Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 November 2024
Sec. Speech and Language

25-year neuroimaging research on spoken language processing: a bibliometric analysis

Yuxuan Zheng,
Yuxuan Zheng1,2*Boning ZhangBoning Zhang3
  • 1School of Interpreting and Translation, Beijing International Studies University, Beijing, China
  • 2AI and Cognition Laboratory, Beijing International Studies University, Beijing, China
  • 3School of English Studies, Beijing International Studies University, Beijing, China

Introduction: Spoken language processing is of huge interest to cognitive and neural scientists, as it is the dominant channel for everyday verbal communication. The aim of this study is to depict the dynamics of publications in the field of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing between 2000 and 2024.

Methods: A bibliometric analysis was conducted to probe this particular subject matter based on data retrieved from Web of Science. A total of 8,085 articles were found, which were analyzed together with their authors, journals of publication, citations and countries of origin.

Results: Results showed a steady increase of publication volume and a relatively high academic visibility of this research field indexed by total citations in the first 25 years of the 21st century. Maps of frequent keywords, institutional collaboration network show that cooperations mainly happen between institutions in the United States, the United Kingdom and Germany. Future trends based on burst detection predict that classification, Alzheimer’s disease and oscillations are potential hot topics.

Discussion: Possible reasons for the result include the aging of the population in developed countries, and the rapid growth of artificial intelligence in the past decade. Finally, specific research avenues were proposed which might benefit future studies.

1 Introduction

Neuroimaging techniques have been proved an extremely useful tool for linguistic research (cf. Gernsbacher and Kaschak, 2003), and have fruited interesting and important findings that furthered our knowledge of the neural underpinnings of language. For example, early neuroimaging studies showed that learning another language leads to the density increment of grey matter in the left inferior parietal cortex, which is mediated by language proficiency and age of acquisition (Mechelli et al., 2004), that the left posterior temporal lobe and the inferior parietal lobe are consistently activated during word retrieval (Warburton et al., 1996), and that post-stroke language recovery relies heavily on the right inferior frontal cortex (Perani et al., 2003). Such findings not only illuminated the quest for the exact brain structures that occasion language, but also bore practical significance regarding the treatment of aphasia. Neuroimaging research on language has now become even more vibrant as imaging techniques advance swiftly.

That notwithstanding, the landscape of neuroimaging research on language is not facile to depict due to the miscellanea within this discipline. There seems to be quite a few contrasting paradigms, if not dichotomies, in this research field, such as the temporal-versus-spatial technique selection (e.g., EEG vs. fMRI), the auditory -versus- articulatory mode of experimentation, the word-versus-sentence stimuli presentation, and the healthy-versus-clinical participant sampling. It is therefore necessary to narrow down the scope before any topic can be reviewed in the area of neuroimaging language research. As far as mode is concerned, research on language processing runs the whole gamut where one end is focused on auditory processing while the other attaches huge importance to articulatory processing. The current paper aims to present a panoramic view of neuroimaging research on spoken/articulatory language processing in the first 25 years of the 21st century across the spectrum of imaging techniques, irrespective of the stimuli selection and sampling population, using the method of bibliometric analysis.

In the field of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing, a model was proposed that attributes human speech production to a dorsal stream in the brain, involving such brain regions as inferior parietal and posterior frontal lobe, as opposed to a ventral stream responsible for speech comprehension that mainly taps the middle and inferior temporal cortices (Hickok and Poeppel, 2004, 2007). To solve this conundrum, different neuroimaging techniques have been employed to investigate auditory and articulatory language processing, including fMRI (Behroozmand et al., 2015; Correia et al., 2020; Hickok et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2012; Riecker et al., 2005), PET (Dufor et al., 2007; Josephs et al., 2010; Schulz et al., 2005; Utianski et al., 2018; Vanlancker-Sidtis et al., 2003), EEG (Antons et al., 2012; Fiedler et al., 2017; Hausfeld et al., 2012; Kühnis et al., 2013; Zoefel and VanRullen, 2016), fNIRS (Defenderfer et al., 2021; Lawrence et al., 2018; Walsh et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2018) etc. Although some studies found that the processing of auditory and spoken language (or overt articulation) activates overlapping brain structures (Papathanassiou et al., 2000), both on the word level and on the sentence level (Price, 2012; Tremblay et al., 2013), the preponderance of evidence seems to indicate separate systems for speech perception and production.

