
95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Hum. Neurosci. , 17 March 2025
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2025.1560138
In daily life, we walk naturally by considering our physical characteristics and formulating appropriate motor plans. However, the impact of changes in body image on walking movements during motor planning remains poorly understood. Therefore, in this study, we examined changes in walking behavior under different conditions where body image was altered. We included 26 participants (13 men and 13 women, aged 18.27 ± 0.52) who performed walking movements under five conditions: eyes open, eyes covered, eyes covered while imagining their bodies becoming larger, eyes covered without imagining altered body size, and eyes open again. As a result, under the condition where participants imagined their bodies becoming larger, their step length, step completion time, and foot lift height increased. To generate a torque larger than the actual body size, the participants made a motor planning with a larger body image, resulting in an increase in step length. Since these results are attributed to the disparity between actual body size and body image, which affects motor planning, our findings have potential applications in rehabilitation and sports coaching settings.
Humans perform walking movements without conscious effort, especially when considering their own physical characteristics and intended movements. Research suggests that this motor planning and motor imagery have equivalent functions (Jeannerod, 1994; Steenbergen et al., 2013; Spruijt et al., 2015). Decety et al. (1994) indicated that mentally imagining a movement activates the same brain regions as when actually performing it (Decety et al., 1994). Movement imagery is defined as “the mental expression of movement without actual bodily movement” (Guillot and Collet, 2005). Considering one’s physical characteristics is considered important in motor imagery.
Walking movements are planned through reverse calculation from the desired speed. These movements are goal-oriented, involving the lower limb. Step length and frequency are adjusted to align with the walking goal at a specific speed (Bruderlin and Calvert, 1989). Previous studies have particularly focused on the transition from a stationary state to the first step taken (Breniere and Do, 1991; Mickelborough et al., 2004; Hass et al., 2008; Vrieling et al., 2008; Ceccato et al., 2009; Bayot et al., 2018; Perera et al., 2023). Research into gait initiation has shown that the greater the electromyogram of the calf muscle of the supporting leg, the faster the forward walking speed (Lepers and Breniere, 1995). Furthermore, studies on the center of pressure (CoP) of the body have indicated that the transition of CoP before walking initiation is related to forward walking speed (Lyon and Day, 1997; Caderby et al., 2014; Yiou et al., 2017). Based on these findings, walking is planned based on the speed and executed while simultaneously controlling the center of gravity and the related muscles.
Motor imagery can encompass various types of movements, and walking movements are particularly useful during rehabilitation. Motor imagery is a useful approach for promoting motor function recovery after cerebrovascular disorders, particularly in relation to walking movements (Peters and Page, 2015). Motor imagery training, involving dorsiflexion and internal/external rotation of the hand joints, improves voluntary control of paralyzed limbs in stroke patients (Stevens and Stoykov, 2003). In a study targeting individuals aged 65 years and above, the incorporation of motor imagery into physical training significantly improved balance ability, walking function, and self-efficacy in preventing falls (Nicholson et al., 2019; Oh and Choi, 2021). Thus, motor imagery is considered effective in improving walking movements.
Recognizing one’s own physical characteristics is crucial to motor imagery. This has been investigated using the concept of body image. Body image is defined as the conscious brain representation of the body, including self-image and appearance perception, which is derived from one’s posture perception (Naito et al., 2016). The discrepancy between body image and actual physical condition is assumed to affect motor performance. For example, in the case of walking, a decrease in step length has been observed when stilts are used to extend the length of the lower limbs (Dominici et al., 2009), because the body image remains the same as the original body. However, the actual body has been extended, resulting in a failure to generate the necessary torque. Thus, the discrepancy between body image and actual physical condition may affect motor performance.
To manipulate body image, visual information plays a crucial role. A person’s body image is thought to be changed by the height of the visible perspective and the presence or absence of visual information in the surrounding environment. Blocking visual information also has a significant impact on movement. Walking with closed eyes results in longer step length and longer time per step (Perry et al., 2001; Wuehr et al., 2013). Additionally, Nishizaki et al. (2022) indicated that raising viewpoints through virtual reality (VR) can make individuals perceive themselves as larger than their actual size, causing increased step length (Nishizaki et al., 2022). Similarly, instructing individuals that their body has become large while raising the viewpoint through VR has also been shown to increase their step length (Sasaki et al., 2021). Thus, visual information plays a significant role in walking movements and posture control, and body image can have a significant impact on movement.
