Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 June 2024
Sec. Speech and Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Methods in Speech and Language: 2023 View all 7 articles

A mapping-knowledge-domain analysis of ERP research on language processing

  • Center for Linguistics and Applied Linguistics, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou, Guangdong, China

The event-related potentials (ERPs) technique represents a newly developed methodology in cognitive neuroscience and has significantly extended the scope of linguistic studies, offering valuable insights into cognitive processes related to language. While extant literature reviews have addressed specific facets of ERP research on language processing, a comprehensive overview of this domain remains notably absent. This study aims to fill this gap by pioneering a mapping-knowledge-domain analysis of ERP research on language processing using Citespace, a visualized bibliometric software. The current study conducted a meticulous survey and evaluation of relevant literature extracted from the Web of Science core collection. Initially, this study outlines the spatial-temporal distribution within this domain. Subsequently, employing document co-citation analysis, keyword co-occurrence analysis, cluster analysis, and burst detection analysis, this study delved deeper into the research landscape. Findings reveal that key areas in ERP research on language processing predominantly focus on sentence comprehension, reading comprehension, and mismatch negativity, with notable emphasis on topics such as speech perception, temporal dynamics, and working memory. The current study advocates for future investigations to concentrate on larger linguistic units, explore the integration of ERP components and their functional significance, and scrutinize individual differences among participants. These directions are imperative for advancing the understanding of language processing mechanisms.

1 Introduction

The human brain continuously generates neural activities, eliciting electrical signals that can be recorded using scalp electrodes. These signals correspond to individuals’ mental states and manifest as brain-wave patterns, recorded by electroencephalograms (EEGs). EEG plots offer information regarding amplitude, wavelength, and frequency of electrical signals. However, an excess of background information in EEGs hinders accuracy, limiting the detailed analysis of the brain’s response to specific stimuli. To address this limitation, researchers have turned to event-related potentials (ERPs) as a solution. ERPs encompass the brain’s electrical activity linked to specific sensory, cognitive, and motor events (Luck, 2014: 4). ERPs are obtained by recording EEG signals within defined time windows corresponding to stimuli. Typically, ERPs involve averaging across multiple trials to minimize the impact of extraneous factors and accentuate the primary effects of stimulus events (Gonzalez-Marquez et al., 2007: 405). Components of ERPs are denoted based on their polarities (negative or positive) and latencies (the time at which they peak after stimulus onset). For instance, the term “P300” designates a positive component occurring 300 ms after stimulus onset.

Modern research on ERPs commenced in 1964 when Gray Walter and his research team identified the first cognitive ERP component, termed the Contingent Negative Variation (CNV) (Walter et al., 1964). Since then, various ERP components have been extensively studied within the framework of language processing, including both written language comprehension and oral speech perception.

Among these, the N400 stands as a seminal contribution in linguistic research. Discovered in 1980 by Kutas and Hillyard, the N400 has emerged as one of the most crucial ERP components associated with language processing. It derives its nomenclature from its occurrence between 200 and 300 ms following stimulus onset, peaking around 400 ms. Kutas and Hillyard’s (1980) study initially linked this component to semantic violation. Over time, the N400 has been widely acknowledged as a measure of semantic processing (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011; Beres, 2017). Recent investigations have further elucidated the effects of the N400, encompassing auditory materials and extending to non-linguistic stimuli (Kutas et al., 1987; Strijkers and Costa, 2011).

Another significant ERP component, the P600, is intensively associated with syntactic processing. Initially detected in syntactically unacceptable or unpredictable sentences (Osterhout and Holcomb, 1992; Hagoort et al., 1993), the P600 subsequently manifested in sentences with intricate structures (Kaan et al., 2000). Thus, it can be inferred that the P600 primarily gauges the re-analysis of grammar or syntax.

The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) is a negative peak triggered approximately 100–250 ms after stimulus onset. It is widely acknowledged for its role in spoken language processing. The amplitude of MMN reflects the memory trace of language components in the human brain (Pulvermüller and Shtyrov, 2006). Expected stimuli typically evoke a less pronounced MMN compared to unexpected ones, with pseudo-words evoking a larger MMN than words (Chen et al., 2020).

The ERP technique has been extensively utilized across various dimensions of language processing research. Primarily, it serves as a valuable instrument for exploring the perception or comprehension of linguistic structures at multiple levels, encompassing phonetic (Sucevic et al., 2015; Whitten et al., 2020), lexical (Zhao et al., 2017; Xia and Peng, 2022), syntactic (Mascelloni et al., 2019; Deng and Zhu, 2020), and even discourse analysis (Li et al., 2018; Rasenberg et al., 2020). Furthermore, ERPs contribute significantly to interdisciplinary investigations within the realm of language processing. Researchers have directed their focus toward unraveling the neural mechanisms associated with bilingualism (Strijkers et al., 2010; Misra et al., 2012), emotional processing (Hinojosa et al., 2010; Chwilla et al., 2011), and language impairment (Leppanen et al., 2010; Olichney et al., 2011), among other domains.

Despite the substantial body of empirical studies, conducting comprehensive literature reviews remains important in elucidating the essence and trends within the subject matter. Existing literature reviews have delineated specific aspects of ERP research on language processing. Some have delved into the effects and functionalities of particular ERP components (Kaan et al., 2000; Pulvermüller and Shtyrov, 2006; Kutas and Federmeier, 2011), while others have assessed specific domains within ERP studies in linguistics (Morgan et al., 2020; Silkes and Anjum, 2021). However, a limited number of reviews offer a comprehensive overview of this research (Beres, 2017). Additionally, prior reviews mainly relied on researchers’ intuitions (Vigliocco et al., 2011), which might constrain the inclusion of all pertinent studies on the subject. Hence, the present study undertakes a systematic literature review of ERP research on language processing, employing a quantitative mapping knowledge domain methodology to address this gap.

