Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 10 January 2020
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensory Processing Across the Lifespan: A 25-Year Initiative to Understand Neurophysiology, Behaviors and Treatment Effectiveness for Sensory Processing View all 13 articles

Multi-sensory Responsiveness and Personality Traits Predict Daily Pain Sensitivity

  • 1Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Health Professions, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 2Chan Division of Occupational Science and Occupational Therapy at the Herman Ostrow School of Dentistry, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Pediatrics, USC Keck School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Background: A continuous effort has been devoted to identifying factors that contribute to individual differences in pain perception. Amongst the personality traits, Neuroticism is assumed to be the most significant moderator of experimental and clinical pain. Multi-sensory responsiveness to daily sensations has been shown to be associated with pain perception. Yet, neither the relationship between personality traits and multi-sensory responsiveness nor the impact of both these factors to pain perception have been examined. Thus, this study aims to explore the contribution of both multi-sensory responsiveness and personality traits to pain perception in a daily context.

Methods: A community-based sample of 204 adults completed the Sensory Responsiveness Questionnaire-Intensity Scale (SRQ-IS); the Big Five Inventory (BFI); and the Pain Sensitivity Questionnaire (PSQ).

Results: The partial eta-square demonstrated that the SRQ-IS Aversive sub-scale score had the strongest relationship with the PSQ-Total score, accounting for 9% of the variation. The regression coefficient relating PSQ-Total score with SRQ-IS Aversive, and BFI sub-scales of Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness-to-Experience scores was found to be r = 0.39 (p < 0.0001), accounting for 16% of the variance, and yielding a large effect size.

Discussion: To the best of our knowledge this is the first study to report on the interplay between aversive responsiveness to daily sensations and personality traits of Neuroticism, Openness-to-Experience, and Extraversion as contributing factors to daily pain sensitivity, amongst which aversive responsiveness was found as the major contributing factor. This study may broaden the understanding of the pain experience variability, both in practice and in experimental research.

Introduction

Pain is a compound multifaceted experience composed of sensory, affective, and cognitive processes (Moayedi and Davis, 2013). There is substantial individual variability in the perception of experimental and clinical pain, as well as in the susceptibility in developing painful conditions, and responding to pain-relieving treatments (Mogil, 1999; Pud et al., 2004, 2006). Continuous efforts have been devoted to identifying factors relevant to understanding this variability (Pud et al., 2004, 2014; Vassend et al., 2013). Increasing evidence indicates that genetic factors (Young et al., 2012; Vassend et al., 2013), demographic characteristics (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity) and personality traits (Riley and Wade, 2004; Pud et al., 2004)—the prompts to think or act in a similar way in response to varied stimuli or situations (Goldberg, 1990), are all related to pain responses. Further, an ecological perspective to painful events in life situations posits that pain is not isolated, and maybe experienced more intensely in individuals who are over-responsive to stimuli derived from other sensory modalities (Bar-Shalita et al., 2015, 2019).

Sensory modulation affects the ability to grade responses to stimuli across one or more sensory systems (ICDL, 2005; Miller et al., 2007); Sensory over-responsivity (SOR) manifests as a condition in which non-painful stimuli are perceived as abnormally irritating, unpleasant (ICDL, 2005; Miller et al., 2007) or painful (Bar-Shalita et al., 2012, 2014; Weissman-Fogel et al., 2018) consequently interfering with participation in daily life (Dunn, 2007; Bar-Shalita et al., 2008; Chien et al., 2016), and in quality of life (Kinnealey et al., 2011; Bar-Shalita et al., 2015). Testing the association between sensory responsiveness and daily pain perception indicated that increased daily pain sensitivity co-occurs with SOR (Bar-Shalita et al., 2015). Furthermore, experimental pain findings suggest atypical pain processing and modulation in subjects with SOR demonstrated by pain hypersensitivity (Bar-Shalita et al., 2014; Weissman-Fogel et al., 2018). Interestingly, while pain hypersensitivity is also related to personality traits (Pud et al., 2004), personality traits are impacted by sensory processing (Dunn, 2001; Croy et al., 2011).