This study aimed to investigate the relationship between motor imagery and motor output by comparing the kinematics of initial walking in the open-eye state, closed-eye state, and closed-eye state with manipulated body image. Participants in this study were asked to imagine an enlarged body image with simple verbal instructions only, and the impact on initial walking was examined. To facilitate imagining an enlarged body image, we blocked visual information to avoid surrounding influences on walking movements (Prokop et al., 1997; Mohler et al., 2007) and achieve more discernible changes. By investigating the kinematics of movements in a state where visual information is blocked and body image is changed, the aim was to elucidate the mechanism of motor output. It was hypothesized that if the body image is larger than the actual body size, the step length and height of lifting the foot would increase. Clarifying the mechanism of motor output is expected to have applications in rehabilitation and sports.
The experiment involved 26 healthy adults (13 men and 13 women, aged 18.27 ± 0.52 years). Participants were allowed to use either their dominant or non-dominant foot for the first step. The sample size was determined based on previous studies (n = 26) (Rochat, 1995; Gabbard and Ammar, 2005; Gabbard et al., 2005; Leclere et al., 2019, 2021). The experiment was conducted between October 7, 2023, and October 16, 2023. Prior to the experiment, the purpose and procedure were explained to the participants, and their written consent was obtained. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kyoto Institute of Technology and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
A schematic of the experiment is presented in Figure 1. The starting point was marked with tape, and the walking tasks were performed barefoot. There were two reasons for conducting the experiment barefoot: firstly, to avoid getting feedback on the weight of the shoes, and secondly, to be able to attach the infrared markers directly to the feet when measuring the kinematics. The tasks started in an upright position with both toes aligned, followed by four steps, then stopped with both feet aligned. The speed was set at a natural walking pace. The leading foot was allowed to step out naturally, without considering the dominant foot. There was also no bias observed in terms of whether the right or left foot was used to start walking, regardless of which foot was dominant. Two infrared cameras (OptiTrack V120 DUO, NeuralPoint) were placed approximately 6 m in front of the participants to capture the walking movements. Reflective markers with a diameter of 16 mm were attached to the tips of the second toes of both feet to obtain three-dimensional (3D) coordinates. These markers were tracked by the two infrared cameras, and the data were obtained with a time resolution of 120 Hz (Gonno et al., 2022).
Figure 1. Overview of the experimental setup. Reflective markers were attached to both toes of the participants, and under the eyes-covered state, they were asked to walk while wearing a blindfold.
The experiment consisted of five phases and the conditions of each are summarized in Table 1. In Phase 1, participants were asked to perform 10 walking tasks in which they took four steps with their eyes open. In Phase 2, they were asked to wear a blindfold and perform 10 walking tasks with their eyes covered. After the trial, they opened their eyes and returned to the starting point on their own. Next, in Phase 3, while still wearing the blindfold and maintaining the same body proportion, participants were instructed to walk 10 times while imagining their bodies becoming larger, as if their heads would touch the ceiling of the laboratory (approximately 4 m in height). In order to help participants imagine themselves as larger, we gave them 10 s to imagine themselves each time they tried. Subsequently, in Phase 4, they were asked to perform three walking tasks using their original body image, while still wearing the blindfold. Finally, in Phase 5, participants were asked again to perform three walking tasks without the blindfold and again using their original body image. This sequence was fixed to investigate whether there was a lasting change in behavior, and Phases 4 and 5 were carried out to rebuild participants’ body image. After the experiment, participants completed a short self-report survey to assess their ability to imagine their bodies getting larger. They rated their level of success on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very well). They were also asked to provide open-ended comments about their subjective experience of imagining an enlarged body.
