Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 02 August 2023
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Dissociations Between Neural Activity and Conscious State: A Key to Understanding Consciousness View all 7 articles

Editorial: Dissociations between neural activity and conscious state: a key to understanding consciousness

  • 1Institute for Neuromodulation and Neurotechnology, University Hospital and University of Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany
  • 2Institute for Advanced Consciousness Studies, Santa Monica, CA, United States
  • 3Vienna Cognitive Science Hub, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 4Department of Computing, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 6Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 7Department of Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

Introduction

In disorders of consciousness (DoC), the behavioral misdiagnosis rate may be as high as 40% (Schnakers et al., 2009). We thus urgently need a way to detect consciousness “from the outside” using objective, physiological biomarkers. Yet abnormal brain activity in severely brain-injured patients doesn't necessarily indicate unconsciousness (Kaplan, 2004; Verellen and Cavazos, 2010). Given this challenge, our editorial explores various strategies for finding true neural markers of (un)consciousness.

Standard approaches for deriving markers of consciousness

One approach is to observe healthy individuals during temporary unconsciousness (e.g., deep sleep) and see how neural activity changes. Based on these results, other researchers may later look for similar changes in neural activity in DoC patients to infer either consciousness or its absence. In our Research Topic, Aamodt et al. followed this approach by correlating the complexity of electroencephalography (EEG) signals with sleep stages in healthy volunteers (more on this later).

A similar approach is to observe surgical patients during the loss of consciousness induced by general anesthesia and see how neural activity changes with this causal manipulation. In our Research Topic, Sun et al. investigated whether EEG alpha oscillations dynamics recorded from adult and pediatric surgical patients during the initial and maintenance phases of general anesthesia predict alpha band activity during emergence from anesthesia, as well as extubation time. Although the authors' findings were negative, the general approach of studying EEG recordings from surgical patients during general anesthesia (Zhang et al., 2001; Brown et al., 2010), as well as volunteers given anesthesia in the laboratory (Alkire and Miller, 2005; Ní Mhuircheartaigh et al., 2013; Purdon et al., 2013), may offer insights that translate not only to detecting covert consciousness in DoC, but also during general anesthesia (so-called “anesthesia awareness”) (Sebel et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2018).

However, the gold standard indications of consciousness are volitional, goal-directed behaviors and verbal report: if patients exhibits these behaviors, then we can hardly doubt that they are conscious. In our Research Topic, for instance, a verbal report study of dream content by Aamodt et al. came to the surprising conclusion that the complexity of spontaneous EEG signals, often associated with one's level of consciousness (Sarasso et al., 2021), does not correspond to dream content in the second stage of non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM2).

In cases of DoC, patients diagnosed with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS, sometimes referred to as the “vegetative state”) lack verbal reports by definition, even if some of them are covertly conscious (Monti et al., 2010). Of course, that is not to say that we cannot learn from studying heterogeneous DoC patients or contrasting their neural activity with that of healthy volunteers. A preliminary study in our Research Topic by Wu et al., for example, found that whole-brain theta band functional EEG connectivity correlated with functional disability in patients emerging from a minimally conscious state, and that frontoparietal theta connectivity was greater in the patient group than in healthy controls. This study offers a promising lead for measuring the recovery of consciousness with EEG, but much more work is needed to learn to what extent theta band connectivity actually relates to consciousness.

Markers of consciousness should generalize across diverse regimes of cortical dynamics

So then, if biomarkers of consciousness are best derived from populations outside of DoC who are capable of volitional behaviors, how can we be sure that such biomarkers will generalize to DoC, where cortical dynamics are often abnormal (Kaplan, 2004; Verellen and Cavazos, 2010)? Indeed, one study in our Research Topic by Maschke et al. found that several common measures of consciousness behaved unexpectedly in three UWS patients treated with the general anesthetic propofol. Perhaps more sophisticated approaches are needed to help us identify biomarkers of consciousness that generalize to abnormal EEGs. One such approach is to recruit subjects who exhibit paradoxical EEG dissociations, i.e., both abnormal cortical dynamics and volitional behaviors during wakefulness which indicate consciousness (Frohlich et al., 2022, 2023b). These participants are behaviorally responsive (albeit not necessarily verbal), giving us confidence in the labels we use for supervised machine learning. For instance, analyses of EEGs from children with rare 15q genetic disorders, who show dissociations between neural oscillations and conscious state (Frohlich et al., 2020; Saravanapandian et al., 2021), demonstrate that the complexity of EEG signals is a common denominator of consciousness shared with healthy children (Frohlich et al., 2022).

