Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 15 February 2023
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Perspective taking in the human brain: complementary evidence from neuroimaging studies with media-based naturalistic stimuli and artificial controlled paradigms

  • 1Brain and Mind Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University School of Science, Espoo, Finland
  • 2International Laboratory of Social Neurobiology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia

Perception and interpretation of various types of events and information in life crucially depend on one’s perspective. A specific perspective can be explicitly adopted, for example, via instructing an experimental subject, implicitly via a priori information given to subjects, and by subjects’ personality traits or cultural background. The neural basis of perspective taking has been addressed in a number of recent neuroimaging studies, some of which have used movies and narratives as media-based stimuli to pursue a holistic understanding of the phenomenon under ecologically valid conditions. Results across these studies suggest that the human brain flexibly adapts to support the information-processing needs of different perspectives, however, also that inferior temporal-occipital areas and posterior-medial parietal areas are engaged across different perspectives. These findings are complemented by studies that have investigated specific aspects of perspective taking with highly controlled experimental designs. They have disclosed involvement of the temporoparietal junction in visual perspective taking and the importance of the affective component of the pain matrix when empathizing with others’ pain. Identification with the protagonists also seems to matter, as dorsomedial vs. ventromedial prefrontal areas are recruited when the protagonist is dissimilar vs. similar to self. Finally, as a translational aspect, perspective taking can, under certain conditions, serve as an effective emotion regulation technique, wherein lateral and medial regions of the prefrontal cortex seem to support reappraisal processes. Together, findings from studies with media-based stimuli and more traditional paradigms complement each other to gain a comprehensive understanding of the neural basis of perspective taking.

Introduction

As famously phrased by Obi-Wan Kenobi, the fictional Jedi-master character in Star Wars “many of the truths we cling to depend greatly on our own point of view” (Return of the Jedi, Lucasfilm Ltd., 1983). Indeed, the same event can be perceived differently by viewers, depending on, for example, their goals, a priori information, their personalities, and their cultural backgrounds. In general, perspective taking is an important and multifaceted human social-cognitive ability. It can refer to emotionally putting oneself into the shoes of others (e.g., “feeling the other’s sadness”), cognitively assessing how another person could see a particular situation (e.g., “based on what he suspects, it is no wonder that he is worried”), adopting of an expert point of view (e.g., an architect seeing an urban environment differently from a layperson), as well as cognitively reappraising one’s situation (e.g., re-thinking a feared outcome as not realistic). Perspective can be taken either explicitly, for example, when a subject adopts a particular perspective, or implicitly, for example, when a priori information or one’s cultural background guides one to look at an event from a particular point of view. Perspective taking has translational utility, as it is widely utilized in psychotherapy (Clark, 2023) and in interventions to alleviate inter group tensions (Halperin et al., 2013).

Elucidation of the neural basis of perspective taking has been the quest in an increasing number of neuroimaging studies. Some have utilized dynamic media-based stimuli such as movies that subjects have viewed from different perspectives. The strength of this approach, facilitated by development of new data analysis approaches, is that perceptual, cognitive, and emotional functions can be engaged in subjects in more ecologically valid ways (Hasson et al., 2010; Jääskeläinen et al., 2021). In general, a pattern of results suggests that there are both brain areas specific to the particulars of a given perspective taking task and brain areas that participate across different perspective taking tasks. In addition, there are a number of studies wherein specific instances of perspective taking have been scrutinized using highly controlled experimental designs. These studies have pinpointed neural mechanisms involved in, for example, feeling the emotions and when adopting the visual perspective of another person. We will go over the findings of these studies, starting with the more holistic results obtained in the studies with media-based naturalistic stimuli. This will be followed by going over the more specific findings from studies that have utilized more controlled tasks, as these offer cues into why certain areas might be engaged when we take perspective under more naturalistic conditions. These findings are summarized in Figure 1 and Table 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A simplified summary schematic of brain regions that are involved in perspective-taking tasks. Areas consistently involved in studies with media-based stimuli include the precuneous, LOC, and IT/O, with the precuneus also being an important part of the DMN that is plotted with yellow color. Areas involved with specific perspectives, such as empathy to other’s pain, are plotted with different colors. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AI, anterior insula; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; DMN, default mode network; DMPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; IT/O, inferior temporal/occipital cortex; LOC, lateral occipital cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; TPJ, temporoparietal junction.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies with different types of perspective taking studies.