The post-experiment survey consisted of a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very well) assessing participants’ ability to imagine their bodies as larger. Out of 26 participants, 20 reported a score of 3 or higher, indicating moderate to successful imagining of an enlarged body. Therefore, only the data from these 20 participants were included in the subsequent kinematic analysis. We focused only on the first-step movement in each walking task owing to the possibility of body image impairment by somatosensory feedback during walking. The time-series 3D position data of the infrared markers on the second toes were preprocessed using a second-order Butterworth low-pass filter with a cutoff frequency of 10 Hz (Yamamoto and Kushiro, 2014). The start of the movement was defined as the onset, when the speed consistently exceeded 30 cm/s for the first 5 frames; the end of the movement was defined as the offset, when the speed consistently remained below 30 cm/s for 5 frames. This threshold of 30 cm/s was based on our previous research into lower limb movement (Oku et al., 2023a,b). The following indices were calculated for each trial: step length (Length)—distance between the markers attached to both toes at the end of the first step; maximum height of the toes (Maximum Height)—maximum height from the ground during the swing phase from the onset to offset of the first step; time taken for one step (Time)—time from the start to the end of the first step. The average values were calculated for each condition.
To assess the reliability of the measurements, we calculated both the coefficient of variation (CV) and the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC(1,k)). The CV was used to examine whether the within-participant variability of the movements changed depending on the phase. The within-participant CV for each variable (step length, maximum height, step time) was calculated for each condition (Phases 1–5) and averaged across participants. A one-way analysis of variance was then performed on the CV of each variable, with phase as a factor. When the main effect was confirmed for each variable, Bonferroni multiple comparisons were performed across phases. However, it is not possible to assess the reliability of performance within a phase based on the CV alone. Therefore, we also calculated the ICC(1,k) to examine the reliability of the 10 repetitions of performance per participant in each phase.
In addition, to examine changes in gait as a function of phase, we performed a one-way analysis of variance with phase as a factor for step length, time and maximum height of the first step. As step length, time and maximum height are each independent variables, we used a one-way ANOVA rather than a MANOVA. When the main effect was confirmed for each variable, Bonferroni’s post hoc comparison was performed across phases.
The motion trajectory in the sagittal plane (Y-Z) is shown in Figure 2. When participants assumed a large body image, the step length increased along with the possibility of higher foot elevation. Additionally, examination of the motion trajectory revealed that instead of following a parabolic curve, the foot can be brought up to the landing point and then lowered straight down.
Figure 2. The trajectory of motion in the sagittal plane (Y-Z direction). The line colors represent the phases: Phase 1 (eyes open), Phase 2 (eyes covered), Phase 3 (enlarged body image with eyes covered), Phase 4 (eyes covered post Test 1), and Phase 5 (eyes open post Test 2).
Figure 3 shows the average step length across participants for each phase. A one-way ANOVA was conducted with phase conditions as factors to determine how step length varied across conditions. The analysis revealed a significant main effect [F (4, 76) = 13.68, p = 1.90 × 10−8, η2 = 0.07]. Subsequently, Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons test revealed significant differences between Phases 1 and 3, Phases 2 and 3, Phases 3 and 4, Phases 3 and 5, and Phases 4 and 5 (p < 0.05). In addition, no significant difference was found between Phases 1 and 5 (p > 0.05).
Figure 3. The average values of step length (Length), foot lift maximum height (Maximum Height), and time per step (Time) among participants for each phase. Phase 1 (eyes open), Phase 2 (eyes covered), Phase 3 (enlarged body image with eyes covered), Phase 4 (eyes covered post Test 1), and Phase 5 (eyes open post Test 2). The bands represent the standard deviation. Single asterisks (*) represent p < 0.05.
The average height at which the participants lifted their feet for each condition is also shown in Figure 3. The one-way ANOVA with the phase conditions as factors revealed a significant main effect [F (4, 76) = 11.13, p = 3.70 × 10−7, η2 = 0.07]. Subsequently, Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons test revealed significant differences between Phases 1 and 3, Phases 2 and 3, Phases 2 and 4, Phases 3 and 4, and Phases 3 and 5 (p < 0.05). In addition, no significant difference was found between Phases 1 and 5 (p > 0.05).
