- Department of Cognitive Science, Institute of Information and Communication Research, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Toruń, Poland
Introduction
It takes more than one human brain to create a human mind.
We are smart, but not because we stand on the shoulders of giants or are giants ourselves. We stand on the shoulders of a very large pyramid of hobbits.
It is difficult to say something unique about the human uniqueness, as it is currently one of the key research problems of contemporary science and the empirical findings are coming from many different disciplines considering different levels of human constitution: from neural information processing to social interactions. In the frame of neurosciences one of the approaches addressing this question involves a comparative analysis of brain volumes among various primates (Dunbar, 1998). For instance, chimpanzees exhibit a brain size roughly one-third that of humans. However, relying solely on brain volume as a metric may not offer the most accurate insight into what sets us apart from other hominins. Recent advancements in our understanding of other Homo species, such as Homo neanderthalensis and Homo heidelbergensis, reveal less pronounced differences in brain volume. Furthermore, the emergence of Homo sapiens dates back ~200,000 to 300,000 years, but a pivotal moment in our cognitive evolution becomes apparent in the fossil record only around 80,000 years ago. This significant cognitive leap suggests that factors beyond sheer brain size play a crucial role in differentiating human cognition from that of our hominin relatives (Hare, 2017). Continuing the research about human uniqueness, at the beginning of 21th century neuroscientists have identified astrocytes that are exclusive to the human brain (Robertson, 2014). These discoveries have led to fresh insights into the intricate local and global structures of the human brain, unveiling their profound associations with the advanced cognitive abilities unique to humans (Oberheim et al., 2006; Zhang and Barres, 2013).
We believe that the question of what makes us humans unique makes sense only in the light of evolution, as Dobzhansky (1973) notably stated: “Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution”. In other words, human uniqueness is a result of evolution and an aggregate of skills developed in the phylogenesis. We claim that the use of tools and the development of language—which can be also considered as a kind of a tool—had a crucial impact on where humans are now on the ladder of evolution. Human beings are able to enhance their natural skills by an incorporation of artifacts and the use of substitutes to create an augmented reality (Clark, 2008). Cognitive processes can be augmented or extended through the use of tools, technology, and other external aids, which should extend our minds beyond its natural boundaries adding new capacities like a larger memory by the use of external sources (e.g., a calendar in the cell phone). This super-ability challenges the traditional view of the mind as being strictly localized within the skull. “Human minds and bodies are essentially open to episodes of deep and transformative restructuring in which new equipment (both physical and “mental”) can become quite literally incorporated into the thinking and acting systems that we identify as our minds and bodies” (Clark, 2008, p. 30–31). We believe that this openness to transformation distinguishes us from other anthropoids. In other words, the use of artifacts as tools transforming the body and the language as a tool transforming the mind has an significant role in human evolution.
Artifacts as tools transforming the body
Theories of the embodied mind hold that the body plays an important role in cognition. According to the thesis about embodied cognition, minds need a body to test and explore the possibilities for incorporating new resources and structures into their embodied acting (Clark, 2008, p. 42). Embodied cognition is intricately tied to adaptive processes, rendering it susceptible to the forces of evolution. Anderson (2007) posits that this evolutionary perspective on cognition is instrumental in unraveling its purpose. Consequently, the underlying implication is that cognition primarily serves the imperative of survival. Notably, cognition is an attribute of organisms that have undergone development within specific environmental parameters. Hence, cognition has progressed hand in hand with the evolution of sensory organs and the nervous system, both crucial for enabling cognitive processes and subsequent actions in a particular ecological setting. In essence, cognition is finely tuned to fit the ecological niche of each distinct species. To encapsulate Anderson's perspective succinctly, “cognition represents a collection of tools with a specialized and interdependent function that has evolved due to their adaptability in nature” (Anderson, 2007, p. 67).
What makes human unique in the use of tools is their unparalleled adaptability and willingness to integrate a diverse array of intricate tools and artifacts into their embodied, perceptual, cognitive, and affective systems (Heersmink, 2022). During tool manipulation, a dynamic relationship emerges among the body, task, and environment, resulting in the transformation of the body-only system into a body-plus-tool system (Mangalam and Fragaszy, 2016). The incorporation of tools into the body schema significantly extends both the physical and sensory reach of human capabilities. Different types of artifacts influence the body schema and expand the horizons of perception and action (Black, 2014). The increasing development of rehabilitative robotic technologies highlights that tools serve not only the purpose of convenience but are also employed in rehabilitation. For instance, exoskeletons aid in addressing the prevalence of mobility impairments stemming from spinal cord injuries and cerebrovascular accidents (Butnaru, 2021).
