Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 06 October 2023
This article is part of the Research Topic Early Prediction of CNS Problems by Combined Ocular Markers (and vice versa) View all 8 articles

No evidence for differential saccadic adaptation in children and adults with an autism spectrum diagnosis

\r\nKaty Tarrit,Katy Tarrit1,2Edward G. FreedmanEdward G. Freedman2Ana A. FranciscoAna A. Francisco3Douwe J. HorsthuisDouwe J. Horsthuis3Sophie Molholm,Sophie Molholm2,3John J. Foxe,*John J. Foxe2,3*
  • 1Information and Computer Sciences Department, University of Hawai’i at Manoa, Honolulu, HI, United States
  • 2The Frederick J. and Marion A. Schindler Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience, Department of Neuroscience, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 3The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Department of Pediatrics and Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States

Background: Altered patterns of eye-movements during scene exploration, and atypical gaze preferences in social settings, have long been noted as features of the Autism phenotype. While these are typically attributed to differences in social engagement and interests (e.g., preferences for inanimate objects over face stimuli), there are also reports of differential saccade measures to non-social stimuli, raising the possibility that fundamental differences in visuo-sensorimotor processing may be at play. Here, we tested the plasticity of the eye-movement system using a classic saccade-adaptation paradigm to assess whether individuals with ASD make typical adjustments to their eye-movements in response to experimentally introduced errors. Saccade adaptation can be measured in infants as young as 10 months, raising the possibility that such measures could be useful as early neuro-markers of ASD risk.

Methods: Saccade amplitudes were measured while children and adults with ASD (N = 41) and age-matched typically developing (TD) individuals (N = 68) made rapid eye-movements to peripherally presented targets. During adaptation trials, the target was relocated from 20-degrees to 15-degrees from fixation once a saccade to the original target location was initiated, a manipulation that leads to systematic reduction in saccade amplitudes in typical observers.

Results: Neither children nor adults with ASD showed any differences relative to TD peers in their abilities to appropriately adapt saccades in the face of persistently introduced errors.

Conclusion: Of the three studies to date of saccade adaptation in ASD, none have shown deficits in saccade adaptation that are sufficient to generalize to the whole or a subgroup of the ASD population. Unlike prior studies, we found no evidence for a slower adaptation rate during the early adaptation phase, and no of evidence greater variance of saccade amplitudes in ASD. In post hoc analysis, there was evidence for larger primary saccades to non-adapted targets, a finding requiring replication in future work.

Introduction

While Autism is primarily associated with atypical social communication and restricted/repetitive behaviors, sensory-motor differences are also very commonly reported (Molloy et al., 2003; Takarae et al., 2004; Nowinski et al., 2005; Brenner et al., 2007; Fournier et al., 2010). For example, postural instability, coordination difficulties, and atypical oculomotor control have all been shown in this population (Vilensky et al., 1981; Jones and Prior, 1985; Kohen-Raz et al., 1992; Rogers et al., 1996; Ghaziuddin and Butler, 1998; Kemner et al., 1998; Klin et al., 2002; Molloy et al., 2003; Takarae et al., 2004; Nowinski et al., 2005; Brenner et al., 2007; Stanley-Cary et al., 2011). Oculomotor functions are of particular interest given that accurate eye movements govern integration between the visual and other sensory systems, are often essential for motor planning, and are implicated in a multitude of other processes potentially relevant to the Autism phenotype, such as visual search, imitation, joint attention, and language (Brenner et al., 2007).

In the context of Autism, eye-movements have been of substantial interest to clinicians and researchers because of the long-noted atypicalities that autistic individuals show in making or maintaining eye-contact in social settings (Hirsch et al., 2022). A key question arises regarding the nature of these gaze differences–that is, (1) are they driven by higher-order social communication difficulties such that they reflect a reluctance to engage socially, or (2) is there a more fundamental oculomotor processing deficit that underlies these differences. Here, we were interested in testing this second thesis that more basic oculomotor physiological differences may exist in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), independent of engagement with socially relevant inputs. We have previously hypothesized that differences in visuo-sensorimotor development in early infancy may underlie some of the differences in visual orienting, communication and social interaction reported in Autism, perhaps as a result of structural cerebellar differences that impact plasticity of the saccadic eye-movement system (Freedman and Foxe, 2018). We set out to test whether simple saccadic eye-movements to basic non-social stimuli, in the context of a saccade adaptation paradigm, would differ in individuals with an Autism diagnosis relative to matched typically developing peers.

Saccades are rapid conjugate eye movements that shift the direction of gaze from one location to another in order to accurately aim the highly-innervated fovea at areas or objects of interest (Fuchs et al., 1985). Modern infrared camera techniques allow for accurate measurement of these eye-movements, and their dynamics and neural substrates have been studied extensively (Hopp and Fuchs, 2004; Alahyane et al., 2008; Kelly et al., 2010; Collins and Wallman, 2012; Belyusar et al., 2013). Once a visual target has been identified, its position in relation to the fovea is calculated and sent as a motor command to activate the extraocular muscles so that the eyes move in the correct direction and to the appropriate location to fixate said target. At the end of a saccadic eye movement, if the eye-movement displacement is erroneous, either undershooting or overshooting the target location, the vector of a subsequent saccade is calculated, and a new motor command generated by the oculomotor system to adjust for the error. It is this oculomotor learning process that has been termed saccade adaptation (Hopp and Fuchs, 2004; Pélisson et al., 2010), allowing the oculomotor system to adjust and maintain the amplitude and direction of saccades by adaptatively adjusting motor commands in response to persistent visual errors (Lappe, 2009; Wong and Shelhamer, 2011).