The results for average time required for the first step in each phase is also presented in Figure 3. The one-way ANOVA with the phase conditions as factors revealed a significant main effect [F (4, 76) = 26.54, p = 8.67 × 10−14, η2 = 0.15]. Subsequently, Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons revealed significant differences between Phases 1 and 3, Phases 2 and 3, Phases 2 and 4, Phases 3 and 4, Phases 3 and 5, and Phases 4 and 5 (p < 0.05). In addition, no significant difference was found between Phases 1 and 5 (p > 0.05).
The calculated CV and ICC(1,k) of the participants are summarized in Table 2. CV was calculated to assess measurement variability between conditions, and ICC was calculated to examine measurement reliability. The average CV among participants was sufficiently low for all variables (Length, Height, and Time) under all conditions. Regarding CV, one-way ANOVA was conducted for each variable, and significant main effects were observed for step length [F (4, 76) = 4.35, p = 0.0032, η2 = 0.113]. The Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons showed that the CV for Phase 4 was significantly higher than that of Phase 5 (p < 0.05). For Maximum Height, a significant main effect was observed [F (4, 76) = 3.63, p = 0.0094, η2 = 0.0974], with Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons showing a significantly higher CV for Phase 4 than that of Phase 5 (p < 0.05). For the CV of Time, a significant main effect was observed [F (4, 760) = 4.74, p = 0.0018, η2 = 0.128], with Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons showing significantly higher CV for Phases 2, 3, and 4 than that of Phase 5 (p < 0.05). Furthermore, significant correlations were found for ICC(1,k) of each variable during all phases.
Table 2. Mean coefficient of variation (mean CV) and intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC(1,k)) of participants for each phase.
The result of this study shows that when participants with a body image larger than their actual proportions started to walk, the step length, time taken, and height at which the foot was lifted increased. Furthermore, confirmation of sufficiently low CV and within-condition ICC(1,k) suggests that the participants were able to consistently and effectively imagine a larger body image. In contrast to the findings of the previous study (Dominici et al., 2009), where the step length decreased when using stilts while maintaining the same body image, in this study, the opposite phenomenon was observed. Previous studies have pointed out that body image and motor planning are equivalent functions (Jeannerod, 1994; Steenbergen et al., 2013; Spruijt et al., 2015). To generate a torque larger than the actual body size, the participants made a motor plan with a larger body image, resulting in an increase in step length, which also suggested an increase in leg-lift height.
Despite differences, the concept of “body image” in this study shares some similarities with that of “body schema.” Body image is defined as a conscious internal representation of the body (Naito et al., 2016), and body schema is defined as a pre-conscious internal representation of the body (Head and Holmes, 1911). Body schemas can be changed by various factors. The measurements sometimes make use of the peripersonal space (PPS), which is generated by the body neurons that react to nearby objects (Graziano et al., 1994) and is considered to play a role in self-protection (Graziano et al., 2004). Stone et al. (2018) argued that the reaction speed increases when objects approach the PPS, defining PPS as the space in which reaction speed increases (Stone et al., 2018). Several studies have reported that PPS can expand body schemas when we actively use tools (e.g., a stick) (Iriki et al., 1996; Bonifazi et al., 2007), which suggests that the change in body schema occurs unconsciously. However, since the participants in this study were asked to consciously perform tasks with a larger body image, body schema is not considered to have affected the results.
The results of this study were not what would be expected from previous research on walking behavior during visual occlusion. Previous research has shown that occlusion of visual information has a significant effect on walking behavior. There are reports that occlusion of visual information increases the disruption of COP during postural control while walking (Duarte and Zatsiorsky, 2002; Schmid et al., 2007). Furthermore, when visual information is blocked during walking, the end time of a step increases and the step length increases to stabilize the center of gravity (Perry et al., 2001; Wuehr et al., 2013). Thus, although different from gait initiation, when visual information is blocked, the center of gravity of the body becomes unstable and step length increases in relation to gait and postural control. Previous research on gait initiation has also reported that the maximum speed of the vertical CoM tends to be slower when the eyes are closed (Chastan et al., 2010). Based on this previous research on walking and gait initiation, it was predicted that step length would increase when the eyes were covered, but as significance was not confirmed in Phases 1 and 2 of the present study, it cannot be said that step length changed when the eyes were covered. A possible reason for this is that the walking movements in this study were performed barefoot. Walking barefoot increases the proprioceptive sensation of the soles of the feet, which stabilizes the COP (Franklin et al., 2015). Therefore, it is assumed that the instability of the COP in the eyes-covered condition reported in previous studies did not occur in this study, and that the step length and time did not change between the open and covered eye conditions.