What is interesting is that the use of tools and the ability to communicate are not exclusive to humans. Crows, for instance, have adapted to urban environments, acquiring knowledge about humans and passing it down to subsequent generations, thereby developing a form of culture (Cornell et al., 2012). Notably, among the Corvus species, the New Caledonian crow has demonstrated the ability to use tools (Bayern et al., 2018). However, other avian species, as well as apes, sea otters, elephants, dolphins, among others, also exhibit tool use (Beck, 1980; Bentley-Condit and Smith, 2010).
Despite the widespread occurrence of tool use in various species, the skills we share with other organisms become unique to humans due to further evolutionary development. Advancements in representational abilities, executive functioning, sensorimotor capabilities, and cognitive skills related to collaborative culture may play a pivotal role in influencing the evolution of tool use (Seed and Byrne, 2010). Modern humans' ability to use tools is closely linked to the evolution of cognitive skills. For example the increased working memory influences various cognitive domains such as theory of mind, symbolic thought, analogical reasoning, and planning (Seed and Byrne, 2010). Our behavior, mind and cognitive abilities are the result of evolution in the broad sense of the word, including both genetic and cultural evolution. A large part of contemporary research and discussions on human uniqueness focuses on the evolution in the cultural aspect (Colagè and d'Errico, 2018; Bender, 2020). The social contexts, shaped by The Social Intelligence Hypothesis (critical view Barrett, 2016), plays a pivotal role in the development of cognitive abilities (e.g., Machiavellian intelligence—Byrne and Whiten, 1988; The social brain hypothesis—Dunbar, 1998; “primate politics”—De Waal, 1982). Of course, non-human primates are inherently social animals, relying on advanced social cognitive abilities, including a rudimentary form of theory of mind, to navigate their complex social environments. Despite the shared social nature of Homo sapiens and nonhuman apes, humans stand out as “The ultra-social animal”, as aptly described by Tomasello (2014). Human cognitive abilities, specifically those related to learning, cooperation, and communication are primarily geared toward the development of culture and processes associated with cultural evolution (Herrmann et al., 2007, 2010; Mesoudi, 2011; Lewens, 2015) as well as cumulative cultural evolution (Tomasello, 1999; Mesoudi, 2016; Mesoudi and Thornton, 2018). This has facilitated the accumulation, transmission, and modification of knowledge, skills, and technological innovations not just in face-to-face interactions but also across generations. Consequently, human creations are no longer solely the product of individual brains but rather of a collective intelligence (Henrich, 2015; Muthukrishna and Henrich, 2016; Henrich and Muthukrishna, 2023; Henrich et al., 2023).
Language as a tool transforming the mind
“Cultural evolution has shown us that one word can be worth a thousand genes” (Szathmáry and Számadó, 2008). In this sense, language serves not only as a means of communication and expression of mental states, but also as a complex cultural artifact that has evolved over time through cumulative cultural processes (Tomasello, 2009; cf. Sterelny, 2014). Tomasello and Rakoczy (2003) outlines a specific hypothesis regarding the transformative role of language in human cognitive development. He suggests that language serves two primary functions: facilitating communication and enabling cognitive representation. He underscores the close interrelation between these functions, emphasizing the significance of uniquely human social cognition in the process. According to Tomasello and Rakoczy (2003), the key lies in understanding how this distinct social cognition enables children to learn and utilize linguistic symbols for interpersonal communication, a process that typically begins around the second year of life. Over time, these interpersonal communication tools are internalized and start being used intrapersonally, ultimately becoming the primary medium for specific types of human cognition. Acquiring language requires specific cognitive abilities, such as joint attention and the comprehension of intentions, as well as active engagement in social experiences and participation in specific social practices, such as scenes of joint attention (Kern and Moll, 2017).
