Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 14 September 2021
This article is part of the Research Topic Role of the Vestibular System in the Perception of Time and Space View all 12 articles

Anxiety and Motion Sickness Susceptibility May Influence the Ability to Update Orientation in the Horizontal Plane of Healthy Subjects

  • Unidad de Investigación Médica en Otoneurología, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Ciudad de México, Mexico

Few studies have evaluated the influence of idiosyncrasies that may influence the judgment of space-time orientation after passive motion. We designed a study to assess the influence of anxiety/depression (which may distort time perception), motion sickness susceptibility (which has been related to vestibular function, disorientation, and to the velocity storage mechanism), and personal habits on the ability to update orientation, after passive rotations in the horizontal plane. Eighty-one healthy adults (22–64 years old) accepted to participate. After they completed an in-house general health/habits questionnaire, the short Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, and the short International Physical Activity Questionnaire, they were exposed to 10 manually driven whole-body rotations (45°, 90°, or 135°), in a square room, with distinctive features on the walls, while seated in the normal upright position, unrestrained, with noise-attenuating headphones and blindfolded. After each rotation, they were asked to report which wall or corner they were facing. To calculate the error of estimation of orientation, the perceived rotation was subtracted from the actual rotation. Multivariate analysis showed that the estimation error of the first rotation was strongly related to the results of the orientation test. The magnitude and the frequency of estimation errors of orientation were independently related to HADS anxiety sub-score and to adult motion sickness susceptibility, with no influence of age, but a contribution from the interaction of the use of spectacles, the quality of sleep and sex. The results suggest that idiosyncrasies may contribute to the space-time estimation of passive self-motion, with influence from emotional traits, adult motion sickness susceptibility, experience, and possibly sleep quality.

Introduction

Behavior in the environment requires a dynamic updating of the perceptions of the body and the surroundings of the body. The spatial updating of self-to-object directions and distances (egocentric relations) that takes place concurrently with the change of spatial relations independent of the position of the perceiver (allocentric relations) depends upon the availability of multisensory information (Klatzky, 1998). Evidence suggests that humans update egocentric, internalized versions of the surroundings to orient themselves as they move (Wang and Spelke, 2000). Though, from an ecological approach to perception and action (Gibson, 1966), perception may not be based on patterns of stimulation available to individual perceptual systems, but may take advantage of “higher order relations” between them (Stoffregen and Riccio, 1988).

During active movements, comparison between the internal prediction of the proprioceptive consequences of self-motion to the actual resultant feedback, input carried by vestibular afferents may be canceled in conditions where there is a match between predicted and actual proprioceptive feedback (Cullen, 2011). Neurons in the vestibular nuclei differentially encode active versus passive head motion; during active movements, distinct classes of neurons allow for reduction of vestibular signals in order to redirect gaze or to stabilize head in space (for review Cullen and Roy, 2004). However, it is unknown yet how this differential processing of head velocity at the vestibular nucleus contributes to other vestibular functions.

In the absence of vision, vestibular signals may update self-orientation in the environment (Crum-Brown, 1875; Mach, 1875). Throughout passive motion, perception of displacement is derived from the same signal that determines perception of velocity, by integration over time (Mergner et al., 1996); while space-time relativity seems to be independent of whether velocity, distance derived by path integration, or both variables are stored in spatial working memory (Glasauer et al., 2007). Besides, vestibular perception of passive rotation in the horizontal plane can be independent of whether subjects are standing or sitting during rotation (Becker et al., 2000); while moving on to active behavior, displacement perception may be modified by proprioceptive and efferent signals, as well as the vestibular afferents at the pace of stepping (Jürgens et al., 1999).

After whole body passive rotations around an earth-vertical axis, without visual cues, subjects can indicate their orientation in space with respect to their initial orientation, while they update their actual orientation with respect to the surroundings (Israël et al., 1996; Wang and Spelke, 2000; Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008). Using simultaneous measurement of oculo-motor and perceptual measures of the vestibular time constant has shown that the perception of angular velocity is based on signals subserved by the velocity storage mechanism (Okada et al., 1999). This mechanism lengthens the time constant of the oculo-motor response to constant head rotation when no vision is available (Raphan et al., 1979), in such a way that the vestibulo-ocular response and the perception of self-motion outlast the duration of the response from the semicircular canals (Grunfeld et al., 2000; Bertolini et al., 2012).