Saccade adaptation is often produced in the laboratory by using paradigms in which the target is surreptitiously moved while the eye is in flight, such as in the McLaughlin paradigm (McLaughlin, 1967) and its variations (Miller et al., 1981; Deubel et al., 1986; Frens and Van Opstal, 1997; Phillips et al., 1997; Straube et al., 1997; Scudder et al., 1998; Wallman and Fuchs, 1998; Noto et al., 1999; Hopp and Fuchs, 2002, 2006; Robinson et al., 2003; Alahyane and Pélisson, 2004, 2005; Takeichi et al., 2005, 2007; Cecala and Freedman, 2008, 2009). It is well-established that the midline cerebellum plays a critical role in saccade adaptation (Barash et al., 1999; Gaymard et al., 2001; Straube et al., 2001; Galea et al., 2009; Jayaram et al., 2012; Avila et al., 2015; Panouillères et al., 2015). In non-human primates, studies have demonstrated that lesioning of the oculomotor cerebellar vermis causes acute hypometria of saccades as well as a severe and relatively chronic deficit in saccade adaptation (Barash et al., 1999) and that inactivation of the caudal fastigial nucleus through acute muscimol injections leads to aberrant saccade dynamics and inaccurate fixations (Goffart et al., 2004). In humans, cerebellar degeneration, infarcts or congenital malformations also lead to disruption of saccade amplitude (Straube et al., 2001).

This is relevant to ASD because there is a large literature reporting potential neuroanatomical differences in the structure of the cerebellum in individuals with ASD (Courchesne et al., 1988, 1994; Takarae et al., 2004; Nowinski et al., 2005; Stanley-Cary et al., 2011). One study using magnetic resonance imaging found that individuals with ASD have significantly smaller neocerebellar lobules VI and VII compared to their typically developing (TD) peers (Courchesne et al., 1988). Another study reported that there were two subgroups in the ASD population, one with vermian hypoplasia and one with vermian hyperplasia (Courchesne et al., 1994). As the cerebellum contributes significantly to visuomotor processing, it is a reasonable proposition that saccadic eye movements might be affected by these structural differences, which would go to our primary thesis here that eye-movement atypicalities in ASD may be due to basic sensory-oculomotor integration differences rather than to a failure to engage in socially relevant interactions.

There is a substantial emerging literature investigating oculomotor functions in ASD by measuring saccade dynamics and metrics, but these have yielded mixed and sometimes contradictory results (Rosenhall et al., 1988; Kemner et al., 1998, 2004; Minshew et al., 1999; van der Geest et al., 2001; Goldberg et al., 2002; Takarae et al., 2004, 2008; Luna et al., 2007; Thakkar et al., 2008; Glazebrook et al., 2009; Mosconi et al., 2009; Pensiero et al., 2009; Stanley-Cary et al., 2011; Johnson et al., 2012; Schmitt et al., 2014; Wilkes et al., 2015; Kovarski et al., 2019; Bast et al., 2021). While some have reported atypical saccade dynamics, others find no obvious differences between ASD and TD populations. Some have reported longer saccade latencies (Goldberg et al., 2002; Glazebrook et al., 2009; Pensiero et al., 2009; Schmitt et al., 2014; Wilkes et al., 2015), while others found no significant differences (Minshew et al., 1999; Johnson et al., 2016; Kovarski et al., 2019). Some studies found a decrease in saccade duration (Bast et al., 2021), and others found significantly longer saccade durations (Stanley-Cary et al., 2011). Other studies have also reported a decrease in saccade amplitude, i.e., hypometria, (Rosenhall et al., 1988; Takarae et al., 2004; Luna et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2012; Bast et al., 2021) while some found no differences in mean accuracy (Johnson et al., 2016). Similarly, some reported more variability in saccade amplitude or latency (Goldberg et al., 2002; Takarae et al., 2004; Glazebrook et al., 2009; Stanley-Cary et al., 2011; Johnson et al., 2012) or an increase in saccadic activity (Kemner et al., 1998). Others found reduced (Rosenhall et al., 1988) or irregular (Pensiero et al., 2009) saccade peak velocity but similar reaction times for individuals with ASD compared to their TD peers (Rosenhall et al., 1988), while some studies reported no group differences in peak velocity (Luna et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2016). In one study, ASD children were found to be as accurate but faster than TD children in looking toward targets (Kovarski et al., 2019). Studies using other tasks such as the anti-saccade task have also reported impaired performance in ASD (Goldberg et al., 2002; Thakkar et al., 2008; Mosconi et al., 2009).

To our knowledge, only two studies have investigated saccade adaptation in ASD to date (Johnson et al., 2013; Mosconi et al., 2013). Johnson et al. (2013) conducted a study on children between 9 and 14 years of age and found that both individuals with High Functioning Autism (HFA) and those classified as Asperger’s (AD) adapted appropriately compared to a TD control group. However, they also reported that the HFA group showed a slower adaptation rate, although it is important to point out that this latter conclusion was reached on the basis of a comparison between cohorts of 10 individuals with HFA and 12 TD individuals. In a much larger study across a considerably larger age-range (8–54 years), however, Mosconi et al. (2013) also reported that their ASD group (N = 56) adapted slower than the TD group (N = 53). In addition, they reported that almost a third of their ASD cohort did not adapt at all, versus only 6% of their TD group. Their ASD group also showed more variability in saccade amplitudes during each trial type, i.e., baseline, adaptation and recovery. Given the paucity of existing data on saccade adaptation in ASD and the lack of entirely consistent results across the two existing studies, we set out here to investigate whether saccade adaptation would be atypical in both children (N = 21: 6–17 years of age) and adults (N = 20: 18–45 years of age) on the Autism Spectrum compared to their TD peers in the context of the so-called “gain-down” saccade adaptation paradigm, a variant of the one pioneered by McLaughlin (1967).

A main motivation for this work is that it has been shown that saccade adaptation can be measured in infants as young as 10 months of age (Lemoine-Lardennois et al., 2016). As such, if saccade adaptation deficits are consistently identified in ASD, this relatively simple-to-deploy measure could prove useful as part of an early risk-assessment battery (Goldberg et al., 2002; Wilkes et al., 2015). We were also specifically interested in assessing whether saccade adaptation deficits might be more severe in children than adults with ASD. While Mosconi et al. (2013) found no association between age and saccade adaptation parameters in their study, we specifically recruited separate groups of children and adults to directly address this issue, since developmental effects across the adult lifespan may have contributed to the relative lack of saccade adaptation deficits that have been seen in the prior work.

