Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 July 2023
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic Perspectives in Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation and Neuromodulation View all 7 articles

Would frontal midline theta indicate cognitive changes induced by non-invasive brain stimulation? A mini review

  • 1Department of Physiological Sciences, Federal University of Espírito Santo, Vitória, Brazil
  • 2Superior School of Sciences of the Santa Casa de Misericórdia de Vitória (EMESCAM), Vitória, Brazil
  • 3FABRA- Higher Education Center, Vitória, Brazil
  • 4Post Graduation Program in Physiological Sciences, Federal University of Espírito Santo, Vitória, Brazil

To the best of our knowledge, neurophysiological markers indicating changes induced by non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) on cognitive performance, especially one of the most investigated under these procedures, working memory (WM), are little known. Here, we will briefly introduce frontal midline theta (FM-theta) oscillation (4–8 Hz) as a possible indicator for NIBS effects on WM processing. Electrophysiological recordings of FM-theta oscillation seem to originate in the medial frontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex, but they may be driven more subcortically. FM-theta has been acknowledged to occur during memory and emotion processing, and it has been related to WM and sustained attention. It mainly occurs in the frontal region during a delay period, in which specific information previously shown is no longer perceived and must be manipulated to allow a later (delayed) response and observed in posterior regions during information maintenance. Most NIBS studies investigating effects on cognitive performance have used n-back tasks that mix manipulation and maintenance processes. Thus, if considering FM-theta as a potential neurophysiological indicator for NIBS effects on different WM components, adequate cognitive tasks should be considered to better address the complexity of WM processing. Future research should also evaluate the potential use of FM-theta as an index of the therapeutic effects of NIBS intervention on neuropsychiatric disorders, especially those involving the ventral medial prefrontal cortex and cognitive dysfunctions.

Introduction

Few years ago, when investigating clinical effects of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) technique applied over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in substance use disorders, we looked for biological evidence. Using low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) focused on event related potential (ERP) P3 segment, we observed that a region located more ventrally in the medial prefrontal cortex (the ventral medial prefrontal cortex, vmPFC) showed the highest electrophysiological change under drug-related cues in patients with alcohol or crack-cocaine use disorders maintaining abstinence after multiple sessions of the bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dlPFC (Nakamura-Palacios et al., 2016). With this analysis, together with few neuroimaging data, we showed that NIBS application over the dorsal region of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) could produce changes in brain activation of the medial frontal region that were not directly target (Nakamura-Palacios et al., 2016). Following, we started to search for what could be underneath this evidence.

Most neuropsychiatric disorders are associated with important dysfunction in frontal regions, more specifically the vmPFC (Schneider and Koenigs, 2017; Hiser and Koenigs, 2018). This brain region plays a crucial role in a multitude of complex psychological functions that underlie adaptive human behavior, notably in value-based decision-making, emotion-related psychophysiology and social cognition (Schneider and Koenigs, 2017).

Brain oscillations have been suggested as biological markers in neuropsychiatric disorders (Yener and Basar, 2013), although Newson and Thiagarajan (2018) pondered that power changes within specific frequency bands may not be specific to a particular disorder. They may overlap substantially across disorders and may show a great variability within disorders (Newson and Thiagarajan, 2018).

However, frontal midline theta oscillations seems to be related to anxiety conditions and have been suggested as an instrument to evaluate symptoms of anxiety in generalized anxiety disorder as its appearance seems to be closely related to improved anxiety symptoms under treatment with anti-anxiety drugs (Suetsugi et al., 2000). It also seems to be associated with depression as the induction of frontal theta oscillations by the repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) facilitates giving-up behavior with a consequent lower tendency of mental rumination, a risk factor for depressive states (Miyauchi and Kawasaki, 2021).

Thus, we wondered if NIBS induced changes in vmPFC activity, and probably a subsequent frontal theta synchronicity within cognitive networks could indicate therapeutic effects of these interventions, especially when aiming to improve cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders (Lefaucheur et al., 2020; Brunoni et al., 2021; Fregni et al., 2021).