The velocity storage mechanism has also been related to motion sickness susceptibility (for review Cohen et al., 2019). However, there are many theories of motion sickness, and some reject explanations in terms of velocity storage (Stoffregen and Riccio, 1988).

In addition, during passive rotation in the dark, updating orientation is dependent on vestibular inputs and processing in the central nervous system, with no opportunity to anticipate motion (for review see Behrendt, 2013; Eichenbaum, 2017). Then, both unpredictability and uncertainty may enable emotional responses during updating orientation in the dark (for review see Lake and LaBar, 2011);

The accuracy to judge angular displacements is highly variable among healthy subjects (Guedry et al., 1971), and overestimation of rotation is more frequent than underestimation (Israël et al., 1995; Marlinsky, 1999; Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008; Anson et al., 2021). It may decrease in old age (Anson et al., 2021), but other individual factors are poorly documented. We designed a study to assess the influence of individual factors on the vestibular contribution to update orientation, after passive rotations in the horizontal plane in young and middle-aged adults.

We selected the following factors that may influence time perception or space-time perception, as well as personal habits that could influence performance: common mental symptoms (anxiety/depression), which have been related to distortions on the awareness of time (Droit-Volet, 2013); susceptibility to motion sickness (in cars, boats, planes, trains, funfair rides), which is related to vestibular function (for review Money, 1970), to disorientation (for review Yardley, 1991), and which could be related to the velocity storage mechanism (for review Cohen et al., 2019); as well as physical activity (Rogge et al., 2021), quality of sleep (Martínez-Gallardo et al., 2020), the use of spectacles (Demer and Crane, 1998), and alcohol/tobacco use (Hafström et al., 2007).

Materials and Methods

Participants

After approval by the Research and Ethics Committees, 81 subjects (mean age 40.0 years ± standard deviation 11.0 years) accepted to participate. All the participants denied having a history of dizziness, vertigo, unsteadiness, migraine, hearing loss, and neurological or psychiatric disorders (submission to psychiatric care of psychopharmacological treatment); none of them had evidence of vestibular dysfunction assessed by neuro-otology evaluation with caloric/rotational tests, and all of them were naive to the study protocol and to the orientation test.

Once they have completed questionnaires to assess the individual factors, they performed an updating orientation test.

Questionnaires

An in-house questionnaire of general health and personal habits.

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983), which is a self−report screening scale that contains 14−items with a Likert scale, seven for anxiety and seven for depression. It has been used to identify possible and probable cases of anxiety disorders in samples from the general population, general practice and psychiatric patients (Bjelland et al., 2002). It is scored by summing the ratings for all the items to yield a total score, and by summing the ratings for the seven items of each subscale to yield two separate sub-scores, which range from 0 to 21. A cut-off score of ≥8 for both subscales gives sensitivities and specificities in the range of 0.70–0.90 (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983; Bjelland et al., 2002), and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient varying from 0.67 to 0.93 (Bjelland et al., 2002).

The short from of the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire (Golding, 2006b), which is a self−report screening scale that contains 18−items, divided into two parts: part A assessing motion sickness during childhood and part B assessing motion sickness during adulthood. It predicts individual differences in motion sickness caused by a variety of stimuli (e.g., cars, boats, planes, trains, funfair rides). Each score ranges from 0 (no susceptibility) to 27 (maximal level of susceptibility) and gives a total score from 0 to 54, with a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.87 (Golding, 2006b).

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (Buysse et al., 1989), which is a self-rated questionnaire to assess sleep quality and sleep disturbances. Nineteen items generate seven scores on: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medication, and daytime dysfunction. The combination of these sub-scores also generates three separate factors to assess (Jia et al., 2019): sleep efficiency; sleep latency and sleep quality. A total score >5 can be considered as bad quality of sleep (Buysse et al., 1989), with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient varying from 0.70 to 0.83 (Mollayeva et al., 2016).

The short form of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) (Craig et al., 2003), which is a self-report instrument to assess the frequency and duration of vigorous, moderate and walking activities, as well as the average sitting time on a weekday during the last 7 days. Although the overall scale tends to overestimate the amount of physical activity, it has shown acceptable correlations with objective measures of activity to assess walking (Lee et al., 2011).