Systematic reviews play a vital role in amalgamating findings from original studies within a specific research domain. These reviews encapsulate the consensus and knowledge gaps within the field, facilitating an evaluation of its trajectory and future prospects (Chen and Song, 2019). In comparison with conventional literature reviews, systematic reviews adopt a more stringent methodology, thereby yielding more substantial outcomes. Bibliometrics, as a quantitative tool, facilitates a comprehensive analysis of academic literature within a particular research sphere and conducts statistical analyses utilizing appropriate algorithms (Yuan and Sun, 2023). Consequently, mapping-knowledge-domain analysis serves as a means to graphically depict these findings. These visual representations illuminate various facets of the research field, including collaborative networks among scholars, social networks established through co-authorship, citation patterns, and co-citation networks (Chen, 2004). In essence, the process of mapping-knowledge-domain analysis enables the evaluation of extensive literature, particularly in research fields with broad coverage. Presently, mapping-knowledge-domain analysis has found widespread application in elucidating the developmental patterns and trends within various disciplines. For instance, in the realm of linguistics, Ahmed et al. (2022) demonstrated the developmental trajectories and emerging trends within cognitive linguistics.

Few studies were published in 1980s when ERP technique was first applied in linguistic research. However, such studies met with a significant increase as the technique became widely-used, especially after 2000. Therefore, the present study employs bibliometric methods to complete a mapping-knowledge-domain analysis, conducting a systematic review of ERP research on language processing in the past two decades (2002–2022). This investigation aims to scrutinize the developmental trajectory, current status, and potential future directions within this research domain. Specifically, our inquiry revolves around several research questions: (1) What is the temporal-spatial distribution of scholarly works pertaining to ERP research on language processing? (2) Which references, journals, and authors receive the highest citation frequencies within the research domain? (3) What are the prevailing focal areas and frequently debated topics within the field? (4) What prospective directions and potential trends characterize the landscape of ERP research on language processing?

2 Methods

2.1 Data collection

For the systematic review of ERP research on language processing, data were sourced from the Science Citation Index-Expanded (SCIE), the Social Science Citation Index (SSCI), and the Arts & Humanities Citation Index (A&HCI) within the Web of Science (WOS) Core Collection by Clarivate Analytics (2021). The Web of Science repository offers access to premier publications along with their citation data across the natural and social sciences, encompassing a time frame since 1975. Consequently, utilizing the core collection of this database ensures the inclusion of high-quality materials essential for this comprehensive analysis.

In accordance with the delineation and scope of the research domain, a systematic literature retrieval strategy was formulated as follows: Initially, keywords “language” and “event-related potential” were selected as primary retrieval terms, applied within the specified temporal boundary from January 1, 2002, to December 31, 2022. Subsequently, articles and reviews were exclusively considered in the document type, while the language criterion was restricted to English. 3797 publications were retrieved from the database. Following the online retrieval of literature, a meticulous manual screening process was executed based on predefined criteria to ensure the relevance and alignment with the subject matter. Two exclusion criteria are set to complete the screening: (1) Publications unrelated to ERP research on language processing. (2) Publications without abstracts. Records in either of abovementioned conditions were systematically excluded from the dataset. Consequently, a total of 3772 publications meeting the stringent inclusion criteria were identified for subsequent analysis. These selected publications were compiled and exported in a plain text file format, encompassing complete records along with cited references. The step-by-step protocol for data collection is presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Procedure of data collection.

2.2 Data analysis

In this study, a mapping-knowledge-domain analysis within ERP research on language processing was conducted employing Citespace 6.1.R6. Citespace is an information visualization software based on Java programming language. It enables the visualization of the knowledge structure and the identification of potential trends by employing co-citation analysis and pathfinder network scaling algorithms (Chen, 2016). Specifically, Citespace generates two distinct visual representations: one focused on co-citation analysis encompassing references, cited authors, and journals, while the other concentrates on co-occurrence analysis involving keywords, clusters, and citation bursts.

Preceding the formal analysis, duplicate records were eliminated using Citespace. According to the records in the current research, all 3772 publications were valid for the formal analysis. Then, several methods of mapping-knowledge-domain analysis were employed to address the pertinent research inquiries. To address questions (1), a comprehensive synthesis of the retrieved articles was presented, encompassing details such as geographic origins and publication timelines. For question (2), a co-citation analysis of references, cited authors, and journals was undertaken to identify the most frequently cited researchers and their contributions. To elucidate question (3), a co-occurrence analysis of keywords and clusters was executed to visually represent the primary themes encapsulated in the relevant studies. Finally, in addressing question (4), burst detection analysis of keywords was utilized to discern emerging trends across distinct temporal segments.

3 Results

3.1 The spatio-temporal distribution of publications

To explore the temporal distribution of literature pertaining to ERP research on language processing, we analyzed the publication statistics across various years using data obtained from WoS. Figure 2 illustrates the chronological progression of ERP research on language processing from 2002 to 2022.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Temporal distribution of literature in ERP research on language processing.

Figures 3, 4 illustrate the top 10 productive countries/regions and organizations between 2002 and 2022 in the research field. Through the examination of these statistics, a broad comprehension of the geographical distribution of ERP research on language processing can be obtained.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regional distribution of literature in ERP research on language processing.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Productive organizations of literature in ERP research on language processing.

3.2 Co-citation analysis of references, cited authors and cited journals

Co-citation analysis stands as a fundamental technique within bibliometric quantitative studies, designed to uncover the specialized domains within a scientific field (Morris and Van der Veer, 2008). This method often involves the identification of specialized clusters based on the co-citation of individual entities. For instance, Author Co-Citation Analysis (ACA) and Document Co-citation Analysis (DCA) are commonly utilized to delineate prominent researchers and their works, thus elucidating the knowledge network within specific research domains (Chen, 2006; Zhao and Strotmann, 2008; Chen et al., 2010). In this present study, we employed co-citation analysis to unveil the most frequently cited authors, references, and journals. Our analysis applied a 1-year time slice and selected the top 20 entities within each slice as parameters in Citespace. Figures 57 portray the outcomes of this analysis.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Frequently cited authors in ERP research on language processing.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Frequently cited journals in ERP research on language processing.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Frequently cited works in ERP research on language processing.