Perspective taking modulates processing of media-based naturalistic stimuli

There are an increasing number of studies that have presented experimental subjects with media-based stimuli (i.e., movies and narratives) during neuroimaging while the subjects have been either explicitly asked or implicitly guided to adopt different perspectives. Such studies have offered important clues into how the brain filters information under ecologically valid, complex and dynamic conditions based on the type of perspective.

In an early study, subjects watched a 10-min clip from the perspectives of a forensic detective (to figure out who is guilty of murder) vs. interior/exterior decorator (to assess how to improve the interiors and exteriors shown in the clip; Lahnakoski et al., 2014). Between-condition differences were assessed using inter-subject correlation (ISC) of brain activity. The ISC compares the response time course in each brain region from one subject to the response time courses obtained in the same brain region from other subjects, identifying brain regions with similar responses, and, thus, also of shared processing and/or understanding across subjects (Hasson et al., 2008; Yeshurun et al., 2021). It was calculated for each voxel as the average Pearson correlation coefficient of brain activity time series across all pairs of subjects (Hasson et al., 2004; Kauppi et al., 2010). The detective perspective, in general, induced higher ISC of brain activity, including in precuneus, than the decorator perspective, which may reflect that the subjects were more idiosyncratic in how they adopted the decorator perspective. Within-perspectives vs. between-perspectives ISC analysis showed higher within-perspectives ISC in lateral occipital areas and in posterior parietal areas that are part of the dorsal attention network (DAN; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), thought to be active when a person is engaging attention in the external environment, as well as in posterior hippocampus (Lahnakoski et al., 2014). Naturally, interpreting this result, one has to keep in mind that the task involved contrasting social (i.e., subjects paying attention to the protagonists for suspicious behaviors) vs. non-social (i.e., subjects paying attention to decorations) perspectives.

Subsequent studies have contrasted two social perspectives. In one such study, individuals listened to an audiobook, thinking that a protagonist exhibited unfounded vs. justified jealousy. Between-condition differences were computed as Eucli dean distances (i.e., as in case of ISC, how similar the BOLD signal was in different conditions). They were observed in the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), superior temporal sulcus (STS), hippocampus, thalamus, precuneus, premotor cortex (PMC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; Yeshurun et al., 2017). Notably, many of these areas overlap with specific functional brain networks. The TPJ, precuneus and VMPFC overlap the default-mode network (DMN) that is activated when the brain is at rest, however, also during watching of social interactions . This suggests that the DMN activation at rest might be due to mind wandering about social interactions (Iacoboni et al., 2004). The VLPFC and PMC overlap the mirror neuron system (MNS), which is activated both when producing and observing actions by others (Rizzolatti, 2005). The VLPFC as well as STS, in turn, overlap a network of areas associated with language processing (Friederici and Gierhan, 2013). Overall, the results can be understood from the perspective that the two conditions differently drove mentalization that would recruit brain areas supporting theory-of-mind, as well as shape the understanding of the plot for which DMN structures seem to be important, in particular the precuneus (Nguyen et al., 2019).