As speaking is an oral activity consisting of tightly coordinated stages (Levelt, 1989), imaging evidences abound that reveal different mechanisms underlying these stages. For instance, it has been shown that speech motor control relies on the caudate nucleus-cerebellum circuit modulated by phonological complexity (Sörös et al., 2006), while the planning for articulation involves the left precentral gyrus of the insula (Dronkers, 1996). In another aspect, neuroimaging findings show that the neural underpinnings of speech production are modulated by diverse linguistic traits. On the word level, the anterior cingulate cortex subserves the monitoring of ongoing word production (Christoffels et al., 2007), and non-word repetition of a novel language incurs the extra activation of the inferior frontal gyrus and the left anterior insula, as compared to the repetition of native language (Moser et al., 2009). The rate of syllabic-level speech production is mirrored in the activation of such motor brain structures as the cerebellum, the primary motor cortex and the thalamus (Riecker et al., 2005; Wildgruber et al., 2001). On the sentence level, compared to jaw and tongue movements, self-initiated sentential speech activates the superior temporal gyri in both hemispheres, and in particular, production of propositional speech relative to non-propositional speech elicits extensive medial and lateral activities (Dhanjal et al., 2008). Geranmayeh et al. (2014) went a step further to try to distinguish overt production of propositional speech from other tasks such as counting and decision with fMRI. Their experiments revealed a left lateralized fronto-temporal–parietal network unique to sentential speech production, partly echoing their previous findings comparing speech and tongue movement (Geranmayeh et al., 2012). Moreover, compared with monolinguals, speaking English as L2 has been found to correlate with increases in fractional anisotropy in more posterior left hemisphere white matter regions (Kuhl et al., 2016). Clinically, neuroimaging research has also fruited key findings for the understanding of the pathology of stuttering (cf. Garnett et al., 2019) and aphasia (Fridriksson et al., 2012; Thompson and den Ouden, 2008). For example, experiments on post-acute aphasia patients using CT and MRI showed that the traditional dorsal speech production route could be extended further, covering more specific brain structures from the supramarginal gyrus through inferior postcentral and precentral sensorimotor regions to premotor cortical regions (Mirman et al., 2015). Given the many findings above, it is not difficult to summarize that the neural mechanisms of spoken language processing are a huge network where diversity outstrips commonality, although some brain structures, such as the superior temporal gyri, have been repeatedly found to be activated across different spoken language experiments. Such disparity might result from different techniques and designs between studies, and calls for further exploration.

Bibliometric analysis (BA) is a quantitative method that comprehensively evaluates the quality of academic journals, the contribution of researchers, the collaboration between institutions and/or countries etc., using statistics such as citations, and software such as CiteSpace (Chen, 2014). BA can reveal not only existing patterns of academic publications in a research field, but also the prospective trends in that discipline. BA has been widely employed to assess linguistic studies (e.g., Guo, 2022; Sun and Lan, 2021; Villalobos et al., 2022; Zhang, 2020) though, it is surprising to know that lacunae of mode-specific BA stand out in the realm of linguistic research, i.e., there is no BA of neuroimaging research on spoken and visual language processing, respectively, in existing literature. This necessitates and justifies the current analysis. Specifically, the current research aims to answer the following three questions: (1) what was the general trend in the first 25 years after 2000 with respect to the publications of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing? (2) what was the collaboration dynamics in this field? (3) what is projected to be the buzzwords in the future?

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design and data selection

Neuroimaging relevant literature on spoken language processing data were retrieved from Web of Science Core Collection (WoSCC), which is one of the most commonly used database and could provide reliable information for bibliometric analysis (Falagas et al., 2008). Four editions were selected including the Science Citation Index Expanded (SCI-expanded), the Social Sciences Citation Index (SSCI), the Arts & Humanities Citation Index (AHCI), and the Emerging Sources Citations Index (ESCI), as neuroimaging language studies are highly interdisciplinary. The query was limited to the topic field, including search title, abstract, author keywords, and keywords plus. The search terms were as follows: (Brain imaging OR neuroimaging OR MRI OR magnetic resonance imaging OR EEG OR electroencephalogram OR MEG OR Magnetoencephalography OR fNIRS OR Functional near-infrared spectroscopy OR PET OR Positron emission tomography OR DTI OR Diffusion tensor imaging OR CT OR computed tomography OR ERP OR event related potential OR Single-photon emission computed tomography OR SPECT OR Diffuse optical imaging OR DOI) AND (Language OR Linguistics) AND (Oral OR Speak OR Spoke OR Speech).

In addition, to make results more precise, the search was limited to the neuroimaging relevant subjects. The publication language was limited to English, and publication type was limited to original articles and reviews. All the searches were conducted on 24 Oct, 2024 with an initial time from 2000. Publication data were exported as “Plain Text File” with the “full record and cited references” for analysis in bibliometric software.

