Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Lang. Sci., 08 July 2024
Sec. Neurobiology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Syntax, the brain, and linguistic theory: a critical reassessment View all 11 articles

Editorial: Syntax, the brain, and linguistic theory: a critical reassessment

  • 1Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States
  • 2Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
  • 3Ikerbasque, Basque Foundation for Science, Bilbao, Spain
  • 4Behavior and Social Sciences, City University of Hong Kong, Kowloon, Hong Kong SAR, China
  • 5Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Lower Saxony, Germany

Introduction

Theoretical syntax no longer plays as prominent a role in neurolinguistics as it used to. A prominent issue is that linguistic theory often has been applied directly to neuroscience (Figure 1, left), but rather should be filtered through a well-articulated processing model (Figure 1, right). The papers in this volume pave the way for a renewed and productive relationship between linguistic theory and neuroscience in the study of syntax and the brain.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Left: some neurolinguistic work has attempted to directly apply linguistic theory to neuroscience, which has led to many pitfalls. Right: the approach we advocate here, in which linguistic theory is integrated with an adequate processing model, which will enable more meaningful results and the development of more effective neurocognitive models.

“Words” are not separable from syntax

A major insight that has guided recent research on the organization of syntax in the brain concerns the tight relationship between syntactic structure and the lexicon. Krauska and Lau review cross-linguistic evidence both from non-European languages such as Iniktitut, Vietnamese, and Hiaki, as well as English and Dutch, illustrating that the concept of the lemma familiar from psycholinguistic research (e.g. Levelt, 1989) is untenable. They propose an alternative model of syntax and the brain in which the posterior temporal lobe generates both what we colloquially call “words” and “sentences”. Gonering and Corina provide a comprehensive review of constructionist and generative syntactic theories and neuroscience research on syntactic processing, and advocate for a theory in which syntactic processing is widely distributed in a dual-stream architecture: a ventral stream for processing nouns and attributive modifiers, and a dorsal stream for processing verbs and relational modifiers (cf. Bornkessel-Schlesewsky and Schlesewsky, 2013). Finally, Matchin points out that lexical items have both syntactic and semantic properties, a consensus across many linguistic theories. Therefore, spatial overlap between lexicality (e.g. word > nonword) and syntactic effects (e.g. complex > simple structures) in functional neuroimaging studies does not provide evidence supporting an inseparability of syntax and semantics in the brain.

Syntactic deficits in patient populations are best explained by a combination of linguistic and domain-general deficits

Linguistic structure is clearly relevant to language disorders: agrammatism and cognitive impairments due to Alzheimer's disease can both be meaningfully characterized in part as a reduction in syntactic complexity. However, it is also critical to incorporate insights from cognitive domains outside of linguistics. Based on the results of Ivanova et al., deficits in working memory may explain some of the major declines in syntactic complexity yet preserved syntactic well-formedness in Alzheimer's disease. Faroqi-Shah claims that a combination of deficits to linguistic structure, speech articulation, and processing capacity may all contribute to the classic pattern of agrammatic speech commonly seen in nonfluent aphasia. Interestingly, Faroqi-Shah's framework takes a plausible middle-of-the-ground approach to agrammatism, differing from theories which focus primarily on deficits from a specific module of syntax derived from linguistic theory (e.g., Grodzinsky, 2000) and differing from theories which eschew any targeted linguistic deficit, but rather a more general processing issue (Kolk, 1995; Fedorenko et al., 2023).

Abstract linguistic structure is critical to online processing

It is common to dismiss the abstract structures that are postulated by some linguistic theories. However, the papers contributed by Greco et al. and Yamaguchi and Ohta indicate that these structures are essential for explaining language processing behavior. Specifically, Greco et al. illustrate the necessity of hierarchical structure to surprisal effects, above and beyond surface-based statistics based on specific words and parts-of-speech. Yamaguchi and Ohta investigated one issue that has often been contentious within the psycholinguistic literature: the extent to which putative phonologically null elements, or empty categories, exert effects on sentence processing similar to overt pronominal elements. They found evidence that the structures containing these putative null elements behave like structures with real reflexive pronouns, and also that multiple distinct types of empty categories must exist, converging with the predictions of syntactic theory.

Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience

One of the pitfalls in previous and current research on the syntax-brain relationship is the failure of sufficient imagination in considering how language might be implemented in the brain, and problematic, unexamined assumptions concerning how language is processed and how that processing relates to brain activity, echoing influential comments by Poeppel and Embick (2005). Călinescu et al. comprehensively review functional neuroimaging on syntax, pointing out the inadequacy of many experimental paradigms in identifying what they claim to. Coopmans and Zaccarella discuss three concepts from syntactic theory: the distinction between competence and performance, the autonomy of syntax, and the abstract nature of syntactic representations, arguing that they are often incorrectly interpreted as applying to online language processing rather than as representational descriptions, or vice versa. Both papers assert that some of the confusion in neurolinguistics may stem from a misunderstanding or misapplication of concepts from linguistics. Uriagereka pushes at the edges of inquiry, suggesting a novel mathematical approach to decomposing syntactic features into an algebraic form. This decomposition may provide a greater opportunity to find a neural correlate of linguistic processing in the form of punctual and distributed representations than is currently evidenced in neurolinguistic research.

Conclusions

The papers in this volume are far from providing confident answers to the questions we posed in this Research Topic. However, they are inspiring in their breadth and their common cause of bringing to light the valuable and significant contributions of syntactic theory, when interpreted carefully, keeping in mind how syntax should be processed algorithmically in real-time, to neurobiology. They should provide a valuable starting point for new researchers looking to enter the field, either linguists who are curious about the brain or neurolinguists looking for theoretical grounding for their work.

Author contributions

WM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL: Writing – review & editing. EZ: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders grant P50 DC014664, supporting WM. SM was supported by BERC 2022-2025 program and Severo Ochoa excellence accreditation CEX2020-001010-S and PID2020-113945RB-I00.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., and Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Reconciling time, space and function: a new dorsal–ventral stream model of sentence comprehension. Brain Lang. 125, 60–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedorenko, E., Ryskin, R., and Gibson, E. (2023). Agrammatic output in non-fluent, including Broca's, aphasia as a rational behavior. Aphasiology 37, 1981–2000. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2022.2143233

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca's area. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 1–21. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00002399

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolk, H. (1995). A time-based approach to agrammatic production. Brain Lang. 50, 282–303.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Poeppel, D., and Embick, D. (2005). “Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience,” in Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones, ed. A. Cutler. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 103–118.

Google Scholar

Keywords: syntax, brain, linguistic theory, neurolinguistics, lexicalism

Citation: Matchin W, Mancini S, Li J and Zaccarella E (2024) Editorial: Syntax, the brain, and linguistic theory: a critical reassessment. Front. Lang. Sci. 3:1441948. doi: 10.3389/flang.2024.1441948

Received: 31 May 2024; Accepted: 10 June 2024;
Published: 08 July 2024.

Edited and reviewed by: Greig I. de Zubicaray, Queensland University of Technology, Australia

Copyright © 2024 Matchin, Mancini, Li and Zaccarella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: William Matchin, matchin@mailbox.sc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.