- 1Public Health, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, United States
- 2Department of Occupational Therapy, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
- 3Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
- 4Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
- 5Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
- 6South African Medical Research Council (SAMRC), Unit on Risk and Resilience in Mental Disorders, Cape Town, South Africa
- 7University Statistical Consulting, LLC, Irvine, CA, United States
- 8Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
- 9Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
- 10Ahmanson-Lovelace Brain Mapping Center, Department of Neurology, Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
- 11Department of Bioengineering, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
- 12Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa
Background: Alcohol and tobacco are known teratogens. Historically, more severe prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) and prenatal tobacco exposure (PTE) have been examined as the principal predictor of neurodevelopmental alterations, with little incorporation of lower doses or ecological contextual factors that can also impact neurodevelopment, such as socioeconomic resources (SER) or adverse childhood experiences (ACEs). Here, a novel analytical approach informed by a socio-ecological perspective was used to examine the associations between SER, PAE and/or PTE, and ACEs, and their effects on neurodevelopment.
Methods: N = 313 mother-child dyads were recruited from a prospective birth cohort with maternal report of PAE and PTE, and cross-sectional structural brain neuroimaging of child acquired via 3T scanner at ages 8–11 years. In utero SER was measured by maternal education, household income, and home utility availability. The child’s ACEs were measured by self-report assisted by the researcher. PAE was grouped into early exposure (<12 weeks), continued exposure (>=12 weeks), and no exposure controls. PTE was grouped into exposed and non-exposed controls.
Results: Greater access to SER during pregnancy was associated with fewer ACEs (maternal education: β = −0.293,p = 0.01; phone access: β = −0.968,p = 0.05). PTE partially mediated the association between SER and ACEs, where greater SER reduced the likelihood of PTE, which was positively associated with ACEs (β = 1.110,p = 0.01). SER was associated with alterations in superior frontal (β = −1336.036, q = 0.046), lateral orbitofrontal (β = −513.865, q = 0.046), caudal anterior cingulate volumes (β = −222.982, q = 0.046), with access to phone negatively associated with all three brain volumes. Access to water was positively associated with superior frontal volume (β=1569.527, q = 0.013). PTE was associated with smaller volumes of lateral orbitofrontal (β = −331.000, q = 0.033) and nucleus accumbens regions (β = −34.800, q = 0.033).
Conclusion: Research on neurodevelopment following community-levels of PAE and PTE should more regularly consider the ecological context to accelerate understanding of teratogenic outcomes. Further research is needed to replicate this novel conceptual approach with varying PAE and PTE patterns, to disentangle the interplay between dose, community-level and individual-level risk factors on neurodevelopment.
1. Introduction
Alcohol and tobacco are established teratogens, as proven in animal models, and consistent with findings in human pediatric samples. Numerous studies have shown that prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) can lead to alterations in children’s physical, cognitive, mental, behavioral and neural development (Glass et al., 2014; Mattson et al., 2019). Since the original recognition of alcohol as a teratogen in humans (Jones and Smith, 1973), the subsequent 50 years of original brain research on FASD has consistently demonstrated structural brain alterations (Riley et al., 1995; Mattson et al., 1996; Archibald et al., 2001; Sowell et al., 2001, 2002, 2008). PAE poses cumulative harm to global health and results in significant economic burdens. PAE can increase demands on health care, special education, justice system, morbidity and mortality, and loss in productivity for both the affected children and their caregivers (Greenmyer et al., 2018; World Health Organization [WHO], 2021). Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (FASD) refer to a range of diagnoses following PAE. Recent estimates of the collective prevalence of FASD suggest even higher rates than historically reported at 3.1–9.9% in the United States (May et al., 2018). A population-based study conducted in South Africa found a prevalence of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS), one of 4 diagnoses under the FASD umbrella term, to be between 5.9–9.1%, and a collective FASD prevalence between 13.5–20.7% (May et al., 2013).
Prenatal tobacco exposure (PTE) is a common co-occurring exposure with PAE (Cornelius and Day, 2009), and has been associated with alterations in speech processing, attention, internalizing and externalizing behavior, and brain development (Cornelius and Day, 2009; El Marroun et al., 2014). Despite our understanding of the teratogenic effect of these substances, PAE in conjunction with PTE continue to occur in substance-using societies and pose significant public health challenges.
Existing literature has attributed brain alterations primarily to the teratogenic effect of PAE, with limited consistent examination of other key and often upstream factors that may also shape brain structural development. Likelihood of prenatal substance exposure is closely associated with availability of socioeconomic resources at individual and neighborhood levels (Karriker-Jaffe, 2013; Coleman-Cowger et al., 2017). In general, socioeconomic resources, PAE and PTE are associated with hardships in prenatal and postnatal experiences, which can also alter a child’s developmental trajectory (Gibson et al., 2009; Lange et al., 2013; Baglivio et al., 2017; Breen et al., 2018; Kambeitz et al., 2019; Luby et al., 2019). Limited recent research provides initial evidence that PAE may interact with low socioeconomic resources (Coles et al., 2019; Uban et al., 2020) to impact child developmental outcomes. With such limited knowledge, more understanding of upstream factors that contribute to teratogenic outcomes on childhood-adolescent brain outcomes is needed.
Existing literature demonstrates that lack of socioeconomic resources is a childhood adversity on its own that leads to disadvantages in executive functioning, memory, and language development (Noble et al., 2006, 2012; Noble and Farah, 2013), and is reflected in development of brain structure (Gonzalez et al., 2020). The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) framework incorporates factors such as emotional and physical abuse, domestic violence toward the mother, household substance use and mental illness, and household member with a history of incarceration. Although conceptually limited access to socioeconomic resources may be an ACE in and of itself, socioeconomic resources and ACEs have distinct differences. ACEs have been shown to be associated with greater risk for health challenges in children, including risk for mental health challenges, development of chronic medical conditions, and regional brain development alterations (Teicher et al., 2012, 2016; Kerker et al., 2015; Luby et al., 2019; Mall et al., 2020; Sevenoaks et al., 2022).
Less is known about how poverty may increase the likelihood of other ACEs (Melchior et al., 2007; Finkelhor et al., 2013). The conceptual model developed by Culhane and Elo (2005) hypothesized that socioeconomic resources can influence either positive or negative individual health behaviors (including substance use during pregnancy), through the availability of social services, exposure to stress, and social norms. These individual health behaviors may partially explain the association between socioeconomic resources, childhood subsequent ACEs, and child neurodevelopmental outcomes. In other words, low socioeconomic resources, presence of PAE and PTE and more ACEs may tend to cluster together, while each has its own impact on child neurodevelopment.