Figure 5 illustrates the leading authorities in ERP research on language processing. Based on the analysis conducted using Citespace, the top 10 scholars and their corresponding citation counts are as follows: Kutas (1832 citations), Friederici (1033 citations), Hagoort (958 citations), Oldfield (814 citations), Osterhout (785 citations), Näätänen (738 citations), Van Petten (700 citations), Holcomb (601 citations), Federmeier (597 citations), and Kuperberg (548 citations). These esteemed scholars have played instrumental roles in shaping the landscape of ERP research on language processing, contributing significantly to the foundational framework of this field.

Figure 6 delineates the most frequently cited journals in ERP research on language processing, signifying their concentrated focus and high caliber publications within this discipline. According to the statistical data derived from Citespace spanning the period from 2002 to 2022, the top 10 journals and their corresponding citation counts are as follows: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (3019 citations), Psychophysiology (2726 citations), Brain and Language (2650 citations), Neuropsychologia (2587 citations), Neuroimage (2179 citations), Science (2061 citations), Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2036 citations), Brain Research (2007 citations), Cognitive Brain Research (1886 citations), and Journal of Memory and Language (1872 citations).

Figure 7 visualizes the most referenced contributions within the domain of ERP research on language processing. These findings align with the outcomes obtained through co-citation analyses of referenced authors and journals. Eminent neurolinguistic scholars such as Friederici, Kutas, and Van Petten are prominently featured in these significant works. Additionally, these works are predominantly disseminated through influential journals like Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Brain Research, Science, among others.

3.3 Co-occurrence analysis of keywords

Keywords hold an essential role in demonstrating the primary theme and scope of a study. When two keywords frequently co-occur, it suggests a potential connection between them. It is commonly understood that the frequency of co-occurrence reflects the strength of their association. Within Citespace, the Betweenness Centrality stands as a metric to gauge this association strength. A higher centrality value assigned to a keyword signifies its relative significance within the network (Chen, 2004; Han et al., 2022; Peng and Hu, 2022). Therefore, this investigation undertakes a co-occurrence analysis to explore the centrality and frequency patterns of keywords in the domain of ERP research on language processing, aiming to identify prevalent focal points. Figure 8 visually presents the highlighted keywords and their interconnections, while Table 1 delineates the top terms alongside their respective frequencies and centrality values.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Diachronic view of the clusters in ERP research on language processing.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Top keywords in ERP research on language processing.

Referring to Figure 9 and Table 1, the predominant keywords in ERP research on language processing encompass event-related potential, language, language comprehension, speech perception, time course, brain, word, working memory, mismatch negativity, and N400.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Keywords’ co-occurrence network in ERP research on language processing.

3.4 Cluster interpretations

Based on the keyword co-occurrence analysis, this study performed a cluster analysis to categorize the terms derived from all 3772 publications. LLR (Log Likelihood Ratio) is a text analysis algorithm used in tasks like classification. It measures the likelihood of a term occurring in one class compared to others. High LLR values indicate terms with strong discriminatory power, aiding in feature selection for accurate classification. The generated clusters, labeled using LLR, resulted in three distinct clusters. Figures 8, 10 present both synchronic and diachronic perspectives of these clusters, while Table 2 encapsulates essential information regarding each cluster.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Synchronic view of the clusters in ERP research on language processing.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of clusters of keywords in ERP research on language processing.

3.5 Burst detection analysis of keywords

Over the past two decades, ERP research on language processing has witnessed significant development, marked by the publication of numerous studies. Our analysis using Citespace revealed that sentence comprehension, mismatch negativity, and reading comprehension have surfaced as focal points. In particular, discussion surrounding speech perception, working memory, and temporal dynamics has been prevalent. This field appears interdisciplinary, amalgamating theories and methodologies from diverse subjects. Hence, it can be inferred that ERP research on language processing presently holds a pioneering role.

To delve deeper into the emerging trends within ERP research on language processing, our study conducted burst detection analysis on 3772 publications’ keywords. Burst terms represent keywords that undergo a remarkable surge within a specific timeframe, indicating ongoing developmental trends (Yuan and Sun, 2023). Figure 11 illustrates the top 25 burst terms:

FIGURE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 11. Burst terms in ERP research on language processing.

Figure 11 presents the evolving trends in ERP research on language processing over the preceding two decades through the examination of burst terms observed in distinct time periods. In this figure, “year” represents the emerging time of the burst term, shown by deep blue column. “Begin” and “end” in red column demonstrates the burst period of the term. “Strength” refers to a measure that quantifies the intensity and significance of the sudden increase in frequency of the term over its burst period. Citespace utilizes an algorithm developed by Jon Kleinberg for burst detection (Kleinberg, 2002).

4 Discussion

4.1 The spatial-temporal distribution of ERP research on language processing

The spatial-temporal distribution of the research pattern is clearly revealed by the visualization data. Figure 2 depicts a consistent upward trend in international publications focusing on ERP research on language processing. Initially emerging as a novel methodology, there existed a relatively limited number of studies employing ERPs in language processing research. Notably, between 2002 and 2004, the yearly publication count remained below 100. However, the field witnessed a subsequent surge in research endeavors. Particularly noteworthy are significant increments observed during three distinct periods: 2006 to 2007, 2010 to 2012, and 2017 to 2021. Remarkably, in 2021, the publication count reached its zenith, registering 244 publications within a single year. Despite minor declines in publication counts observed in 2008, 2010, and 2017, each downturn was promptly followed by obvious upswings. These observations underscore the emergence and burgeoning interest in ERP research on language processing in recent years. Notably, it is posited that significant discoveries concerning ERP components and advancements in research methodologies have propelled the proliferation of studies in this field (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011; Lopez-Calderon and Luck, 2014; Bates et al., 2015). Consequently, the number of publications experiences substantial growth during specific temporal periods.