2.2 Data analysis

Text files extracted from Web of Science were then imported into three bibliometric programs in order to examine the development and status of relevant research. Biblioshiny is a widely used web interface based on R program to conduct bibliometric analysis, which extracts the features of publications across four different level metrics: countries, journals, institutions, and authors. First, raw data (in zip format) were imported into Biblioshiny. Then the raw data were filtered with default values, in agreement with those in the initial data selection stage. After that, Biblioshiny returned results of annual scientific production (shown in Figure 1) and most global cited documents (shown in Table 1) from 2000 to 2024. VOSviewer is another program to construct and view bibliometric maps (van Eck and Waltman, 2010), and could generate maps of institutions and countries based on co-authorship data, as well as maps of keywords based on co-occurrence data. In VOSviewer, a map based on bibliographic data was created by selecting “co-occurrence” and “author keywords” as the analysis types and “full counting” as the counting method. Then the minimum number of co-occurrence of a keyword was set to be 25 for thresholding. Altogether 171 keywords were above this threshold and thus were kept. For each of the 171 keywords, the total strength of the cooccurrence links with other keywords was calculated. The keywords with the greatest total link strength were kept. Finally, 30 keywords were selected for visualization (shown in Figure 2). Such procedure was then repeated for producing collaboration results, with “co-authorship” and “countries” replacing “co-occurrence” and “author keywords” respectively in the parameter setting. Forty documents were chosen as the inclusion threshold for a country to appear on the collaboration map, and a total of 15 countries were finally visualized (shown in Figure 3). Lastly, the same procedure was applied to producing the institutional collaboration map, with the only difference being the parameter “organizations” rather than “countries.” As a result, 50 institutions were included in the map (shown in Figure 4). CiteSpace is a bibliometric program for calculating references with the strongest citation bursts. In CiteSpace, data were categorized according to the co-occurring author keywords and keywords plus, and the k value in the g-index was set as 2 to extract a moderate size network. Finally, results of keywords with the strongest citation bursts and document co-citation analysis were visualized (shown in Figures 5, 6 respectively).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Number of publications by year (blue dot-dash line) and the trend of publication (orange line).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Top 10 most-cited papers in the area of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing from 2000 to 2024.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Density visualization of keywords co-occurrence.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Overlays visualization of country collaboration of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing (2014-2017).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Network visualization of institutional collaboration in the field of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Top 9 keywords with the strongest citation bursts in neuroimaging research on spoken language processing generated by Citespace.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Results of the cluster-based document co-citation analysis (DCA).

3 Results

A total number of 8,085 articles were retrieved spanning the first 25 years of the 21st century. Figure 1 shows the dynamics of total annual publications over the period of 2000–2024, and the projected volume till 2030 based on a linear model (the orange line in Figure 1). The annual publication curve presents a roughly spiral growth of research production from 2000 to 2024, and the number of total publications across such period almost quadrupled. The linear fitting has a very high explanatory power for the data with an R2 value of 0.956. According to the linear model, there will be over 500 publications in the area of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing in 2025, almost 5 times more than the number in 2000, indicating a lasting heat on this specific research branch.

Table 1 lists the top 10 most cited academic papers on the topic of neuroimaging research of spoken language processing, with relevant information of the publication such as the source journal, first author and number of total citations. First, all of the 10 articles have been cited for at least 1,100 times, showing the vast interest aroused by the topic of neuroimaging spoken language processing. NeuroImage is the journal that publishes the most number of highly cited papers, followed by Cognition. Clearly, most of the highly-cited papers are review articles. Eight of the 10 papers are published before 2010, and no article after 2012 enters the top 10 most-cited list, which might suggest that this research avenue lacks substantial breakthrough after 2012.

Figure 2 is a density visualization that presents the most frequently used keywords in neuroimaging research on spoken language processing, the warmer the color, the higher the frequency. First, as an umbrella term, language is the most frequently used keyword in the existing research literature, accompanied by fMRI in one common co-occurrence network that also includes neuroimaging. Following that is the second most popular keyword cluster of ERPs, together with EEG and speech perception that forms another relatively large co-occurrence network. Obviously, the second and third clusters are technique-related keyword networks. Ranking third is the word speech with some peripheral words such as development and auditory cortex. Other smaller clusters are dominated by such keywords as aphasia, mmn (mismatch negativity) and N400. Results of the keywords cooccurrence analysis demonstrate that among all imaging techniques fMRI is the most popular one, and that neuroimaging spoken language research has been implemented more on clinical populations than on healthy participants.

Figure 3 portrays the contribution by and collaboration between different countries (top 15) over the period of 2014–2017 in the field of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing. The size of the node represents the production volume of a country, while the color of the node indicates the time of publication from that country. In terms of total publication frequencies, USA comes first with 3,181 publications, followed by Germany (1179) and UK (1041). Other countries with over 500 publications include China, France, Netherlands and Canada. With respect to the time of publication, countries such as UK, USA and Germany have had a relatively longer history of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing, whereas China, Australia and Spain are newly into this field although all have produced sizeable amount of articles. USA is the hub for academic collaborations with 1,550 internationally co-authored publications. Note that the time line only shows the collaboration dynamics between 2014 and 2017 in order to achieve the best contrastive visual effect. When the timeline was stretched to cover a 5-year period anywhere between 2000 and 2024, the contrast as manifested by line colors would be washed out, indicating an interesting fact that cooperations in this field saw intensive new dynamics between 2014 and 2017 and were in a quite stable state either before 2014 or after 2017.