The five-factor model of personality dimensions (Goldberg, 1990) includes (1) Agreeableness—being sympathetic, kind, and affectionate; (2) Conscientiousness—being organized, thorough, and reliable; (3) Extraversion—being talkative, energetic, and assertive; (4) Openness to experience—having wide interests and being imaginative and insightful; and (5) Neuroticism—being tense, moody, and anxious. The personality trait of Neuroticism is considered to be among the most significant moderators of experimental and clinical pain (Wade and Price, 2000; Boggero et al., 2014). Since, individuals with SOR demonstrate enhanced experimental pain ratings, as well as daily pain hypersensitivity (Bar-Shalita et al., 2012, 2014, 2015), we hypothesized that Neuroticism together with SOR will best explain the variance of daily pain sensitivity than either of these factors alone. Importantly, the five-factor model presents traits that are clearly dimensional (Chaplin et al., 1988), thus personality can be best understood by assessing the ranks on these five bipolar factors (McCrae and John, 1992). Yet, neither the importance of Neuroticism nor the association of the other personality traits with pain responses have been sufficiently studied. Of note, since the presence of pre-existing pain may alter the perception of pain sensation (Apkarian et al., 2011; Woolf, 2011), or influence the self-reporting of personality traits (Fishbain et al., 2006), and since we aimed at contributing to a better understanding of the pre-existing individual factors that may impact pain perception, this study investigated a non-clinical, healthy sample.

Materials and Methods

This is a cross-sectional study, approved by the institutional ethics review committee, and all participants provided written consent before enrolling in the study.

Participants

The participant population has been included in a previous publication, authored by both authors of this article (Bar-Shalita and Cermak, 2016). A non-clinical convenience sample of 204 adults [51.5% (N = 105) men] participated in this study. Mean (SD) age was 27.4 (3.71) years (age range 23–40 years). The study sample included 48.5% of university students, while the rest (51.5%) were recruited off-campus and reported work as their main occupation. Eighty-five percent were native-born while the rest (15%; n = 30) were born in Europe, the USA, and Africa. Forty-seven percent had up to 12 years of education, while 53% had higher education. As for family status 76% were single and the rest were married. Exclusion criteria stipulated pregnancy, frequent or chronic pain conditions, neurodevelopmental conditions including autism and ADHD, neurological deficits including speech, vision, hearing or behavioral abnormalities, a history of psychopathology as well as any restrictions to self-reporting.

Instrumentation

The Sensory Responsiveness Questionnaire-Intensity Scale (SRQ-IS; Bar-Shalita et al., 2009a)

A self-report questionnaire assessing responses to daily sensations, aiming at clinically identifying sensory modulation dysfunction. The scale consists of a set of 58 items that represent typical scenarios encountered occasionally throughout daily life. Each scenario involves one sensory stimulus in one modality including auditory, visual, gustatory, olfactory, vestibular and somatosensory stimuli excluding pain. The items are worded in a manner that attributes a hedonic/aversive valence to the situation [e.g., Aversive sample item: It bothers me the way new clothes feel; Hedonic sample item: I enjoy loud noises (such as a vacuum cleaner, construction work)]. The participant rates the intensity of the hedonic/aversive response to the situation using a 5-point scale with the anchors “not at all” attached to the score of “1” and “very much” attached to the score of “5.” Two scores are computed: sensory responsiveness questionnaire (SRQ)-Aversive (32 items) assessing SOR and SRQ-Hedonic (26 items) assessing sensory under-responsivity (Mean SD 1.87 + 0.26; 2.10 + 0.33, respectively). The SRQ has been demonstrated to have content, criterion and construct validity, as well as internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.90–0.93) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.71- 0.84; p < 0.001–0.005; Bar-Shalita et al., 2009a).

The Big Five Inventory (BFI; John et al., 1991)

A 44-item self-report questionnaire assessing five broadband personality traits: Extraversion, encompassing such traits such as talkative, energetic, and assertive; Agreeableness, being sympathetic, kind, and affectionate; Conscientiousness, being organized, thorough, and reliable; Neuroticism, being tense, moody, and anxious; and Openness to experience, having wide interests and being imaginative and insightful. The response format utilizes a 5-point Likert scale varying from “total disagreement” attached to the score of “1” to “total agreement” attached to the score of “5.” A sum score for each of the five personality dimensions is used to build a personality profile. The Big Five Inventory (BFI) questionnaire has been demonstrated to have content, convergent and discriminant validity, as well as internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.79–0.87; Mean 0.83; Worrell and Cross, 2004; John et al., 2008).