Indeed, one major goal of NIBS, such as TMS, tDCS, or transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), is to promote neuroenhancement and neurorehabilitation using the potential of protocols to promote changes in cortical excitability and generate neuroplasticity in circuits underlying working memory (WM) and executive functions (EFs) (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014; Hara et al., 2021; Antal et al., 2022). However, neurophysiological markers associated with their impact on cognition and behavior are little known. In a recent systematic review and metanalysis, Mendes et al. (2022) suggested that the parietal ERP P3 amplitude could be a potential neural signature for changes induced by frontal anodal tDCS on cognitive performance, more specifically during oddball and n-back tasks.

Considering the evidence in the literature showing that theta waves consistently occur in the frontal midline during the processing of memory and emotion related to WM and EFs, we started to explore whether this electrophysiological rhythm could indicate the effects of NIBS on these cognitive processes. We first searched for articles published in the last 10 years (2012–2022) on the MEDLINE/PUBMED database, using the terms (defined by Medical Subject Headings–MeSH): “Theta Rhythm” combined (using the Boolean operator AND) with “Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation,” “Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation,” “Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation,” “Medial Prefrontal Cortex,” resulting in 478 articles. 365 were excluded initially because of title inadequacy followed by abstracts that did not fit within the scope of our goal. The remaining 113 publications underwent full reading, of which 49 were considered relevant to be included in this brief review. Additionally, 14 articles were subsequently included to support the context.

Frontal midline theta oscillations were first named FM-theta (or Fmθ) by Ishihara and Yoshi (1972) when they were observed in juvenile delinquents performing continuous arithmetic calculations. The recordings of theta rhythms possibly originate in the medial frontal cortex (MFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Tollner et al., 2017), but the thalamus possibly drives them, although this requires further investigation (Mitchell et al., 2008).

Frontal midline-theta has been associated with broad cognitive functions (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Hsieh and Ranganath, 2014; Ishii et al., 2014; Herweg et al., 2020) and it has been suggested that it would indicate different processes depending on task requirements (Eschmann et al., 2018). According to Mitchell et al. (2008), the cognitive function of FM-theta at any particular temporal point will depend on which other structures the frontal region is interacting with in that moment, resulting in very distinct cognitive competences sharing a common level or means of computation. Additionally, Berger et al. (2019) suggested a role of gatekeeper to FM-theta, which would provide an efficient mechanism allowing or preventing remote neocortical areas to have access to PFC cognitive resources depending on cognitive demands.

The characteristics of FM-theta were detailed by Mitchell et al. (2008) and some will be depicted here. These theta oscillations are maximal in the frontal-midline regions (F3, Fz, F4) observed in electrophysiological (EEG) studies. It is mostly found around 6 Hz in sinusoidal waveforms with amplitude around 50–75 μV during behavioral tasks. It usually appears in discrete bursts for a few seconds but may range from 1 to >10 s; it tends to “wax and wane” and maybe phase- and time-locked related to behavioral tasks.

It is also essential to consider a few limitations. For instance, it seems that not all individuals display theta oscillations on EEGs signals during cognitive task performance, being of low occurrence, especially in old adults (Mitchell et al., 2008). Thus, it appears to be age-related and is most common in young adults, with its occurrence decreasing significantly after 30 years of age (Mitchell et al., 2008). However, these data were mainly based on FM-theta spontaneous induction during cognitive task performance. When using signal processing techniques and, especially, when it is evoked by events (Mitchell et al., 2008) using time-locked paradigms (Doppelmayr et al., 2000; Kieffaber et al., 2023), its occurrence is larger.

WM and FM-theta

Here we will bring evidence proving the relationship between FM-theta and WM. Let us start by defining WM. It can be defined as a system or a process constituted by multiple components or a working system assembling distinct cognitive processes (Baddeley, 2003; Repovs and Baddeley, 2006). It contains two distinct components (Smith and Jonides, 1999) or sub-systems (Ratcliffe et al., 2022). One is a short-term storage for active maintenance, or a representational system, holding task-relevant information content for a limited period, possibly distributed in posterior regions, such as occipital/parietal or temporal areas, depending on task demands (Kawasaki et al., 2014). The other component is a set of “executive processes” associated with goal-directed behavior that operates on the content of the storage or representational system managed by the PFC and related networks (Smith and Jonides, 1999; Friedman and Robbins, 2022; Menon and D’Esposito, 2022; Ratcliffe et al., 2022).