It was also observed that it was difficult to reconstruct the usual body image when visual information was blocked. As mentioned above, there was no significant difference between Phase 1 (eyes open) and Phase 2 (eyes covered), but on the other hand, step length and time were shorter in Phase 4 (eyes covered) than in Phase 5 (eyes open). In addition, walking time was significantly shorter in the second eye covering (Phase 4) than in the first eye covering (Phase 2), and no significant difference was observed in any of the variables between the first eye opening (Phase 1) and the second eye opening (Phase 5). From these results it appears that step length and time decreased significantly in Phase 4, rather than step length increasing in Phase 5. In the results for CV, all variables were significantly greater in Phase 4 than in Phase 5 and there was a large amount of variation between trials. This suggests that when the body image, which had been enlarged in Phase 3, was returned to its original size, the difficulty of reconstructing the body image had an effect and the variation between trials increased. It is thought that it was more difficult to be aware of their normal body image than the body image they imagined in this study, which was so large that their head would touch the ceiling, in the absence of visual information. Thus, it is likely that the difficulty in reconstructing a normal body image without visual information led to a change in movement and more caution, resulting in shorter step lengths and times. The mechanism of walking movements in reconstructing a normal body image in the absence of visual information is a topic for future study.
A limitation of this study is that the instructions on body image were provided verbally. This could have caused variations in perception of the body “becoming larger,” depending on the individual. This study mainly targeted kinesthetic motor imagery, but it was not possible to fully control visual motor imagery. In this study, visual information was blocked by blindfolds, but it is possible to reproduce movement in the mind without visual input. Such visual motor imagery includes both first and third person perspectives, but it is thought that there was variation between individuals in terms of which motor imagery was used. The use of VR as a means of controlling such visual motor imagery is a topic for future research. By using VR to visually control the ‘state of having a larger body’ and examining motor output, it will be possible to investigate the effects of different types of motor imagery.
The aim of this study was to elucidate the mechanism of motor output by investigating the kinematics of movement when visual information is blocked and body image is changed. In this study, we observed that, as the body image became larger, the step length increased, resulting in an increase in time and leg-lift height. These findings suggests that changing body image could have a considerable impact on actual walking movement. This underscores the importance of body image in motor planning and highlights potential applications in rehabilitation and performance optimization, where cognitive strategies might be used to influence physical movement.
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24520528.v1.
The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of Kyoto Institute of Technology. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
KO: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ST: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. YN: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. CF: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. NK: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Bayot, M., Dujardin, K., Tard, C., Defebvre, L., Bonnet, C. T., Allart, E., et al. (2018). The interaction between cognition and motor control: a theoretical framework for dual-task interference effects on posture, gait initiation, gait and turning. Neurophysiol. Clin. 48, 361–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2018.10.003
Bonifazi, S., Farne, A., Rinaldesi, L., and Ladavas, E. (2007). Dynamic size-change of peri-hand space through tool-use: spatial extension or shift of the multi-sensory area. J. Neuropsychol. 1, 101–114. doi: 10.1348/174866407X180846
Breniere, Y., and Do, M. C. (1991). Control of gait initiation. J. Mot. Behav. 23, 235–240. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1991.9942034
Bruderlin, A., and Calvert, T. W. (1989). Goal-directed, dynamic animation of human walking. International Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques 23, 233–242. doi: 10.1145/74334.74357
Caderby, T., Yiou, E., Peyrot, N., Begon, M., and Dalleau, G. (2014). Influence of gait speed on the control of mediolateral dynamic stability during gait initiation. J. Biomech. 47, 417–423. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.11.011
Ceccato, J. C., De Seze, M., Azevedo, C., and Cazalets, J. R. (2009). Comparison of trunk activity during gait initiation and walking in humans. PLoS One 4:e8193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008193
Chastan, N., Westby, G. W., Du Montcel, S. T., Do, M. C., Chong, R. K., Agid, Y., et al. (2010). Influence of sensory inputs and motor demands on the control of the Centre of mass velocity during gait initiation in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 469, 400–404. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.12.038