From the insights gleaned in Figures 3, 4, a dominant presence is observed in ERP research on language processing, particularly attributed to the United States. Over a span of two decades, US-based researchers have contributed significantly, accounting for approximately 35% of the total studies in this field, amassing a total of 1308 publications. Outstanding academic institutions such as the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (with 203 articles), the University of California San Diego (with 127 articles), and the Radboud University Nijmegen (with 120 articles) have significantly bolstered the advancement of ERP research on language processing. Geographically, the epicenters of ERP research on language processing are chiefly concentrated in Europe and North America. Nevertheless, an encouraging trend is observed in Asia, particularly in China (with 430 articles), evidencing substantial growth and promising advancements in catching up with the forefront of ERP research.

4.2 Research focuses on ERP research on language processing

Results from co-citation analysis of references (Figure 7 and Table 3), co-occurrence analysis of keywords (Figure 9 and Table 1) and cluster interpretation (Figures 8, 10 and Table 2) have unraveled several key points in the ERP research on language processing from 2002 to 2022. Discussion will concern three main topics, namely the ERP components, methodologies and techniques, as well as research scope.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Top 10 cited works in ERP research on language processing.

Several ERP components have been instrumental in exploring the neurocognitive mechanisms of language processing. The N400, illustrated by Kutas and Federmeier (2000) as an index of semantic processing, is a significant electrophysiological signature in language processing. Studies highlight its role in sentence processing and neural mechanisms of language comprehension, suggesting a dynamic and modality-specific semantic processing network predominantly in the left hemisphere (Koelsch et al., 2004; Frank et al., 2015). Kutas and Federmeier (2011) extended this by demonstrating that the N400 is evoked by multimodal stimuli, including written, spoken, and sign languages, even non-linguistic stimuli like drawings and videos. This broadened the exploration of the N400 into perception, attention, and memory, reflecting how the brain utilizes top-down and bottom-up information to understand the world. Lau et al. (2008) anatomically deconstructed the N400 component, proposing a neurocognitive model for semantic processing that includes core regions like the left posterior temporal cortex (lPTC), left anterior temporal cortex (lATC), angular gyrus (AG), and left inferior frontal gyrus (lIFG). This model suggests that lexical semantic information is stored in the lPTG and higher-level semantic processes involve the lATG and AG, with the lIFG implicated in selecting and retrieving specific lexical representations.

The P600, another crucial ERP component, normally associated with syntactic processing, was interpreted by Brouwer et al. (2012) through the Retrieval-Integration Account. They found that semantically anomalous sentences evoked a P600 rather than the expected N400, suggesting that the P600 reflects the integration of lexical information within contexts and plays a role in constructing or updating mental representations.

Mismatch negativity (MMN) is closely related to sound processing and signifies the activation of memory networks for language sounds and spoken words (Pulvermüller and Shtyrov, 2006). Shtyrov and Pulvermüller (2002) observed distinct MMN responses to word stimuli compared to pseudowords, supporting immediate word processing upon identification. Jacobsen et al. (2004) demonstrated that MMN is evoked by low-frequency vowels, indicating that the auditory processing network can pre-attentively extract vowel formant structures. MMN also indicates syntactic processing. Hahne et al. (2001) found that combined physical and syntactic violations evoked larger MMNs than either alone, suggesting parallel and independent early-stage processing. Pulvermüller et al. (2007) evidenced the autonomy of syntactic processing, while Hasting et al. (2007) revealed functionally distinct neural mechanisms underlying subject-verb agreement and word class information. What’s more, studies on MMN encompass multimodal materials (Chen et al., 2022). Ishida and Nittono (2022) explored anomalies in music, finding that syntactic anomalies evoke early right anterior negativity (ERAN) and acoustic anomalies trigger MMN. Ding et al. (2022) showed that neutral and fear emotions induce greater MMN compared to positive emotions, revealing a processing bias toward negative emotions in body gestures.

The Left Anterior Negativity (LAN) and Early Left Anterior Negativity (ELAN) are components associated with syntax comprehension, typically observed over the left anterior scalp regions. ELAN, appearing within 100–200 ms after stimulus onset, is involved in early syntactic processing, while LAN, occurring around 300–500 ms, reflects sensitivity to syntactic violations or grammatical errors (Palolahti et al., 2005; Steinhauer and Drury, 2012).

Methodologies in ERP research are crucial for study quality. Luck (2014) provides a comprehensive guide on ERPs in cognitive neuroscience, covering theoretical foundations, experimental paradigms, and data analysis techniques. Lopez-Calderon and Luck (2014) introduced ERPLAB, an open-source toolbox for ERP data analysis compatible with MATLAB, enhancing the ease of offline analysis of ERP data. Bates et al. (2015) detailed the application of linear mixed-effects models using the lme4 package in R, offering a more refined model for predicting underlying patterns compared to traditional ANOVA analysis.

ERP studies on language processing often use visual stimuli, emphasizing reading comprehension. Ditman et al. (2007) proposed the self-paced reading design in ERP research for a more precise examination of reading processing. Metzner et al. (2017) integrated ERPs and eye-movement techniques, suggesting alternative strategies in sentence comprehension, such as reanalysis (P600) or tolerance of insufficient interpretations (N400).

Novel methods like time-frequency analysis and microstate analysis are gaining attention. Time-frequency analysis decomposes ERPs into frequency components over time, offering insights into cognitive functions (Herrmann et al., 2014; Morales and Bowers, 2022). Microstate analysis studies dynamic brain activity associated with cognitive processes, segmenting the ERPs signal into temporally stable topographic states, representing synchronized neural activity (Ott and Jäncke, 2013; Croce et al., 2021).