Thus, WM processing requires the storage buffer and central EFs to be coordinated with the contribution of different brain regions with diverse functional roles. The neural mechanisms governing these processes seem to be signaled by theta and alpha waves detected in EEG recordings (Kawasaki et al., 2010). The relationship between theta and alpha oscillations was examined by Riddle et al. (2020) in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) acquired during a retro-cued delayed WM task performance. They confirmed a model in which theta oscillations are excitatory to neural activity at frontal sites and alpha oscillations are inhibitory at posterior (parietal) sites, and they need to be balanced for WM performance (Riddle et al., 2020).

Behavioral tasks that seem to better examine the neural mechanisms underlying WM functions are those with delayed response (Funahashi, 2017). In these tasks a cued stimulus presented is no longer shown during a delay period, usually for seconds or longer. Over this time interval the information regarding the stimulus must be kept online (and manipulated or not according to the given instruction). By the end of the delay period the information needs to be retrieved from the temporary storage (maintenance) to allow the response to be given for task completion.

Patterns of enhanced FM-theta coherence, meaning increased synchronization between two cortical sites, seem specific to the delay period in delayed response tasks (Sarnthein et al., 1998). Moreover, FM-theta power seems to increase parametrically with memory load and is sustained during the delay period (Jensen and Tesche, 2002). Additionally, Sauseng et al. (2002) showed that evoked theta oscillations spread from anterior to posterior EEG recording sites when an individual retrieves information previously encoded in long-term memory system. They reverse the direction to frontal areas when information is successfully retrieved, a phenomenon they suggested reflects the transfer of information between WM and long-term memory systems.

Eschmann et al. (2018) found greater FM-theta activity (indicated by frequency power) in conditions with a high need for cognitive control. Still, it was focally activated in frontal sites in proactive (delayed matching to sample) tasks, yet broadly distributed in a reactive task (Stroop test). Ratcliffe et al. (2022), using a delayed matching to sample task and introducing a delayed procedure in one- and two-back tasks, showed focal FM-theta power increases with WM engagement during the delay interval. Decoding the WM content is driven by posterior sites, which grows with functional theta coupling to fronto-medial channels. In addition, they observed that FM-theta frequency speed (ranging from 4 to 8 Hz) dropped toward the lower end (peaking at 5 Hz) with increased WM load (one- vs. two-back tasks).

Indeed, FM-theta power seems to rise in conditions with high WM load and task difficulty, and the increase in theta activity during WM also appears to predict later long-term memory retrieval for correct responses as it has not been observed when the responses are incorrect (Eschmann et al., 2018).

Kawasaki et al. (2010), found that FM-theta was observed when information was manipulated during delay periods in auditory and visual WM tasks, but not during maintenance periods, whereas alpha wave increases in both manipulation and maintenance periods in the temporal area for the auditory WM tasks and the parietal area for visual WM tasks. Furthermore, FM-theta phase synchronization (the phase relation of theta oscillation between two different brain regions) (Fell and Axmacher, 2011) occurred only between the to-be-manipulated modality-related brain regions, such as frontoparietal synchronization for the visual modality and frontotemporal synchronization for the auditory-verbal modality (Kawasaki et al., 2014).

However, according to Berger et al. (2019) FM-theta occurs irrespectively if information is manipulated or not (retention only) during the delay period (Berger et al., 2019). According to these authors, the complexity of cognitive demand (number of items and if it is a simple retention or if mental manipulation is required) seems to define where the posterior (parietal and/or temporal) fast rhythm (gamma) will be nested into prefrontal slow waves (peak or trough expressed by FM-theta). When fast rhythms are nested in FM-theta troughs, they engage the dynamic coupling of fronto-temporo-parietal control networks, allowing the performance of a complex WM task. When gamma is nested in FM-theta peak a network decoupling seems to happen when performing a less demanding WM task.