Materials and methods

Participants and phenotypic assessments

A total of 41 individuals with an Autism Spectrum Diagnosis (ASD) and 68 age-matched Typically Developing (TD) controls (age range: 6 to 45 years old) were recruited (see Table 1 below for demographics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characterization of the typically developing and ASD groups included in the analyses: Age, biological sex, and IQ.

Participants all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (20/30). Participants with corrected vision were asked to remove their glasses and wear COMO SPORT L glasses with lenses adjusted to their visual acuity for the duration of the experiment. Participants were excluded from the study if they had a history of seizures, traumatic brain injury, psychiatric or neurodevelopmental conditions other than autism (not including ADHD or anxiety), or if they were taking any antiepileptic or anticonvulsant medication, as these can impact eye movements (Aschoff, 1968; Cohen et al., 1985; Tedeschi et al., 1989; Remler et al., 1990; Zaccara et al., 1992; Goldman and Schultz-Ross, 1993; Hilton et al., 2004; Lo et al., 2008; Reilly et al., 2008; Park et al., 2015; Lunn et al., 2016). The current study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards (IRB) of the University of Rochester Medical Center and of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine. Written informed consent was obtained from participants or their parent/legal guardian. Assent appropriate for age and developmental level was obtained from participants where applicable. Participants were compensated at a rate of $15 per hour for their time in the laboratory.

Phenotyping sessions lasted about one and a half hours for TD participants and 2 h for individuals with ASD. The Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale, 2nd Edition (WASI-II) (Wechsler, 2011) was used to assess verbal and non-verbal abilities. Those reporting a diagnosis of ASD additionally completed Module 3 or Module 4 of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Edition (ADOS-2) (Gotham et al., 2007). Individuals with ASD were assessed by a clinical psychologist, research reliable on the ADOS-2. TD participants were assessed by either a clinical psychologist or a post-doctoral fellow supervised by a clinical psychologist. Two participants who were initially recruited into the autism group were excluded from the analysis, as despite valid autism diagnosis by a qualified professional based on DSM criteria at a younger age, these individuals no longer met instrument classification for ASD on the ADOS-2. All individuals in the ASD group had a positive ADOS-2 score and met DSM-5 criteria for autism.

Stimuli and experimental task

Eye movements were recorded using the video-based eye tracker EyeLink 1,000 (SR Research, Ottawa, Canada), with an average accuracy of 0.15 degrees and a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. Only one eye was monitored. The amplitudes of primary saccades [i.e., the initial eye movements from the centrally presented fixation cross (T0) to the target location (T1)] were identified and extracted off-line using a custom MATLAB script (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). Noisy trials were excluded and only trials without blinks and in which participants looked toward the intended target were included in the analysis. The amplitude of primary saccades was then plotted as a function of the trial number, i.e., the order in which they occurred during the experiment, including control trials–which occurred pre- and post-adaptation trials.

During the experiment, participants sat on a chair facing the center of a computer monitor, placed 78.8 cm away from the participant, where stimuli were presented. At the beginning of the experiment, the camera was calibrated on the selected eye by asking the participant to make saccades to nine fixation crosses randomly presented either at the center of the screen, in each direction along the middle line (top, bottom, left, and right of the center) or along the corners. The experimental paradigm as can be seen on Figure 1 below was a gain-down saccadic adaptation paradigm adapted from the one introduced by McLaughlin (1967) and consisted of the presentation of a series of 68 pre-adaptation or control trials, followed by a series of 240 adaptation trials, interspersed with control trials, and a final series of 68 post-adaptation or control trials.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Gain-down saccadic adaptation experimental paradigm adapted from McLaughlin. Panel (A): Illustration of a control trial, Panel (B): Illustration of an adaptation trial.

Initial control trials consisted of the presentation of a central target (T0) that participants were required to fixate within ± 0.75° for a variable interval of either 600, 800, 1,000, or 1,200 ms. Breaking fixation resulted in aborting the trial and the beginning of a new trial. At the end of this interval, the T0 target was turned off and a secondary target (T1) was presented at one of the following locations: either 20° to the left or to the right of the T0 location, or 10° above or below T0. Participants were then instructed to saccade as accurately as possible to the T1 target. As soon as they started a saccade toward T1, the T1 target disappeared and after a fixed interval of 1,500 ms the next trial started. After 68 of these control trials, the series of adaptation and interspersed control trials started. Adaptation trials were like control trials. The difference was that adaptation trials were only presented on the right side of the central target, and after T1 was turned off, it was relocated to a T2 target located 15° from the central target, i.e., 5°closer to T0 compared to T1. The T2 target was then displayed for an interval of 500 ms before it was turned off followed by a blank screen for an interval of 1,000 ms before the start of the next trial. During the series of adaptation trials, control trials in other directions related to the center of the screen (left, top, or bottom) were randomly interspersed to prevent participants from predicting the location of the next target. After presentation of 200 adaptation trials, a set of 68 post-adaptation control trials were presented. This involved presentation of the T1 at 20 degrees without any subsequent movement, the same as the original pre-adaptation control trials. This final series of control trials served as a recovery phase.