Figure 4 illustrates the academic cooperation between global institutions. The whole collaboration network is characterized by seven clusters. The green cluster dominated by University College London and Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences shows the cooperation mainly between British and German academic institutions, with a fairly large share of contributions from other universities in these two countries such as Oxford University and Humboldt University. As is shown by the red and yellow nodes, US universities form two big clusters characterized by more domestic coordination than international partnership, albeit some connections with Chinese institutions (e.g., Beijing Normal University) and British ones (e.g., King’s College London). In contrast, Canadian universities marked with purple nodes maintained extensive relations with other institutions across continents. There is also a small orange cluster consisting of three Dutch institutions, i.e., Radboud University Nijmegen, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, and Maastricht University. It seems that Dutch academic units are a bit more self-reliant than their counterparts in other countries, especially Canada. Lastly, there are two minor networks showing the collaboration of Finnish, Spanish and Swiss universities.

Figure 5 shows the top 9 keywords that have had a surging appearance during a specific period of time between 2000 and 2024 using burst detection. It is conspicuous that in the early noughties research was characterized by specific neuroimaging techniques like PET, fMRI and Wada test, together with such explanatory terms as localization and cerebral blood flow. Among them, localization had the longest and most enduring popularity spanning the period of 2000–2011. After that the research hotspot transitioned to temporal lobe epilepsy, a common brain disease in the neuroimaging research literature. From 2016 onwards, the concept of classification, Alzheimer’s disease and oscillations attracted academic attention from the neuroimaging spoken language research community. In particular, oscillations showed a very high citation strength since 2020, still trending and are projected to be intriguing topics in the future.

Figure 6 shows the result of document co-citation analysis (DCA) across the first quarter of the 21st century. Altogether 8 co-citation clusters were identified, the largest of which was labelled arcuate fasciculus (cluster 0), followed by mismatch negativity (cluster 1). Less strong but more recent were the themes of neural tracking (cluster 2) and post-stroke aphasia (cluster 3), which were also situated in the farther position on the DCA map, indicating a relatively high independence of such themes. After that clusters 4, 5 and 6 were closely connected to clusters 1 and 2, with theme labels of using fMRI, cortical organization and word production, respectively. Lastly, cluster 7 reflected the early co-citation network under the theme of phonological processing, which was not so strong perhaps due to obsolescence. The modularity Q = 0.745 and silhouette S = 0.918 show that the clustering has very high validity and reliability.

4 Discussion

A bibliometric analysis of the first 25 years of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing was implemented based on data retrieved from Web of Science. Results show that the total number of publications in this area has been growing steadily and will continue to grow until 2030, that it has a relatively high academic visibility with its top 10 papers all having over 1,100 citations, that research in this area is largely implemented with fMRI and EEG, that academic collaborations mainly happen among European, North American and East Asian countries/institutions, and that potential hot topics in the future may include such keywords as classification, Alzheimer’s disease and oscillations.

The reason behind the publication boom from 2000 to 2024 might be threefold. In a sense speaking takes precedence over writing in linguistics (for a comprehensive review on the relationship between written and spoken language cf. Olson, 1996), and therefore spoken language processing is of great interest to linguists, as evidenced by the fact that early attempts which gave birth to the discipline of neurolinguistics were all based on spoken language processing (e.g., Broca, 1861; Wernicke, 1874). The fast development and the wider application of various neuroimaging techniques in academic research in the 21st century accounts for another big share of publication increase in neuroimaging research on spoken language processing (cf. Scott, 2021). Lastly, the mode of spoken language processing has clinical significance, i.e., speaking has been included in test batteries or used as a screening method for diseases of various kinds, such as stroke (e.g., Breier et al., 2008; Ilves et al., 2014; Sreedharan et al., 2019; Sreedharan et al., 2020), Parkinson’s disease (Dick et al., 2018; Hyder et al., 2021; Lee and Van Lancker Sidtis, 2020), Alzheimer’s disease (Deters et al., 2017; Pistono et al., 2021; Radanovic et al., 2009) and autism spectrum disorder (Arnold et al., 2009; Ghaziuddin and Gerstein, 1996; Volden, 2004), among both young populations (Jarrold et al., 2013; Sah and Torng, 2015; Wu et al., 2020) and senior participants (Rusz et al., 2013; Tjaden et al., 2013; Yorkston et al., 2017).

In terms of the most cited papers in this research area as listed in Table 1, the results are quite revealing as the papers with the most citations are also the most significant ones in pushing forward the understanding of the neural mechanisms of spoken language processing. For example, Kutas and Federmeier (2011) disentangled the many functions of the N400 ERP component in language processing, and reorganized previous empirical findings with a clear categorization that includes discourse processing, predictive processing, word recognition, bilingualism, semantic memory, recognition memory etc., providing a comprehensive framework for later studies to refer to. Similarly, an early review by Friederici (2002) has been cited for over a thousand times for its ground-breaking proposal of the neural basis for auditory sentence processing that mainly involves the bilateral temporo-frontal network including the left anterior STG for syntactic processing, the left MTG for semantic processing and the right posterior STG for prosodic processing. In a word, the top 10 most cited papers are also the most influential ones in this research avenue, in a sense confirming the effectiveness of citation analysis. Note however, the highly cited papers in the table are all review articles and no empirical or methodological paper was present. This could be a BA-induced bias, and warrants caution in terms of generalization of the analysis.