Updating Orientation Test

To assess self-orientation relative to the distinctive features of an unfamiliar room, participants were seated in the normal upright position, unrestrained on a hydraulic barber’s chair (with a gyroscope on the headrest), in the center of a squared room (2 × 2 m), which contained fixed features positioned in the middle of each wall, in such a way that the features and corners subtended 45° with respect to the subject. They were asked to remember the location of each of the features while they were rotated in the light, and faced each of the walls. Then, blindfolded and wearing noise-attenuating headphones, they were exposed to two sets of five manually driven whole body rotations of 45° (2 s) or 90° (3 s) or 135° (4 s), to the right or to the left, in an unpredictable sequence, balanced for amplitude, direction, and order, with 10 s in-between (to allow post-rotatory sensations to fade). After the first set of five rotations and return to the start position, the eye mask was removed and the subject had a short rest of 1–2 min before commencing the second set of rotations. The actual rotation sequence included five rotations to the right and five rotations to the left (four rotations of 45°, five rotations of 90°, and 1 rotation of 135°); the sequence of small and large rotations and vice versa were similar to the right than to the left; whenever the first rotation of the first set was to the right, then the first rotation of the second set (after the rest) was to the left, and vice versa. Participants were instructed to report which wall or corner they were facing at the moment, just after each rotation (Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008).

The estimation error of each rotation was computed by subtracting the reported rotation from the actual rotation. Average estimation errors were calculated for all rotations and for each rotation size (error magnitude), and the number of correct estimations (error frequency) was also considered. All participants were tested during the morning and early afternoon.

Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using STATISTICA software (Tulsa, OK., StatSoft Inc.). According to data distribution (Kolmogorov Smirnov test), the results are described using median and quartiles 1 and 3 (Q1–Q3). The exploratory bivariate analysis was performed using Pearson correlation coefficient to assess linear correlations, and Mann Whitney U test to compare subgroups by age (≤ or >40 years old), by sex, and by HADS anxiety sub-score ≥8. To further assess linear correlations, analysis of covariance was performed. Considering the data distribution and plausible not linear effects, to assess the contribution of each independent variable to predict the estimation errors and correct estimations, after controlling for all other independent variables, a multivariable analysis was performed using a Generalized Linear Model with Wald test (Lindsey, 1997). The significance level was set at 2-tailed 0.05.

Results

General Description

The characteristics of the participants and the scores on the Motion Sickness Questionnaire and the HADS are described in Table 1. The majority of participants was right-handed and had a university degree; circa half of them were health workers and a third of them were administrative workers. The report of physical activity was varied: 27.1% (n = 22) reported low physical activity, 29.6% (n = 24) reported moderate physical activity and 43.2% (n = 35) reported high physical activity. The use of alcohol was frequent but moderate (53%, n = 43) as well as the report of bad quality of sleep (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index >5) (51%, n = 42).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. General characteristics and questionnaire scores of the 81 adults participating in the study.

The results of the orientation test are shown in Table 1. Inaccurate estimation of orientation was observed after 34.3% of all rotations, with a typical error of 45°; overestimation of rotation was more frequent than underestimation of rotation (85.3% versus 14.7%). The median estimation error for all rotations was 18° (Q1–Q3 = 9°–22.5°); it was the same for the two sets of rotations (18°, 9°–27°), and it was consistent with the median estimation error for the 90° rotations (18°, 9°–27°). The median of the total correct estimations was 7 (5–8).

Exploratory Bivariate Analysis

Comparisons by age are shown in Table 2. Compared to young adults (21–40 years old), middle-aged participants (41–64 years old) showed the lowest scores on the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire total score and sub-scores (p ≤ 0.008), and the highest scores on the Pittsburgh Quality of Sleep Index total score and sub-scores (p ≤ 0.03), with no differences between subgroups on the average estimation error or the correct estimations.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Median, Quartile 1 (Q1) and Quartile 3 (Q3) of the Pittsburgh sleep quality index, International Physical Activity Questionnaire, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire, and updating orientation results by age (young adults ≤40 years versus middle age >40 years).

Comparisons by sex are shown in Table 3. Although men spent more energy on physical activity (met-minutes per week) than women (p = 0.021), there were no other differences by sex.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Median, Quartile 1 (Q1) and Quartile 3 (Q3) of the Pittsburgh sleep quality index, International Physical Activity Questionnaire, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire, and updating orientation results by sex.

Comparison by HADS anxiety sub-score showed that participants with a sub-score ≥8 reported fewer accurate estimations (Z = -2.013, p = 0.044) and larger overestimation of rotation (Z = 1.985, p = 0.047) than those with a sub-score <8.