The ERP technique has found diverse applications in various linguistic research domains. Van Petten and Luka (2012) synthesized ERP studies on prediction during language comprehension, noting the N400 as an indicator of benefits from semantic context and positive components like P300 and P600 addressing prediction costs. Friederici (2002) emphasized the neural underpinnings of auditory sentence processing, positing that comprehension is organized within a bilateral temporo-frontal network. Kuperberg (2007) reviewed studies on the P600, suggesting that language comprehension involves competing neural processing streams: a semantic stream reliant on long-term memory and a combinatorial stream for morphosyntactic rules. This clash triggers reanalysis, evident in the P600. Chow and Phillips (2013) investigated the “Semantic P600” phenomenon in Chinese, finding that well-formed role-reversed sentences evoked P600 activation, highlighting the dependence of online semantic processing on surface syntax. Deng et al. (2016) revealed early syntactic processing (ELAN) and semantic-syntactic integration (N400) in Chinese verbs. Stowe et al. (2018) emphasized the need for updated experimental designs to explain late components in sentence wrap-up. Recent studies highlight pragmatic knowledge’s role in sentence comprehension. Kuperberg et al. (2007) proposed an animacy-based P600 effect influencing thematic-semantic relations. Van Berkum et al. (2007) posited active referential processing during sentence comprehension. Casado et al. (2020) revealed that motor sequencing affects early syntactic processing. Figurative language studies by Ferretti et al. (2021) and Shen et al. (2022) focused on proverbs and metaphors in sentence processing. Studies on reading comprehension and development, such as Stites and Laszlo (2017), linked ERP components with children’s reading development, finding that N250 predicted phonological awareness. Tabullo et al. (2020) found that higher reading skills were associated with reduced N400 during expected word processing. Troyer et al. (2022) explored hemispheric processing asymmetries, highlighting the left hemisphere’s engagement with specific semantic information and the right hemisphere’s involvement in broader semantic connections.

4.3 Trends and prospects in ERP research on language processing

Referring to the outcomes from burst detection analysis (Figure 11), several findings of developing trends are discovered. Besides, future directions may be predicted based on the statistics.

The first finding denotes a transition toward examining larger linguistic units (Nieuwland et al., 2019). Throughout the early 21st Century, ERPs investigations in language predominantly centred on lexical processing, evident from burst terms like “lexical decision” (2003–2005) and “word” (2003–2005). Initial studies were primarily focused on unraveling the neurocognitive mechanisms involved in processing individual words, pivotal in understanding language structures. During this phase, some studies isolated word stimuli for examination (Barber et al., 2004; Carreiras et al., 2005), while others incorporated these stimuli into phrases or sentences (Allen et al., 2003; Weber-Fox et al., 2003; Schiller, 2006). Nonetheless, the primary emphasis remained on the lexical level. The proliferation of studies concentrating on lexical processing established a robust foundation for ERP research on language processing. Consequently, subsequent investigations broadened their scope to encompass varied linguistic materials, highlighted by the emergence of the burst term “sentence comprehension” (2020–2022). Beyond lexical-semantic attributes, sentence processing encompasses the integration of diverse linguistic information. Recent studies have frequently delved into neural mechanisms associated with syntactic processing (Jackson et al., 2020; Hao et al., 2021) and pragmatic processing (Allegretta et al., 2021; Jouen et al., 2021; Schoknecht et al., 2022).

The second key finding underscores the growing integration and synthesis observed in ERP research on language processing. Initially, most studies were centred around specific ERP components, aiming to explain their distinct functions. For instance, early investigations proposed the N400 as an indicator of semantic violation (Quiroz-G, 2003) and the P600 as a marker of syntactic violation (Kuperberg et al., 2003). This trend is evident from burst terms like “semantic processing” (2002–2004) and “evoked potential” (2005–2009). In more recent years, the emergence of new burst terms such as “component” (2015–2022), “N400” (2019–2022), and “integration” (2019–2022) has spurred a re-examination of ERP components. A substantial body of studies now argues that the same component may exhibit diverse effects or functions across different experimental contexts (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011; Niharika and Rao, 2020). Moreover, the appearance of the burst term “eye movement” (2018–2019) suggests a trend toward integrating the ERP technique with other methodologies in linguistic research to acquire more robust evidence (Kessler et al., 2021).

The third significant finding highlights the increasing attention given to individual participant characteristics in ERP research on language processing. Burst terms such as “individual difference” (2017–2022) and “children” (2015–2018) signify this emerging trend. Investigations into second language acquisition have begun to identify individual variations by considering between-subject factors such as second language ability (Bice and Kroll, 2021; Liang and Chen, 2022; Grey, 2023). Furthermore, discussions regarding individual factors like gender or age have gained traction in recent studies (Cheimariou et al., 2019; Choisdealbha et al., 2022; Padrón et al., 2023).

In general, ERP research on language processing is poised to advance along several key trajectories in the future. Firstly, there will be a continued trend toward investigating larger linguistic units, so as to find out human brain’s processing mechanisms of complex language structures. Secondly, there will be a synthesis and re-examination of ERP components, exploring their multifaceted nature and roles across different linguistic contexts. Thirdly, there will be an increased focus on individual participant characteristics, to better understand variability in language processing outcomes. Lastly, there will be a growing integration and update of ERP techniques with other methodologies, to provide a more comprehensive understanding of language processing mechanisms. Overall, these trends are expected to aid the development of more nuanced models of language processing.

4.4 Strengths and limitations

This study is the first mapping-knowledge-domain analysis on ERP research on language processing, which pioneers the systematic review of the topic. 3772 relevant publications from the WoS core collection are exclusively extracted for precise visualization analysis.

The database in the current study is still far from inclusive, with only WoS core collection included. Publications from other databases are not considered properly, for example, PubMed and Scopus. Besides, merely publications written in English have been selected, which might lead to a bias in the analysis. Finally, the research scope in the current study is limited in language comprehension and perception. However, in language-related ERP study, language production also plays an essential role. Further research should discuss the neurocognitive mechanisms of language production in detail.