Analytical approach

Multiple saccade metrics were of interest: (1) the amplitude of primary saccades, (2) the early and the late phase of adaptation (as defined below), (3) the mean amplitude of all control trials which occurred to the left of the central target during the entire experiment (i.e., during both series of control trials and the series of adaptation trials mixed with control trials), and (4) the within-participant variability in saccade amplitude to initial control trials. The early phase was calculated as the difference between the mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation control trials and the mean amplitude of the first twenty adaptation trials, while the late phase was calculated as the difference between the mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation control trials and the mean amplitude of the last twenty adaptation trials. A two-way repeated measures ANCOVA was performed to test for differences in adaptation between the two groups as well as to investigate the impact of age on saccadic adaptation across groups. A univariate ANCOVA was used to determine if eye movement amplitudes differed between individuals with ASD and their age-matched controls, i.e., to verify if individuals with ASD tended to undershoot (hypometria) or overshoot (hypermetria) the visual target. Finally, an independent samples t-test was performed to look at the variability of eye movements in each group. Variability was calculated by deriving the standard deviation of the last twenty pre-adaptation control trials for everyone separately, and normalized by dividing this by the individuals’ mean saccade amplitude across those last twenty trials. All analyses were accomplished using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and SPSS (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

For illustrative purposes, Figure 2 below provides examples of adaptation from two TD participants and two individuals with an ASD diagnosis, providing an example of a faster and a slower adapter for each group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Saccadic adaptation examples to illustrate fast (top plots) and slow (bottom plots) adapters in both TD and ASD groups. The amplitude of primary saccadic eye movements is plotted as a function of trial number, i.e., the order in which they occurred during the experiment. Pre-adaptation or control trials (left) and post-adaptation or control trials (right) are plotted in blue. Adaptation trials are plotted in yellow.

Trials to the left (in green) correspond to the first series of control trials (the baseline phase), trials in the middle (in yellow) are adaptation trials during which the systematic shift of the target was introduced, and trials to the right (in green) correspond to the final series of control trials (i.e., the recovery phase), which was identical to the initial baseline phase. In the case of the faster TD adapter (TD1, top-right panel), the participant made saccades to the T1 target with a mean amplitude of 17.56 (SD = 0.72) during the baseline phase, but within the first 20 adaptation trials, the mean amplitude decreased to 16.19 (SD = 0.83) as a result of the introduction of the inward target shifts. This decrease in saccade amplitude continued to develop over the remaining adaptation trials, until the mean amplitude of the last 20 adaptation trials reached 11.83 (SD = 2.98). For the slower adapting TD (TD2, bottom-right panel), saccades to the target during baseline had a mean amplitude of 18.93 (SD = 0.93). During the first 20 adaptation trials, the mean amplitude remained high at 19.52 (SD = 0.87), showing no evidence for adaptation to the introduced errors. Over the remaining adaptation trials, the mean amplitude decreased gradually to 16.57 (SD = 0.91). Likewise, for the sample rapid-adapter from the ASD cohort (ASD1, top-left panel), a mean saccade amplitude of 16.38 (SD = 1.12) was observed during baseline trials. Within the first 20 adaptation trials, the mean amplitude decreased to 14.68 (SD = 1.03) and this decrease continued to evolve over subsequent adaptation trials until the mean amplitude reached 13.62 (SD = 0.77). In the case of the slow ASD adapter (ASD2, bottom-left panel), baseline trials had a mean amplitude of 15.93 (SD = 1.34), with little change seen during the initial 20 adaptation trials (Mean = 15.55; SD = 1.39), and only a very modest decrease observed over the subsequent adaptation trials (Mean = 13.78; SD = 1.30).

We set out to examine the early phase (i.e., the response to the first 20 adaptation trials) versus the late phase (i.e., the last 20 adaptations trials) of adaptation, given prior work suggesting that adaptation might be slower to emerge in participants with ASD. Figure 3 (Children; 6–17 years old) and Figure 4 (Adults; 18–45 years old), as can be seen below, show the mean amplitude of the baseline pre-adaptation trials, compared to the mean amplitude of the first twenty adaptation trials (Panel A1). In panel A2, the distributions of the differences between baseline and early-adaptation trials are plotted for each group (TD and ASD). The same plotting convention is used to show the comparison between baseline trials and the late adaptation trials in panel B of both figures.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results from Children (ages 6–17 years): Panel (A): Early Phase for the 6–17 age range; (A1): Mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation trials and of the first twenty adaptation trials, (A2): Mean amplitude difference between pre-adaptation and adaptation trials from (A1). Panel (B): Late Phase for the 6–17 age range; (B1): Mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation trials and of the last twenty adaptation trials, (B2): Mean amplitude difference between pre-adaptation and adaptation trials from (B1).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Results from Adults (ages 18–45 years): Panel (A): Early Phase for the 18–45 age range; (A1): Mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation trials and of the first twenty adaptation trials, (A2): Mean amplitude difference between pre-adaptation and adaptation trials from (A1). Panel (B): Late Phase for the 18–45 age range; (B1): Mean amplitude of the last twenty pre-adaptation trials and of the last twenty adaptation trials, (B2): Mean amplitude difference between pre-adaptation and adaptation trials from (B1).

A two-way repeated measures ANCOVA did not reveal effects of age [F(1,106) = 0.007, p = 0.936], of group [F(1,106) = 0.001, p = 0.981], or of the interaction between group and phase [F(1,106) = 0.401, p = 0.761] in saccade adaptation. However, the main effect of phase was significant [F(1,106) = 26.883, p < 0.001], reflecting the fact that the amplitude of adaptation was significantly higher in the late phase compared to the early one for both populations.

To test for differences in amplitude of eye movements between groups–that is, whether individuals with ASD tended to undershoot (hypometria) or overshoot (hypermetria) the target –we calculated the mean amplitude of all control trials in the opposite direction of adaptation trials and performed a univariate ANCOVA. Figure 5 below shows individuals with ASD tended to make longer saccades to the target in this paradigm when compared to their age-matched controls.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mean amplitude of all control trials in the opposite direction of adaptation trials per group and both age range; (A): Children, 6–17 years old and (B): Adults, 18–45 years old.

A statistically significant difference was found between ASD and TD groups in terms of amplitude of primary saccade when taking age into account: F(1,106) = 10.381, p = 0.002. The effect size of this difference was, nevertheless, small (η2 = 0.089, TD: Mean = 16.666; SD = 0.351, and ASD: Mean = 18.509; SD = 0.452).