The Pain Sensitivity Questionnaire (PSQ; Ruscheweyh et al., 2009)

A 17-item self-report questionnaire assessing daily pain sensitivity based on pain intensity ratings of imagined painful daily life situations in different somatosensory sub-modalities. The Pain Sensitivity Questionnaire (PSQ) is suggested as an alternative to experimental pain procedures that evaluate pain sensitivity in healthy and chronic pain patients (Ruscheweyh et al., 2009, 2012). Pain intensity is rated on a scale with the anchors “not painful at all” attached to the score of “0” and “worst pain imaginable” attached to the score of “10.” The PSQ provides a total score (PSQ-total) and two subscale scores: PSQ-moderate (sample item: Imagine you burn your tongue on a very hot drink) and PSQ-minor (sample item: Imagine you prick your finger tip on the thorn of a rose). The PSQ has been demonstrated to have content, criterion and construct validity, as well as internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.92 for PSQ-total, 0.81 for PSQ-minor and 0.91 for PSQ-moderate), and test-retest reliability (ICCs = 0.83, 0.86 and 0.79, respectively; Ruscheweyh et al., 2009).

Procedure

A convenience sample of participants, recruited using a snowball sampling, were contacted by phone. Information regarding the study was provided by the researcher while inclusion criteria were verified. Eligible participants attended a session, where after completing a consent form and a medical and demographic questionnaire, they were administered the SRQ, BFI, and PSQ. The latter three questionnaires were completed on a counter-balanced order to avoid sequential effects and to balance the possible influence of fatigue and attention span. The session lasted for approximately 45 min, with the researcher present and available for participants’ queries.

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed with SAS V9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Continuous variables are summarized with a mean and standard deviation and categorical variables are presented by a count and percentage. Pearson’s correlation coefficient is presented between pairs of continuous variables with a level of significance. Linear regression was performed to assess multiple correlation coefficients (R) regression coefficients and effect sizes (partial eta-square) with 95% confidence limits presented. All statistical tests were two-sided and tested at a 5% level of significance. Since this was an exploratory study with no existing previous data relating to SRQ and BFI, adjustments for multiple testing were not performed and nominal p-values are presented.

Results

Descriptive statistics (Mean; SD) for the three study measures (BFI; SRQ; PSQ) is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics (Mean; SD, Min, Median and Max) for the three study measures (BFI; SRQ; PSQ; N = 204).

Association Between Personality Traits (BFI) and Sensory Responsiveness (SRQ)

Low to moderate statistically significant correlations were found between SRQ scores and BFI scores in the total sample (n = 204; Table 2). The SRQ-Aversive score showed significant correlations with all BFI scores except Openness-to-Experience. The SRQ-Hedonic score correlated significantly with two of the five BFI scores; such that a negative weak correlation was found with the Neuroticism score, whereas a weak positive correlation was found with the Openness-to-Experience score (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson correlation coefficients between the SRQ (Aversive and Hedonic) scores and the BFI (Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience) scores (N = 204).

Association Between Personality Traits (BFI), Sensory Responsiveness (SRQ) and Daily Pain Sensitivity (PSQ)

The total, moderate and minor scores on the PSQ were found to have statistically significant low correlations with the SRQ-Aversive score but not with the SRQ-Hedonic score. Furthermore, the PSQ scores were found to have statistically significant low positive correlations with Neuroticism and negative correlations with Openness-to-Experience (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between the SRQ (Aversive and Hedonic), the BFI (Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience) and the PSQ (Total, Moderate, Minor and Non-painful) scores (N = 204).

Assessing Contributing Factors to Pain Perception

In order to assess the contributions of both sensory and personality factors to pain perception, all variables that were significantly correlated with the PSQ scores were entered into multivariate model. These variables also were found either to be correlated with the PSQ scores or with the SRQ scores in the univariate analyses (Tables 2, 3).

The PSQ-Total score was significantly correlated with the SRQ-Aversive score and the Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness to Experience scores of the BFI (Table 4; multivariate correlation coefficient r = 0.39, p < 0.0001).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regression coefficients relating the PSQ-Total score with the SRQ-Aversive, and BFI-Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness-to-Experience scores (R2 = 0.156).

The partial eta-square demonstrated that the SRQ-Aversive score has the strongest relationship with the PSQ-Total score, accounting for about 9% of the variance which is considered a medium effect size. The personality components of Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness-to-Experience each contributed about 2% to the total variation (Table 5). SRQ-Aversive, Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness-to-Experience together, as noted above, account for 16% of the variation, i.e., the model has a large effect size (Cohen, 1988). The resulting linear equation is as follows:

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The contribution of each parameter (SRQ-Aversive and BFI: Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness-to-Experience scores to the variation of the PSQ-Total score (Partial eta-square with 95% confidence limits).