5 Conclusion

Appearing from cognitive neuroscience, the Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) technique has emerged as an elite tool in linguistic research. Offering feasibility and high temporal precision, this technique provides a unique vantage point for probing the neural underpinnings of language processing. Despite a burgeoning number of studies in ERP research on language processing since the turn of the 21st century, there exists a dearth of systematic reviews applying bibliometric methods to offer a comprehensive overview of this thriving research domain. This study undertook a mapping-knowledge-domain analysis using Citespace, a visualization-centric bibliometric tool, to analyze 3772 relevant publications in the recent two decades (2002–2022) in the core collection of Web of Science. Several key findings have emerged from this comprehensive analysis. Over the past two decades, the volume of publications in ERP research on language processing has shown a consistent upward trajectory. Firstly, the United States and the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences were identified as the leading contributors in terms of publications in this field. Co-citation analysis illuminated the frequently cited authors, journals, and references, outlining the fundamental pillars of ERP research on language processing. Subsequent co-occurrence and cluster analyses of keywords highlighted pivotal topics and current focal points in this domain. Prominent keywords encompassed themes such as working memory, speech perception, and time course, among others. Three primary clusters surfaced: sentence comprehension, mismatch negativity, and reading comprehension. Moreover, a burst detection analysis underscored potential future directions, suggesting the ascension of larger linguistic units as a burgeoning interest. Beyond individual words and sentences, this trajectory endeavors to unravel the intricate neurocognitive processes underlying more extensive language structures such as paragraphs and discourses. Another critical area involves integrating ERP components and delineating their functional roles, thereby enriching our understanding of how these components collectively contribute to cognitive functions like syntactic and semantic integration. Furthermore, exploring individual differences within ERP profiles, encompassing factors such as linguistic proficiency and cognitive styles, promises to refine the applicability and relevance of research findings, potentially paving the way for personalized models of language processing.

The current study draws a picture of the existing status in ERP research on language processing, which may shed light on following research in the future. Although there are some limitations, this work tries to provide implication for future research in the field.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

YS: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review and editing. XL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research is funded by the Bilingual Cognition and Development Laboratory Open Project in 2022 “ERP Study on the Influence of Second Language Proficiency Differences on the Metaphorical Processing of Novel Verbs” (BCD202202).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahmed, A., Al-Khawlani, A., Almaghlouth, S., and Alfadda, H. (2022). Cognitive linguistics: analysis of mapping knowledge domains. J. Intell. 10, 1–41. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence10040093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allegretta, R., Pyke, W., and Galli, G. (2021). ERP evidence of age-related differences in emotional processing. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 1261–1271. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06053-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M., Badecker, W., and Osterhout, L. (2003). Morphological analysis in sentence processing: an ERP study. Lang. Cogn. Process. 18, 405–430. doi: 10.1080/01690960244000054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, H., Vergara, M., and Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport 15, 545–548. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200403010-00032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Beres, A. (2017). Time is of the essence: a review of electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in language research. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 42, 247–255. doi: 10.1007/s10484-017-9371-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bice, K., and Kroll, J. (2021). Grammatical processing in two languages: how individual differences in language experience and cognitive abilities shape comprehension in heritage bilinguals. J. Neurolinguist. 58, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100963

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., and Hoeks, J. (2012). Getting real about semantic illusions: rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Res. 1446, 127–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.055

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., and Barber, H. (2005). Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1803–1817. doi: 10.1162/089892905774589217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Casado, P., Martín-Loeches, M., Muñoz, F., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., Jiménez-Ortega, L., Sánchez-García, J., et al. (2020). On the interplay between motor sequencing and linguistic syntax: electrophysiological evidence. J. Neurolinguist. 53, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.100874

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheimariou, S., Farmer, T., and Gordon, J. (2019). Lexical prediction in the aging brain: the effects of predictiveness and congruency on the N400 ERP component. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 26, 781–806. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2018.1529733

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, A., Peter, V., and Burnham, D. (2022). Development of neural discrimination of pitch across speech and music in the first year of life, a mismatch response study. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 37, 1153–1168. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2022.2051571

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. (2004). Searching for intellectual turning points: progressive knowledge domain visualization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 5303–5310. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307513100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. (2006). CiteSpace II: detecting and visualizing emerging trends and transient patterns in scientific literature. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 57, 359–377. doi: 10.1002/asi.20317

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. (2016). CiteSpace: a Practical Guide for Mapping Scientific Literature. New York: Nova Science Publishers.

Google Scholar

Chen, C., Ibekwe-SanJuan, F., and Hou, J. (2010). The structure and dynamics of co-citation clusters: a multiple-perspective co-citation analysis. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 61, 1386–1409.

Google Scholar

Chen, C., and Song, M. (2019). Visualizing a field of research: a methodology of systematic scientometric reviews. PLoS One 14:e0223994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T., Hsieh, M., Lin, Y., Chan, P., and Cheng, C. (2020). Mismatch negativity to different deviant changes in autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 766–777. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.10.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choisdealbha, A., Attaheri, A., Rocha, S., Brusini, P., Flanagan, S., Mead, N., et al. (2022). Neural detection of changes in amplitude rise time in infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 54, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, W., and Phillips, C. (2013). No semantic illusion in the “Semantic P600” phenomenon: ERP evidence from Mandarin Chinese. Brain Res. 1506, 76–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.02.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chwilla, D., Virgillito, D., and Vissers, C. (2011). The relationship of language and emotion: N400 support for an embodied view of language comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2400–2414. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarivate Analytics, (2021). Web of Science Platform. Available online at: https://clarivate.com/webofsciencegroup/solutions/webofscience-platform/ (accessed October 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Croce, P., Spadone, S., Zappasodi, F., Baldassarre, A., and Capotosto, P. (2021). rTMS affects EEG microstates dynamic during evoked activity. Cortex 138, 302–310. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.02.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., Wu, Q., Wang, J., Feng, L., and Xiao, Q. (2016). Event-related potentials reveal early activation of syntax information in Chinese verb processing. Neurosci. Lett. 631, 19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.07.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., and Zhu, H. (2020). To move or not to move: an ERP study on the processing of fictive and literal motion expressions. Lingua 244, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102908

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, X., Chen, Y., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., and Liu, J. (2022). The automatic detection of unexpected emotion and neutral body postures: a visual mismatch negativity study. Neuropsychologia 164, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditman, T., Holcomb, P., and Kuperberg, G. (2007). An investigation of concurrent ERP and self-paced reading methodologies. Psychophysiology 44, 927–935. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00593.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferretti, T., Hall, D., and Mansour, F. (2021). Interpreting pragmatic markers following proverbs. Can. J. Exp. Psychol.-Rev. Can. Psychol. Exp. 75, 197–203. doi: 10.1037/cep0000231

