., and Yamamoto, K. R. (2009). DNA binding site sequence directs glucocorticoid receptor structure and activity. Science 324, 407–410. doi: 10.1126/science.1164265
Meltser, I., and Canlon, B. (2011). Protecting the auditory system with glucocorticoids. Hear. Res. 281, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.06.003
Mergia, E., Friebe, A., Dangel, O., Russwurm, M., and Koesling, D. (2006). Spare guanylyl cyclase NO receptors ensure high NO sensitivity in the vascular system. J. Clin. Invest. 116, 1731–1737. doi: 10.1172/JCI27657
Mifsud, K. R., and Reul, J. M. (2016). Acute stress enhances heterodimerization and binding of corticosteroid receptors at glucocorticoid target genes in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 11336–11341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605246113
Muller, D., Hida, B., Guidone, G., Speth, R. C., Michurina, T. V., Enikolopov, G., et al. (2009). Expression of guanylyl cyclase (GC)-a and GC-B during brain development: evidence for a role of GC-B in perinatal neurogenesis. Endocrinology 150, 5520–5529. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-0490
Nadhimi, Y., and Llano, D. A. (2021). Does hearing loss lead to dementia? A review of the literature. Hear. Res. 402:108038. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2020.108038
Nelissen, E., Argyrousi, E. K., Van Goethem, N. P., Zhao, F., Hines, C. D. G., Swaminath, G., et al. (2021). Soluble Guanylate Cyclase stimulator Vericiguat enhances long-term memory in rats without altering cerebral blood volume. Biomedicine 9, 1–20. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines9081047
Nelissen, E., Possemis, N., Van Goethem, N. P., Schepers, M., Mulder-Jongen, D. A. J., Dietz, L., et al. (2022). The sGC stimulator BAY-747 and activator runcaciguat can enhance memory in vivo via differential hippocampal plasticity mechanisms. Sci. Rep. 12:3589. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-07391-1
Obradovic, D., Tirard, M., Nemethy, Z., Hirsch, O., Gronemeyer, H., and Almeida, O. F. (2004). DAXX, FLASH, and FAF-1 modulate mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor-mediated transcription in hippocampal cells--toward a basis for the opposite actions elicited by two nuclear receptors? Mol. Pharmacol. 65, 761–769. doi: 10.1124/mol.65.3.761
Oitzl, M. S., and de Kloet, E. R. (1992). Selective corticosteroid antagonists modulate specific aspects of spatial orientation learning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 62–71. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.1.62
Park, S., Park, J. M., Kim, S., Kim, J. A., Shepherd, J. D., Smith-Hicks, C. L., et al. (2008). Elongation factor 2 and fragile X mental retardation protein control the dynamic translation of arc/Arg3.1 essential for mGluR-LTD. Neuron 59, 70–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.023
Patel, N. S., Klett, J., Pilarzyk, K., Lee, D. I., Kass, D., Menniti, F. S., et al. (2018). Identification of new PDE9A isoforms and how their expression and subcellular compartmentalization in the brain change across the life span. Neurobiol. Aging 65, 217–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.01.019
Plath, N., Ohana, O., Dammermann, B., Errington, M. L., Schmitz, D., Gross, C., et al. (2006). Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation of synaptic plasticity and memories. Neuron 52, 437–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.024
Polman, J. A., de Kloet, E. R., and Datson, N. A. (2013). Two populations of glucocorticoid receptor-binding sites in the male rat hippocampal genome. Endocrinology 154, 1832–1844. doi: 10.1210/en.2012-2187
Potter, L. R. (2011). Regulation and therapeutic targeting of peptide-activated receptor guanylyl cyclases. Pharmacol. Ther. 130, 71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2010.12.005
Ramirez-Amaya, V., Vazdarjanova, A., Mikhael, D., Rosi, S., Worley, P. F., and Barnes, C. A. (2005). Spatial exploration-induced arc mRNA and protein expression: evidence for selective, network-specific reactivation. J. Neurosci. 25, 1761–1768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4342-04.2005
Reul, J. M., and de Kloet, E. R. (1985). Two receptor systems for corticosterone in rat brain: microdistribution and differential occupation. Endocrinology 117, 2505–2511. doi: 10.1210/endo-117-6-2505
Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., and Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 289–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135620
Rodriguez, J. J., Davies, H. A., Silva, A. T., De Souza, I. E., Peddie, C. J., Colyer, F. M., et al. (2005). Long-term potentiation in the rat dentate gyrus is associated with enhanced arc/Arg3.1 protein expression in spines, dendrites and glia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2384–2396. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04068.x
Roozendaal, B., and McGaugh, J. L. (2011). Memory modulation. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 797–824. doi: 10.1037/a0026187
Sanderson, T. M., and Sher, E. (2013). The role of phosphodiesterases in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 74, 86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.01.011
Sarabdjitsingh, R. A., Pasricha, N., Smeets, J. A., Kerkhofs, A., Mikasova, L., Karst, H., et al. (2016). Hippocampal fast Glutamatergic transmission is transiently regulated by Corticosterone Pulsatility. PLoS One 11:e0145858. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145858
Satake, S., Inoue, T., and Imoto, K. (2012). Paired-pulse facilitation of multivesicular release and intersynaptic spillover of glutamate at rat cerebellar granule cell-interneurone synapses. J. Physiol. 590, 5653–5675. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.234070