In another study, subjects watched a movie, thinking that the main protagonists were either genetic or adopted sisters. Higher ISC was observed in the “genetic” condition in several areas overlapping the DAN (superior parietal areas) and the DMN (VMPFC, precuneus, and posterior cingulate cortex), as well as other regions including inferior and lateral prefrontal, superior temporal, and occipital areas (Bacha-Trams et al., 2017). Especially the medial DMN structures suggest that the enhanced ISC was due to more similar mentalization and plot-interpretation, whereas ISC in the DAN suggest a more similar deployment of attention, though it is important to keep in mind the caveats of reverse inference (Poldrack, 2011). Finally, a recent study contrasted adopting two social perspectives, as subjects were instructed to put themselves into the shoes of a potential organ donor vs. recipient sister in a movie depicting a moral dilemma of a healthy sister refusing to donate her kidney to save her sick sister. Even though, the differences in ISC were distributed across the brain, the results showed the engagement of empathy circuitry, including the anterior insula (AI), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and somatosensory areas, in the perspective of the sick sister (Bacha-Trams et al., 2020). As for the perspective of the healthy sister, they highlighted the role of moral dilemma processing areas, specifically DLPFC and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; Bacha-Trams et al., 2017).

In addition to explicit perspective-taking tasks and implicit a priori information, such factors as cultural family background and personality traits can shape the perspective from which one interprets and feels the world. In a recent study, subjects with two different family cultural-ethnic backgrounds listened to a 71-min audiobook during fMRI. Differences in ISC between the two groups were noted in lateral aspects of the temporal lobe, LOC, visual areas, posterior cingulate cortex, and precuneus. This suggests that the cultural background, in subtle ways, influenced how the audiobook was heard at the level of single word semantics, visual imagery elicited by the story, and at the level of interpretation of the plot of the audiobook (Hakonen et al., 2022). Behavioral findings paralleled these neuroimaging findings as the subject groups exhibited significant differences in word-lists that they produced in order to describe what had been on their minds while they heard the audiobook during neuroimaging (Hakonen et al., 2022). As other findings of note, it was observed that the precuneus exhibited higher activity during exposure to statements exhibiting pro-outgroup than pro-ingroup viewpoints in members of groups involved in an intergroup conflict (Bruneau and Saxe, 2010). Finally, temperamental traits have been noted to modulate brain activity when watching arousing sports movie s (Bierzynska et al., 2019).

Overall, the studies with ecologically valid media-based stimuli suggest that the human brain flexibly adapts to the specific information-processing needs of the perspectives. However, inferior temporal-occipital areas and posterior-medial parietal areas seem to be quite consistently recruited across different perspectives. One drawback of using media-based stimuli is that it is difficult to discern which specific factors resulted in recruitment of specific brain areas. Fortunately, there are studies that have studied this question by looking at specific factors in isolation. These will be reviewed next.

Neural correlates of visual perspective taking

Visual perspective taking, the ability to see the visuospatial world from the perspective of another person, can be seen as a basic mechanism that supports higher-level perspective taking. Visual perspective taking has been studied, for example, with tasks where an avatar is surrounded by objects and the subjects are to assess the spatial arrangement of the objects (e.g., which one is ahead of the other) from third-person vs. first-person perspectives, and/or manipulate objects taking into account what the avatar is seeing. In a study where subjects adopted a visual perspective of an avatar relative to surrounding objects, increased activity was observed in precuneus, bilateral frontal areas, cerebellum, as well as left-hemisphere temporal and occipitoparietal areas (Vogeley et al., 2004). These findings pinpointed areas specifically involved in adopting the third-person visual perspective. The involvement of these areas was confirmed in a subsequent study (Kaiser et al., 2008). In addition, spatial perspective-taking (a protagonist in a narrative being “here” vs. “there”) was observed to be linked to temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and posterior medial parietal areas (Mano et al., 2009). Finally, a transcranial-current stimulation study causally confirmed the involvement of TPJ bilaterally in visual perspective taking (Santiesteban et al., 2015). Taken together, these findings indicate brain regions that are recruited when taking the visual-spatial perspective of another person (see Figure 1). These results were extended in a study wherein visual perspective was adopted to take into account the intentions of another, resulting in activation of the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) as well as IFG and temporal pole (TP), indicated as important for social cognition (Dumontheil et al., 2010).