Finally, an independent samples t-test revealed no statistically significant difference between the ASD and TD group in the normalized standard deviation based on the mean of the last 20 control trials for each individual participant [t(107) = −1.182, two-sided p = 0.240].

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate potential differences in saccade metrics between individuals with ASD and age-matched TD in the context of a classic gain-down saccadic adaptation paradigm (McLaughlin, 1967). The primary question was whether saccade adaptation to systematically introduced errors would be disrupted in this population and whether this might be more evident during the early phase of adaptation, as suggested by two previous studies in ASD populations (Johnson et al., 2013; Mosconi et al., 2013). We also measured the accuracy of saccades directed toward the hemifield opposite to that used for the adaptation task, to assess the presence of saccadic undershoot or overshoot in ASD. The primary analysis showed clear evidence for adaptation, both during the initial “early” phase (i.e., the first 20 adaptation trials), and again during the last 20 “late” adaptation trials. A main effect of phase confirmed that the later phase resulted in greater adaptation; that is, individuals continued to adapt across the adaptation trials. Our primary hypothesis, however, that sensory-oculomotor deficits would lead to slower and reduced saccade adaptation in ASD, was not supported by the data. This was found to be the case for both children and adults with ASD, providing no evidence for a developmental component. Also, in contrast to prior findings, there was no evidence that early adaptation was weaker in ASD, and again, this was the case for both the children and the adult groups.

In a secondary analysis, we assessed whether primary saccades directed toward the hemifield opposite to that used for the adaptation task would show anomalous saccade metrics. Here, we did find evidence for a tendency toward hypermetric saccades in ASD, but it should be emphasized that, while statistically significant, the effect size was found to be small. It should also be emphasized that we did not predict this finding and as such, it should be considered post hoc and exploratory at this stage and would require replication before any strong conclusions should be reached. A finding of hypermetric saccades also stands in contrast to previous work, as the majority of studies that did find primary saccade differences, tended to report hypometria (Johnson et al., 2012, 2013; Schmitt et al., 2014; Kovarski et al., 2019; Bast et al., 2021), although, there are also studies reporting no evidence for differences in saccade amplitude in ASD (Minshew et al., 1999; Luna et al., 2007; Takarae et al., 2008; Johnson et al., 2016; Kovarski et al., 2019).

Perhaps one of the more consistent findings in the literature on saccade metrics in ASD has been the finding of greater within-participant variability in saccade amplitudes (Minshew et al., 1999; Goldberg et al., 2002; Takarae et al., 2004; Nowinski et al., 2005; Glazebrook et al., 2009; Stanley-Cary et al., 2011; Johnson et al., 2012). Here, in contrast, we found no evidence for this greater variability when we looked at the within-participant variance (normalized standard deviations) in primary saccades to the last 20 control trials that were delivered before the adaptation phase of the experiment was initiated.

What then of the putative link between cerebellar anomalies in ASD and eye-movement disturbances? One possible strategy would be to perform some stratification of the ASD population, either by first identifying a subgroup of individuals with ASD who show clear cerebellar structural differences on neuroimaging and asking whether this subgroup also show saccadic dysfunctions, or by separating out subgroups of ASD individuals who show anomalous saccade metrics (i.e., lack of adaptation, hypermetria) and asking if this subgroup would show greater instances of structural cerebellar differences. Of course, given the current results, instances of non-adaptation were just as common in the TD population as in the ASD, and the finding of a slight relative hypermetria in ASD stands in contrast to several previous studies that reported precisely the opposite.

Taken together, the current findings suggest that if there are differences in saccade metrics in ASD, these are of small effect size and very subtle. Considering our current findings and the lack of consistent evidence for saccade differences in the literature, it seems highly unlikely that there is a fundamental sensory-motor processing deficit in the oculomotor system in Autism. Of course, gaze and fixation differences have been widely observed and reported in Autism (Klin et al., 2002; Hirsch et al., 2022), leaving open the question as to what drives them. Although not tested here, it may well be the case that the alternate hypothesis, that these differences are related to social motivational factors, for instance a reluctance or difficulty in engaging with peers, is the more parsimonious explanation.

It is worthwhile considering what might account for differences in the results of the present study and that of Mosconi et al. (2013) the other relatively large sample study on saccade adaptation in ASD, where subtle saccade adaptation differences were reported for early stages of adaptation. Both studies had a considerable baseline period against which to assess saccade adaptation, used similar criteria for assessing early stages of adaptation (20 to 30 trials), and included ASD and TD participants of average and above cognitive ability over a similar age range. It is possible that differences in paradigm might be a factor. In the current study, the target was presented at 20 degrees from central fixation and reduced by 25% to induce saccade adaptation whereas in Mosconi et al. (2013) it was presented at 12 degrees and reduced by 16%. Thus, the use of targets of different eccentricity and the different magnitude discrepancies between T0 and T1 could contribute to the different results. However, in both cases, most participants adapted, indicating that both were effective saccade adaptation paradigms. Whereas in our study, there was no significant group difference in number of individuals that did not adapt, in Mosconi et al. (2013), there was a substantial increase in the number of non-adapters in the ASD group and very few TD non-adapters. One possibility is that there is a trade-off between magnitude of T0 and T1 difference and initiation/speed of adaptation, and that larger magnitude differences are required, on average, for autistic individuals. Future work in which this parameter is varied will help to address this possible explanation. However, our ASD sample also did not show increased variability as it has been shown in some other studies, suggesting that differences between studies may reflect variability from sample to sample. The findings of the current study speak to a lack of robustness regarding differences in ASD in saccadic adaptation, hyper- versus hypo- metric saccades, and saccadic variability, suggesting that, unfortunately, saccades and saccadic adaptation do not hold promise as early biomarkers for the detection of ASD.