PSQ-Total = 15.32 + 18.94*SRQ-Aversion + 6.56*Extraversion + 5.50*Neuroticism–7.41*Openness-to-Experience. Similar patterns were found for the PSQ- Moderate and Minor scores (data not shown).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that investigated the contribution of personality traits and multi-sensory responsiveness to the individual variance in daily pain perception in a non-clinical healthy sample, in an attempt to explore potential pre-existing individual factors that may affect pain perception. Results demonstrate that aversive responsiveness to sensations and the personality traits of Neuroticism, Openness-to-Experience and Extraversion all contribute to daily pain sensitivity. Specifically, individuals who were most sensitive to pain tended to be high in aversive responsiveness to multi-sensory stimuli and in Neuroticism while low in Openness-to-Experience, and in Extraversion, with sensory aversive responsiveness measuring SOR, was found as the major contributing factor to pain sensitivity.

Sensory Responsiveness and Personality Traits

People vary in the way they perceive their environment (Croy et al., 2011) which contributes to the characterization of their personality traits (McCrae et al., 2000). The “sensory filter” hypothesis is based on the notion that people do not have an objective picture of the environment surrounding them, but rather a person-specific filtered one (Croy et al., 2011). Accordingly, an individual’s sensory processing capacity would partly form such a sensory-filter system that is applied when perceiving sensory events, robustly impacting the perceived world, and in turn, influencing one’s customary thoughts, emotions and behavior relative to the environment, which characterize personality traits (McCrae et al., 2000). Thus, when considering the trajectory that determines the way people perceive the environment, it seems tenable that the sensory processing ability may influence the way the world is conceived, which then develops into a pattern of behavioral responses. But at the same time, the sensory system’s capacities and personality traits may both share the same genetic origins (Croy et al., 2011). Moreover, basic behavioral characteristics may be predisposed but also are developed and shaped with accumulating experiences within the environment (Croy et al., 2011). Hence, elusive shaping of underlying genetic elements of personality are environmentally enabled, and an individual pattern of sensory responses may be related and contribute to personality characteristics (McCrae et al., 2000). Indeed, research has demonstrated significant individual variability in sensory abilities (McCrae et al., 2000), as well as in the tolerance to the pain sensory system (John et al., 1991; Fillingim et al., 2009; Paine et al., 2009).

Personality Traits and Pain Perception

The five-factor model of personality, considered to have a biological basis (Jang et al., 2002), was designed to supply a comprehensive taxonomy of traits using five basic dimensions (Goldberg, 1990). Positive traits are as interesting and significant as the more familiar negative traits when studying the factors underlying individual variability in pain perception (Vassend et al., 2013). This study demonstrates that when examining the association between personality traits and daily pain sensitivity, the subscales of Neuroticism (positive correlation) and Openness-to-Experience (negative correlation) were found significantly associated. Pud et al. (2014) sub-grouped healthy individuals according to different pain modality sensitivities and personality profiles, and found that the personality trait of harm avoidance was the most likely to determine pain perception. Harm avoidance, according to Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Theory, is defined as a tendency to respond intensely to previously established signals of aversive stimuli and to passively avoid novelty (Paine et al., 2009). In the present study, Neuroticism was found to have the strongest correlations (among all five personality traits) to all three daily pain sensitivity measures. While Neuroticism is characterized by tenseness, moodiness, and anxiety (Martínez et al., 2011; Littman-Ovadia and Lavy, 2012), it is the trait most similar to harm avoidance (Pud et al., 2014). Openness-to-Experience, which this study found negatively correlated to daily pain sensitivity, denotes having wide interests and being imaginative and insightful (Littman-Ovadia and Lavy, 2012). It seems that Openness-to-Experience could serve as the opposite anchor of harm avoidance. Notably, while Pud et al. (2014) tested the relation between pain sensitivity and personality dimensions in the lab, our findings not only support their results, but have the additional advantage of being able to be extrapolated to environments outside the lab.