Empathetic perspective-taking

Empathy refers to feeling the emotions of others, including their pain. This is one of the most researched areas of perspective taking. In pioneering studies, it was shown that seeing one’s loved one in pain activated the AI and ACC that are associated with affective components of pain, yet failed to activate somatosensory and caudal cingulate cortex that were activated when feeling pain oneself (Singer et al., 2004; Jackson et al., 2005). These findings suggest that these areas are involved when adopting an empathetic perspective towards others’ suffering.

In another study, adopting empathetic self-perspective vs. other-perspective, to pictures of hands and feet in pain, resulted in elevated activity in the left supramarginal gyrus. In the reverse contrast, other-perspective vs. self-perspective, stronger activity was seen in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), middle cingulate, pallidum, as well as the superior temporal sulcus (STS; van der Heiden et al., 2013). The more wide-spread activation and delayed hemodynamic responses, when looking at the pictures empathizing with others’ pain, were taken to suggest that adopting the other-perspective is more effort-demanding than the self-perspective (van der Heiden et al., 2013).

Researchers often connect empathetic perspective taking with the MNS, which is activated both during one’s own actions and the perception of others’ actions (Rizzolatti, 2005). A recent meta-analysis confirms that emotional and cognitive empathy are moderately correlated with MNS activity (Bekkali et al., 2021). Together, these findings suggest that taking an empathetic perspective to others’ suffering, we observe a pattern of brain activity that partially overlaps with what is seen when we feel pain ourselves (see Figure 1).

Perspective taking when the other is viewed as similar vs. dissimilar to self

There are neuroimaging findings indicating that different brain mechanisms are utilized when mentalizing about others who are similar to self vs. others who are dissimilar to self (Mitchell et al., 2006). Seeing others as similar to self can result from identifying shared group membership (e.g., national, ethnic, religious, political, place of work, or supporters of the same sports team). Lack of such identification, or seeing the other as belonging to another group, would result in one seeing the other as dissimilar to self. It seems, then, that taking the perspective of similar vs. dissimilar others relies on differential brain mechanisms. Interestingly, DMPFC areas implicated in cases of the perception of dissimilar others partially overlap the DMPFC activation elicited when adopting the visual perspective of an avatar (Kaiser et al., 2008). In contrast, the VMPFC activity during the mentalization about others similar to self coincided with areas engaged during autobiographical memory recall. Thus, in the case of watching movies, the perceived similarity of the protagonists to oneself likely modulates the neural mechanisms that are recruited in a perspective taking task.

Emotion regulation via perspective taking

Emotion regulation refers to attempts to influence emotions in ourselves (or others) and encompasses up- and down-regulation of positive and negative emotions (McRae and Gross, 2020). Coping is a similar term, limited to down-regulation of negative emotions (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Cognitive reappraisal is one of the most effective emotion regulation strategies (Kalokerinos et al., 2017; Mohammed et al., 2021). Using this strategy, individuals consider other perspectives and potential antecedents to change how they perceive or interpret events and, consequently, regulate their emotions, be it up- or down-regulation (Wang et al., 2017). For example, identifying with transgressors through perspective taking facilitates forgiveness (Menahem and Love, 2013). Specific manuals have been elaborated to control anger using perspective-taking (Day et al., 2008). Narrative approaches are especially effective for perspective-taking skills (Bamberg, 1991).

In laboratory studies, instructions for emotion regulation are typically given before a stimulus which would provoke a strong negative (or positive) emotion. EEG studies showed an increased late positive potential amplitude in the reappraisal condition (Langeslag and Surti, 2017; Cao et al., 2020) and stronger frontal EEG activity associated with better emotion regulation (Dennis and Solomon, 2010). Cao et al. (2021) demonstrated that TMS over the left VLPFC shifted valence ratings in a more positive direction during positive reappraisal. Dörfel et al. (2014), using fMRI, showed that reappraisal of negative stimuli involved the left VLPFC and orbitofrontal gyrus. Allard and Kensinger (2014) observed activation of lateral (DLPFC, VLPFC, OFC) and medial prefrontal cortex (ACC), while subjects hedonically regulated their responses to unpleasant movies. Higher levels of VMPFC further impacted the regulation network associated with lower anger experience during the high-anger clips and lower trait anger levels (Jacob et al., 2018). These findings implicate frontal cortical mechanisms in emotion regulation, both provoked by non-naturalistic and naturalistic stimuli. Finally, a recent intracranial EEG study showed that the temporal pole regulates amygdala responses provoked by both pictures and movies (Sonkusare et al., 2020).