Some support for the importance of considering socioeconomic resources and ACEs in PAE exists within samples including biological birthing parents. It is established in perinatal literature that socioeconomic resources are associated with PAE, partly via differential patterns and profiles of co-exposures. Lower maternal income is associated with a higher odds ratio of prenatal exposure to marijuana and tobacco (Coleman-Cowger et al., 2017). Women with residence in disadvantaged neighborhoods were more likely to experience substance exposed pregnancies to tobacco and other drugs in comparison to women living in middle-class neighborhoods (Karriker-Jaffe, 2013). Social capital of the country in which women resided was significantly associated with PTE (Shoff and Yang, 2013). Levels of neighborhood assistance accounted for significant variances of type of PAE and PTE after controlling for individual-level characteristics such as race, age, public assistance, and prenatal care (Finch et al., 2001). The potential bidirectionality between upstream socioeconomic resources factors of prevalence of PAE/PTE is not understood well.
In addition to systemic factors, prenatal substance exposure status may serve as indicators of other adverse circumstances within the home environment that shape children’s living experiences. For instance, alcohol use for women has been associated with higher risk for experiencing intimate partner violence, which may be associated with an unstable household environment for the children (O’Connor et al., 2006). Literature shows that maternal ACEs is associated with increased risk of PTE as well as adverse experiences of offspring, such as intimate partner violence and child maltreatment (Pear et al., 2017; Buffarini et al., 2022). It is possible that maternal cumulative exposure to adversity, including ACEs and poverty, increases the risk of prenatal tobacco exposure, which links to a subsequent elevated ACEs in children.
Expanding upon current understanding of how socioeconomic resources and ACEs contribute to PAE- and PTE-related structural brain alterations, we applied a novel conceptual model in the present analyses to examine PAE and PTE as mediators of socioeconomic resources and postnatal ACEs, and to examine the effects of socioeconomic resources, PAE/PTE, and ACEs on brain outcomes. Rather than framing prenatal substance exposure as primary predictors of brain alterations, this intentional reframing of prenatal substance exposure as a mediator is warranted, given the commonly co-occurring of between prenatal substance, socioeconomic resources (Bingol et al., 1987; McLachlan et al., 2020) and ACEs (Kambeitz et al., 2019; Andre et al., 2020): all factors known to individually impact brain development (Rivkin et al., 2008; Dannlowski et al., 2012; Noble et al., 2012; Luby et al., 2013; Bick and Nelson, 2016).
For the first aim, we hypothesized that fewer socioeconomic resources would be associated with more ACEs, and presence or absence of PAE or PTE would partially mediate this relationship (Figure 1). The second aim examined whether socioeconomic resources-related resources, PAE or PTE, or ACEs altered cortical brain structural development among children and adolescents (Figure 1). We hypothesized that lower socioeconomic resources, the presence of PAE or PTE, and higher ACEs would be associated with smaller cortical volumes.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Study design and participant recruitment
The current study involved a subsample of the existing birth cohort of the Prenatal Alcohol in Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDs) and Stillbirth (PASS) Network recruited from Cape Town, South Africa (Dukes et al., 2014). For the original PASS cohort, pregnant women were recruited during their routine antenatal care at the Belhar antenatal clinic and Bishop Lavis Midwife Obstetric Unit between August 2007 and January 2015. Enrollment of pregnant women started between the 6th week of gestation and delivery day. Pregnant women within this cohort originated from Bishop Lavis and Belhar communities: both low-income urban suburbs that developed as a direct result of apartheid in of Cape Town, South Africa. Historically, both communities have experienced high rates of prenatal alcohol exposure, SIDS and socioeconomic inequalities (May et al., 2000). A detailed report on the recruitment methodology of the original PASS study has been published elsewhere (Dukes et al., 2014).
For the study reported here, birth parent/legal guardian and their child were recruited from the PASS birth cohort among those with surviving children 8–12 years later. This age range was selected for the dynamic pubertal maturation that occurs during the transition from childhood to early adolescence. This period was hypothesized to be more opportunistic for observing lasting brain alternations following prenatal conditions. Further, the neuroimaging protocols were adapted from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study, designed for collecting MRI data at this age range. Female birth parents and their children were approached for neuroimaging and other neuropsychological measures in the townships around Cape Town, South Africa. This current analysis includes a sample of 313 birth parent/legal guardian–child participant dyads. The demographics of the study sample is presented in Table 1.
Inclusion criteria for the birth parents were (1) at least 16 years of age and (2) spoke either English or Afrikaans. Children were between 8 and 11 years of age at the acquisition of the MRI brain scan. Exclusion criteria were (1) history of traumatic brain injury, (2) presence of major medical or central nervous system disorders, and (3) MRI contraindications, such as orthodontic braces and ferromagnetic metal implants.
2.2. Measurements
2.2.1. Structural magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition
A 3-Tesla Siemens Skyra scanner at the Cape Universities Imaging Center (CUBIC) was used to acquire whole-brain T1-weighted images for all participants. The total acquisition time was around 45 min, and only data from the structural scan was analyzed for the current study. The image was acquired through a multi-echo T1w MPRAGE sequence, with acquisition parameters as following: 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxel size, 176 slices, slice thickness 1.00 mm, FOV 256 × 256, TR = 2,530 ms, TE = (1.61; 3.44, 5.27; 7.1 ms), TI = 1,240 ms, flip angle = 7 degrees.
2.2.2. Image processing
FreeSurfer’S v5.3 recon-all pipeline was utilized as metrics for volumetric segmentation. Briefly, the FreeSurfer pipeline includes motion correction (Reuter et al., 2010), non-uniform intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), skull-strip (Ségonne et al., 2004), Talairach transformation and volumetric labeling of cortical and subcortical regions (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl, 2004), tessellation of gray/white matter boundaries for topology correction and cortical surface construction (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl, 2004), parcellation of white and gray matter and derivation of cortical and subcortical matrices. A detailed description of all steps can be found elsewhere: https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/FreeSurferMethodsCitation. The structural MRI sequence was adapted from the US-based ABCD Study © that was designed to optimize pediatric neuroimaging for similar age ranges (9.0–10.99 years old): covering both late childhood and early adolescence matching the age range and pubertal maturation of participants in the present study.
2.2.3. ROIs
The overlapping cortical and subcortical regions that have been historically shown to be impacted by PAE and PTE, socioeconomic resources, and ACEs were selected as Regions of interests (ROIs) (Cortical: superior frontal, medial and lateral orbitofrontal, rostral, and caudal anterior cingulate regions; Subcortical: hippocampus, thalamus, amygdala, caudate, nucleus accumbens, and putamen). Volumes of ROIs were analyzed bilaterally across left and right hemispheres.
2.2.4. Socioeconomic resources measures
Socioeconomic resources measures included monthly household income in South African rand (ZAR), the number of school grades completed by the birth parent, dichotomous (yes/no) utility variables that recorded the availability of electricity, phone (landline and/or mobile phone), flushing toilet, and running water in the household (Myer et al., 2008). Socioeconomic resources measures were included individually in the analysis (e.g., household income, utility availability, and maternal education).