Of course, non-human primates are also social animals. At the core of their strategies for coping in a complex social environment lie advanced cognitive abilities related to social cognition, such as, for example, theory of mind to some extent. One hypothesis suggests that great apes possess cognitive abilities similar to humans, but their primary function is competition rather than cooperation (cf. Cheney and Seyfarth, 2007; Tomasello, 2023). In other words, their ability to read the minds of others may be used in the context of cooperation, but the main goal is competition. As support for these theories, it is pointed out that great apes more effectively handle certain tasks when they are related to competition than when they involve cooperation or communication (Hare et al., 2001; Hare and Tomasello, 2004).
Language as a tool fulfills an operative psychological function, as its internalization within the context of social interactions facilitates the semiotic mediation of mental processes (cf. Trevarthen, 1998; Brinck and Liljeenfors, 2013). Here, a linguistic symbol is regarded as a means that transforms elementary mental functions into higher functions (Wertsch, 1985; Tomasello, 1999). Clark (1998, 2008) highlights the importance of understanding the role of language in shaping human cognition and behavior within the context of our dynamic and adaptive interaction with the environment. This dynamism includes both its transformative character and the socio-cultural aspects. “While numerous species engage in representation and communication, humans uniquely employ a single system for both functions. Human language serves dual roles: it functions as an intra-individual representational system and, simultaneously, as an inter-individual communication system” (Astington and Baird, 2005, p. 6). Communicative function of language and its cognitive function are two aspects of one process. For instance, the development of abilities such as joint attention enables social interactions that influence the capacity to adopt perspectives and understand communicative intentions in specific language discourses.
The genesis of higher psychological functions is rooted in language-based social interactions. A sign integrated into the natural developmental process, embraced by the child within the cultural developmental trajectory, restructures the cognitive system and its basic mental functions—such as attention, memory, perception, and thinking—into higher functions such as metacognition (cf. Żuromski et al., 2022). In the process of internalization, language undergoes a fundamental shift in function, initially serving as a means for controlling others' behavior in the social speech and later transforming into an executive and self-regulation function for cognitive processes and behavior in the private and inner speech (Wertsch, 1985; Vygotsky, 1987). In line with contemporary perspectives, Fernyhough (2008, 2009) posits that both the sequence of explanations and Vygotsky's exploration of verbal thinking provide insights into the processes of dialogical thinking, rooted in external (social) dialogue (Vygotsky, 1997, 1999). In dialogical exchange of beliefs, the connection between self and others is contained. The inherent tension between “me” and “other” serves as a natural force shaping the development of an autonomous and self-aware subject. An outcome of interaction through language is metacognition, which evolves alongside language acquisition and initial social interactions (Heyes, 2016; Heyes et al., 2020).
To conclude, on an evolutionary time scale, our cognitive abilities and our minds are the result of evolution in a broad sense, both genetic and cultural. In this first sense, much of our cognitive endowment is in fact inherited from sharing a common ancestor with apes. The consequences of the processes of the natural and cultural evolution encompass not only innovations like tools, technology, institutions, social norms, advanced mathematical notations, and intricate scientific theories but also specific and distinctive cognitive abilities termed as “cognitive gadgets” (Heyes, 2018a). These cognitive gadgets include language (Tomasello, 2005; Christiansen and Chater, 2016; Sterelny, 2016; Planer and Sterelny, 2021), theory of mind (Heyes and Frith, 2014; Moore, 2020), empathy (Heyes, 2018b), and emotions (Barrett, 2017; Lindquist et al., 2022). These socio-cultural cognitive abilities are transformative—they alter abilities already shared with other nonhuman apes, such as causal understanding, spatial memory, and tool use (Moll, 2018). In this context, primatology, evolutionary psychology, comparative psychology, and neuroscience provide insights into the evolution of human cognitive abilities. This knowledge extends beyond our own species to encompass other members of the Homo genus (e.g., Homo erectus or Homo neanderthalensis) as well as great apes (chimpanzees, orangutans, etc.). As Herrmann et al. (2007, p. 1361) suggest: “Humans' especially powerful skills of social-cultural cognition early in ontogeny serve as a kind of ‘bootstrap' for the distinctively complex development of human cognition in general”. Referring to the processes of cumulative cultural evolution, which enable the accumulation and modification of innovations across generations, we can explain how humans are involved not only in creating tools but also in developing advanced technologies for tool creation. The various forms of human activity find their basis in distinctly human cognitive mechanisms—the fundamental processes of the human mind. Throughout the course of cultural evolution, these underlying cognitive mechanisms have undergone transformation (Heyes, 2019, p. 1).