Weak linear correlations were observed between the results of the orientation test and the HADS score and sub-scores (anxiety sub-score and depression sub-score) (Pearson’s r < 0.3, p < 0.05), as well as the motion sickness total score and adult sub-score (Pearson’s r < 0.3, p < 0.05) (Appendix 1). The strongest linear correlations were observed between the estimation error of the first rotation and the average estimation error, the total correct estimations and the estimation error for the 90° rotations (Pearson’s r from 0.48 to 0.67, p < 0.00001) (Appendix 1).

Further covariance analysis confirmed that inaccurate estimation of the first rotation was linearly related to increased average estimation error, independently from HADS anxiety sub-score ≥8 and from the motion sickness susceptibility adult sub-score (F = 15.630, p = 0.0001) (Figure 1); while no significant linear interaction between these two variables was observed (F = 0.672, p = 0.4).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean and standard error of the mean of the average estimation error during the updating orientation test, according to accuracy/inaccuracy of the estimation of the first rotation and evidence of Anxiety/No anxiety (HADS anxiety sub-score ≥8), of 81 healthy volunteers.

Multivariable Analysis

The Estimates with their Standard Error and the Wald statistic with p values for all the variables included in the model are shown in Table 4A. The anxiety sub-score of the HADS and the adult sub-score of the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire had independent contributions to both the average estimation error (p ≤ 0.009), and the total correct estimations (p ≤ 0.023), with a contribution of the use of spectacles on the total correct estimations (p ≤ 0.035), but no influence was observed from age, physical activity, smoking, or alcohol use. In addition, in men, the use of spectacles and bad quality of sleep was related to larger average estimation error (p = 0.010) and less total correct estimations (p = 0.0004).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Coefficient estimates and standard error (S.E.) of the estimates are described with Wald statistic and p values for the average estimation error and the total correct estimations without including the first rotation estimation error (A) and including it (B).

To confirm the independent contribution of the estimation error of the first rotation to the overall results of the orientation test, including plausible no linear effects, a second multivariable analysis was performed including this variable (despite collinearity). The Estimates with their Standard Error and the Wald statistic with p values for all the variables included in the model are shown in Table 4B. A highly significant relationship was observed between the estimation of the first rotation and the overall results of the test (p < 0.0001); participants who made an accurate estimation of the first rotation had lower average estimation error than those who made an inaccurate first estimation.

Discussion

In young and middle-aged healthy subjects, assessment of the influence of individual factors on updating orientation during passive rotations in the horizontal plane showed contributions from anxiety and adult motion sickness susceptibility (in cars, boats, planes, trains, funfair rides), as well as an interaction among the use of spectacles, the quality of sleep and sex, with no independent influence from age or sex. Estimation of the first rotation was related to the accuracy in updating orientation during the following rotations.

The finding of an association between HADS anxiety sub-score and the results of the orientation test is consistent with the relationship between emotion and vestibular function (Balaban and Thayer, 2001; Viaud-Delmon et al., 2011; Preuss et al., 2014; Coelho and Balaban, 2015). The spectrum of this relationship comprises from the dizziness related to psychiatric disorders (for review see Viaud-Delmon et al., 2011) to the neural network of vestibular inputs (Balaban and Thayer, 2001; Balaban, 2002). However, in this study we also considered the influence of affective states on the subjective estimation of time (Pariyadath and Eagleman, 2007; Tanaka and Yotsumoto, 2017). Since anxiety has been related to distortions of the awareness of time, including overestimation of short time intervals (for review see Droit-Volet, 2013).

In agreement with previous reports using the same or a similar orientation task (Israël et al., 1995; Marlinsky, 1999; Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008; Anson et al., 2021), in this study, over-estimation of rotation was more frequent than underestimation. The orientation test design included rotations of increasing amplitude/duration, this allowed overestimation of duration to be easily interpreted as increased amplitude, which could give rise to overestimation errors. Participants with a HADS anxiety sub-score ≥8 were prone to overestimate the rotations. We propose the hypothesis that distortion of the duration of rotation could have influenced the percept of displacement. However, variation of the velocity among rotations (11°/s) could have interfered with this result.

The association of motion sickness susceptibility (in cars, boats, planes, trains, funfair rides), with errors on updating orientation suggests that the unknown idiosyncrasy related to the variability of motion sickness susceptibility among subjects (Lentz and GuedryJr., 1978; Golding, 2006a) may also contribute to distortions on the space-time perception of passive rotations. We suggest that a link between these two variables could be the velocity storage mechanism and its processing, by means of the source signal to perceive both displacement and duration of rotation. The finding of lower motion sickness susceptibility scores in participants older than 40 years is consistent with the report of its decline with age, in both healthy subjects and vestibular patients (Paillard et al., 2013; Jones et al., 2019).