Conclusion

This study examined possible saccadic eye-movement differences between individuals with ASD and their age-matched TD peers in the context of a gain-down saccadic adaptation task. While studies reported in the literature have yielded mixed results regarding the ability of individuals with ASD to adapt saccade amplitudes in response to imposed visual errors, the data presented here provide no evidence for differential saccadic adaptation in Autism, either in children or adults on the spectrum. Nor was there evidence for a slower rate of adaptation in ASD in either children or adults. We also assessed whether primary saccades during initial control trials would be more variable at the individual participant level in ASD participants but found no evidence for greater variability in this cohort. The data did point to significantly larger primary saccades to non-adapted targets in both children and adults with Autism, although this was a post hoc finding and the effect size was small. Overall, in a sizable cohort of ASD children and adults (N = 41), this study found scant evidence for differences in saccade metrics in Autism.

Data sharing

Upon acceptance for publication, the authors will coordinate with the editorial office to ensure that the full and appropriately de-identified datasets are uploaded to a publicly available repository and that the appropriate link is made to the article file (e.g., Figshare, Dryad).

Data availability statement

The data presented in this study are deposited in the following repository: doi: dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24187761.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Subjects Review Boards of the University of Rochester and the Albert Einstein College of Medicine approved all the experimental procedures (respectively STUDY00002036 and 2009-523). All adult participants provided written informed consent and for all individuals below 18 their parents provided written informed consent and each participant in this age range provided written informed assent in accordance with the tenets laid out in the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

KT, JF, and EF designed and implemented the study. KT, AF, and DH coordinated the data collection. KT analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AF constructed the illustrations. SM took primary responsibility for coordinating recruitment and phenotyping of participants at the Albert Einstein collaborating site. All authors provided editorial input to KT on subsequent drafts and read and approved the final version of this manuscript.

Funding

Initial funding for this work was provided through a pilot grant to EF and JF from the Schmitt Program on Integrative Neuroscience (SPIN), administered through the Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience at the University of Rochester. Eye-movement recordings were conducted at the Translational Neuroimaging and Neurophysiology Core and participant phenotyping was conducted at the Human Recruitment and Phenotyping Core of the University of Rochester Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (UR-IDDRC), which is supported by a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P50 HD103536–to JF). Phenotyping and eye-tracking at the Albert Einstein collaborating site were conducted through the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (RFK-IDDRC), which is supported by a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P50 HD105352–to SM).

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the contribution of University of Rochester undergraduate students Sri Nuvvula, Chloe Pacteau, and Suzan Hoffman, and of Albert Einstein College of Medicine study coordinator Elise Taverna for their assistance with recruitment and data collection, and of clinical psychologists Leona Oakes, Ph.D and Juliana Bates, Ph.D for her contributions to phenotyping of participants.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typically developing; ANCOVA, analysis of covariance; HFA, high functioning autism.

References

Alahyane, N., Fonteille, V., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Nighoghossian, N., Pelisson, D., et al. (2008). Separate neural substrates in the human cerebellum for sensory-motor adaptation of reactive and of scanning voluntary saccades. Cerebellum 7, 595–601. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0065-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alahyane, N., and Pélisson, D. (2004). Eye position specificity of saccadic adaptation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 45, 123–130. doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-0570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alahyane, N., and Pélisson, D. (2005). Long-lasting modifications of saccadic eye movements following adaptation induced in the double-step target paradigm. Learn. Mem. 12, 433–443. doi: 10.1101/lm.96405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aschoff, J. (1968). The effect of diazepam (Valium) on the saccadic eye movements in man. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 211, 325–332. doi: 10.1007/BF00404467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avila, E., Van Der Geest, J. N., Kengne Kamga, S., Verhage, M. C., Donchin, O., and Frens, M. A. (2015). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation effects on saccade adaptation. Neural Plast. 2015:968970. doi: 10.1155/2015/968970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barash, S., Melikyan, A., Sivakov, A., Zhang, M., Glickstein, M., and Thier, P. (1999). Saccadic dysmetria and adaptation after lesions of the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 10931–10939. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-10931.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bast, N., Mason, L., Freitag, C. M., Smith, T., Portugal, A. M., Poustka, L., et al. (2021). Saccade dysmetria indicates attenuated visual exploration in autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 149–159. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belyusar, D., Snyder, A. C., Frey, H.-P., Harwood, M. R., Wallman, J., and Foxe, J. J. (2013). Oscillatory alpha-band suppression mechanisms during the rapid attentional shifts required to perform an anti-saccade task. NeuroImage 65, 395–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenner, L. A., Turner, K. C., and Müller, R. A. (2007). Eye movement and visual search: Are there elementary abnormalities in autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1289–1309. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0277-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cecala, A. L., and Freedman, E. G. (2008). Amplitude changes in response to target displacements during human eye–head movements. Vis. Res. 48, 149–166. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cecala, A. L., and Freedman, E. G. (2009). Head-unrestrained gaze adaptation in the rhesus macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 164–183. doi: 10.1152/jn.90735.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A., Ashby, L., Crowley, D., Land, G., Peck, A., and Miller, A. (1985). Lamotrigine (BW430C), a potential anticonvulsant. Effects on the central nervous system in comparison with phenytoin and diazepam. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 20, 619–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.1985.tb05120.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, T., and Wallman, J. (2012). The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 3342–3348. doi: 10.1152/jn.00746.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courchesne, E., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A., Lincoln, A. J., Haas, R. H., et al. (1994). Abnormality of cerebellar vermian lobules VI and VII in patients with infantile autism: Identification of hypoplastic and hyperplastic subgroups with MR imaging. Am. J. Roentgenol. 162, 123–130. doi: 10.2214/ajr.162.1.8273650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courchesne, E., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Hesselink, J. R., and Jernigan, T. L. (1988). Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. N. Engl. J. Med. 318, 1349–1354. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198805263182102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deubel, H., Wolf, W., and Hauske, G. (1986). Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. Hum. Neurobiol. 5, 245–253.