A framework for studying neural signatures of consciousness

To summarize, we present a categorization matrix in Figure 1 which describes a framework for studying neural signatures of consciousness. In this framework, most study populations can be placed along two dimensions: cortical dynamics (normal or abnormal) and conscious state labels (known or unknown). While we feel that this matrix is a useful tool for thinking about study designs, not every study will, at present, fit cleanly into it. For example, in our Research Topic, Huntley et al. measured the perturbational complexity index (PCI) (Casali et al., 2013; Comolatti et al., 2019), a highly accurate (Casarotto et al., 2016) surrogate for consciousness, in three patients with severe Alzheimer's disease (AD). Each patient yielded PCI values at or slightly below the threshold for consciousness. Should severe AD be viewed, then, in the upper right quadrant of Figure 1, with PCI values dissociated from consciousness? Or should severe AD be viewed in the lower right quadrant, as a DoC (Huntley et al., 2021) in which PCI accurately tracks consciousness? We hope that once AD and other ambiguous conditions are better understood, the approach in Figure 1 will be broadly applicable.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Matrix of study populations for investigating neural biomarkers of consciousness broken down by the presence or absence of abnormal cortical dynamics and the presence or absence of high-confidence data labels to guide supervised machine learning. In the top left quadrant, such labels are available, but the risk of overfitting a binary classifier model to normal EEG features is high. This includes most studies of sleep and general anesthesia [but note that cognitive-motor dissociation sometimes occurs under anesthesia (Sebel et al., 2004); see also (Nilsen et al.)]. In the bottom left quadrant, cortical dynamics are normal (so we risk overfitting), and we don't even know if our subjects possess consciousness. This includes human fetuses and newborns (Frohlich et al., 2023a) and other animal species, especially those that are only distantly related to humans (Birch, 2022). Next, the bottom right quadrant includes subjects with abnormal cortical dynamics whose level of consciousness is uncertain, namely, DoC patients. Finally, the top right quadrant represents our ideal study population: cortical dynamics are abnormal and lack the features that we might otherwise overfit a classifier model to in the top left quadrant. Nonetheless, these participants are behaviorally responsive, giving us labels with which to train a classifier. Besides children with 15q disorders (Frohlich et al., 2022), this quadrant also includes healthy adults challenged with drugs like tiagabine (Nutt et al., 2015; Barnett et al., 2020; Darmani et al., 2021) which result in paradoxical pharmacological dissociations (Frohlich et al., 2023b). Alzheimer's disease (Huntley et al.) can be understood as an ambiguous case that might fall under either the top right or bottom right quadrant.

Finally, we feel humbled by the perspective shared by Nilsen et al. in our Research Topic, who ask, “Are we really ‘unconscious' in unconscious states?” The authors invite us to revisit common assumptions in light of a few key points: behavioral signs and subjective reports of consciousness or its absence are notoriously unreliable and, furthermore, “[s]tates presumed to be unconscious are not always devoid of experience.” Indeed, from birth until death, do the lights of consciousness ever switch off completely?

Conclusions

We advocate for studying dissociations between neural activity and conscious state as a key to identifying universal markers of consciousness. We look forward to further studies that will challenge not only candidate biomarkers of consciousness, but also our frameworks for understanding them.

Author contributions

JF: Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing. JC: Writing—review and editing. PM: Writing—review and editing. DT: Writing—review and editing. DB: Writing—review and editing.

Acknowledgments

We extend our gratitude to the authors and reviewers who participated in our Research Topic.

Conflict of interest

JF was a former employee of F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. (October 2016–July 2017).