The independent contribution of motion sickness susceptibility and trait anxiety (by HADS anxiety sub-score) to the estimation error of orientation is in agreement with the report that the relationship between trait anxiety and motion sickness susceptibility can be weak in healthy subjects and not evident in patients with vestibular disease (Paillard et al., 2013).

The relationship between the estimation accuracy observed during the first rotation of the orientation test and the performance during the whole test could be explained by the test itself. Since participants were blindfolded, and they had no opportunity to contrast or reset their space-time perception of rotation with other sensory cues, they had to rely on their vestibular inputs and use the memory trace of their initial motion perception as they rotated to face each new direction. In addition, the strongest correlation with the estimation error of the 90° rotations could have been influenced by the reference axis. During passive rotations without vision, evidence suggests that the spatial framework used to organize the egocentric space orientation may include an orthogonal system related to the frame of the human body, with an ideal angle of 90° for subjective estimations of turns in the range of 15°–165° (Sadalla and Montello, 1989).

Several factors may have contributed to the finding that the use of spectacles and bad quality of sleep were related to larger average estimation error and less correct estimations, particularly in men. In healthy subjects, visual conditions can modify the vestibular responses (Gonshor and Jones, 1976). In laboratory situations, including passive head rotation, the wearing of telescopic spectacles can modify the gain of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (Demer and Crane, 1998). However, an objective evaluation of the corrected refraction errors would be required to adequately assess this factor. On the other hand, sleep disturbances have been related to several deficits including attention, memory, cognitive processes and emotional reactivity (for review see McCoy and Strecker, 2011); likewise, they can alter vestibular responses (Collins, 1988; Quarck et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2018) and they can affect the perception of motion during magnetic vestibular stimulation (Martínez-Gallardo et al., 2020). Evidence supports that, after sleep deprivation, the vestibulo-ocular reflex gain may increase in response to velocity steps (Quarck et al., 2006), or the responses may decrease according to the time of sleep deprivation (Collins, 1988). Moreover, in this study, since participants were blindfolded, we cannot disregard the effect of darkness on the alertness of subjects with bad quality of sleep (de Zeeuw et al., 2019). In addition, recent evidence suggests that sleep deprivation could have a stronger effect on balance (i.e., postural stability) in men, compared to women (Ołpińska-Lischka et al., 2021).

In agreement with previous reports (Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008; Anson et al., 2021), the analysis of this study showed no independent influence of sex on updating orientation after passive rotations in the horizontal plane. This finding was consistent with the observed lack of influence of physical activity on the performance of the orientation test, given that men reported more physical activity than women. Previous reports support that men may out-perform women on virtual navigation tasks (Astur et al., 1998) and way-finding outdoors (Silverman et al., 2000). However, men may prefer an allocentric strategy to orient them-selves (Dabbs et al., 1998), while women may prefer a landmark-based strategy (Choi et al., 2006). Albeit the orientation test of this study implied a combination of egocentric and allocentric strategies, an egocentric framework may have been used to update object position while rotating. This is consistent with the evidence supporting that the ability to accurately locate objects in small-scale environments appears to depend on the perception of the current egocentric distances and directions of objects, with a continuous update of these perceptions relative to the environment geometry through time, which can be used as a source for reorienting over motion (for review see Cheng et al., 2013), while path integration using the head as a reference is egocentric by nature. Moreover, in this study, the reported localization was not by chance; which supports that egocentric spatial perceptions persisted over orientation/disorientation.

We also observed no significant correlation between the age of the participants and the average estimation error or the total correct estimations. Young and middle-aged adults showed similar results on the orientation test, despite clear differences on the motion sickness susceptibility scores. These findings support that the decline in spatial orientation that has been observed in the elderly could be attributed to factors that may develop late in life (Anson et al., 2021), but may not be evident yet in young and middle-aged subjects (Jáuregui-Renaud et al., 2008).

An interaction among these variables is represented in Figure 2. The results suggest that updating orientation in the horizontal plane during passive rotations, without vision, would be the result of the space-time perception of the stimuli, in the context of individual expectations and idiosyncrasy. However, to estimate both displacement and duration of motion, idiosyncrasy may play a part in central integration of the sensory inputs; while anxiety and bad sleep may influence this processing, in the context of individual experiences and expectations.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Theoretical model of the interaction among individual factors during passive rotation without vision to update orientation in the horizontal plane.