Google Scholar

Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: A synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedman, E. G., and Foxe, J. J. (2018). Eye movements, sensorimotor adaptation and cerebellar-dependent learning in autism: Toward potential biomarkers and subphenotypes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 47, 549–555. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frens, M. A., and Van Opstal, A. J. (1997). Monkey superior colliculus activity during short-term saccadic adaptation. Brain Res. Bull. 43, 473–483. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(97)80001-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, A., Kaneko, C., and Scudder, C. (1985). Brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 307–337. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.001515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galea, J. M., Jayaram, G., Ajagbe, L., and Celnik, P. (2009). Modulation of cerebellar excitability by polarity-specific noninvasive direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29, 9115–9122. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2184-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaymard, B., Rivaud-Pechoux, S., Yelnik, J., Pidoux, B., and Ploner, C. (2001). Involvement of the cerebellar thalamus in human saccade adaptation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 554–560. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01669.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghaziuddin, M., and Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: A further report. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 42, 43–48. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.1998.00065.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glazebrook, C., Gonzalez, D., Hansen, S., and Elliott, D. (2009). The role of vision for online control of manual aiming movements in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 13, 411–433. doi: 10.1177/1362361309105659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffart, L., Chen, L. L., and Sparks, D. L. (2004). Deficits in saccades and fixation during muscimol inactivation of the caudal fastigial nucleus in the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 3351–3367. doi: 10.1152/jn.01199.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, M. C., Lasker, A. G., Zee, D. S., Garth, E., Tien, A., and Landa, R. J. (2002). Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia 40, 2039–2049. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00059-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, M. J., and Schultz-Ross, R. A. (1993). Adverse ocular effects of anticonvulsants. Psychosomatics 34, 154–158. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(93)71906-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., and Lord, C. (2007). The autism diagnostic observation schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 613–627.

Google Scholar

Hilton, E. J. R., Hosking, S. L., and Betts, T. (2004). The effect of antiepileptic drugs on visual performance. Seizure 13, 113–128. doi: 10.1016/S1059-1311(03)00082-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, J., Zhang, X., Noah, J. A., Dravida, S., Naples, A., Tiede, M., et al. (2022). Neural correlates of eye contact and social function in autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One 17:e0265798. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0265798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, J. J., and Fuchs, A. F. (2002). Investigating the site of human saccadic adaptation with express and targeting saccades. Exp. Brain Res. 144, 538–548. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1077-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, J. J., and Fuchs, A. F. (2004). The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 27–53. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, J. J., and Fuchs, A. F. (2006). Amplitude adaptation occurs where a saccade is represented as a vector and not as its components. Vis. Res. 46, 3121–3128. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jayaram, G., Tang, B., Pallegadda, R., Vasudevan, E. V. L., Celnik, P., and Bastian, A. (2012). Modulating locomotor adaptation with cerebellar stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 2950–2957.

Google Scholar

Johnson, B. P., Lum, J. A., Rinehart, N. J., and Fielding, J. (2016). Ocular motor disturbances in autism spectrum disorders: Systematic review and comprehensive meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 260–279. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., Papadopoulos, N., Tonge, B., Millist, L., White, O., et al. (2012). A closer look at visually guided saccades in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:99. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., White, O., Millist, L., and Fielding, J. (2013). Saccade adaptation in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Neuroscience 243, 76–87.

Google Scholar

Jones, V., and Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 15, 37–46.

Google Scholar

Kelly, S. P., Foxe, J. J., Newman, G., and Edelman, J. A. (2010). Prepare for conflict: EEG correlates of the anticipation of target competition during overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1690–1700. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07219.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemner, C., Van der Geest, J. N., Verbaten, M. N., and van Engeland, H. (2004). In search of neurophysiological markers of pervasive developmental disorders: Smooth pursuit eye movements? J. Neural Transm. 111, 1617–1626. doi: 10.1007/s00702-004-0164-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., Camfferman, G., and Van Engeland, H. (1998). Abnormal saccadic eye movements in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 28, 61–67.

Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., and Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohen-Raz, R., Volkman, F. R., and Cohen, D. J. (1992). Postural control in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 22, 419–432.

Google Scholar

Kovarski, K., Siwiaszczyk, M., Malvy, J., Batty, M., and Latinus, M. (2019). Faster eye movements in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 12, 212–224.

Google Scholar

Lappe, M. (2009). What is adapted in saccadic adaptation? J. Physiol. 587(Pt 1):5.

Google Scholar

Lemoine-Lardennois, C., Alahyane, N., Tailhefer, C., Collins, T., Fagard, J., and Doré-Mazars, K. (2016). Saccadic adaptation in 10–41 month-old children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:241. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, C., Shorvon, S. D., Luxon, L. M., and Bamiou, D.-E. (2008). Saccadic eye movements and anti-epileptic drugs. Epilepsy Res. 78, 93–101.

Google Scholar

Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., and Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 474–481.

Google Scholar

Lunn, J., Donovan, T., Litchfield, D., Lewis, C., Davies, R., and Crawford, T. (2016). Saccadic eye movement abnormalities in children with epilepsy. PLoS One 11:e0160508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, S. C. (1967). Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept. Psychophys. 2, 359–362.

Google Scholar

Miller, J. M., Anstis, T., and Templeton, W. B. (1981). Saccadic plasticity: Parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. J. Exp. Psychol. 7:356.

Google Scholar

Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., and Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology 52, 917–922.

Google Scholar

Molloy, C. A., Dietrich, K. N., and Bhattacharya, A. (2003). Postural stability in children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33, 643–652.

Google Scholar

Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D’cruz, A.-M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., and Stanford, L. D. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Med. 39, 1559–1566. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosconi, M. W., Luna, B., Kay-Stacey, M., Nowinski, C. V., Rubin, L. H., Scudder, C., et al. (2013). Saccade adaptation abnormalities implicate dysfunction of cerebellar-dependent learning mechanisms in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). PLoS One 8:e63709. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noto, C. T., Watanabe, S., and Fuchs, A. F. (1999). Characteristics of simian adaptation fields produced by behavioral changes in saccade size and direction. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2798–2813. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.6.2798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowinski, C. V., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., Takarae, Y., and Sweeney, J. A. (2005). Oculomotor studies of cerebellar function in autism. Psychiatry Res. 137, 11–19.