2.2.5. Prenatal substance exposure measures
The PASS study collected prospective information on PAE and PTE using a modified Timeline Follow-Back (TLFB) during pregnancy. The TLFB measure was modified to be administered in the participant’s language of choice (Afrikaans in the current analytical sample), and prompts to the researchers were inserted to facilitate precision of administration among participants and across repeated time points within participants during pregnancy (Dukes et al., 2014).
Data on PAE and PTE was collected up to three times during pregnancy (20–24, 28–32, and 34+ gestational weeks) and 1 month post-delivery using the TLFB. Detailed information to accurately measure the total grams of alcohol consumed on a drinking day were collected. Standard drinks were calculated based on the type of alcohol consumed, whether the drinks contained ice, if drinks were shared amongst others, and the volume potentially consumed as measured by the size of container.
Timing data for PAE was grouped into three PAE categories: (1) early PAE, (2) extended PAE, and (3) no PAE. The no PAE group included children whose birth mothers reported consuming no alcohol in all three trimesters. The early PAE group included children whose mother reported having one or more drinks during the first trimester (<12 weeks into pregnancy) but not in the second or the third trimesters, while the extended PAE group included children whose mother reported consuming one or more drinks in two or all trimesters of their pregnancy. Available PTE data was grouped dichotomously into (1) PTE at any time in utero or (2) no PTE exposure throughout in utero development.
2.2.6. ACEs measure
Because no prior ACE questionnaires existed that were validated for youth in the Cape Town Flats, validated ACEs items from existing literature were compiled across several questionnaires. Individual ACE items were selected in close consultation with research staff in Cape Town to determine which items were: (1) relevant to the lived experiences of the youth participants in their culture; (2) did not require mandatory reporting if endorsed to avoid harming rapport between researchers and the community members within the Cape Town Flats; and (3) retained original meaning after being translated and back-translated into Afrikaans, as determined by the US and South African researchers.
The final ACE questionnaire consisted of 14 dichotomous questions (Supplementary Table 1). The children were asked questions which related to whether they had witnessed sexual abuse, or had experienced emotional and physical, neglect and parental separation, substance use, incarceration and mental illness within the household, homelessness or violence, and loss of a loved one. Child participants completed the questionnaire with the research assistant in their preferred language of either English or Afrikaans. A summary score was calculated by counting the total number of questions that the child endorsed.
2.3. Statistical analysis
CRAN R v.4.1 was used to perform statistical analyses (Bates et al., 2015; Kuznetsova et al., 2017; R Development Core Team, 2019; Wickham et al., 2019, 2022; Heinzen et al., 2021).
2.3.1. Mediation analysis
To test whether PAE or PTE were mediators between socioeconomic resources and ACEs, we applied the Baron and Kenny criteria for mediation analysis (Baron and Kenny, 1986). The analytic flow is shown in Figure 1. For the first step, we examined the association between socioeconomic resources variables and ACE total score. A generalized linear model (GLM) was fitted with ACE total score as the outcome variable and household income, maternal education, phone, water, and electricity availability as the explanatory variable with a link function for the Gaussian distribution. For the second step, we tested the association between the explanatory variable and the mediator. A GLM was fitted with the same socioeconomic resources variables as the explanatory variables, and PAE or PTE as the outcome variable, with a link function for the binomial distribution. If PAE or PTE was significantly associated with socioeconomic resources variables, it was then included in the last step of the analysis. For the third and last step of the mediation analysis, we examined the direct effect between socioeconomic resources and ACE total score by adjusting for the potential mediator. The condition for a partial mediation was met if (1) the main outcome variable, ACE total score, and the mediator, PAE or PTE, were significantly associated with socioeconomic resources variables; (2) the mediator was significant in the third step analysis; (3) the absolute values of the estimate of the explanatory variables were reduced when the mediator was included.
2.3.2. sMRI analysis
To examine whether PAE/PTE, socioeconomic resources, or ACE were associated with brain volume alterations in the prefrontal and striatum areas, we applied a linear mixed-effects model using the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015; Kuznetsova et al., 2017). Because PAE/PTE, socioeconomic resources and ACE were significantly associated with each other, we examined the effect of PAE/PTE, socioeconomic resources, and ACE on the ROI volumes separately. Hemisphere was included as a within-subject variable. Our models were constructed as follows: first, we constructed a reduced model with only the primary relationship; then, we built up from the reduced model by adjusting for covariates, including child age and sex. In the case of PAE and PTE, we constructed a third model with an interaction term between PTE and PAE and the covariates. We used AIC comparison and log likelihood ratio test to determine whether including the covariates provided a better fit to the model and whether an interaction was appropriate. Lastly, a false discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied to all individual explanatory variables across the 11 ROIs. The results were considered significant if the q-value, the FDR analog of the p-value, was less than 0.05.
2.4. Ethics
The data collection was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of University of Cape Town (HREC UCT REF 248/2014). The Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of Stellenbosch University gave their ethical approval (REF 248/2014). The Institutional Review Board (IRB) at Children’s Hospital in Los Angeles approved the processing of de-identified neuroimaging data (CHLA-19-00228). The IRB at University of California, Irvine approved the analysis of de-identified data (UCI #212354).
3. Results
Detailed demographic information is presented in Table 1. Of the 313 enrollees, 229 participants (mean age: 9.91 years; 131 (48.7%) male) had available PAE and PTE data. Among them, 50 had early PAE (exposure during the 1st trimester), 100 had extended PAE, and 95 had no PAE. A 110 had no PTE and 159 had PTE. Fifty-five had only PAE, 43 had only PTE, and 116 had both PAE (early or extended) and PTE. On average, the total number of ACEs endorsed was 3.8. The average maternal education was 9.98 years, while the mean monthly household income was 817.12 ZAR (equivalent to $45.55 US dollars). Age, sex, parent-reported and child-reported pubertal development scale did not differ by PAE status (p > 0.05). Birth weight also did not differ by PTE and PAE (p > 0.05).
3.1. Mediation analysis
In the first-step mediation analysis, we examined the primary relationship between specific socioeconomic resources-related resources and total ACE scores. Lower maternal education (β = −0.293,p = 0.01) and no phone access (β = −0.968,p = 0.05) were both associated with higher ACE total scores. For the second-step analysis, socioeconomic resources was regressed against the two potential mediators, PAE and PTE. Lower household income (β = −0.001,p = 0.01) and lower maternal education (β = −0.248,p = 0.05) were associated with PTE, while no phone access only (β = −1.210,p = 0.05) was associated with PAE (i.e., early PAE, extended PAE, no PAE). In the third step of mediation analysis, PTE (i.e., yes PTE, no PTE) and PAE were included, respectively in the primary association models to test the direct association between socioeconomic resources and ACE after adjusting for the mediators. PAE was not a significant explanatory variable when the model included socioeconomic resources variables, and therefore PAE did not fulfill the criteria as a mediator. PTE remained significant when added to the socioeconomic resources-ACE model (β = 1.110,p = 0.01), where the presence of PTE was associated with higher ACE total scores. Moreover, the absolute value of the effect estimates of household income and maternal education were reduced after PTE was included in the model (Table 2). Therefore, PTE fulfilled the criteria as a partial mediator between socioeconomic resources and ACE, while PAE did not fulfill the criteria as a partial mediator.