The main limitation of this study was its cross-over design. Repeated measures would allow less individual cofactors increasing the variance, and better assessment of age effects on performance; though performing a known task would have an effect on the results. A second limitation of the study is the reliance en self-report instruments; also, the apparent lack of influence of alcohol/tobacco use that is not conclusive, due to the low frequency of smokers and the moderate alcohol consumption among participants. Another limitation was that the manually driven rotations of the orientation test introduced velocity variability among the three amplitudes tested (11°/s), precluding an accurate assessment according to the space-time characteristics of the stimuli. However, the consistent findings open new research questions to study the influence of individual factors in the perception of self-motion, including the possibility that distortion of time perception might interfere with displacement perception, as well as the influence of the immediate perception of motion on updating space-time orientation.

In conclusion, in young and middle-aged healthy adults, susceptibility to motion sickness and anxiety may influence the space-time perception of earth-vertical passive rotations in the horizontal plane, with influence from individual traits and experiences, and possibly sleep quality.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité Local de Investigación en Salud y Ética en Investigación no. 3601 del Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KJ-R conceived and designed the study and the vestibular stimuli, supervised the selection of participants, analyzed and interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. MA-T selected and evaluated the participants, performed the stimuli, collected the data, and revised the manuscript. JM-P coordinated the evaluation of participants, validated and administered the data, and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anson, E., Ehrenburg, M. R., Simonsick, E. M., and Agrawal, Y. (2021). Association between vestibular function and rotational spatial orientation perception in older adults. J. Vest. Res. [Preprint]. doi: 10.3233/VES-201582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astur, R. S., Ortiz, M. L., and Sutherland, R. J. (1998). A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: a large and reliable sex difference. Behav. Brain Res. 93, 185–190. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00019-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balaban, C. D. (2002). Neural substrates linking balance control and anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 77, 469–475. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00935-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balaban, C. D., and Thayer, J. F. (2001). Neurological bases for balance-anxiety links. J. Anxiety Disord. 15, 53–79. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(00)00042-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, W., Jürgens, R., and Boß, T. (2000). Vestibular perception of self-rotation in different postures: a comparison between sitting and standing subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 131, 468–476. doi: 10.1007/s002219900321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrendt, R. P. (2013). Conscious experience and episodic memory: hippocampus at the crossroads. Front. Psychol. 4:304. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertolini, G., Ramat, S., Bockisch, C. J., Marti, S., Straumann, D., and Palla, A. (2012). Is vestibular self-motion perception controlled by the velocity storage? Insights from patients with chronic degeneration of the vestibulo-cerebellum. PLoS One 7:e36763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone/0036763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T., and Neckelmann, D. (2002). The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale. An updated literature review. J. Psychosom. Res. 52, 69–77.

Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, I. I. I. C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, K., Huttenlocher, J., and Newcombe, N. S. (2013). 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1033–1054. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0416-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J., McKillop, E., and Ward, M. (2006). Sex-specific relationships between route-learning strategies and abilities in a large-scale environment. Environ. Behav. 38, 791–801. doi: 10.1177/0013916506287004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coelho, C. M., and Balaban, C. D. (2015). Visuo-vestibular contributions to anxiety and fear. Neurosc. Biobehavl. Rev. 48, 148–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, B., Dai, M., Yakushin, S. B., and Cho, C. (2019). The neural basis of motion sickness. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 973–982. doi: 10.1152/jn.00674.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, W. E. (1988). Some effects of sleep loss on vestibular responses. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 59, 523–529.

Google Scholar

Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. L., Sjostrom, M. L., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B., et al. (2003). International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 35, 1381–1395.

Google Scholar

Crum-Brown, A. (1875). On the sense of rotation and the anatomy and physiology of the semicircular canals of the internal ear. J. Anat. Physiol. 8, 327–331.

