Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 September 2023
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Maintain that Brain - Protecting and Boosting Cognitive Flexibility View all 6 articles

Editorial: Maintain that brain - protecting and boosting cognitive flexibility

  • 1Behavioral Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, Burlington, VT, United States
  • 2Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 3University Hospital of Old Age Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 4Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Studies have consistently demonstrated that cognitively stimulating activities can protect against the negative effects of brain pathology on cognition in aging (Brem and Sensi, 2018). These stimulating activities are thought to increase adaptive behavior via compensatory processes, a phenomenon also known as cognitive reserve (CR) (Barulli and Stern, 2013). CR is a fascinating concept because it has been proven to be linked to many different mechanisms: starting from general cognitive functions (Roldán-Tapia et al., 2012) and narrowing it down to creativity (Colombo et al., 2018) and more specifically to flexibility of thought (Fusi et al., 2021a). Cognitive flexibility serves as a foundational cognitive trait that enables individuals to adapt more effectively to a rapidly changing world. It empowers individuals to embrace uncertainty, learn from experiences, and navigate complex challenges. Enhancing cognitive flexibility can lead to heightened adaptability, ultimately improving an individual's ability to thrive in diverse environments and situations.

This path can be explained by reflecting on the fact that although it is generally accepted that more efficient executive functioning is a main mediator of this adaptive behavior, little is known regarding the exact underlying cognitive and neurophysiological mechanisms. One potential explanation is that increased adaptability can be explained via an underlying enhanced cognitive flexibility (Fusi et al., 2021a).

Papers included in this Research Topic embrace all the spectrum of components that allow to protect our brain, with a target focus on flexibility of thought, or cognitive flexibility.

If, as mentioned above, the relationship between CR and creativity has been reported in the literature, an understudied aspect is linked to some of the neurological bases of this association. An interesting insight into this is provided by the paper by Nobukawa et al. that focused on functional connectivity in older adults. Their results highlighted how older participants with high creativity exhibited high functional connectivity overall, with no significant effect of their IQ. This finding seems to support the idea that creativity entails widespread brain connectivity. Interestingly, but not surprisingly, a similar conclusion has been reported when focusing on the CR (Stampanoni Bassi et al., 2019). The paper by Khachatryan et al. using an oddball paradigm, follows and expands this line of research, by relating the P300 amplitude and latency to different proxies of cognitive reserve. Specifically, they found that education and leisure activities were linearly related to the P300 latency, whereas more complex, non-linear relations between the P300 amplitude and education and working activities were found. This study therefore identifies distinct neural components underlying different proxies that are presumed to increase reserve.

Because of the connection between CR and cognitive flexibility, and because of the fact that they tend to be widespread in the brain, strategies that aim to improve adaptability via the improvement of cognitive flexibility have been reported to translate best into preservation and enhancement of cognitive abilities and therefore successful aging (e.g., Buitenweg et al., 2017; Colombo et al., 2022).

Yet, the effect on some specific CR proxies have been investigated to a lesser extent. Some of these proxies include emotion regulation (Huang et al., 2019; Colombo et al., 2023) as well as behavior within different social situations (Ihle et al., 2018). The paper by Allaert et al. addresses this aspect in an interesting way, by using non-invasive brain stimulation (specifically, tDCS) as a form of intervention. Their results suggest that attenuated self-referential attention in specific emotion-inducing social situations may be a neurocognitive mechanism through which tDCS reduces emotional reactivity.

Another open question is how differently our brains cope with damage when pathology is present if compared to normal aging. A first answer can be found in the study by Jansen et al. that focuses on the effect of cognitive reserve on cognition in older adults with different levels of cognitive decline: from subjective cognitive complaints to Alzheimer's dementia. Their results demonstrate how with more advanced levels of cognitive impairment, the positive effects of cognitive reserve proxies on cognition become less pronounced. This suggests that our ability to adapt, presumably mediated via cognitive reserve, may be functioning best in case of relatively intact cognition or only mild levels of cognitive impairment. That being said, this relationship has been proven not to be linear, but to be most likely moderated by many other factors, like, for example, the time given to perform divergent thinking tasks (Fusi et al., 2021b). If the type of intervention and the way it is administered plays a role in the results, as indirectly suggested by the findings of Jansen et al. seeing how different interventions might affect brain coping mechanisms would be important.