3.2. sMRI analysis
All models were adjusted for age (months) and biological sex (at birth), as the covariates significantly improved model fit as evident in log likelihood ratio tests (p < 0.05). The PAE and PTE models did not include the interaction term between PTE and PAE, as in all cases the interaction term did not significantly improve model fit. After FDR correction, PTE was significantly associated with the lower volumes of lateral orbitofrontal region (β = −331.000, q = 0.033) and accumbens areas (β = −34.800, q = 0.033) (Figure 2A). PAE was associated with increased thalamus, accumbens area and caudate before the FDR correction (p < 0.05), but these associations did not carry on with the FDR correction (q > 0.05). ACE total score was not significantly associated with the volumes of any of the 11 brain ROIs. Phone access was associated with the smaller volumes of superior frontal (β = −1336.036, q = 0.046), lateral orbitofrontal (β = −513.865, q = 0.046), and the caudal anterior cingulate (β = −222.982, q = 0.046) (Figure 2B). Water access was associated with larger volumes of the superior frontal region (β=1569.527, q = 0.013) (Figure 2C). Uncorrected p-values and full model estimates are presented in Supplementary Table 2.
Figure 2. Scatter plots showing significant regions post FDR correction. Error bars show 95% confidence interval. (A) Significant associations between prenatal tobacco exposure and brain volume. (B) Significant associations between phone access and brain volume. (C) Significant associations between water access and brain volume.
4. Discussion
The present analyses examined a novel reframing of PAE and PTE as mediators for the association between socioeconomic resources and postnatal ACEs on cortical brain volumes. Within a very low socioeconomic resources context, and with prospective community-levels of prenatal substance exposure, we found that PTE, but not PAE, partially mediated the association between less in utero socioeconomic resources and subsequent more postnatal ACEs for the youth. Lower socioeconomic resources during pregnancy were associated with higher likelihood of PTE, and in turn PTE was associated with higher total number of endorsed ACEs. Both socioeconomic resources and PTE were associated with smaller volumes in prefrontal and striatum regions. Lower socioeconomic resources during pregnancy were associated with increased likelihood of subsequent PAE. However, given null brain findings with community-levels of PAE in this cohort, whether PAE plays a similar mediating role between socioeconomic resources, ACEs and brain outcomes as PTE does remains unknown, particularly for populations experiencing higher PAE known to cause clinical FASD, or within higher resourced contexts.
Our results demonstrate that PTE, but not PAE, was associated with lower cortical volume in the lateral orbitofrontal region and nucleus accumbens. While lateral orbitofrontal has been consistently associated with processing of rewards and punishments, as well as emotional and social regulations (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004), nucleus accumbens serves to integrate information from frontal and temporal regions and facilitate action (Floresco, 2015). These functional correlates of lateral orbitofrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens are consistent with the negative association between PTE and global cognition in children between 9 to 12 years old found in the current literature, suggesting a potential brain-behavior relationship (Fried et al., 1998; Gonzalez et al., 2023). Our results are consistent with prior studies from this same birth cohort representing community patterns of PAE and PTE, showing more widespread cortical and subcortical brain alterations with PTE compared to PAE at ages 6 years old (Uban et al., 2023) and ages 8–12 years (Marshall et al., 2022). Compared to alcohol, tobacco use is less likely to be cut back during pregnancy and more likely to be associated with tobacco exposure after pregnancy (Leech et al., 1999; Cornelius and Day, 2000). Even among women who reduce their tobacco use or quit spontaneously during pregnancy, postpartum relapse is common (Crume, 2019). It is likely that children who had PTE were also exposed to prolonged second-hand smoke perinatally [from birth parent and/or others smoking around child (Paavola et al., 1996; Scherrer et al., 2012)], which has been known to increase the risk of poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes in children (Chen et al., 2013). Therefore, it is possible that the structural brain development differences observed in our analysis were the result of accumulated tobacco exposure via maternal systems from both maternal use as well as use from others via second-hand smoke exposure during perinatal development. The potential for PTE from others’ use may be a mechanism for reaching higher doses of exposure, and/or longer durations of exposure across postnatal developmental stages, unlike PAE. Together, these mechanisms for PTE that are unique from those of PAE may explain the more widespread effects on brain development at ages 6 (Uban et al., 2023) and 8–11 years as seen here and in Marshall et al. (2022). Interestingly, PTE dose-response relationships did not show significant results after corrections for multiple comparisons (Marshall et al., 2022), suggesting that PTE exposure from the postnatal period, or relating to perinatal tobacco exposure from others around the pregnant person or baby may be driving PTE outcomes more than maternal use in pregnancy alone. Data on the existence of postnatal tobacco exposure was not collected in our sample, which limited our ability to disentangle prenatal from postnatal tobacco exposure. Future research may investigate whether there is a dose-response relationship between prenatal and postnatal tobacco exposure and structural brain development.
While we are aware of the teratogenic effects of exposure to substances in utero, there exist other mechanistic pathways by way of hypoxia that might affect brain development, including the presence of obstetric complications. Obstetric complications, including preeclampsia, eclampsia, and gestational diabetes, can affect brain volumes (Rätsep et al., 2016; Luo et al., 2022). Our analyses are potentially limited by not accounting for obstetric complications that may affect brain morphology in utero with continuing effects seen in the growth trajectory of the developing brain through adolescence. Additionally, PTE has been shown to be associated with lower birth weight, smaller head circumference, and shorter length in newborns (Cornelius and Day, 2000). Specifically in our sample, birth weight did not differ by PTE or PAE status, again suggesting that prospective data most likely reflects community-level patterns of exposure and not necessarily high doses that are commonly seen in clinical FASD research samples.
Our analysis showed that phone access (landline and/or mobile phone) and running water access in utero were associated with volumes of the frontal regions in our sample of children between 8 to 11 years old in Cape Town, South Africa. Most of the existing literature on the impact of socioeconomic resources on child brain development have included samples from United States. How socioeconomic resources influence child development may be substantially different in a community where access to basic needs is inconsistent. Phone access is not universal in Cape Town, South Africa, because the necessary hardware to support phone service is expensive due to importation and little domestic manufacturing, and cellular data prices are exorbitantly high for lower resourced communities (Walton and Donner, 2012). Running water access for the Cape Town participants in this study is also negatively impacted by the legacy of racial inequality, where restricted access to clean and consistent water supply remains common (Enqvist and Ziervogel, 2019). Therefore, phone and water are likely proxies of the physical environment, such as access to governmental supports for maintaining utilities or exposure to environmental toxins or nutrition. Together, these physical environmental factors may impact child brain development and are potentially associated with housing amenity-based factors impacting ventilation of cooking, sanitation, and neighborhood safety. Given that our sample was derived from a low-resource community, it is also possible that the associations between utility access and structural brain volume may not generalize to communities in developed countries with more resources and infrastructure. Additionally, access to socioeconomic resources has been intertwined with cross-generational race/ethnicity-based oppression. Thus, the brain alterations we found as a function of socioeconomic resources in this sample may also reflect the impact of experienced racism, in addition to environmental exposures and poverty. Further research is needed to assess the interaction between racism, environmental exposure and poverty, and their collective impact on brain development.