Author contributions
DŻ: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. AP-Ż: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The cost of the publication was covered by the Department of Cognitive Science.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Anderson, M. L. (2007). “How to study the mind: An introduction to embodied cognition,” in Brain Development in Learning Environments: Embodied and Perceptual Advancements, eds. F. Santoianni, C. Sabatano (Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing).
Astington, J. W., and Baird, J. A. (Eds.). (2005). “Introduction: why language matters,” in Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind (Oxford University Press), 3–25. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0001
Barrett, L. (2016). “The (r) evolution of primate cognition: does the social intelligence hypothesis lead us around in anthropocentric circles?” in The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of the Social Mind (Routledge), 35–50.
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life Of The Brain. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Bayern, A. M. P., Danel, S., Auersperg, A. M. I., Mioduszewska, B., and Kacelnik, A. (2018). Compound tool construction by New Caledonian crows. Sci. Rep. 8, 15676. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33458-z
Beck, B. (1980). Animal Tool Behaviour: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. New York: Garland STPM Pub.
Bender, A. (2020). The role of culture and evolution for human cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. 12, 1403–1420. doi: 10.1111/tops.12449
Bentley-Condit, V., and Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour 147, 185–32A. doi: 10.1163/000579509X12512865686555
Black, D. (2014). Where bodies end and artefacts begin: tools, machines and interfaces. Body Soc. 20, 31–60. doi: 10.1177/1357034X13506946
Brinck, I., and Liljeenfors, R. (2013). The developmental origin of metacognition. Infant Child Dev. 22, 85–101. doi: 10.1002/icd.1749
Butnaru, D. (2021). Exoskeletons, rehabilitation and bodily capacities. Body Soc. 27, 28–57. doi: 10.1177/1357034X211025600
Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.
Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon Metaphysics: The Evolution of a Social Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Christiansen, M. H., and Chater, N. (2016). Creating Language: Integrating Evolution, Acquisition, and Processing. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Clark, A. (2008), Supersizing the Mind. Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Colagè, I., and d'Errico, F. (2018). Culture: the driving force of human cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. 12, 654–672. doi: 10.1111/tops.12372
Cornell, H. N., Marzluff, J. M., and Pecoraro, S. (2012). Social learning spreads knowledge about dangerous humans among American crows. Proc. R. Soc. B. 279, 499–508. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0957
Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am. Biol. Teach. 35, 125–129. doi: 10.2307/4444260
Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Dev. Rev. 28, 225–262. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.03.001
Fernyhough, C. (2009). “Dialogic thinking,” in Private Speech, Executive Functioning, and the Development of Verbal Self-Regulation, eds. A. Winsler, C. Fernyhough, and I. Montero (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 42–52.
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annu Rev Psychol 68, 155–186.
Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Anim. Behav. 61, 139–151. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1518
Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim. Behav. 68, 571–581. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011
Heersmink, R. (2022). Human uniqueness in using tools and artifacts: flexibility, variety, complexity. Synthese 200, 442. doi: 10.1007/s11229-022-03892-8
Henrich, J. (2015). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species and Making Us Smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Henrich, J., Blasi, D. E., Curtin, C. M., Davis, H. E., Hong, Z., Kelly, D., et al. (2023). A cultural species and its cognitive phenotypes: implications for philosophy. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 14, 349–386. doi: 10.1007/s13164-021-00612-y
Henrich, J., and Muthukrishna, M. (2023). What makes us smart? Top. Cogn. Sci. 1–25. doi: 10.1111/tops.12656
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317, 1360–1366 doi: 10.1126/science.1146282
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 5, e12438. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012438
Heyes, C. (2016). Who knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends Cognit. Sci. 20, 2014–2213. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.007
Heyes, C. (2018a). Cognitive Gadgets: The Cultural Evolution of Thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Heyes, C. (2018b). Empathy is not in our genes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 95, 499–507 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.001
Heyes, C. (2019). Précis of cognitive gadgets: the cultural evolution of thinking. Behav. Brain Sci. 42:e169. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X18002145
Heyes, C., Bang, D., Shea, N., Frith, C. D., and Fleming, S. M. (2020). Knowing ourselves together: the cultural origins of metacognition. Trends Cognit. Sci. 24, 349–362. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.007
Heyes, C. M., and Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science 344, 1243091. doi: 10.1126/science.1243091
Kern, A., and Moll, H. (2017). On the transformative character of collective intentionality and the uniqueness of the human. Philosoph. Psychol. 30, 319–337. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2017.1295648
Lindquist, K. A., Jackson, J. C., Leshin, J., Satpute, A. B., and Gendron, M. (2022). The cultural evolution of emotion. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 1, 669–681. doi: 10.1038/s44159-022-00105-4
Mangalam, M., and Fragaszy, D. M. (2016). Transforming the body-only system into the body-plus-tool system. Anim. Behav. 117, 115–122 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.04.016
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural evolution: Integrating psychology, evolution and culture. Curr. Opini. Psychol. 7, 17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.001
Mesoudi, A., and Thornton, A. (2018). What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proc. R. Soc. B. 285, 20180712. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0712
Moll, H. (2018). The transformative cultural intelligence hypothesis: evidence from young children's problem-solving. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 9, 161–175. doi: 10.1007/s13164-017-0342-7
Moore, R. (2020). The cultural evolution of mind-modelling. Synthese 199, 1751–1776. doi: 10.1007/s11229-020-02853-3
Muthukrishna, M., and Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 371, 20150192 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0192
Oberheim, N. A., Wang, X., Goldman, S., and Nedergaard, M. (2006). Astrocytic complexity distinguishes the human brain. Trends Neurosci. 29, 10. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.08.004
Planer, R., and Sterelny, K. (2021). From Signal to Symbol: the Evolution of Language. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Robertson, J. M. (2014). Astrocytes and the evolution of the human brain. Med. Hypothes. 82, 4. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.12.004
Seed, A., and Byrne, R. (2010). Animal tool-use. Curr Biol. 20, R1032– R1039. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.042
Sterelny, K. (2014). A paleolithic reciprocation crisis: symbols, signals, and norms. Biol. Theory 9, 65–77. doi: 10.1007/s13752-013-0143-x
Sterelny, K. (2016). Cumulative cultural evolution and the origins of language. Biol. Theory 11, 1. doi: 10.1007/s13752-016-0247-1
Szathmáry, E., and Számadó, S. (2008). Language: a social history of words. Nature 456, 40–41. doi: 10.1038/456040a
Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate: Based on the 2008 Tanner Lectures on Human Values at Stanford University. Cambridge: MIT.
Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 187–194. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015
Tomasello, M. (2023). Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: a theory. Anim. Cogn. 26, 25–35. doi: 10.1007/s10071-022-01662-0
Tomasello, M., and Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind Lang. 18, 121–147. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00217
Trevarthen, C. (1998). “The concept and foundation of infant intersubjectivity”, in Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, ed. S. Bråten (Cambridge University Press), 15–46.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. I: Problems of General Psychology, eds. R. W. Rieber and A.S. Carton. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). “The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky,” in Vol. IV: The History of the Development of Higher Mental Functions, ed. R. W. Rieber (New York: Plenum Press).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1999). “The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky,” in Vol. VI: Scientific Legacy, ed. R.W. Rieber (New York: Plenum Press).
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Zhang, Y., and Barres, B. A. (2013). A smarter mouse with human astrocytes. Bioessays 35, 876–880. doi: 10.1002/bies.201300070
Keywords: language, evolution, culture, tool, embodied cognition, human uniqueness, social skill acquisition
Citation: Żuromski D and Pacholik-Żuromska A (2024) Socio-cultural brain reprogramming–The uniqueness of human cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1331213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1331213
Received: 06 November 2023; Accepted: 23 February 2024;
Published: 12 March 2024.
Edited by:
Idan Segev, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, IsraelReviewed by:
Yossi Yovel, Tel Aviv University, IsraelCopyright © 2024 Żuromski and Pacholik-Żuromska. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Anita Pacholik-Żuromska, apz@umk.pl