Savitska, D., Hess, M., Calis, D., Marchetta, P., Harasztosi, C., Fink, S., et al. (2022). Stress affects central compensation of neural responses to Cochlear Synaptopathy in a cGMP-dependent way. Front. Neurosci. 16:864706. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.864706
Schwabe, L., Joels, M., Roozendaal, B., Wolf, O. T., and Oitzl, M. S. (2012). Stress effects on memory: an update and integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1740–1749. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.002
Shimba, A., and Ikuta, K. (2020). Control of immunity by glucocorticoids in health and disease. Semin. Immunopathol. 42, 669–680. doi: 10.1007/s00281-020-00827-8
Singer, W., Geisler, H. S., and Knipper, M. (2013a). The Geisler method: tracing activity-dependent cGMP plasticity changes upon double detection of mRNA and protein on brain slices. Methods Mol. Biol. 1020, 223–233. doi: 10.1007/978-1-62703-459-3_15
Singer, W., Geisler, H. S., Panford-Walsh, R., and Knipper, M. (2016). Detection of excitatory and inhibitory synapses in the auditory system using fluorescence immunohistochemistry and high-resolution fluorescence microscopy. Methods Mol. Biol. 1427, 263–276. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-3615-1_15
Singer, W., Zuccotti, A., Jaumann, M., Lee, S. C., Panford-Walsh, R., Xiong, H., et al. (2013b). Noise-induced inner hair cell ribbon loss disturbs central arc mobilization: a novel molecular paradigm for understanding tinnitus. Mol. Neurobiol. 47, 261–279. doi: 10.1007/s12035-012-8372-8
Sohal, V. S., Zhang, F., Yizhar, O., and Deisseroth, K. (2009). Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459, 698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991
Son, H., Hawkins, R. D., Martin, K., Kiebler, M., Huang, P. L., Fishman, M. C., et al. (1996). Long-term potentiation is reduced in mice that are doubly mutant in endothelial and neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Cells 87, 1015–1023. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81796-1
Suga, N. (2020). Plasticity of the adult auditory system based on corticocortical and corticofugal modulations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 461–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.021
Thomson, A. M. (2000). Facilitation, augmentation and potentiation at central synapses. Trends Neurosci. 23, 305–312. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01580-0
Tzingounis, A. V., and Nicoll, R. A. (2006). Arc/Arg3.1: linking gene expression to synaptic plasticity and memory. Neuron 52, 403–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.016
van Weert, L., Buurstede, J. C., Mahfouz, A., Braakhuis, P. S. M., Polman, J. A. E., Sips, H. C. M., et al. (2017). NeuroD factors discriminate mineralocorticoid from glucocorticoid receptor DNA binding in the male rat brain. Endocrinology 158, 1511–1522. doi: 10.1210/en.2016-1422
Wall, M. J., and Correa, S. A. L. (2018). The mechanistic link between arc/Arg3.1 expression and AMPA receptor endocytosis. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 77, 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2017.09.005
Wang, J., and Puel, J. L. (2020). Presbycusis: an update on Cochlear mechanisms and therapies. J. Clin. Med. 9, 1–22. doi: 10.3390/jcm9010218
Wang, X., Zhang, C., Szabo, G., and Sun, Q. Q. (2013). Distribution of CaMKIIalpha expression in the brain in vivo, studied by CaMKIIalpha-GFP mice. Brain Res. 1518, 9–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.04.042
Waung, M. W., Pfeiffer, B. E., Nosyreva, E. D., Ronesi, J. A., and Huber, K. M. (2008). Rapid translation of arc/Arg3.1 selectively mediates mGluR-dependent LTD through persistent increases in AMPAR endocytosis rate. Neuron 59, 84–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.014
Wingenfeld, K., and Otte, C. (2019). Mineralocorticoid receptor function and cognition in health and disease. Psychoneuroendocrinology 105, 25–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.010
Yilmaz-Rastoder, E., Miyamae, T., Braun, A. E., and Thiels, E. (2011). LTP- and LTD-inducing stimulations cause opposite changes in arc/arg3.1 mRNA level in hippocampal area CA1 in vivo. Hippocampus 21, 1290–1301. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20838
Zhang, H., and Bramham, C. R. (2021). Arc/Arg3.1 function in long-term synaptic plasticity: emerging mechanisms and unresolved issues. Eur. J. Neurosci. 54, 6696–6712. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14958
Zhang, L., Wang, J., Sun, H., Feng, G., and Gao, Z. (2022). Interactions between the hippocampus and the auditory pathway. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 189:107589. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2022.107589
Keywords: glucocorticoid receptor, mineralocorticoid receptor, NO-GC, GC-A, cognition
Citation: Calis D, Hess M, Marchetta P, Singer W, Modro J, Nelissen E, Prickaerts J, Sandner P, Lukowski R, Ruth P, Knipper M and Rüttiger L (2023) Acute deletion of the central MR/GR steroid receptor correlates with changes in LTP, auditory neural gain, and GC-A cGMP signaling. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 16:1017761. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2023.1017761
Edited by:
Clint L. Makino, Boston University, United StatesReviewed by:
Clive R. Bramham, University of Bergen, NorwayJenna Penney, University of Guelph, Canada
Copyright © 2023 Calis, Hess, Marchetta, Singer, Modro, Nelissen, Prickaerts, Sandner, Lukowski, Ruth, Knipper and Rüttiger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marlies Knipper, ✉ marlies.knipper@uni-tuebingen.de
†These authors have contributed equally to this work