Socioeconomic resources, but not ACEs, were related to lower cortical volumes, and less socioeconomic resources were associated increased likelihood of PTE and PAE. The presence of PAE/PTE may be a symptom of existing socioeconomic inequities, which may continue to independently and/or interactively impact the postnatal experience of the child. It is possible that, in this sample of participants, PTE and PAE are symptoms of less access to resources. Substances, including tobacco and alcohol, are commonly used to cope with stressors, including those relating to additional economic and low resourced living conditions (O’Connor et al., 2011; Peer et al., 2014; Watt et al., 2014). PTE may be reflective of additional needs to cope with stressors in a lower socioeconomic resources context in our study. Indeed, tobacco use among women in low resourced communities around Cape Town has been associated with poverty and more psychosocial stress (Peer et al., 2014). Previously, more adverse life events and a perception of lack of control over one’s environment were found to be associated with an increased risk of tobacco use among this population (Peer et al., 2014). Thus, the present study provides evidence to extend established socioeconomic resources and PTE associations to the period of pregnancy, and subsequent ACEs endorsed by their children. With intentional incorporation of these factors in PAE brain research, more can be understood about the complex interplay between co-occurring contributing factors with PAE/PTE on brain structure development. It is possible that teratogenic potential of PAE/PTE may differ as a function of many factors, including co-occurring exposures, socioeconomic resources, and variable postnatal experiences.
We did not find an association between the total number of ACEs and brain volume in the prefrontal and striatum regions. While the 14-point ACE scoring system captures the grouped experience of the adverse events, the cumulative score is not specific to the three domains of neglect, abuse, and household challenges scored on the ACE scale. Moreover, the scoring of events such as this on a linear scale deprives us of the sensitivity to the chronicity and intensity of the events. The screening of ACEs may not fully capture the breadth of adverse events experienced by children living in post-apartheid South Africa and perhaps better serves as preliminary data on ACEs for the PASS birth cohort. There are cultural differences in how people experience, and express abuse, neglect, and household challenges compared to the U.S. population with whom these ACE items were first developed. We attempted to minimize the cultural effects through forward- and back-translation, but there may be persisted issues of cultural validity with the measure. In addition, children may not remember adverse events that happened when they were very young and therefore might not report these events accurately, if at all. Given that early childhood is an especially sensitive developmental period, the limitation of the child self-report may also have contributed to our lack of findings. ACEs requiring mandatory reporting were not assessed and may have artificially created a ceiling effect on total ACE scores. Lastly, resilience is known to be important as an interacting force to ACEs and warrants further investigation to understand how it relates to PAE and/or PTE, socioeconomic resources and ACEs for these brain outcomes.
Although not directly tested, known mechanisms implicate stress systems for underlying, in part, the impact of socioeconomic resources and ACEs on brain development. The toxic stress model hypothesizes that poverty and maltreatment influence levels of adversity, which contribute to toxic stress and allostatic load and thus affect brain and cognitive development (McEwen and McEwen, 2017). In this model, toxic stress activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and thereby alters brain structures involved in neuroendocrine functioning, such as the limbic system and the prefrontal cortex (McEwen and McEwen, 2017). Indeed, a substantial body of literature has demonstrated associations between child maltreatment and altered structural and functional connectivity of the fronto-limbic regions (Hanson et al., 2010; Teicher et al., 2012; McLaughlin et al., 2014, 2016; Herzberg and Gunnar, 2020). Similar HPA and brain alterations have been found to associate with PAE. Animal models of implicate HPA-dysregulation as a key mechanism of lasting harm of PAE on brain structural alterations in prefrontal and the limbic regions (Uban et al., 2010, 2013). Few studies to date have examined PAE, PTE, socioeconomic resources, and HPA-function and warrant future investigation.
Additional contextual characteristics of the present study should be noted. Firstly, in the US, the majority of participants in historical PAE brain literature have been recruited from clinical FASD populations and almost always raised as adoptees, outside of their racial/ethnic/culture of origin (Uban et al., 2020). Here, the birth cohort from South Africa was comprised of child and adolescent participants raised by the biological mother, effectively eliminating cultural mismatch or not being raised by the biological mother as drivers of brain alterations observed with PAE. Second, given the prospective nature, PAE and PTE patterns reflected community-level patterns of PAE. The majority of PAE-focused published work identified participants with established facial dysmorphology or severe patterns of PAE, commonly associated with diagnoses such as fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) or partial FAS (pFAS) (Coles et al., 2020). Community-patterns of PAE with consideration of PTE may better capture FASD-related diagnoses that have been historically underrepresented in FASD clinical brain research, such as alcohol related neurodevelopmental disorder (ARND).
The birth cohort data leveraged in this study is from a low-resource community in Cape Town, South Africa, which has experienced cross-generational stressors through displacement and race/ethnicity-based oppression through historical apartheid. Specific to the Cape Town Flats region where the study participants reside, the physical environment is limited by lasting infrastructure challenges, in part due to the legacy of Apartheid (Henri and Grunebaum, 2005; O’Connell, 2018). Black communities were displaced from the Cape Town city area and rendered to the peripheral where basic infrastructure is lacking even today (Henri and Grunebaum, 2005; O’Connell, 2018). Systemic race-based oppression spanning generations combined with lack of resources have often led to experiences of toxic stress and substance use (Watt et al., 2014). This community has historically been labeled as having high FASD prevalence in research (Croxford and Viljoen, 1999; May et al., 2000; Olivier et al., 2016). Our study is contextualized with this consideration of poverty and systemic race-based oppression. Future research may further examine specific pathways through which poverty and psychosocial stress during pregnancy, as well as PAE/PTE, become associated with ACEs endorsed by children, and whether interventions and community services may disrupt the intergenerational transmission of adversity in this population.