Contributors to Pain Perception

This study found that the main contributor to pain likelihood was the SRQ-Aversive score, which surprisingly far exceeded the importance of personality traits. The SRQ Aversive sub-scale contains items that reflect irritation from daily non-noxious sensations. We have previously reported that individuals with over-responsiveness to daily sensation demonstrate hyperalgesia and lingering sensation to experimental pain stimuli (Bar-Shalita et al., 2009b, 2012, 2014, 2019). Indeed, individuals who are sensory over-responsive process sensory stimulus more intensely, longer and become overwhelmed by everyday sensory experiences (ICDL, 2005; Miller et al., 2007; Davies et al., 2010). Consequently, one of their main adaptive coping mechanisms reported is avoidance (Kinnealey et al., 1995). Harm avoidance, which was previously found as the principal factor that seems to determine pain perception (Pud et al., 2014), and was reported to be highly associated with Neuroticism as well (Caseras et al., 2003), leads to fear-avoidance behavior (Conrad et al., 2007), and worsens pain perception (Pud et al., 2004; Vlaeyen and Linton, 2006). Specifically, higher Harm avoidance was found correlated to less efficient endogenous analgesia, assuming to characterize pro-nociceptive individuals (Nahman-Averbuch et al., 2016). Thus, we suggest that the predisposition of aversive responsiveness to sensations can lead to avoidance. These, in turn, evolve into fear-avoidance behaviors which consequently may be demonstrated as a pro-nociceptive pain perception (Bar-Shalita et al., 2019). Using a multivariate model enabled a more authentic examination allowing a dimensional perspective of all factors tested. As such this is the first study to indicate that these three personality traits (Extraversion, Neuroticism and Openness to Experience) similarly contribute to pain perception. Moreover, our findings demonstrate that pain perception has a stronger link to the sensory domain than to the personality domain.

Study Limitations

There are limitations to this study that warrant attention: this study measured daily pain perception through self-report. Although the measure used (PSQ) is suggested by its authors as an experimental pain procedures alternative for evaluating pain sensitivity in healthy and chronic pain patients (Ruscheweyh et al., 2009, 2012), objective experimentally induced pain measures were not carried out in this study. Further, though the study population varied in geographical and vocational variables, with approximately 50% of university students, this study utilized a convenience sample. Moreover, distribution in most demographic variables may indicate that these did not impact research findings, however future research should investigate personality and sensory responsiveness together with cultural, religious, previous painful experiences, and ethnicity to better capture the pain sensitivity phenomenon. Finally, though we found a large effect size, a causal relationship cannot be claimed using this study design.

Conclusion

The presence of pain may either alter the perception of pain sensations (Apkarian et al., 2011; Woolf, 2011), or influence the self-reporting of personality traits (Fishbain et al., 2006). Thus, in order to shed light on the pre-existing individual factors that could affect pain perception, this study investigated a non-clinical, healthy sample. Findings illuminate the key role that sensory responsiveness has in daily pain sensitivity and may have an important implication in preventing pain as well as in pain therapy. Moreover, the similar contribution of Openness-to-Experience and Extraversion as Neuroticism in predicting pain highlights the complexity of pain perception. Effective pain treatment can only be achieved by approaching the entire person, rather than the biological pathology (de Meij and van Kleef, 2016). Hence, the identification of sensory responsiveness patterns and specific personality traits can together allude to the pain perception profile and contribute to an individually tailored multidisciplinary pain management therapy.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by School of Occupational Therapy, Hebrew University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Both authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Azriel O., Block I., Matanis R. and Yemini T. for assisting with data collection, Dr. Lisa Deutsch for statistical consultation.