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., Tadary, B., Woods, R., et al. (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature 371, 600–602. doi: 10.1038/371600a0
Dominici, N., Daprati, E., Nico, D., Cappellini, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., and Lacquaniti, F. (2009). Changes in the limb kinematics and walking-distance estimation after shank elongation: evidence for a locomotor body schema? J. Neurophysiol. 101, 1419–1429. doi: 10.1152/jn.91165.2008
Duarte, M., and Zatsiorsky, V. M. (2002). Effects of body lean and visual information on the equilibrium maintenance during stance. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 60–69. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1154-1
Franklin, S., Grey, M. J., Heneghan, N., Bowen, L., and Li, F. X. (2015). Barefoot vs common footwear: a systematic review of the kinematic, kinetic and muscle activity differences during walking. Gait Posture 42, 230–239. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.05.019
Gabbard, C., and Ammar, D. (2005). Visual cues and perceived reachability. Brain Cogn. 59, 287–291. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.07.006
Gabbard, C., Ammar, D., and Rodrigues, L. (2005). Perceived reachability in hemispace. Brain Cogn. 58, 172–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.10.001
Gonno, M., Kida, N., Nomura, T., Matsui, T., Azuma, Y., Hiramoto, M., et al. (2022). Relationship between standing trunk extension angle and medial elbow injuries in young baseball pitchers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:3895. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19073895
Graziano, M. S., Cooke, D. F., Taylor, C. S., and Moore, T. (2004). Distribution of hand location in monkeys during spontaneous behavior. Exp. Brain Res. 155, 30–36. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1701-4
Graziano, M. S., Yap, G. S., and Gross, C. G. (1994). Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266, 1054–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.7973661
Guillot, A., and Collet, C. (2005). Contribution from neurophysiological and psychological methods to the study of motor imagery. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 387–397. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.09.004
Hass, C. J., Waddell, D. E., Wolf, S. L., Juncos, J. L., and Gregor, R. J. (2008). Gait initiation in older adults with postural instability. Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon) 23, 743–753. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2008.02.012
Head, H., and Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34, 102–254. doi: 10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102
Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., and Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport 7, 2325–2330. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199610020-00010
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 187–202. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026
Leclere, N. X., Sarlegna, F. R., Coello, Y., and Bourdin, C. (2019). Sensori-motor adaptation to novel limb dynamics influences the representation of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia 131, 193–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.05.005
Leclere, N. X., Sarlegna, F. R., Coello, Y., and Bourdin, C. (2021). Opposing force fields induce direction-specific sensorimotor adaptation but a non-specific perceptual shift consistent with a contraction of peripersonal space representation. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 31–46. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05945-1
Lepers, R., and Breniere, Y. (1995). The role of anticipatory postural adjustments and gravity in gait initiation. Exp. Brain Res. 107, 118–124. doi: 10.1007/BF00228023
Lyon, I. N., and Day, B. L. (1997). Control of frontal plane body motion in human stepping. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 345–356. doi: 10.1007/PL00005703
Mickelborough, J., Van Der Linden, M. L., Tallis, R. C., and Ennos, A. R. (2004). Muscle activity during gait initiation in normal elderly people. Gait Posture 19, 50–57. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00016-X
Mohler, B. J., Thompson, W. B., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Pick, H. L. Jr., and Warren, W. H. Jr. (2007). Visual flow influences gait transition speed and preferred walking speed. Exp. Brain Res. 181, 221–228. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0917-0
Naito, E., Ota, J., and Murata, A. (2016). Body representation in the brain. Neurosci. Res. 104, 1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.12.014
Nicholson, V., Watts, N., Chani, Y., and Keogh, J. W. (2019). Motor imagery training improves balance and mobility outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. J. Physiother. 65, 200–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jphys.2019.08.007
Nishizaki, Y., Fukada, C., and Kida, N. (2022). The change in viewpoint causes the change in action. The 37th Annual Meeting of Japanese Cognitive Science Society (in Japanese).