Google Scholar

Panouillères, M. T. N., Miall, R. C., and Jenkinson, N. (2015). The role of the posterior cerebellum in saccadic adaptation: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. J. Neurosci. 35, 5471–5479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4064-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, K. M., Shin, K. J., Ha, S. Y., Park, J., Kim, S. E., Kim, H. C., et al. (2015). Can the adverse effects of antiepileptic drugs be detected in saccadic eye movements? Seizure 25, 33–36.

Google Scholar

Pélisson, D., Alahyane, N., Panouilleres, M., and Tilikete, C. (2010). Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1103–1120.

Google Scholar

Pensiero, S., Fabbro, F., Michieletto, P., Accardo, A., and Brambilla, P. (2009). Saccadic characteristics in autistic children. Funct. Neurol. 24, 153–158.

Google Scholar

Phillips, J. O., Fuchs, A. F., Ling, L., Iwamoto, Y., and Votaw, S. (1997). Gain adaptation of eye and head movement components of simian gaze shifts. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 2817–2821. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.5.2817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, J. L., Lencer, R., Bishop, J. R., Keedy, S., and Sweeney, J. A. (2008). Pharmacological treatment effects on eye movement control. Brain Cogn. 68, 415–435.

Google Scholar

Remler, B. F., Leigh, R. J., Osorio, I., and Tomsak, R. L. (1990). The characteristics and mechanisms of visual disturbance associated with anticonvulsant therapy. Neurology 40:791.

Google Scholar

Robinson, F. R., Noto, C. T., and Bevans, S. E. (2003). Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1235–1244.

Google Scholar

Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., and Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Dev. 67, 2060–2073.

Google Scholar

Rosenhall, U., Johansson, E., and Gillberg, C. (1988). Oculomotor findings in autistic children. J. Laryngol. Otol. 102, 435–439.

Google Scholar

Schmitt, L. M., Cook, E. H., Sweeney, J. A., and Mosconi, M. W. (2014). Saccadic eye movement abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder indicate dysfunctions in cerebellum and brainstem. Mol. Autism 5:47.

Google Scholar

Scudder, C. A., Batourina, E. Y., and Tunder, G. S. (1998). Comparison of two methods of producing adaptation of saccade size and implications for the site of plasticity. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 704–715. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanley-Cary, C., Rinehart, N., Tonge, B., White, O., and Fielding, J. (2011). Greater disruption to control of voluntary saccades in autistic disorder than Asperger’s disorder: Evidence for greater cerebellar involvement in autism? Cerebellum 10, 70–80. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0229-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straube, A., Deubel, H., Ditterich, J., and Eggert, T. (2001). Cerebellar lesions impair rapid saccade amplitude adaptation. Neurology 57, 2105–2108.

Google Scholar

Straube, A., Fuchs, A. F., Usher, S., and Robinson, F. R. (1997). Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 874–895.

Google Scholar

Takarae, Y., Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., and Sweeney, J. A. (2008). Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 980–989. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takarae, Y., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., and Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Oculomotor abnormalities parallel cerebellar histopathology in autism. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 1359–1361. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.022491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeichi, N., Kaneko, C. R. S., and Fuchs, A. F. (2005). Discharge of monkey nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis neurons changes during saccade adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1938–1951. doi: 10.1152/jn.00113.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeichi, N., Kaneko, C. R. S., and Fuchs, A. F. (2007). Activity changes in monkey superior colliculus during saccade adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 4096–4107.

Google Scholar

Tedeschi, G., Casucci, G., Allocca, S., Riva, R., Costanzo, A. D., Tata, M., et al. (1989). Neuroocular side effects of carbamazepine and phenobarbital in epileptic patients as measured by saccadic eye movements analysis. Epilepsia 30, 62–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1989.tb05282.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thakkar, K. N., Polli, F. E., Joseph, R. M., Tuch, D. S., Hadjikhani, N., Barton, J. J. S., et al. (2008). Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Brain 131, 2464–2478. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Camfferman, G., Verbaten, M. N., and van Engeland, H. (2001). Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: A saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 614–619. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01070-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilensky, J. A., Damasio, A. R., and Maurer, R. G. (1981). Gait disturbances in patients with autistic behavior: A preliminary study. Arch. Neurol. 38, 646–649. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1981.00510100074013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallman, J., and Fuchs, A. F. (1998). Saccadic gain modification: Visual error drives motor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2405–2416. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.5.2405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence-second edition [Database record]. APA PsycTests. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/t15171-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkes, B. J., Carson, T. B., Patel, K. P., Lewis, M. H., and White, K. D. (2015). Oculomotor performance in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 38, 338–344. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, A. L., and Shelhamer, M. (2011). Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 1130–1140. doi: 10.1152/jn.00394.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaccara, G., Gangemi, P., Messori, A., Parigi, A., Massi, S., Valenza, T., et al. (1992). Effects of oxcarbazepine and carbamazepine on the central nervous system: Computerised analysis of saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements. Acta Neurol. Scand. 85, 425–429. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06041.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder (ASD), eye-tracking, eye movements, saccades, saccadic adaptation

Citation: Tarrit K, Freedman EG, Francisco AA, Horsthuis DJ, Molholm S and Foxe JJ (2023) No evidence for differential saccadic adaptation in children and adults with an autism spectrum diagnosis. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 17:1232474. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1232474

Received: 31 May 2023; Accepted: 24 August 2023;
Published: 06 October 2023.

Edited by:

Reza Rastmanesh, American Physical Society, United States

Reviewed by:

George Fragulis, University of Western Macedonia, Greece
John Stins, VU Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2023 Tarrit, Freedman, Francisco, Horsthuis, Molholm and Foxe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John J. Foxe, john_foxe@urmc.rochester.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.