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alkire, M. T., and Miller, J. (2005). General anesthesia and the neural correlates of consciousness. Prog. Brain Res. 150, 229–597. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50017-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett, L., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Seth, A. K. (2020). Decreased directed functional connectivity in the psychedelic state. NeuroImage 209:116462. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birch, J. (2022). The search for invertebrate consciousness. Nous 56, 133–153. doi: 10.1111/nous.12351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, E. N., Lydic, R., and Schiff, N. D. (2010). General anesthesia, sleep, and coma. N. Engl. J. Med. 363, 2638–2650. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0808281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casali, A. G., Gosseries, O., Rosanova, M., Boly, M., Sarasso, S., Casali, K. R., et al. (2013). A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Sci. Transl. Med. 5:198ra105. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casarotto, S., Comanducci, A., Rosanova, M., Sarasso, S., Fecchio, M., Napolitani, M., et al. (2016). Stratification of unresponsive patients by an independently validated index of brain complexity. Ann. Neurol. 80, 718–729. doi: 10.1002/ana.24779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comolatti, R., Pigorini, A., Casarotto, S., Fecchio, M., Faria, G., Sarasso, S., et al. (2019). A fast and general method to empirically estimate the complexity of brain responses to transcranial and intracranial stimulations. Brain Stimul. 12, 1280–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darmani, G., Nieminen, J., Bergmann, T., Ramezanpour, H., and Ziemann, U. (2021). A degraded state of consciousness in healthy awake humans? Brain Stimul. 14, 710–712. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frohlich, J., Bayne, T., DallaVecchia, A., Kirkeby-Hinrup, A., Mediano, P. A., Moser, J., et al. (2023a). Not with a “zap” but with a “beep”: measuring the origins of perinatal experience. NeuroImage 273:120057. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frohlich, J., Bird, L. M., Dell'Italia, J., Johnson, M. A., Hipp, J. F., and Monti, M. M. (2020). High-voltage, diffuse delta rhythms coincide with wakeful consciousness and complexity in angelman syndrome. Neurosci. Conscious. 2020:niaa005. doi: 10.1093/nc/niaa005.eCollection2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frohlich, J., Chiang, J. N., Mediano, P. A., Nespeca, M., Saravanapandian, V., Toker, D., Dell'Italia, J., et al. (2022). Neural complexity is a common denominator of human consciousness across diverse regimes of cortical dynamics. Commun. Biol. 5:1374. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-04331-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frohlich, J., Mediano, P. A., Bavato, F., and Gharabaghi, A. (2023b). Paradoxical pharmacological dissociations result from drugs that enhance delta oscillations but preserve consciousness. Commun. Biol. 6:654. doi: 10.1038/s42003-023-04988-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Z., Vlisides, P. E., Tarnal, V. C., Janke, E. L., Keefe, K. M., Collins, M. M., et al. (2018). Brain imaging reveals covert consciousness during behavioral unresponsiveness induced by propofol. Sci. Rep. 8:13195. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31436-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntley, J. D., Fleming, S. M., Mograbi, D. C., Bor, D., Naci, L., Owen, A. M., et al. (2021). Understanding Alzheimer's disease as a disorder of consciousness. Alzheimer's Dement. 7:e12203. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12203.eCollection2021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, P. W. (2004). The EEG in metabolic encephalopathy and coma. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 21, 307–318. doi: 10.1097/01.WNP.0000145004.22230.D5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, M. M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Pickard, J. D., Tshibanda, L., et al. (2010). Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N. Engl. J. Med. 362, 579–589. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ní Mhuircheartaigh, R., Warnaby, C., Rogers, R., Jbabdi, S., and Tracey, I. (2013). Slow-wave activity saturation and thalamocortical isolation during propofol anesthesia in humans. Sci. Transl. Med. 5:208ra148. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nutt, D., Wilson, S., Lingford-Hughes, A., Myers, J., Papadopoulos, A., and Muthukumaraswamy, S. (2015). Differences between magnetoencephalographic (MEG) spectral profiles of drugs acting on GABA at synaptic and extrasynaptic sites: a study in healthy volunteers. Neuropharmacology 88, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purdon, P. L., Pierce, E. T., Mukamel, E. A., Prerau, M. J., Walsh, J. L., Wong, K. F. K., et al. (2013). Electroencephalogram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness from propofol. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, E1142–E1151. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221180110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarasso, S., Casali, A. G., Casarotto, S., Rosanova, M., Sinigaglia, C., Massimini, M., et al. (2021). Consciousness and complexity: a consilience of evidence. Neurosci. Conscious. 7, 1–24. doi: 10.1093/nc/niab023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saravanapandian, V., Nadkarni, D., Hsu, S.-H., Hussain, S. A., Maski, K., Golshani, P., et al. (2021). Abnormal sleep physiology in children with 15q11. 2-13.1 duplication (dup15q) syndrome. Mol. Autism 12, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s13229-021-00460-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J., Ventura, M., Boly, M., Majerus, S., et al. (2009). Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurology 9:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-9-35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebel, P. S., Bowdle, T. A., Ghoneim, M. M., Rampil, I. J., Padilla, R. E., Gan, T. J., and Domino, K. B. (2004). The incidence of awareness during anesthesia: a multicenter united states study. Anesth. Anal. 99, 833–839. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000130261.90896.6C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verellen, R. M., and Cavazos, J. E. (2010). Post-traumatic epilepsy: an overview. Therapy 7, 527–531. doi: 10.2217/THY.10.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X.-S., Roy, R. J., and Jensen, E. W. (2001). EEG complexity as a measure of depth of anesthesia for patients. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 48, 1424–1433. doi: 10.1109/10.966601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, consciousness, sleep, general anesthesia, disorders of consciousness, biomarkers, coma, unresponsive wakefulness syndrome

Citation: Frohlich J, Crone JS, Mediano PAM, Toker D and Bor D (2023) Editorial: Dissociations between neural activity and conscious state: a key to understanding consciousness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1256168. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1256168

Received: 10 July 2023; Accepted: 20 July 2023;
Published: 02 August 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Christopher W. Tyler, Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, United States

Copyright © 2023 Frohlich, Crone, Mediano, Toker and Bor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joel Frohlich, jfneuro@pm.me

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.