FM-theta, WM, and NIBS

In this section we will introduce NIBS effects on WM followed by electrophysiological evidence under these conditions. Although FM-theta has been consistently observed during many cognitive tasks, here we will focus on WM. This is because this cognitive function is one of the most studied under NIBS. Most of these studies have investigated the effects of the stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) on the performance of n-back tasks, but, unfortunately, showing mixed results.

N-back tasks involve the continuous presentation of items, in which the participant must indicate whether an actual item presented is the same as presented one-, two-, three-, or n-times back, usually with no delay introduced, so items are presented consecutively. A clear relationship exists between this task and FM-theta spectral activity, attributed to the allocation of attentional resources related to WM (Mitchell et al., 2008). However, WM components are not well distinguished with this method.

In their meta-analysis, Brunoni and Vanderhasselt (2014) conclude that repetitive application of TMS (rTMS) over the dlPFC significantly increases accuracy and hastens response time. In contrast, tDCS improves only one parameter, the response time, with no effect on accuracy.

However, Zaehle et al. (2011) observed that anodal tDCS increases WM performance measured by a letter two-back task and amplifies oscillatory power in the theta and alpha bands in the occipito-parietal region. The cathodal tDCS, on the other hand, decreases oscillatory power in the theta and alpha bands in these posterior electrode sites and interferes with the regular repetition-related increase in WM performance. With these findings they suggested that tDCS over the left dlPFC would modulate WM performance by altering the underlying oscillatory brain activity according to its polarity (Zaehle et al., 2011).

Hoy et al. (2013) found greater theta event-related synchronization and alpha event-related desynchronization at the frontal (F3) site immediately following stimulation with a low dose of the anodal tDCS over the left dlPFC, which improved two-back task performance.

Using a delayed WM task, Jones et al. (2020) observed that anodal tDCS over the right dlPFC (F4) alternating with the right parietal cortex (P4), in a counterbalanced order, increased the phase-amplitude coupling between PFC theta oscillations and parietal gamma activity which was associated with WM training enhancement.

Differently from tDCS, a frequency-modulated brain stimulation can be implemented with TMS and tACS (Lage et al., 2016; Lefaucheur, 2019; Hosseinian et al., 2021). These techniques can be applied at similar endogenous frequencies of interest, such as theta, alpha, gamma, or at high frequencies watching for repercussions over neural oscillations.

Studies investigating the tACS, especially in theta and gamma frequencies, most over the left dlPFC, with some including parietal cortex, show inconsistent results on WM, especially in young, healthy participants or in a single session. Yet, it seems more favorable, either in one or in multiple sessions, in low performers such as old adults and patients with neurodegenerative diseases (Al Qasem et al., 2022).

Chander et al. (2016) observed that when FM-theta oscillations are phase locked by tACS applied over FPz and Pz in young subjects, the power increase of endogenous FM-theta is blocked, and WM performance measured by a two-back task is impaired. This suggests that endogenous FM-theta phase regulation is required for the stabilization and maintenance of temporal order information for correct responses in n-back tasks (Chander et al., 2016). Using similar electrode locations (FPz and Pz) but with tACS with frequency below the individual theta frequency, Vosskuhl et al. (2015) observed an increase in short-term memory capacity measured by forward digit span during the tACS, but, the WM, measured by backward digit span and three-back task, was not affected. Considering a theta-gamma coding theory, in which each gamma wave represents one memory item and each theta wave represent the list of items stored to be recall, Vosskuhl et al. (2015) suggested that lower theta frequencies would fit more gamma waves onto each theta wave.

When applied bilaterally over the dlPFC, tACS improved verbal WM accuracy in young subjects (Meiron and Lavidor, 2014), and when combined with TMS, both applied bilaterally over F3 and F4, in a complex phase-synchronized low-intensity electric and magnetic stimulation technique as developed by Hosseinian et al. (2021), a narrowband 6-Hz theta oscillations was induced and stabilized and processing of WM was enhanced.