Google Scholar

Cullen, K. E. (2011). The neural encoding of self-motion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 587–595. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullen, K. E., and Roy, J. E. (2004). Signal processing in the vestibular system during active versus passive head movements. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1919–1933. doi: 10.1152/jn.00988.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dabbs, J., Chang, E. L., Strong, R., and Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 19, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(97)00107-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Zeeuw, J., Papakonstantinou, A., Nowozin, C., Stotz, S., Zaleska, M., Hädel, S., et al. (2019). Living in biological darkness: objective sleepiness and the pupillary light responses are affected by different metameric lighting conditions during daytime. J. Biol. Rhythms 34, 410–431. doi: 10.1177/0748730419847845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demer, J. L., and Crane, B. T. (1998). Vision and vestibular adaptation. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 119, 78–88. doi: 10.1016/s0194-5998(98)70176-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. J. Physiol. Paris 107, 255–264. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichenbaum, H. (2017). Time (and space) in the hippocampus. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 17, 65–70.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Glasauer, S., Schneider, E., Grasso, R., and Ivanenko, Y. P. (2007). Space-time relativity in self-motion reproduction. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 451–461. doi: 10.1152/jn.01243.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golding, J. F. (2006a). Motion sickness susceptibility. Auton. Neurosci. 129, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2006.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golding, J. F. (2006b). Predicting individual differences in motion sickness susceptibility by questionnaire. Pers. Individ. Diff. 41, 237–248. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonshor, A., and Jones, G. M. (1976). Short-term adaptive changes in the human vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. J. Physiol. 256, 361–379. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grunfeld, E. A., Okada, T., Jauregui-Renaud, K., and Bronstein, A. M. (2000). The effect of habituation and plane of rotation on vestibular perceptual responses. J. Vestib. Res. 10, 193–200.

Google Scholar

Guedry, F. E., Stockwell, C. W., and Gilson, R. D. (1971). Comparison of subjective responses to semicircular canal stimulation produced by rotation about different axes. Acta Otolar. 72, 101–106. doi: 10.3109/00016487109122461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hafström, A., Modig, F., Karlberg, M., and Fransson, P. A. (2007). Increased visual dependence and otolith dysfunction with alcohol intoxication. NeuroReport 18, 391–394. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e328013e3eb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Israël, I., Bronstein, A. M., Kanayama, R., Faldon, M., and Gresty, M. A. (1996). Visual and vestibular factors influencing vestibular “navigation”. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 411–419.

Google Scholar

Israël, I., Sievering, D., and Koenig, E. (1995). Self-rotation estimate about the vertical axis. Acta Oto Laryngol. 115, 3–8. doi: 10.3109/00016489509133338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jáuregui-Renaud, K., Sang, Y. P. F., Gresty, M. A., Green, D. A., and Bronstein, A. M. (2008). Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 276–283. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.122119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, Y., Chen, S., Deutz, N. E. P., Bukkapatnam, S. T. S., and Woltering, S. (2019). Examining the structure validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 17, 209–221. doi: 10.1007/s41105-018-00201-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. L. H., Le, V. C., Ebert, S. M., Sienko, K. H., Reed, M. P., and Sayer, J. R. (2019). Motion sickness in passenger vehicles during test track operations. Ergonomics 6210, 1357–1371. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2019.1632938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jürgens, R., Boß, T., and Becker, W. (1999). Estimation of self-turning in the dark: comparison between active and passive rotation. Exp. Brain Res. 128, 491–504. doi: 10.1007/s002210050872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klatzky, R. L. (1998). “Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: definitions, distinctions, and interconnections,” in Spatial Cognition. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 1404, eds C. Freksa, C. Habel, and K. F. Wender (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

Lake, J., and LaBar, K. S. (2011). Unpredictability and uncertainty in anxiety: a new direction for emotional timing research. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, P. H., Macfarlane, D. J., Lam, T. H., and Stewart, S. M. (2011). Validity of the international physical activity questionnaire short form (IPAQ-SF): a systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act 8:115. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lentz, J. M., and Guedry, F. E. Jr. (1978). Motion sickness susceptibility: a retrospective comparison of laboratory tests. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 49, 1281–1288.

Google Scholar

Lindsey, J. K. (1997). Applying Generalized Linear Models. Diepenbeek: Springer.

Google Scholar

Mach, E. (1875). Grundlinien der Lehre von den Bewegungsempfindungen. Leipzig: Englemann.