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, S., Otten, L., Galli, G., and Vigliocco, G. (2015). The ERP response to the amount of information conveyed by words in sentences. Brain Lang. 140, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Marquez, M., Mittelberg, I., Coulson, S., and Spivey, M. (2007). Methods in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Grey, S. (2023). Variability in native and nonnative language: an ERP study of semantic and grammar processing. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 45, 137–166. doi: 10.1017/S0272263122000055

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagoort, P., Brown, C., and Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP-measure of syntactic processing. Lang. Cogn. Process. 8, 439–484. doi: 10.1080/01690969308407585

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahne, A., Schröger, E., and Friederici, A. (2001). Segregating early physical and syntactic processes in auditory sentence comprehension. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 305–309. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200203040-00012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, Y., Peng, Z., and Chen, H. (2022). Bibliometric assessment of world scholars’ international publications related to conceptual metaphor. Front. Psychol. 13:1071121. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1071121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, Y., Duan, X., and Zhang, L. (2021). Processing aspectual agreement in an inflexionless language: an ERP study of Mandarin Chinese. Brain Sci. 11, 1–18. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasting, A., Kotz, S., and Friederici, A. (2007). Setting the stage for automatic syntax processing: the mismatch negativity as an indicator of syntactic priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 386–400. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C., Rach, S., Vosskuhl, J., and Strüber, D. (2014). Time-frequency analysis of event-related potentials: a brief tutorial. Brain Topogr. 27, 438–450. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0327-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinojosa, J., Méndez-Bértolo, C., and Pozo, M. (2010). Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology 47, 748–757. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00982.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishida, K., and Nittono, H. (2022). Relationship between early neural responses to syntactic and acoustic irregularities in music. Eur. J. Neurosci. 56, 6201–6214. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15856

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, C., Lorimor, H., and van Hell, J. (2020). The ERP correlates of thematic role assignment for passive versus active sentences. J. Neurolinguist. 54, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100886

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, T., Schröger, E., and Sussman, E. (2004). Pre-attentive categorization of vowel formant structure in complex tones. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 473–479. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.03.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jouen, A., Cazin, N., Hidot, S., Madden-Lombardi, C., Ventre-Dominey, J., and Dominey, P. (2021). Common ERP responses to narrative incoherence in sentence and picture pair comprehension. Brain Cogn. 153, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bc.2021.105775

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., and Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Lang. Cogn. Process. 15, 159–201. doi: 10.1080/016909600386084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R., Weber, A., and Friedrich, C. (2021). Activation of literal word meanings in idioms: evidence from eye-tracking and ERP experiments. Lang. Speech 64, 594–624. doi: 10.1177/0023830920943625

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinberg, J. (2002). Bursty and hierarchical structure in streams. Data Min. Knowl. Discov. 7, 373–397. doi: 10.1023/A:1014390908505

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., and Friederici, A. (2004). Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 302–307. doi: 10.1038/nn1197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperberg, G. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: challenges to syntax. Brain Res. 1146, 23–49. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperberg, G., Kreher, D., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., and Holcomb, P. (2007). The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active english sentences: evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Lang. 100, 223–237. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperberg, G., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., and Holcomb, P. (2003). Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 117–129. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00086-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 463–470. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., Neville, H., and Holcomb, P. (1987). A preliminary comparison of the N400 response to semantic anomalies during reading, listening and signing. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39, 325–330.

Google Scholar

Lau, E., Phillips, C., and Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 920–933. doi: 10.1038/nrn2532

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leppanen, P., Hamalainen, J., Salminen, H., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., Lohvansuu, K., et al. (2010). Newborn brain event-related potentials revealing atypical processing of sound frequency and the subsequent association with later literacy skills in children with familial dyslexia. Cortex 46, 1362–1376. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Deng, N., Yang, Y., and Wang, L. (2018). Process focus and accentuation at different positions in dialogues: an ERP study. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 255–274. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2017.1387278

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, L., and Chen, B. (2022). The non-native phonetic perception mechanism utilized by bilinguals with different L2 proficiency levels. Int. J. Biling. 26, 368–386. doi: 10.1177/13670069211058275

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Calderon, J., and Luck, S. (2014). ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. (2014). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Mascelloni, M., Zamparelli, R., Vespignani, F., Gruber, T., and Mueller, J. (2019). Distinct neural processes for memorizing form and meaning within sentences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:412. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00412

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzner, P., Malsburg, T., Vasishth, S., and Rösler, F. (2017). The importance of reading naturally: evidence from combined recordings of eye movements and electric brain potentials. Cogn. Sci. 41, 1232–1263. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12384

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S., and Kroll, J. (2012). When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. J. Mem. Lang. 67, 224–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales, S., and Bowers, M. (2022). Time-frequency analysis methods and their application in developmental EEG data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 54, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, E., van der Meer, A., Vulchanova, M., Blasi, D., and Baggio, G. (2020). Meaning before grammar: a review of ERP experiments on the neurodevelopmental origins of semantic processing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 441–464. doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01677-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S., and Van der Veer, M. (2008). Mapping research specialties. Annu. Rev. Inform. Sci. Technol. 42, 213–295.

Google Scholar

Nieuwland, M., Coopmans, C., and Sommers, R. (2019). Distinguishing old from new referents during discourse comprehension: evidence from ERPs and oscillations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:398. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00398

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niharika, M., and Rao, K. (2020). Processing syntax: perspectives on language specificity. Int. J. Neurosci. 130, 841–851. doi: 10.1080/00207454.2019.1707818

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olichney, J., Yang, J., Taylor, J., and Kutas, M. (2011). Cognitive event-related potentials: biomarkers of synaptic dysfunction across the stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 26, 215–228. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-0047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osterhout, L., and Holcomb, P. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 785–806.