Using TMS alone delivered in theta or alpha frequencies over the left frontal or left parietal region, matching or not to the task-driven theta or alpha oscillations, seen by neuroimaging analysis during a retro-cued delayed WM task, Riddle et al. (2020) demonstrated a causal role for theta and alpha neural oscillations in, respectively, prioritization and suppression of WM representations. They suggested that an optimal excitatory-inhibitory (or theta-alpha) balance at the fronto-parietal network is required to manage a successful WM performance (Riddle et al., 2020).

Employing TMS at high frequency to frontal (Fz), temporal (TP7) or parietal (Pz) areas paired with EEG during an auditory delayed WM task, Miyauchi et al. (2016) explored the directionality of the theta phase synchronization between frontal and sensory areas. They found that TMS increased theta phase synchronization when delivered over sensory areas, but not when delivered over frontal area, suggesting that theta phase synchrony induced by TMS during WM processing was bottom-up directed.

Berger et al. (2019) also delivered a fast (50 Hz) triple-pulse of TMS over the right temporo-parietal site during the through of the FM-theta cycle and found that a delayed task performance was disrupted. This result proved a causal relationship between the nesting of posterior (temporo-parietal) gamma bursts into specific FM-theta phases. They suggested that task-relevant temporo-parietal neural activity must be pulsed and synchronized to FM-theta waves to guide an efficient WM performance (Berger et al., 2019).

Neurochemical and molecular mechanisms underlying FM-theta and NIBS

So far, evidence is growing that NIBS may induce FM-theta oscillations related to WM processing. To that end, they likely share biochemical and molecular mechanisms.

Few neurochemistry studies have associated the variation in FM-theta with variation in monoamine (dopamine, noradrenaline, and serotonin) systems. It seems that not only their levels but the balance between them is important in the control of this frontal rhythm (Mitchell et al., 2008).

Anodal tDCS and TMS enhance excitatory synaptic transmissions possibly by facilitating cortical glutamate transmission and suppressing gamma-aminobutyric acid transmission. They also modulate positively or negatively the activities of dopamine, serotonin, and acetylcholine transmissions, events that may change the balance between excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Chervyakov et al., 2015; Yamada and Sumiyoshi, 2021).

The molecular basis of the synchronicity of theta oscillation between the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala and hippocampus in conditioning responses associated with fear and reward in rodents, seems to be associated with the expression of receptors and proteins fundamental to the process of brain plasticity, such as NMDA (N-methyl D-Aspartate) receptors, dopamine receptors, c-FOS and CREB (cyclic-AMP response element binding protein) (Bocchio et al., 2017; Anders et al., 2018).

These molecular mechanisms also mediate cortical changes induced by NIBS (Pelletier and Cicchetti, 2014; Chervyakov et al., 2015; Yamada and Sumiyoshi, 2021). Animal studies have shown that tDCS enhances the expression of NMDA and BDNF (Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor) and involves AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid) receptor modulation and GAP-43 (cortical growth-associated protein) expression, possibly related to improving cognitive performance (Wu et al., 2017; de Souza Custodio et al., 2018; Martins et al., 2019; Nakamura-Palacios et al., 2021). However, Serrano et al. (2022) observed a reduction of serum BDNF which was correlated with the improvement of WM and other cognitive tasks induced by home-based anodal tDCS in patients with fibromyalgia. Regarding TMS, long-lasting therapeutic effects seem to be related to LTP (long-term potentiation) and LTD (long-term depression) involving post-synaptic NMDA receptors, changing BDNF production in stimulated and remote brain regions, but increased, unchanged, or decreased in the serum and cerebrospinal fluid (Chervyakov et al., 2015; Lefaucheur, 2019). There is also evidence that tACS may also induce plasticity mediated by NMDA receptors in the motor cortex, because the after-effects of 20 Hz tACS was suppressed by a NMDA antagonist (Wischnewski et al., 2019, 2023) and may potentially induce BDNF changes (He et al., 2023).

Conclusion

Bearing in mind experimental limitations and limited evidence, FM-theta could be a potential electrophysiological indicator for NIBS effects on distinct WM components. Furthermore, FM-theta oscillations could also indicate the potential therapeutical effects of NIBS on neuropsychiatric disorders associated with vmPFC and cognitive dysfunctions. Evidently, this hypothesis needs to be specifically investigated in the future.