References

Apkarian, A. V., Hashmi, J. A., and Baliki, M. N. (2011). Pain and the brain: specificity and plasticity of the brain in clinical chronic pain. Pain 152, S49–S64. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., and Cermak, S. A. (2016). Atypical sensory modulation and psychological distress in the general population. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 70:7004250010. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2016.018648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Deutsch, L., Honigman, L., and Weissman-Fogel, I. (2015). Ecological aspects of pain in sensory modulation disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 45, 157–167. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Granovsky, Y., Parush, S., and Weissman-Fogel, I. (2019). Sensory modulation disorder (SMD) and pain: a new perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 13:27. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Seltzer, Z., Vatine, J. J., Yochman, A., and Parush, S. (2009a). Development and psychometric properties of the sensory responsiveness questionnaire (SRQ). Disabil. Rehabil. 31, 189–201. doi: 10.1080/09638280801903096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Vatine, J.-J., Seltzer, Z., and Parush, S. (2009b). Psychophysical correlates in children with sensory modulation disorder (SMD). Physiol. Behav. 98, 631–639. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Vatine, J. J., and Parush, S. (2008). Sensory modulation disorder: a risk factor for participation in daily life activities. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 50, 932–937. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03095.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Vatine, J.-J., Parush, S., Deutsch, L., and Seltzer, Z. (2012). Psychophysical correlates in adults with sensory modulation disorder. Disabil. Rehabil. 34, 943–950. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2011.629711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Shalita, T., Vatine, J. J., Yarnitsky, D., Parush, S., and Weissman-Fogel, I. (2014). Atypical central pain processing in sensory modulation disorder: absence of temporal summation and higher after-sensation. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 587–595. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3767-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boggero, I. A., Smart, L. M., Kniffin, T. C., and Walker, R. L. (2014). Neuroticism and resting mean arterial pressure interact to predict pain tolerance in pain-free adults. Pers. Individ. Dif. 69, 140–143. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caseras, X., Avila, C., and Torrubia, R. (2003). The measurement of individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems: a comparison of personality scales. Pers. Individ. Dif. 34, 999–1013. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00084-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaplin, W. F., John, O. P., and Goldberg, L. R. (1988). Conceptions of states and traits: dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 541–557. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chien, C.-W., Rodger, S., Copley, J., Branjerdporn, G., and Taggart, C. (2016). Sensory processing and its relationship with children’s daily life participation. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 36, 73–87. doi: 10.3109/01942638.2015.1040573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Conrad, R., Schilling, G., Bausch, C., Nadstawek, J., Wartenberg, H. C., Wegener, I., et al. (2007). Temperament and character personality profiles and personality disorders in chronic pain patients. Pain 133, 197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croy, I., Springborn, M., Lötsch, J., Johnston, A. N., and Hummel, T. (2011). Agreeable smellers and sensitive neurotics-correlations among personality traits and sensory thresholds. PLoS One 6:e18701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, P. L., Chang, W. P., and Gavin, W. J. (2010). Middle and late latency ERP components discriminate between adults, typical children and children with sensory processing disorders. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 4:16. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Meij, N., and van Kleef, M. (2016). The quality of pain centers, how should it be assessed? Pain Pract. 16, 7–11. doi: 10.1111/papr.12332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, W. (2001). The sensations of everyday life: empirical, theoretical, and pragmatic considerations. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 55, 608–620. doi: 10.5014/ajot.55.6.608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, W. (2007). Supporting children to participate succesfully in everyday life by using sensory processing knowledge. Infants Young Child. 20, 84–101. doi: 10.1097/01.iyc.0000264477.05076.5d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fillingim, R. B., King, C. D., Ribeiro-Dasilva, M. C., Rahim-Williams, B., and Riley, J. L. (2009). Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. J. Pain 10, 447–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbain, D. A., Cole, B., Cutler, R. B., Lewis, J., Rosomoff, H. L., and Rosomoff, R. S. (2006). Chronic pain and the measurement of personality: do states influence traits? Pain Med. 7, 509–529. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00239.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: the big-five factor structure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59, 1216–1229. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ICDL. (2005). Regulatory Sensory Processing Disorder Access 1. In: Diagnostic Manual for Infancy and Early Childhood: Mental Health, Developmental, Regulatory-Sensory Processing and Language Disorders and Learning Challenges (ICDL-DMIC). Bethesda, MD: Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental and Learning Disorders.

Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., and Vernon, P. A. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 33, 83–101. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00137-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory—Versions 4A and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research.

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Soto, C. J. (2008). “Paradigm shift to the integrative big five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues,” in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (Vol. 3), eds O. P. John, R. W. Robins and L. A. Pervin (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 114–158.

Google Scholar

Kinnealey, M., Koenig, K. P., and Smith, S. (2011). Relationships between sensory modulation and social supports and health-related quality of life. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 65, 320–327. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2011.001370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinnealey, M., Oliver, B., and Wilbarger, P. (1995). A phenomenological study of sensory defensiveness in adults. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 49, 444–451. doi: 10.5014/ajot.49.5.444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littman-Ovadia, H., and Lavy, S. (2012). Character strengths in Israel: hebrew adaptation of the via inventory of strengths. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 28, 41–50. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000089