Another notable point in the results is the prediction that classification, Alzheimer’s disease and in particular, oscillations, are the trending topics in the research domain of neuroimaging experiments on language. The three trending topics are intertwined to some extent, with oscillations lying in the central position. First of all, classification is a process of obtaining specific EEG features after artifact removal and feature extraction, for the purpose of automatic application of EEG signals in practical settings such as brain-computer interface (BCI) (Craik et al., 2019). The rising popularity of the buzzword classification to some degree was occasioned by the booming development of artificial intelligence in the past decade, as manifested by the many deep learning algorithms and approaches employed in EEG classification (cf. He et al., 2021; Rabcan et al., 2020). Additionally, the trending of classification also echoes cluster 2 (neural tracking) on the DCA map, as the former can be a means to the latter (Lotte et al., 2018). Second, the mounting population of senior citizens in developed countries gave rise to the rapid growth of Alzheimer’s disease in recent years, the prediction and modeling of which is closely connected with neural oscillations (Goriely et al., 2020; Jafari et al., 2020). In fact, neural oscillations has been found to be a crucial index of a broader range of brain diseases (Weiss et al., 2023), which is in line with the result of DCA (cluster 3 post-stroke aphasia that is trending now). Lastly, the emerging hot topic of oscillations is possibly propelled by two reasons. In one aspect, oscillations have long been identified as the moderator for cognition (Ward, 2003), in another they serve and parallel the current speech processing technology advancement (Poeppel and Assaneo, 2020).

The fruitful results aforementioned notwithstanding, one stream of research that has been quite popular yet not captured by the current analysis, is language studies simultaneously using two or more imaging techniques (such as synchronous EEG-fMRI and/or EEG-NIRs). Such multimodal approach is not only necessary but also feasible (Wallois et al., 2012). However, so far it has not been used in spoken language processing studies, possibly due to technical difficulties related to the imaging techniques. Therefore it is quite promising to combine temporal and spatial imaging techniques in spoken language processing studies in the future.

5 Limitations

Despite the many revealing networks of collaboration between authors, academic institutions and countries presented, the current research has several limitations. First, the field of neuroimaging research is heavily reliant on the financial conditions of an institution or a country, as a set of EEG equipment costs at least tens of thousands of dollars and that of fMRI, millions. It is therefore reasonable to find the preponderance of neuroimaging research literature was produced in developed countries, while developing and underdeveloped countries could not afford such facilities. Consequently, the inequality in accessing the imaging techniques led to the bias of country/institution production in the BA results. Second, the method of BA per se is not without flaws. The variability in the searching process, e.g., difference in the types of publications selected (i.e., journal articles, conference papers or other forms) and languages covered could result in missing data, hence influencing the final analysis. Lastly, different analyses on the same subject matter may yield different results due to the discrepancy of underlying algorithms (e.g., Kleminski et al., 2022), an example of which is the citation-based article ranking that does not present empirically transformative research. This also adds to the inconsistency and bias of BA results.

6 Conclusion

Bibliometric analysis of the first 25 years of academic journal publications in the field of neuroimaging research on spoken language processing presents that, with fMRI and EEG being the most popular research techniques, this field is right in the spotlight. It receives attention particularly from NeuroImage, and institutions in the United States, the United Kingdom and Germany. In the future more efforts are likely to be directed to probing such keywords as Alzheimer’s disease and oscillations.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: www.webofscience.com.

Author contributions

YZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Software, Writing review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The research was financed by Beijing Social Science Fund (23YYC016). Yuxuan Zheng applied for the fund.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Antons, J.-N., Schleicher, R., Arndt, S., Moller, S., Porbadnigk, A. K., and Curio, G. (2012). Analyzing speech quality perception using electroencephalography. IEEE J. Select. Topics Signal Process. 6, 721–731. doi: 10.1109/JSTSP.2012.2191936

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., and Diehl, J. J. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition 110, 131–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Behroozmand, R., Shebek, R., Hansen, D. R., Oya, H., Robin, D. A., Howard, M. A. III, et al. (2015). Sensory–motor networks involved in speech production and motor control: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 109, 418–428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breier, J. I., Hasan, K. M., Zhang, W., Men, D., and Papanicolaou, A. C. (2008). Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 29, 483–487. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0846

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broca, P. (1861). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Bull. Soc. Anat. 6, 330–357.

Google Scholar

Chen, C. (2014). The citespace manual. College Comput. Inform. 1, 1–84.