Author contributions

EN-P conceived the original idea, directed the discussion of references included in the text, assembled the parts worked by the other authors, and wrote the main text. AF schematized the search keys in the literature with all the other authors and drafted the introduction. QA, LP, MZ, CR, and PL worked and drafted parts of the text included in the main text. All authors discussed the content and contributed to the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Al Qasem, W., Abubaker, M., and Kvasnak, E. (2022). Working memory and transcranial-alternating current stimulation-state of the art: Findings, missing, and challenges. Front. Psychol. 13:822545. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.822545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anders, Q. S., Klauss, J., Rodrigues, L. C. M., and Nakamura-Palacios, E. M. (2018). FosB mRNA expression in peripheral blood lymphocytes in drug addicted patients. Front. Pharmacol. 9:1205. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.01205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Luber, B., Brem, A. K., Bikson, M., Brunoni, A. R., Cohen Kadosh, R., et al. (2022). Non-invasive brain stimulation and neuroenhancement. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 7, 146–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2022.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. J. Commun. Disord. 36, 189–208. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00019-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, B., Griesmayr, B., Minarik, T., Biel, A. L., Pinal, D., Sterr, A., et al. (2019). Dynamic regulation of interregional cortical communication by slow brain oscillations during working memory. Nat. Commun. 10:4242. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12057-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocchio, M., Nabavi, S., and Capogna, M. (2017). Synaptic plasticity, engrams, and network oscillations in amygdala circuits for storage and retrieval of emotional memories. Neuron 94, 731–743. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., Nitsche, M. A., and Loo, C. K. E. (2021). Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Switzerland: Springer Nature.

Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., and Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 86, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chander, B. S., Witkowski, M., Braun, C., Robinson, S. E., Born, J., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2016). tACS phase locking of frontal midline theta oscillations disrupts working memory performance. Front. Cell Neurosci. 10:120. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chervyakov, A. V., Chernyavsky, A. Y., Sinitsyn, D. O., and Piradov, M. A. (2015). Possible mechanisms underlying the therapeutic effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:303. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Souza Custodio, J. C., Martins, C. W., Lugon, M., de Melo Rodrigues, L. C., de Figueiredo, S. G., and Nakamura-Palacios, E. M. (2018). Prefrontal BDNF levels after anodal epidural direct current stimulation in rats. Front. Pharmacol. 9:755. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Schwaiger, J., Stadler, W., and Rohm, D. (2000). The time locked theta response reflects interindividual differences in human memory performance. Neurosci. Lett. 278, 141–144. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00925-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eschmann, K. C. J., Bader, R., and Mecklinger, A. (2018). Topographical differences of frontal-midline theta activity reflect functional differences in cognitive control abilities. Brain Cogn. 123, 57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2011). The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 105–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fregni, F., El-Hagrassy, M. M., Pacheco-Barrios, K., Carvalho, S., Leite, J., Simis, M., et al. (2021). Evidence-based guidelines and secondary meta-analysis for the use of transcranial direct current stimulation in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 256–313. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyaa051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Robbins, T. W. (2022). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 72–89. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funahashi, S. (2017). Working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Sci. 7:49. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7050049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hara, T., Shanmugalingam, A., McIntyre, A., and Burhan, A. M. (2021). The effect of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) on attention and memory function in stroke rehabilitation patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Diagnostics 11:227. doi: 10.3390/diagnostics11020227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Liu, S., Chen, L., Ke, Y., and Ming, D. (2023). Neurophysiological mechanisms of transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 17:1091925. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1091925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herweg, N. A., Solomon, E. A., and Kahana, M. J. (2020). Theta oscillations in human memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 208–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiser, J., and Koenigs, M. (2018). The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 638–647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosseinian, T., Yavari, F., Kuo, M. F., Nitsche, M. A., and Jamil, A. (2021). Phase synchronized 6 Hz transcranial electric and magnetic stimulation boosts frontal theta activity and enhances working memory. Neuroimage 245:118772. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Emonson, M. R., Arnold, S. L., Thomson, R. H., Daskalakis, Z. J., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). Testing the limits: Investigating the effect of tDCS dose on working memory enhancement in healthy controls. Neuropsychologia 51, 1777–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, L. T., and Ranganath, C. (2014). Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage 85, 721–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishihara, T., and Yoshi, N. (1972). Multivariate analytic study of EEG and mental activity in juvenile delinquents. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 33, 71–80. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(72)90026-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishii, R., Canuet, L., Ishihara, T., Aoki, Y., Ikeda, S., Hata, M., et al. (2014). Frontal midline theta rhythm and gamma power changes during focused attention on mental calculation: an MEG beamformer analysis. Front. Hum Neurosci. 8:406. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1395–1399. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Johnson, E. L., and Berryhill, M. E. (2020). Frontoparietal theta-gamma interactions track working memory enhancement with training and tDCS. Neuroimage 211:116615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, M., Kitajo, K., and Yamaguchi, Y. (2010). Dynamic links between theta executive functions and alpha storage buffers in auditory and visual working memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1683–1689. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07217.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, M., Kitajo, K., and Yamaguchi, Y. (2014). Fronto-parietal and fronto-temporal theta phase synchronization for visual and auditory-verbal working memory. Front. Psychol. 5:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kieffaber, P. D., Osborne, J., Norton, E., and Hilimire, M. (2023). Deconstructing the functional significance of the error-related negativity (ERN) and midline frontal theta oscillations using stepwise time-locking and single-trial response dynamics. Neuroimage 274:120113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lage, C., Wiles, K., Shergill, S. S., and Tracy, D. K. (2016). A systematic review of the effects of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognition. J. Neural Transm. 123, 1479–1490. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1592-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefaucheur, J. P. (2019). Transcranial magnetic stimulation. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 160, 559–580. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-64032-1.00037-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefaucheur, J. P., Aleman, A., Baeken, C., Benninger, D. H., Brunelin, J., Di Lazzaro, V., et al. (2020). Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): An update (2014-2018). Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 474–528. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martins, C. W., de Melo Rodrigues, L. C., Nitsche, M. A., and Nakamura-Palacios, E. M. (2019). AMPA receptors are involved in prefrontal direct current stimulation effects on long-term working memory and GAP-43 expression. Behav. Brain Res. 362, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiron, O., and Lavidor, M. (2014). Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendes, A. J., Pacheco-Barrios, K., Lema, A., Goncalves, O. F., Fregni, F., Leite, J., et al. (2022). Modulation of the cognitive event-related potential P3 by transcranial direct current stimulation: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132, 894–907. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and D’Esposito, M. (2022). The role of PFC networks in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 90–103. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01152-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, D. J., McNaughton, N., Flanagan, D., and Kirk, I. J. (2008). Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Prog. Neurobiol. 86, 156–185. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyauchi, E., and Kawasaki, M. (2021). Behavioural effects of task-relevant neuromodulation by rTMS on giving-up. Sci. Rep. 11:22250. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-01645-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyauchi, E., Kitajo, K., and Kawasaki, M. (2016). TMS-induced theta phase synchrony reveals a bottom-up network in working memory. Neurosci. Lett. 622, 10–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura-Palacios, E. M., Lopes, I. B., Souza, R. A., Klauss, J., Batista, E. K., Conti, C. L., et al. (2016). Ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) as a target of the dorsolateral prefrontal modulation by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in drug addiction. J. Neural Transm. 123, 1179–1194. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1559-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura-Palacios, E. M., Ronchete, C. F., Felippe, L. V., Ferreira, L. V. B., Anders, Q. S., and Rodrigues, L. C. M. (2021). “Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Substance Use Disorders,” in Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Neuropsychiatric Disorders, eds A. R. Brunoni, M. A. Nitsche, and C. K. Loo (Switzerland: Springer Nature), 533–564.