Google Scholar

Marlinsky, V. V. (1999). Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man–II. Estimations of rotations about the earth-vertical axis. Neuroscience 90, 395–401. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00449-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, T., Gauthier, A., Ying, Z., Benguigui, N., Moussay, S., Bulla, J., et al. (2018). Effect of sleep deprivation on diurnal variation of vertical perception and postural control. J. Appl. Physiol. 125, 167–174. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00595.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Gallardo, S., Miguel-Puga, J. A., Cooper-Bribiesca, D., Bronstein, A. M., and Jáuregui-Renaud, K. (2020). Derealization and motion-perception related to repeated exposure to 3T magnetic resonance image scanner in healthy adults. J. Vestib. Res. 30, 69–80. doi: 10.3233/ves-201577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCoy, J. G., and Strecker, R. E. (2011). The cognitive cost of sleep lost. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 564–582. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mergner, T., Rumberger, A., and Becker, W. (1996). Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity? Brain Res. Bull. 40, 467–471. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollayeva, T., Thurairajah, P., Burton, K., Mollayeva, S., Shapiro, C. M., and Colantonio, A. (2016). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index as a screening tool for sleep dysfunction in clinical and non-clinical samples: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 25, 52–73. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Money, K. E. (1970). Motion sickness. Physiol. Rev. 50, 1–39.

Google Scholar

Okada, T., Grunfeld, E., Shallo-Hoffmann, J., and Bronstein, A. M. (1999). Vestibular perception of angular velocity in normal subjects and in patients with congenital nistagmus. Brain 122, 1293–1303. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.7.1293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ołpińska-Lischka, M., Kujawa, K., and Maciaszek, J. (2021). Differences in the effect of sleep deprivation on the postural stability among men and women. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3796. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paillard, A. C., Quarck, G., Paolino, F., Denise, P., Paolino, M., Golding, J. F., et al. (2013). Motion sickness susceptibility in healthy subjects and vestibular patients: effects of gender, age and trait-anxiety. J. Vestib. Res. 23, 203–209. doi: 10.3233/ves-130501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pariyadath, V., and Eagleman, D. (2007). The effect of predictability on subjective duration. PLoS One 2:e1264. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, N., Hasler, G., and Mast, F. W. (2014). Caloric vestibular stimulation modulates affective control and mood. Brain Stimul. 7, 133–140. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quarck, G., Ventre, J., Etard, O., and Denise, P. (2006). Total sleep deprivation can increase vestibulo-ocular responses. J. Sleep Res. 15, 369–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00550.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raphan, T., Matsuo, V., and Cohen, B. (1979). Velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex arc (VOR). Exp. Brain Res. 35, 229–248.

Google Scholar

Rogge, A. K., Hamacher, D., Cappagli, G., Kuhne, L., Hötting, K., Zech, A., et al. (2021). Balance, gait, and navigation performance are related to physical exercise in blind and visually impaired children and adolescents. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 1111–1123. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06038-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadalla, E. K., and Montello, D. R. (1989). Remembering changes in direction. Environ. Behav. 21, 346–363. doi: 10.1177/0013916589213006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., and Olshansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding. further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 201–213. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00036-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoffregen, T. A., and Riccio, G. E. (1988). An ecological theory of orientation and the vestibular system. Psychol. Rev. 95, 3–14. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, R., and Yotsumoto, Y. (2017). Passage of time judgments is relative to temporal expectation. Front. Psychol. 8:187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viaud-Delmon, I., Venault, P., and Chapouthier, G. (2011). Behavioral models for anxiety and multisensory integration in animals and humans. Progr. Neuro Psychopharma Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1391–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, R. F., and Spelke, E. S. (2000). Updating egocentric representations in human navigation. Cognition 77, 215–250. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00105-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yardley, L. (1991). Orientation perception, motion sickness and vertigo: Beyond the sensory conflict approach. Brit. J. Audiol. 25, 405–413. doi: 10.3109/03005369109076616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370.

Google Scholar

Appendix 1

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlation matrix between the updating orientation test results and the age, the first rotation estimation error, the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) total scores and sub-scores.

Keywords: orientation, vestibular, anxiety, motion sickness, adults

Citation: Alcantara-Thome M, Miguel-Puga JA and Jauregui-Renaud K (2021) Anxiety and Motion Sickness Susceptibility May Influence the Ability to Update Orientation in the Horizontal Plane of Healthy Subjects. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 15:742100. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2021.742100

Received: 15 July 2021; Accepted: 25 August 2021;
Published: 14 September 2021.

Edited by:

Pierre Denise, INSERM U1075 Université de Caen Normandie - Pôle des Formations et de Recherche en Santé, France

Reviewed by:

Gilles Clement, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France
Thomas A. Stoffregen, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States

Copyright © 2021 Alcantara-Thome, Miguel-Puga and Jauregui-Renaud. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kathrine Jauregui-Renaud, kathrine.jauregui@imss.gob.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.