Google Scholar

Christoffels, I. K., Formisano, E., and Schiller, N. O. (2007). Neural correlates of verbal feedback processing: an fMRI study employing overt speech. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 868–879. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, J. M., Caballero-Gaudes, C., Guediche, S., and Carreiras, M. (2020). Phonatory and articulatory representations of speech production in cortical and subcortical fMRI responses. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61435-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, A., He, Y., and Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2019). Deep learning for electroencephalogram (EEG) classification tasks: a review. J. Neural Eng. 16:031001. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab0ab5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Defenderfer, J., Forbes, S., Wijeakumar, S., Hedrick, M., Plyler, P., and Buss, A. T. (2021). Frontotemporal activation differs between perception of simulated cochlear implant speech and speech in background noise: an image-based fNIRS study. NeuroImage 240:118385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deters, K. D., Nho, K., Risacher, S. L., Kim, S., Ramanan, V. K., Crane, P. K., et al. (2017). Genome-wide association study of language performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Lang. 172, 22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.04.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhanjal, N. S., Handunnetthi, L., Patel, M. C., and Wise, R. J. (2008). Perceptual systems controlling speech production. J. Neurosci. 28, 9969–9975. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2607-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, J., Fredrick, J., Man, G., Huber, J. E., and Lee, J. (2018). Sentence production in Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 32, 804–822. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2018.1444791

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dronkers, N. F. (1996). A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384, 159–161. doi: 10.1038/384159a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., and Démonet, J.-F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: a PET study. NeuroImage 34, 1692–1707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Falagas, M. E., Pitsouni, E. I., Malietzis, G. A., and Pappas, G. (2008). Comparison of PubMed, Scopus, web of science, and Google scholar: strengths and weaknesses. FASEB J. 22, 338–342. doi: 10.1096/fj.07-9492LSF

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiedler, L., Wöstmann, M., Graversen, C., Brandmeyer, A., Lunner, T., and Obleser, J. (2017). Single-channel in-ear-EEG detects the focus of auditory attention to concurrent tone streams and mixed speech. J. Neural Eng. 14:036020. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa66dd

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridriksson, J., Hubbard, H. I., Hudspeth, S. G., Holland, A. L., Bonilha, L., Fromm, D., et al. (2012). Speech entrainment enables patients with Broca’s aphasia to produce fluent speech. Brain 135, 3815–3829. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws301

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnett, E. O. D., Chow, H. M., Choo, A. L., and Chang, S. E. (2019). Stuttering severity modulates effects of non-invasive brain stimulation in adults who stutter. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:411. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geranmayeh, F., Brownsett, S. L., Leech, R., Beckmann, C. F., Woodhead, Z., and Wise, R. J. (2012). The contribution of the inferior parietal cortex to spoken language production. Brain Lang. 121, 47–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geranmayeh, F., Wise, R. J., Mehta, A., and Leech, R. (2014). Overlapping networks engaged during spoken language production and its cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 34, 8728–8740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0428-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gernsbacher, M. A., and Kaschak, M. P. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of language production and comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 91–114. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145128

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghaziuddin, M., and Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic speaking style differentiates Asperger syndrome from high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 26, 585–595. doi: 10.1007/BF02172348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goriely, A., Kuhl, E., and Bick, C. (2020). Neuronal oscillations on evolving networks: dynamics, damage, degradation, decline, dementia, and death. Phys. Rev. Lett. 125:128102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.125.128102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X. (2022). A bibliometric analysis of child language during 1900-2021. Front. Psychol. 13:862042. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.862042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausfeld, L., De Martino, F., Bonte, M., and Formisano, E. (2012). Pattern analysis of EEG responses to speech and voice: influence of feature grouping. NeuroImage 59, 3641–3651. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

He, C., Liu, J., Zhu, Y., and Du, W. (2021). Data augmentation for deep neural networks model in EEG classification task: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:765525. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.765525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., Buchsbaum, B., Humphries, C., and Muftuler, T. (2003). Auditory–motor interaction revealed by fMRI: speech, music, and working memory in area Spt. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 673–682. doi: 10.1162/089892903322307393

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition 92, 67–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyder, R., Jensen, M., Højlund, A., Kimppa, L., Bailey, C. J., Schaldemose, J. L., et al. (2021). Functional connectivity of spoken language processing in early-stage Parkinson’s disease: an MEG study. NeuroImage 32:102718. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102718

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilves, P., Tomberg, T., Kepler, J., Laugesaar, R., Kaldoja, M. L., Kepler, K., et al. (2014). Different plasticity patterns of language function in children with perinatal and childhood stroke. J. Child Neurol. 29, 756–764. doi: 10.1177/0883073813489350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jafari, Z., Kolb, B. E., and Mohajerani, M. H. (2020). Neural oscillations and brain stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 194:101878. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101878

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarrold, W., Mundy, P., Gwaltney, M., Bailenson, J., Hatt, N., McIntyre, N., et al. (2013). Social attention in a virtual public speaking task in higher functioning children with autism. Autism Res. 6, 393–410. doi: 10.1002/aur.1302