Google Scholar

Ott, C., and Jäncke, L. (2013). Processing of self-initiated speech-sounds is different in musicians. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:41. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Padrón, I., Fraga, I., Poch, C., Vieitez, L., and Hinojosa, J. (2023). Does pleasantness affect the grammatical brain? an ERP study on individual differences. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 38, 1463–1477. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2023.2166678

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palolahti, M., Leino, S., Jokela, M., Kopra, K., and Paavilainen, P. (2005). Event-related potentials suggest early interaction between syntax and semantics during on-line sentence comprehension. Neurosci. Lett. 384, 222–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.04.076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, Z., and Hu, Z. (2022). A bibliometric analysis of linguistic research on COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 13:5487. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1005487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., and Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Language outside the focus of attention: the mismatch negativity as a tool for studying higher cognitive processes. Prog. Neurobiol. 79, 49–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A., and Carlyon, R. (2007). Syntax as a reflex: neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain Lang. 104, 244–253. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quiroz-G, Y. (2003). N400: an electrophysiological measure of semantic processing. Rev. Neurologia 36, 1176–1180.

Google Scholar

Rasenberg, M., Rommers, J., and van Bergen, G. (2020). Anticipating predictability: an ERP investigation of expectation-managing discourse markers in dialogue comprehension. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1624789

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, N. (2006). Lexical stress encoding in single word production estimated by event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. 1112, 201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoknecht, P., Roehm, D., Schlesewsky, M., and Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2022). The interaction of predictive processing and similarity-based retrieval interference: an ERP study. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 37, 883–901. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2022.2026421

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, L., Li, X., Huang, S., Huang, Y., Gao, X., You, Z., et al. (2022). Comprehending scientific metaphors in the bilingual brain: evidence from event-related potentials. Front. Psychol. 13:1037525. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1037525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shtyrov, Y., and Pulvermüller, F. (2002). Neurophysiological evidence of memory traces for words in the human brain. Neuroreport 13, 521–525. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200203250-00033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silkes, J., and Anjum, J. (2021). The role and use of event-related potentials in aphasia: a scoping review. Brain Lang. 219, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.bl.2021.104966

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinhauer, K., and Drury, J. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain Lang. 120, 135–162. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stites, M., and Laszlo, S. (2017). Time will tell: a longitudinal investigation of brain-behavior relationships during reading development. Psychophysiology 54, 798–808. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12844

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stowe, L., Kaan, E., Sabourin, L., and Taylor, R. (2018). The sentence wrap-up dogma. Cognition 176, 232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strijkers, K., and Costa, A. (2011). Riding the lexical speedway: a critical review on the time course of lexical selection in speech production. Front. Psychol. 2:365–372. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strijkers, K., Costa, A., and Thierry, G. (2010). Tracking lexical access in speech production: electrophysiological correlates of word frequency and cognate effects. Cereb. Cortex 20, 912–928. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sucevic, J., Savic, A., Popovic, M., Styles, S., and Kovic, V. (2015). Balloons and bavoons versus spikes and shikes: ERPs reveal shared neural processes for shape-sound-meaning congruence in words, and shape-sound congruence in pseudowords. Brain Lang. 145-146, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.band1.2015.03.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabullo, A., Shalom, D., Sevilla, Y., Gattei, C., París, L., and Wainselboim, A. (2020). Reading comprehension and predictability effects on sentence processing: an event-related potential study. Mind Brain Educ. 14, 32–50. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, M., McRae, K., and Kutas, M. (2022). Wrong or right? Brain potentials reveal hemispheric asymmetries to semantic relations during word-by-word sentence reading as a function of (fictional) knowledge. Neuropsychologia 170, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Berkum, J., Koornneef, A., Otten, M., and Nieuwland, M. (2007). Establishing reference in language comprehension: an electrophysiological perspective. Brain Res. 1146, 158–171. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Petten, C., and Luka, B. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: benefits, costs, and ERP components. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 176–190. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D., Druks, J., Barber, H., and Cappa, S. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 407–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter, W., Winter, A., Copper, R., McCallum, W., and Aldridge, V. (1964). Contingent negative variation: electric sign of sensorimotor association + expectancy in human brain. Nature 203, 380–384. doi: 10.1038/203380a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber-Fox, C., Davis, L., and Cuadrado, E. (2003). Event-related brain potential markers of high-language proficiency in adults. Brain Lang. 85, 231–244. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00587-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitten, A., Key, A., Mefferd, A., and Bodfish, J. (2020). Auditory event-related potentials index faster processing of natural speech but not synthetic speech over nonspeech analogs in children. Brain Lang. 207, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, Q., and Peng, G. (2022). The roles of object and action, and concreteness and imageability, in the distinction between nouns and verbs: an ERP study on monosyllabic words in Chinese. J. Neurolinguist. 61, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2021.101026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, G., and Sun, Y. (2023). A bibliometric study of metaphor research and its implications (2010-2020). South. Afr. Linguist. Appl. Lang. 41, 1–21. doi: 10.2989/16073614.2022.2113413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, B., Dang, J., and Zhang, G. (2017). EEG source reconstruction evidence for the noun-verb neural dissociation along semantic dimensions. Neuroscience 359, 183–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, D., and Strotmann, A. (2008). Information science during the first decade of the web: an enriched author cocitation analysis. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 59, 916–937. doi: 10.1002/asi.20799

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ERPs, language, systematic review, Citespace, bibliometrics

Citation: Sun Y and Luo X (2024) A mapping-knowledge-domain analysis of ERP research on language processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1352753. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1352753

Received: 08 December 2023; Accepted: 30 May 2024;
Published: 12 June 2024.

Edited by:

Anastasios M. Georgiou, Cyprus University of Technology, Cyprus

Reviewed by:

Mikio Kubota, Washington University in St. Louis, United States
Daniel C. Gallagher, Kyushu University, Japan

Copyright © 2024 Sun and Luo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaoyang Luo, c3RldmVuOTgwMUBhbGl5dW4uY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.