In summary, our findings support the hypothesis that contextual factors, such as access to socioeconomic resources, may impact brain development through multiple pathways, including a direct pathway through the availability of certain resources and an indirect pathway through increasing the risk of teratogenic exposure (e.g., tobacco). These socioeconomic resources are entangled with cross-generational race/ethnicity-based oppression and poverty stemming from the legacy of the Apartheid. Therefore, our findings may not necessarily reflect differences in brain development due solely to poverty. Future studies may conceptualize the teratogen exposure as one factor embedded within a web of contextual factors that also influence brain development. Intentional incorporation of contextual factors that can also drive differences in brain development are needed to expand future teratogenic research, and to help destigmatize birth parents. Understanding varying patterns of PAE and PTE in the context of broader socioeconomic resources influences and their connections with postnatal ACEs can present novel policy-level and community-level interventions. This broader understanding of PAE and PTE outcomes may lead to support and awareness for affected individuals that is consistent with current recommendations to address social determinants of substance use.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of the University of Cape Town (HREC UCT REF 248/2014), the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of Stellenbosch University (REF 248/2014), the Institutional Review Board at Children’s Hospital in Los Angeles (CHLA-19-00228), and the Institutional Review Board at the University of California, Irvine (UCI #212354). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
Author contributions
SB, DJ, DS, KN, SJ, HO, and KU acquired the funding. YX, VR, SB, DJ, BS, KD, WC, JI, EK, KN, and KU contributed to the data acquisition and processing. YX, VR, HA, and KU contributed to the statistical analysis. YX wrote the manuscript. VR, SB, DJ, BS, KD, DS, HA, and KU contributed to the manuscript. ERS contributed to the funding and manuscript. All authors contributed to the conceptualization of the manuscript and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This project was supported by funding from NIAAA (K01 AA026889 to KU and R01 AA025653 to ERS and DS). This publication was made possible in part by a grant from Carnegie Corporation of New York, supporting the author DJ.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for ERS’s contribution to the overall PASS brain imaging study design, securing funding, and the development of this manuscript.
Conflict of interest
HA was employed by University Statistical Consulting, LLC.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Author disclaimer
The statements made and views expressed are solely the responsibility of the authors.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnint.2023.1104788/full#supplementary-material
References
Andre, Q. R., Mcmorris, C. A., Kar, P., Ritter, C., Gibbard, W. B., Tortorelli, C., et al. (2020). Different brain profiles in children with prenatal alcohol exposure with or without early adverse exposures. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 4375–4385. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25130
Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A., Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., and Jernigan, T. L. (2001). Brain dysmorphology in individuals with severe prenatal alcohol exposure. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 148–54.
Baglivio, M. T., Wolff, K. T., Epps, N., and Nelson, R. (2017). Predicting adverse childhood experiences. Crime Delinquency 63, 166–188. doi: 10.3390/children8090761
Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48.
Bick, J., and Nelson, C. A. (2016). Early adverse experiences and the developing brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 177–196. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.252
Bingol, N., Schuster, C., Fuchs, M., Iosub, S., Turner, G., Stone, R. K., et al. (1987). The influence of socioeconomic factors on the occurrence of fetal alcohol syndrome. Adv. Alcohol Substance Abuse 6, 105–118. doi: 10.1300/J251v06n04_08
Breen, M. S., Wingo, A. P., Koen, N., Donald, K. A., Nicol, M., Zar, H. J., et al. (2018). Gene expression in cord blood links genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders with maternal psychological distress and adverse childhood outcomes. Brain Behav. Immun. 73, 320–330. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2018.05.016
Buffarini, R., Hammerton, G., Coll, C. V. N., Cruz, S., Da Silveira, M. F., and Murray, J. (2022). Maternal adverse childhood experiences (ACES) and their associations with intimate partner violence and child maltreatment: Results from a Brazilian birth cohort. Prev. Med. 155:106928. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106928
Chen, R., Clifford, A., Lang, L., and Anstey, K. J. (2013). Is exposure to secondhand smoke associated with cognitive parameters of children and adolescents?-a systematic literature review. Ann. Epidemiol. 23, 652–661. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2013.07.001
Cheng, T. W., Magis-Weinberg, L., Guazzelli Williamson, V., Ladouceur, C. D., Whittle, S. L., Herting, M. M., et al. (2021). A researcher’s guide to the measurement and modeling of puberty in the ABCD study§at baseline. Front. Endocrinol. 12:608575. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.608575
Coleman-Cowger, V. H., Schauer, G. L., and Peters, E. N. (2017). Marijuana and tobacco co-use among a nationally representative sample of US pregnant and non-pregnant women: 2005–2014 National survey on drug use and health findings. Drug Alcohol Depend. 177, 130–135. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.03.025
Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., Granovska, I. V., Pashtepa, A. O., Plotka, L. D., Dolhov, V. B., et al. (2019). Gestational age and socioeconomic status as mediators for the impact of prenatal alcohol exposure on development at 6?months. Birth Defects Res. 111, 789–796. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1408
Coles, C. D., Kalberg, W., Kable, J. A., Tabachnick, B., May, P. A., and Chambers, C. D. (2020). Characterizing alcohol-related neurodevelopmental disorder: Prenatal alcohol exposure and the spectrum of outcomes. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 44, 1245–1260. doi: 10.1111/acer.14325
Cornelius, M. D., and Day, N. L. (2000). The effects of tobacco use during and after pregnancy on exposed children. Alcohol Res. Health 24, 242–249.
Cornelius, M. D., and Day, N. L. (2009). Developmental consequences of prenatal tobacco exposure. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 22, 121–125. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328326f6dc
Croxford, J., and Viljoen, D. (1999). Alcohol consumption by pregnant women in the Western Cape. S. Afr. Med. J. 89, 962–5.
Crume, T. (2019). Tobacco use during pregnancy. Clin. Obstetr. Gynecol. 62, 128–141. doi: 10.1016/j.placenta.2022.09.012
Culhane, J. F., and Elo, I. T. (2005). Neighborhood context and reproductive health. Am. J. Obstetr. Gynecol. 192, S22–S29. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2005.01.071
Dannlowski, U., Stuhrmann, A., Beutelmann, V., Zwanzger, P., Lenzen, T., Grotegerd, D., et al. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 286–293. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021
Dukes, K. A., Burd, L., Elliott, A. J., Fifer, W. P., Folkerth, R. D., Hankins, G. D. V., et al. (2014). The safe passage study: Design, methods, recruitment, and follow-up approach. Paediatr. Perinatal Epidemiol. 28, 455–465. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12136
El Marroun, H., Schmidt, M. N., Franken, I. H. A., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Hofman, A., Van Der Lugt, A., et al. (2014). Prenatal tobacco exposure and brain morphology: A prospective study in young children. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 792–800. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.273
Enqvist, J. P., and Ziervogel, G. (2019). Water governance and justice in Cape Town: An overview. Wires Water 6:e1354.
Finch, B. K., Vega, W. A., and Kolody, B. (2001). Substance use during pregnancy in the state of California. USA. Soc. Sci. Med. 52, 571–583. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00161-1
Finkelhor, D., Shattuck, A., Turner, H., and Hamby, S. (2013). Improving the adverse childhood experiences study scale. JAMA Pediatr. 167, 70–5. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.420
Fischl, B. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation. Neuron 33, 341–355.