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Josephs, K. A., Duffy, J. R., Fossett, T. R., Strand, E. A., Claassen, D. O., Whitwell, J. L., et al. (2010). Fluorodeoxyglucose F18 positron emission tomography in progressive apraxia of speech and primary progressive aphasia variants. Arch. Neurol. 67, 596–605. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.78

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleminski, R., Kazienko, P., and Kajdanowicz, T. (2022). Analysis of direct citation, co-citation and bibliographic coupling in scientific topic identification. Journal of Information Science, 48, 349–373. doi: 10.1177/0165551520962775

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhl, P. K., Stevenson, J., Corrigan, N. M., van den Bosch, J. J., Can, D. D., and Richards, T. (2016). Neuroimaging of the bilingual brain: structural brain correlates of listening and speaking in a second language. Brain Lang. 162, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.07.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnis, J., Elmer, S., Meyer, M., and Jäncke, L. (2013). The encoding of vowels and temporal speech cues in the auditory cortex of professional musicians: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia 51, 1608–1618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, R. J., Wiggins, I. M., Anderson, C. A., Davies-Thompson, J., and Hartley, D. E. (2018). Cortical correlates of speech intelligibility measured using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Hear. Res. 370, 53–64. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y.-S., Turkeltaub, P., Granger, R., and Raizada, R. D. (2012). Categorical speech processing in Broca's area: an fMRI study using multivariate pattern-based analysis. J. Neurosci. 32, 3942–3948. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3814-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B., and Van Lancker Sidtis, D. (2020). Subcortical involvement in formulaic language: studies on bilingual individuals with Parkinson's disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 4029–4045. doi: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Lotte, F., Bougrain, L., Cichocki, A., Clerc, M., Congedo, M., Rakotomamonjy, A., et al. (2018). A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces: a 10 year update. J. Neural Eng. 15:031005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aab2f2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O'Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431:757. doi: 10.1038/431757a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirman, D., Chen, Q., Zhang, Y., Wang, Z., Faseyitan, O. K., Coslett, H. B., et al. (2015). Neural organization of spoken language revealed by lesion–symptom mapping. Nat. Commun. 6:6762. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7762

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moser, D., Baker, J. M., Sanchez, C. E., Rorden, C., and Fridriksson, J. (2009). Temporal order processing of syllables in the left parietal lobe. J. Neurosci. 29, 12568–12573. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5934-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, D. R. (1996). Towards a psychology of literacy: on the relations between speech and writing. Cognition 60, 83–104. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(96)00705-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papathanassiou, D., Etard, O., Mellet, E., Zago, L., Mazoyer, B., and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2000). A common language network for comprehension and production: a contribution to the definition of language epicenters with PET. NeuroImage 11, 347–357. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0546

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Tettamanti, M., Rosa, M., Scifo, P., Miozzo, A., et al. (2003). A fMRI study of word retrieval in aphasia. Brain Lang. 85, 357–368. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00561-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pistono, A., Senoussi, M., Guerrier, L., Rafiq, M., Giméno, M., Péran, P., et al. (2021). Language network connectivity increases in early Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 82, 447–460. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201584

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poeppel, D., and Assaneo, M. F. (2020). Speech rhythms and their neural foundations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 322–334. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0304-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage 62, 816–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabcan, J., Levashenko, V., Zaitseva, E., and Kvassay, M. (2020). Review of methods for EEG signal classification and development of new fuzzy classification-based approach. IEEE Access 8, 189720–189734. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2020.3031447

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radanovic, M., Diniz, B. S., Mirandez, R. M., Novaretti, T. M., Flacks, M. K., Yassuda, M. S., et al. (2009). Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease among Brazilian Portuguese speakers: the influence of education. Int. Psychogeriatr. 21, 1081–1087. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209990639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riecker, A., Mathiak, K., Wildgruber, D., Erb, M., Hertrich, I., Grodd, W., et al. (2005). fMRI reveals two distinct cerebral networks subserving speech motor control. Neurology 64, 700–706. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152156.90779.89

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusz, J., Cmejla, R., Tykalova, T., Ruzickova, H., Klempir, J., Majerova, V., et al. (2013). Imprecise vowel articulation as a potential early marker of Parkinson's disease: effect of speaking task. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134, 2171–2181. doi: 10.1121/1.4816541

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sah, W.-H., and Torng, P.-C. (2015). Narrative coherence of mandarin-speaking children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: an investigation into causal relations. First Lang. 35, 189–212. doi: 10.1177/0142723715584227

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, G., Varga, M., Jeffires, K., Ludlow, C., and Braun, A. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of human vocalization: an H215O PET study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1835–1847. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, P. J. H. (2021). A picture is worth a thousand words: the power of neuroimaging. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 12, 2553–2554. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.1c00388