The study by Stalpaert et al. provides a first answer with a specific reference to patients with Primary Progressive Aphasia. Their results highlight how patients who received speech-language therapy showed specific neurophysiological changes, suggesting that this therapy might be more effective for patients with a non-fluent variant of aphasia.

In summary, the Research Topic of studies published in this Frontiers Research Topic shed light on a range of understudied aspects of CR including the relationship between CR and other cognitive, behavioral, and neural components, the modulation of CR proxies through non-invasive brain stimulation, and the role of CR in therapeutic outcomes and in protective effects during pathological aging. While the relationship between CR and cognition is complex and moderated by various factors, promising results warrant further investigation of interventions targeting CR to enhance cognitive abilities.

Author contributions

BC: Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing. A-KB: Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing. JO: Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Barulli, D., and Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brem, A. K., and Sensi, S. L. (2018). Towards combinatorial approaches for preserving cognitive fitness in aging. Trends Neurosci. 41, 885–897. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buitenweg, J. I., Van de Ven, R. M., Prinssen, S., Murre, J. M., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Cognitive flexibility training: a large-scale multimodal adaptive active-control intervention study in healthy older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 529. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, B., Antonietti, A., and Daneau, B. (2018). The relationships between cognitive reserve and creativity. A study on American aging population. Front. Psychol. 9, 764. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, B., Fusi, G., Pabla, S., and Caravita, S. C. S. (2022). Challenge your brain. blogging during the COVID lockdown as a way to enhance well-being and cognitive reserve in an older population. JIN 21, 4. doi: 10.31083/j.jin2104114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, B., Hamilton, A., Telazzi, I., and Balzarotti, S. (2023). The relationship between cognitive reserve and the spontaneous use of emotion regulation strategies in older adults: a cross-sectional study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 12, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s40520-023-02424-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusi, G., Colombo, B., Zanetti, M., Crepaldi, M., Rozzini, L., Rusconi, M. L., et al. (2021a). The effect of psychological symptoms on divergent thinking in healthy older adults. Creativity Res. J. 33, 302–310. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2021.1892413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusi, G., Lavolpe, S., Crepaldi, M., and Rusconi, M. L. (2021b). The controversial effect of age on divergent thinking abilities: a systematic review. J. Creat. Behav. 55, 374–395. doi: 10.1002/jocb.461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. M., Fan, Y. T., Lee, S. H., Liu, H. L., Chen, Y. L., Lin, C., et al. (2019). Cognitive reserve-mediated neural modulation of emotional control and regulation in people with late-life depression. Soc. Cognit. Aff. Neurosci. 14, 849–860. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ihle, A., Oris, M., Sauter, J., Rimmele, U., and Kliegel, M. (2018). Cognitive reserve and social capital accrued in early and midlife moderate the relation of psychological stress to cognitive performance in old age. Dement. Ger. Cognit. Dis. 45, 190–197. doi: 10.1159/000488052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roldán-Tapia, L., García, J., Cánovas, R., and León, I. (2012). Cognitive reserve, age, and their relation to attentional and executive functions. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 19, 2–8. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2011.595458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stampanoni Bassi, M., Iezzi, E., Gilio, L., Centonze, D., and Buttari, F. (2019). Synaptic plasticity shapes brain connectivity: implications for network topology. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20, 6193. doi: 10.3390/ijms20246193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, creativity, brain health, cognitive flexibility, cognitive reserve

Citation: Colombo B, Brem A-K and Oosterman J (2023) Editorial: Maintain that brain - protecting and boosting cognitive flexibility. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1279374. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1279374

Received: 17 August 2023; Accepted: 21 August 2023;
Published: 12 September 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2023 Colombo, Brem and Oosterman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Colombo, bcolombo@champlain.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.