Oh, D. S., and Choi, J. D. (2021). Effects of motor imagery training on balance and gait in older adults: a randomized controlled pilot study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:650. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18020650
Oku, K., Tanaka, S., and Kida, N. (2023a). Direction and distance dependency of reaching movements of lower limb. PLoS One 18:e0290745. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0290745
Oku, K., Tanaka, S., and Kida, N. (2023b). Dissociation of perception and motor execution of lower limb in multi-directional movements. Sci. Rep. 13:17237. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-44544-2
Perera, C. K., Gopalai, A. A., Gouwanda, D., Ahmad, S. A., and Salim, M. S. B. (2023). Sit-to-walk strategy classification in healthy adults using hip and knee joint angles at gait initiation. Sci. Rep. 13:16640. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-43148-0
Perry, S. D., Santos, L. C., and Patla, A. E. (2001). Contribution of vision and cutaneous sensation to the control of Centre of mass (COM) during gait termination. Brain Res. 913, 27–34. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02748-2
Peters, H. T., and Page, S. J. (2015). Integrating mental practice with task-specific training and behavioral supports in poststroke rehabilitation: evidence, components, and augmentative opportunities. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. N. Am. 26, 715–727. doi: 10.1016/j.pmr.2015.06.004
Prokop, T., Schubert, M., and Berger, W. (1997). Visual influence on human locomotion. Modulation to changes in optic flow. Exp. Brain Res. 114, 63–70. doi: 10.1007/PL00005624
Rochat, P. (1995). Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 59, 317–333. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1995.1014
Sasaki, K., Kida, N., Fukada, C., and Nishizaki, Y. (2021). Effects of viewpoint transformation and verbal instruction on human behavior in VR. The 26th Annual Conference of the Virtual Reality Society of Japan (in Japanese).
Schmid, M., Nardone, A., De Nunzio, A. M., Schmid, M., and Schieppati, M. (2007). Equilibrium during static and dynamic tasks in blind subjects: no evidence of cross-modal plasticity. Brain 130, 2097–2107. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm157
Spruijt, S., Van Der Kamp, J., and Steenbergen, B. (2015). Current insights in the development of children's motor imagery ability. Front. Psychol. 6:787. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00787
Steenbergen, B., Jongbloed-Pereboom, M., Spruijt, S., and Gordon, A. M. (2013). Impaired motor planning and motor imagery in children with unilateral spastic cerebral palsy: challenges for the future of pediatric rehabilitation. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55, 43–46. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12306
Stevens, J. A., and Stoykov, M. E. (2003). Using motor imagery in the rehabilitation of hemiparesis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84, 1090–1092. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(03)00042-X
Stone, K. D., Kandula, M., Keizer, A., and Dijkerman, H. C. (2018). Peripersonal space boundaries around the lower limbs. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 161–173. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5115-0
Vrieling, A. H., Van Keeken, H. G., Schoppen, T., Otten, E., Halbertsma, J. P., Hof, A. L., et al. (2008). Gait initiation in lower limb amputees. Gait Posture 27, 423–430. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.05.013
Wuehr, M., Schniepp, R., Pradhan, C., Ilmberger, J., Strupp, M., Brandt, T., et al. (2013). Differential effects of absent visual feedback control on gait variability during different locomotion speeds. Exp. Brain Res. 224, 287–294. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3310-6
Yamamoto, S., and Kushiro, K. (2014). Direction-dependent differences in temporal kinematics for vertical prehension movements. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 703–711. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3783-y
Keywords: motor image, motor planning, eyes close (EC), walking, step length, foot height
Citation: Oku K, Tanaka S, Nishizaki Y, Fukada C and Kida N (2025) Impact of body image on the kinematics of gait initiation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 19:1560138. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1560138
Received: 13 January 2025; Accepted: 27 February 2025;
Published: 17 March 2025.
Edited by:
Jean Blouin, CNRS, FranceReviewed by:
Marie Fabre, Université Jean Monnet, FranceCopyright © 2025 Oku, Tanaka, Nishizaki, Fukada and Kida. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Noriyuki Kida, a2lkYUBraXQuYWMuanA=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.