Google Scholar

Newson, J. J., and Thiagarajan, T. C. (2018). EEG frequency bands in psychiatric disorders: a review of resting state studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:521. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelletier, S. J., and Cicchetti, F. (2014). Cellular and molecular mechanisms of action of transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence from in vitro and in vivo models. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18:yu047. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyu047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliffe, O., Shapiro, K., and Staresina, B. P. (2022). Fronto-medial theta coordinates posterior maintenance of working memory content. Curr. Biol. 32, 2121–2129.e2123. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.03.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repovs, G., and Baddeley, A. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 139, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddle, J., Scimeca, J. M., Cellier, D., Dhanani, S., and D’Esposito, M. (2020). Causal evidence for a role of theta and alpha oscillations in the control of working memory. Curr. Biol. 30, 1748–1754.e1744. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarnthein, J., Petsche, H., Rappelsberger, P., Shaw, G. L., and von Stein, A. (1998). Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 7092–7096. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Gruber, W., Doppelmayr, M., Stadler, W., and Schabus, M. (2002). The interplay between theta and alpha oscillations in the human electroencephalogram reflects the transfer of information between memory systems. Neurosci. Lett. 324, 121–124. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00225-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B., and Koenigs, M. (2017). Human lesion studies of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 107, 84–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrano, P. V., Zortea, M., Alves, R. L., Beltrán, G., Bavaresco, C., Ramalho, L., et al. (2022). The effect of home-based transcranial direct current stimulation in cognitive performance in fibromyalgia: A randomized, double-blind sham-controlled trial. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 24:17. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.992742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283, 1657–1661.

Google Scholar

Suetsugi, M., Mizuki, Y., Ushijima, I., Kobayashi, T., Tsuchiya, K., Aoki, T., et al. (2000). Appearance of frontal midline theta activity in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Neuropsychobiology 41, 108–112. doi: 10.1159/000026641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tollner, T., Wang, Y., Makeig, S., Muller, H. J., Jung, T. P., and Gramann, K. (2017). Two independent frontal midline theta oscillations during conflict detection and adaptation in a simon-type manual reaching task. J. Neurosci. 37, 2504–2515. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1752-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2015). Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci 9:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wischnewski, M., Alekseichuk, I., and Opitz, A. (2023). Neurocognitive, physiological, and biophysical effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 27, 189–205. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wischnewski, M., Engelhardt, M., Salehinejad, M. A., Schutter, D., Kuo, M. F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2019). NMDA receptor-mediated motor cortex plasticity after 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2924–2931. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y. J., Lin, C. C., Yeh, C. M., Chien, M. E., Tsao, M. C., Tseng, P., et al. (2017). Repeated transcranial direct current stimulation improves cognitive dysfunction and synaptic plasticity deficit in the prefrontal cortex of streptozotocin-induced diabetic rats. Brain Stimul. 10, 1079–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, Y., and Sumiyoshi, T. (2021). Neurobiological mechanisms of transcranial direct current stimulation for psychiatric disorders; neurophysiological, chemical, and anatomical considerations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:631838. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.631838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yener, G. G., and Basar, E. (2013). Brain oscillations as biomarkers in neuropsychiatric disorders: following an interactive panel discussion and synopsis. Suppl. Clin. Neurophysiol. 62, 343–363. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-7020-5307-8.00016-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaehle, T., Sandmann, P., Thorne, J. D., Jancke, L., and Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: frontal midline theta (FM-theta), executive functions (EFs), working memory (WM), cognitive control, non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), transcranial alternate current stimulation (tACS), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Citation: Nakamura-Palacios EM, Falçoni Júnior AT, Anders QS, de Paula LdSP, Zottele MZ, Ronchete CF and Lirio PHC (2023) Would frontal midline theta indicate cognitive changes induced by non-invasive brain stimulation? A mini review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1116890. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1116890

Received: 05 December 2022; Accepted: 29 June 2023;
Published: 14 July 2023.

Edited by:

Sandra Carvalho, University of Aveiro, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Augusto José Mendes, Université de Genève, Switzerland
Diego Pinal, University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Nakamura-Palacios, Falçoni Júnior, Anders, de Paula, Zottele, Ronchete and Lirio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ester Miyuki Nakamura-Palacios, emnpalacios@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.