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörös, P., Sokoloff, L. G., Bose, A., McIntosh, A. R., Graham, S. J., and Stuss, D. T. (2006). Clustered functional MRI of overt speech production. NeuroImage 32, 376–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreedharan, S., Arun, K., Sylaja, P., and Kesavadas, C. S. (2019). Functional connectivity of language regions of stroke patients with expressive aphasia during real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging based Neurofeedback. Brain Connect. 9, 613–626. doi: 10.1089/brain.2019.0674

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreedharan, S., Chandran, A., Yanamala, V. R., Sylaja, P. N., Kesavadas, C., and Sitaram, R. (2020). Self-regulation of language areas using real-time functional MRI in stroke patients with expressive aphasia. Brain Imaging Behav. 14, 1714–1730. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00106-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Y., and Lan, G. (2021). Research trends in ‘trans-’ studies on writing: a bibliometric analysis. System 103:102640. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2021.102640

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, C. K., and den Ouden, D.-B. (2008). Neuroimaging and recovery of language in aphasia. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 8, 475–483. doi: 10.1007/s11910-008-0076-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tjaden, K., Lam, J., and Wilding, G. (2013). Vowel acoustics in Parkinson's disease and multiple sclerosis: comparison of clear, loud, and slow speaking conditions. J. Speech, Language & Hearing Research 56, 1485–1502. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0259)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., Deschamps, I., and Gracco, V. L. (2013). Regional heterogeneity in the processing and the production of speech in the human planum temporale. Cortex 49, 143–157. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Utianski, R. L., Whitwell, J. L., Schwarz, C. G., Senjem, M. L., Tosakulwong, N., Duffy, J. R., et al. (2018). Tau-PET imaging with [18F] AV-1451 in primary progressive apraxia of speech. Cortex 99, 358–374. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Eck, N. J., and Waltman, L. (2010). Software survey: VOSviewer, a computer program for bibliometric mapping. Scientometrics 84, 523–538. doi: 10.1007/s11192-009-0146-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanlancker-Sidtis, D., McIntosh, A. R., and Grafton, S. (2003). PET activation studies comparing two speech tasks widely used in surgical mapping. Brain Lang. 85, 245–261. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00596-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Villalobos, D., Povedano-Montero, J., Fernández, S., López-Muñoz, F., Pacios, J., and del Río, D. (2022). Scientific research on verbal fluency tests: a bibliometric analysis. J. Neurolinguistics 63:101082. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2022.101082

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Volden, J. (2004). Conversational repair in speakers with autism spectrum disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 39, 171–189. doi: 10.1080/13682820410001663252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallois, F., Mahmoudzadeh, M., Patil, A., and Grebe, R. (2012). Usefulness of simultaneous EEG–NIRS recording in language studies. Brain Lang. 121, 110–123. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, B., Tian, F., Tourville, J., Yücel, M., Kuczek, T., and Bostian, A. (2017). Hemodynamics of speech production: an fNIRS investigation of children who stutter. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04357-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warburton, E., Wise, R. J., Price, C. J., Weiller, C., Hadar, U., Ramsay, S., et al. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects studies with PET. Brain 119, 159–179. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 553–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, E., Kann, M., and Wang, Q. (2023). Neuromodulation of neural oscillations in health and disease. Biology 12:371. doi: 10.3390/biology12030371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex: eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslau: Cohn & Weigert.

Google Scholar

Wildgruber, D., Ackermann, H., and Grodd, W. (2001). Differential contributions of motor cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum to speech motor control: effects of syllable repetition rate evaluated by fMRI. NeuroImage 13, 101–109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0672

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H., Lu, F., Yu, B., and Liu, Q. (2020). Phonological acquisition and development in Putonghua-speaking children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 34, 844–860. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2019.1702720

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorkston, K., Baylor, C., and Britton, D. (2017). Speech versus speaking: the experiences of people with Parkinson's disease and implications for intervention. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 26, 561–568. doi: 10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X. (2020). A bibliometric analysis of second language acquisition between 1997 and 2018. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 42, 199–222. doi: 10.1017/S0272263119000573

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Zhou, Y., and Yuan, J. (2018). Speech prosodies of different emotional categories activate different brain regions in adult cortex: an fNIRS study. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18683-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoefel, B., and VanRullen, R. (2016). EEG oscillations entrain their phase to high-level features of speech sound. NeuroImage 124, 16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuroimaging, spoken language processing, bibliometric analysis, Alzheimer's disease, oscillations

Citation: Zheng Y and Zhang B (2024) 25-year neuroimaging research on spoken language processing: a bibliometric analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1461505. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1461505

Received: 08 July 2024; Accepted: 11 November 2024;
Published: 27 November 2024.

Edited by:

Xi-Nian Zuo, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Siti Atiyah Ali, University of Malaysia Sarawak, Malaysia
Mani Abdul Karim, XIM University, India

Copyright © 2024 Zheng and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuxuan Zheng, enl4QGJpc3UuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.