Martínez, M. P., Sánchez, A. I., Miró, E., Medina, A., and Lami, M. J. (2011). The relationship between the fear-avoidance model of pain and personality traits in fibromyalgia patients. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 18, 380–391. doi: 10.1007/s10880-011-9263-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hřebířková, M., Avia, M. D., et al. (2000). Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 173–186. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J. Pers. 60, 175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. J., Anzalone, M. E., Lane, S. J., Cermak, S. A., and Osten, E. T. (2007). Concept evolution in sensory integration: a proposed nosology for diagnosis. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 61, 135–140. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.2.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moayedi, M., and Davis, K. D. (2013). Theories of pain: from specificity to gate control. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 5–12. doi: 10.1152/jn.00457.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogil, J. S. (1999). The genetic mediation of individual differences in sensitivity to pain and its inhibition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 96, 7744–7751. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.7744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahman-Averbuch, H., Yarnitsky, D., Sprecher, E., Granovsky, Y., and Granot, M. (2016). Relationship between personality traits and endogenous analgesia: the role of harm avoidance. Pain Pract. 16, 38–45. doi: 10.1111/papr.12256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paine, P., Kishor, J., Worthen, S. F., Gregory, L. J., and Aziz, Q. (2009). Exploring relationships for visceral and somatic pain with autonomic control and personality. Pain 144, 236–244. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pud, D., Eisenberg, E., Sprecher, E., Rogowski, Z., and Yarnitsky, D. (2004). The tridimensional personality theory and pain: harm avoidance and reward dependence traits correlate with pain perception in healthy volunteers. Eur. J. Pain 8, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/s1090-3801(03)00065-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pud, D., Treister, R., and Eisenberg, E. (2014). Sub-grouping healthy subjects’ sensitivity to pain and its relationship to personality traits: results of a cluster analysis. Appl. Math. 5, 1705–1712. doi: 10.4236/am.2014.511163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pud, D., Yarnitsky, D., Sprecher, E., Rogowski, Z., Adler, R., and Eisenberg, E. (2006). Can personality traits and gender predict the response to morphine? An experimental cold pain study. Eur. J. Pain 10, 103–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, J. L., and Wade, J. B. (2004). Psychological and Demographic Factors that Modulate the Different Stages and Dimensions of Pain. Seattle, WA: IASP.

Ruscheweyh, R., Marziniak, M., Stumpenhorst, F., Reinholz, J., and Knecht, S. (2009). Pain sensitivity can be assessed by self-rating: development and validation of the pain sensitivity questionnaire. Pain 146, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruscheweyh, R., Verneuer, B., Dany, K., Marziniak, M., Wolowski, A., Çolak-Ekici, R., et al. (2012). Validation of the pain sensitivity questionnaire in chronic pain patients. Pain 153, 1210–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassend, O., Røysamb, E., and Nielsen, C. S. (2013). Five-factor personality traits and pain sensitivity: a twin study. Pain 154, 722–728. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlaeyen, J. W., and Linton, S. J. (2006). Are We “Fear-Avoidant”? Pain 124, 240–241. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.06.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, J. B., and Price, D. D. (2000). “Nonpathological factors in chronic pain: implications for assessment and treatment,” in Personality characteristics of patients with pain, eds R. J. Gatchel and J. N. Weisberg (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 89–107.

Google Scholar

Weissman-Fogel, I., Granovsky, Y., and Bar-Shalita, T. (2018). Sensory over-responsiveness among healthy subjects is associated with a pronociceptive state. Pain Pract. 18, 473–486. doi: 10.1111/papr.12619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolf, C. J. (2011). Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain 152, S2–S15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worrell, F. C., and Cross, W. E. Jr. (2004). The reliability and validity of big five inventory scores with african american college students. J. Multicult. Couns. Devel. 32, 18–32. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1912.2004.tb00358.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, E. E., Lariviere, W. R., and Belfer, I. (2012). Genetic basis of pain variability: recent advances. J. Med. Genet. 49, 1–9. doi: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2011-100386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sensory over responsiveness, sensory modulation, pain sensitivity, pain perception, risk factor, personality traits

Citation: Bar-Shalita T and Cermak SA (2020) Multi-sensory Responsiveness and Personality Traits Predict Daily Pain Sensitivity. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 13:77. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00077

Received: 19 September 2019; Accepted: 19 December 2019;
Published: 10 January 2020.

Edited by:

Elysa Jill Marco, Cortica, United States

Reviewed by:

Guilherme Lucas, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Aviva Yochman, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel

Copyright © 2020 Bar-Shalita and Cermak. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tami Bar-Shalita, dGJzaGFsaXRhQHBvc3QudGF1LmFjLmls

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.