Floresco, S. B. (2015). The nucleus accumbens: An interface between cognition, emotion, and action. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 25–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115159
Fried, P. A., Watkinson, B., and Gray, R. (1998). Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 20, 293–306. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(97)00091-3
Gibson, C. L., Morris, S. Z., and Beaver, K. M. (2009). Secondary exposure to violence during childhood and adolescence: Does neighborhood context matter? Justice Q. 26, 30–57. doi: 10.1177/2378023121992941
Glass, L., Ware, A. L., and Mattson, S. N. (2014). Neurobehavioral, neurologic, and neuroimaging characteristics of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 125, 435–462. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62619-6.00025-2
Gonzalez, M. R., Palmer, C. E., Uban, K. A., Jernigan, T. L., Thompson, W. K., and Sowell, E. R. (2020). Positive economic, psychosocial, and physiological ecologies predict brain structure and cognitive performance in 9–10-year-old children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:578822. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.578822
Gonzalez, M. R., Uban, K. A., Tapert, S. F., and Sowell, E. R. (2023). Prenatal tobacco exposure associations with physical health and neurodevelopment in the ABCD cohort. Health Psychol. doi: 10.1037/hea0001265 [Epub ahead of print].
Greenmyer, J. R., Klug, M. G., Kambeitz, C., Popova, S., and Burd, L. (2018). A multicountry updated assessment of the economic impact of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: Costs for children and adults. J. Addict. Med. 12, 466–473. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0000000000000438
Hanson, J. L., Chung, M. K., Avants, B. B., Shirtcliff, E. A., Gee, J. C., Davidson, R. J., et al. (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: A tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. J. Neurosci. 30, 7466–7472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0859-10.2010
Heinzen, E., Sinnwell, J., Atkinson, E., Gunderson, T., and Dougherty, G. (2021). arsenal: An arsenal of ‘R’ functions for large-scale statistical summaries.
Henri, Y., and Grunebaum, H. (2005). Re-historicising trauma: Reflections on violence and memory in current-day Cape Town. Cape Town: Direct Action Centre for Peace and Memory.
Herzberg, M. P., and Gunnar, M. R. (2020). Early life stress and brain function: Activity and connectivity associated with processing emotion and reward. NeuroImage 209:116493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116493
Jones, K. L., and Smith, D. W. (1973). Recognition of the fetal alcohol syndrome in early infancy. Lancet 302, 999–1001. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(73)91092-1
Kambeitz, C., Klug, M. G., Greenmyer, J., Popova, S., and Burd, L. (2019). Association of adverse childhood experiences and neurodevelopmental disorders in people with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) and non-FASD controls. BMC Pediatr. 19:498. doi: 10.1186/s12887-019-1878-8
Karriker-Jaffe, K. J. (2013). Neighborhood socioeconomic status and substance use by U.S. adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133, 212–221. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.033
Kerker, B. D., Zhang, J., Nadeem, E., Stein, R. E. K., Hurlburt, M. S., Heneghan, A., et al. (2015). Adverse childhood experiences and mental health, chronic medical conditions, and development in young children. Acad. Pediatr. 15, 510–517. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2015.05.005
Kringelbach, M. L., and Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progr. Neurobiol. 72, 341–372. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmertest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26.
Lange, S., Shield, K., Rehm, J., and Popova, S. (2013). Prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in child care settings: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics 132, e980–e995. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0066
Leech, S. L., Richardson, G. A., Goldschmidt, L., and Day, N. L. (1999). Prenatal substance exposure: Effects on attention and impulsivity of 6-year-olds. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 21, 109–118. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(98)00042-7
Luby, J. L., Tillman, R., and Barch, D. M. (2019). Association of timing of adverse childhood experiences and caregiver support with regionally specific brain development in adolescents. JAMA Netw. Open 2:e1911426. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.11426
Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harms, M. P., Babb, C., et al. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 167, 1135–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139
Luo, S., Hsu, E., Lawrence, K. E., Adise, S., Herting, M. M., Buchanan, T., et al. (2022). Associations between prenatal exposure to maternal diabetes and child adiposity markers: Mediating effects of brain structure. medRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2022.02.03.22270398
Mall, S., Platt, J. M., Temmingh, H., Musenge, E., Campbell, M., Susser, E., et al. (2020). The relationship between childhood trauma and schizophrenia in the Genomics of Schizophrenia in the Xhosa people (Sax) study in South Africa. Psychol. Med. 50, 1570–1577. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001703
Marshall, A. T., Bodison, S. C., Uban, K. A., Adise, S., Jonker, D., Charles, W., et al. (2022). The impact of prenatal alcohol and/or tobacco exposure on brain structure in a large sample of children from a South African birth cohort. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 46, 1980–1992. doi: 10.1111/acer.14945
Mattson, S. N., Bernes, G. A., and Doyle, L. R. (2019). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A review of the neurobehavioral deficits associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 43, 1046–1062.
Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. L., Sobel, D. F., and Jones, K. L. (1996). A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 20, 1088–1093.
May, P. A., Blankenship, J., Marais, A.-S., Gossage, J. P., Kalberg, W. O., Barnard, R., et al. (2013). Approaching the prevalence of the full spectrum of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in a South African population-based study. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 818–830. doi: 10.1111/acer.12033
May, P. A., Brooke, L., Gossage, J. P., Croxford, J., Adnams, C., Jones, K. L., et al. (2000). Epidemiology of fetal alcohol syndrome in a South African community in the Western Cape Province. Am. J. Public Health 90, 1905–1912. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.12.1905
May, P. A., Chambers, C. D., Kalberg, W. O., Zellner, J., Feldman, H., Buckley, D., et al. (2018). Prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in 4 US communities. JAMA 319, 474–482.
McEwen, C. A., and McEwen, B. S. (2017). Social structure, adversity, toxic stress, and intergenerational poverty: An early childhood model. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 43, 445–472.
McLachlan, K., Zhou, D., Little, G., Rasmussen, C., Pei, J., Andrew, G., et al. (2020). Current socioeconomic status correlates with brain volumes in healthy children and adolescents but not in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:223. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00223
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., and Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 578–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Gold, A. L., Duys, A., Lambert, H. K., Peverill, M., et al. (2016). Maltreatment exposure, brain structure, and fear conditioning in children and adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1956–1964.
Melchior, M., Moffitt, T. E., Milne, B. J., Poulton, R., and Caspi, A. (2007). Why do children from socioeconomically disadvantaged families suffer from poor health when they reach adulthood? A life-course study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 166, 966–974. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm155
Myer, L., Stein, D. J., Grimsrud, A., Seedat, S., and Williams, D. R. (2008). Social determinants of psychological distress in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 66, 1828–1840. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.025
Noble, K. G., and Farah, M. J. (2013). Neurocognitive consequences of socioeconomic disparities: The intersection of cognitive neuroscience and public health. Dev. Sci. 16, 639–640. doi: 10.1111/desc.12076
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., and Sowell, E. R. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 15, 516–527.
Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., and Mccandliss, B. D. (2006). Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Dev. Sci. 9, 642–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00542.x
O’Connor, M. J., Frankel, F., Paley, B., Schonfeld, A. M., Carpenter, E., Laugeson, E. A., et al. (2006). A controlled social skills training for children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Consult Clin. Psychol. 74, 639–648.
O’Connor, M. J., Tomlinson, M., Leroux, I. M., Stewart, J., Greco, E., and Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (2011). Predictors of alcohol use prior to pregnancy recognition among township women in Cape Town, South Africa. Soc. Sci. Med. 72, 83–90.
Olivier, L., Viljoen, D., and Curfs, L. (2016). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: Prevalence rates in South Africa: The new millennium. S. Afr. Med. J. 106, 103–106.
Paavola, M., Vartiainen, E., and Puska, P. (1996). Predicting adult smoking: The influence of smoking during adolescence and smoking among friends and family. Health Educ. Res. 11, 309–315.
Pear, V. A., Petito, L. C., and Abrams, B. (2017). The role of maternal adverse childhood experiences and race in intergenerational high-risk smoking behaviors. Nicotine Tob. Res. 19, 623–630. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntw295
Peer, N., Lombard, C., Steyn, K., and Levitt, N. (2014). Differential patterns of tobacco use among black men and women in Cape Town: The cardiovascular risk in black South Africans study. Nicotine Tob. Res. 16, 1104–1111. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu042
R Development Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Rätsep, M. T., Paolozza, A., Hickman, A. F., Maser, B., Kay, V. R., Mohammad, S., et al. (2016). Brain structural and vascular anatomy is altered in offspring of pre-eclamptic pregnancies: A pilot study. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 37, 939–945. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A4640
Reuter, M., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2010). Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: A robust approach. NeuroImage 53, 1181–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020
Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. L., Sobel, D. F., and Jones, K. L. (1995). Abnormalities of the corpus callosum in children prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 1198–1202.
Rivkin, M. J., Davis, P. E., Lemaster, J. L., Cabral, H. J., Warfield, S. K., Mulkern, R. V., et al. (2008). Volumetric MRI study of brain in children with intrauterine exposure to cocaine, alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana. Pediatrics 121, 741–750. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1399
Scherrer, J. F., Xian, H., Pan, H., Pergadia, M. L., Madden, P. A. F., Grant, J. D., et al. (2012). Parent, sibling and peer influences on smoking initiation, regular smoking and nicotine dependence. Results from a genetically informative design. Addict. Behav. 37, 240–247. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.10.005
Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage 22, 1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032
Sevenoaks, T., Fouche, J.-P., Phillips, N., Heany, S., Myer, L., Zar, H. J., et al. (2022). Childhood trauma and mental health in the Cape Town adolescent antiretroviral cohort. J. Child Adolesc. Trauma 15, 353–363. doi: 10.1007/s40653-021-00362-0
Shoff, C., and Yang, T.-C. (2013). Understanding maternal smoking during pregnancy: Does residential context matter? Soc. Sci. Med. 78, 50–60.
Sled, J. G., Zijdenbos, A. P., and Evans, A. C. (1998). A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 17, 87–97.
Sowell, E. R., Mattson, S. N., Kan, E., Thompson, P. M., Riley, E. P., and Toga, A. W. (2008). Abnormal cortical thickness and brain-behavior correlation patterns in individuals with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Cereb. Cortex 18, 136–144. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm039
Sowell, E. R., Mattson, S. N., Thompson, P. M., Jernigan, T. L., Riley, E. P., and Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive correlates: Effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurology 57, 235–244. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.2.235
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Mattson, S. N., Tessner, K. D., Jernigan, T. L., Riley, E. P., et al. (2002). Regional brain shape abnormalities persist into adolescence after heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Cereb. Cortex 12, 856–865. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.856
Teicher, M. H., Anderson, C. M., and Polcari, A. (2012). Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, E563–E572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115396109
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., and Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 652–666.
Uban, K. A., Bodnar, T., Butts, K., Sliwowska, J. H., Comeau, W., and Weinberg, J. (2010). “Direct and indirect mechanisms of alcohol teratogenesis: Implications for understanding alterations in brain and behavior in FASD,” in Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. management and policy perspectives of FASD, eds E. P. Riley, S. Clarren, J. Weinberg, and E. Jonsson (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell), 71–108.
Uban, K. A., Comeau, W. L., Ellis, L. A., Galea, L. A. M., and Weinberg, J. (2013). Basal regulation of Hpa and dopamine systems is altered differentially in males and females by prenatal alcohol exposure and chronic variable stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1953–1966. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.017
Uban, K. A., Jonker, D., Donald, K. A., Bodison, S. C., Brooks, S. J., Kan, E., et al. (2023). Associations between community-level patterns of prenatal alcohol and tobacco exposure on brain structure in a non-clinical sample of 6-year-old children: A South African pilot study. Acta Neuropsychiatr. doi: 10.1017/neu.2022.34 [Epub ahead of print].
Uban, K. A., Kan, E., Wozniak, J. R., Mattson, S. N., Coles, C. D., and Sowell, E. R. (2020). The relationship between socioeconomic status and brain volume in children and adolescents with prenatal alcohol exposure. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:85. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00085
Walton, M., and Donner, J. (2012). Public access, private mobile: The interplay of shared access and the mobile internet for teenagers in Cape Town. Global impact study research report series. Cape Town: University of Cape Town.
Watt, M. H., Eaton, L. A., Choi, K. W., Velloza, J., Kalichman, S. C., Skinner, D., et al. (2014). “It’s better for me to drink, at least the stress is going away”: Perspectives on alcohol use during pregnancy among South African women attending drinking establishments. Soc. Sci. Med. 116, 119–125. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.06.048
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., Mcgowan, L. D. A., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4:1686.
Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., and Müller, K. (2022). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. R package version 1.0.8.
Keywords: socioeconomic resources, prenatal substance exposure, neurodevelopment, adverse childhood experiences, prenatal alcohol exposure, prenatal tobacco exposure
Citation: Xia Y, Rebello V, Bodison SC, Jonker D, Steigelmann B, Donald KA, Charles W, Stein DJ, Ipser J, Ahmadi H, Kan E, Sowell ER, Narr KL, Joshi SH, Odendaal HJ and Uban KA (2023) Contextualizing the impact of prenatal alcohol and tobacco exposure on neurodevelopment in a South African birth cohort: an analysis from the socioecological perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 17:1104788. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1104788
Received: 22 November 2022; Accepted: 19 June 2023;
Published: 18 July 2023.
Edited by:
Elizabeth B. Torres, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, United StatesReviewed by:
Yuliya S. Nikolova, University of Toronto, CanadaAdriana Laura Burgueño, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Argentina
Copyright © 2023 Xia, Rebello, Bodison, Jonker, Steigelmann, Donald, Charles, Stein, Ipser, Ahmadi, Kan, Sowell, Narr, Joshi, Odendaal and Uban. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yingjing Xia, yingjinx@uci.edu; Kristina A. Uban, kuban@hs.uci.edu