Distancing is another emotion regulation strategy that involves, for example, watching emotional pictures, with an understanding that they are from a movie rather than real. Ochsner and Gross (2008) consider distancing to be a subtype of reappraisal. Koenigsberg et al. (2010) found that distancing oneself from aversive images provoked an increased activation in dorsal ACC, medial prefrontal cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex, precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex, inferior parietal sulcus, and middle and superior temporal gyrus and decreased activation in the amygdala. Using EEG, Sulpizio et al. (2021) found a decrease in theta and beta bands in posterior regions. A multivariate pattern classification of fMRI data further revealed distributed patches of posterior cortical activation (Powers et al., 2020). Overall, distancing involves posterior brain areas in comparison to reappraisal or reinterpretation, which is more related to frontal areas.

Concluding remarks

Recently, the neural basis of perspective taking has been studied with media-based stimuli such as movies and narratives. Looking across these studies, it seems that the brain flexibly recruits the areas that are needed given the requirements of a given perspective, however, inferior temporal-occipital areas and posterior-medial parietal areas seem to be engaged across perspectives. Studies conducted with precisely controlled stimuli and tasks help further clarify specific factors contribut ing to the recruitment of brain areas. For example, taking the visual perspective of another person seems to result in recruitment of TPJ. When putting oneself into the shoes of another person, similar to oneself, structures associated with autobiographical memory function are recruited, and when empathizing the pain of others is called upon, the affective parts of the pain matrix, that is AI and ACC, are recruited. While sensory aspects of perspective taking may lie in posterior regions, empathy and reappraisal are mainly provided by the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex. We envision that continued efforts of combining media-based and more traditional experimental designs, with the latter offering specific localizer tasks, will pave the way for a comprehensive understanding of the neural basis of perspective taking. Open questions include how the brain switches between perspectives across unfolding twists of a plot in a movie, and how fundamental attentional, memory, and executive processes relate to perspective taking, such as those utilized when actively inhibiting thinking of or remembering something (Anderson and Green, 2001; Nowicka et al., 2011; Detre et al., 2013; Marzi et al., 2014; Pierguidi et al., 2016). As a translational aspect, perspective taking may provide some emotion regulation strategies; for example, to decrease amygdala activity to negative stimuli as during emotional distancing (Koenigsberg et al., 2010).

Author contributions

Both authors contributed equally to the writing of the mini-review manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The work was financed by the Russian Science Foundation, project #22-48-08002, https://rscf.ru/project/22-48-08002/ and carried out using HSE Automated system of non-invasive brain stimulation with the possibility of synchronous registration of brain activity and registration of eye movements (Reg. num 354937).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allard, E. S., and Kensinger, E. A. (2014). Age-related differences in neural recruitment during the use of cognitive reappraisal and selective attention as emotion regulation strategies. Front. Psychol. 5:296. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, M. C., and Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature 410, 366–369. doi: 10.1038/35066572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacha-Trams, M., Glerean, E., Dunbar, R. I., Lahnakoski, J. M., Ryyppö, E., Sams, M., et al. (2017). Differential inter-subject correlation of brain activity when kinship is a variable in moral dilemma. Sci. Rep. 7:14244. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14323-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacha-Trams, M., Ryyppö, E., Glerean, E., Sams, M., and Jääskeläinen, I. P. (2020). Social perspective-taking shapes brain hemodynamic activity and eye movements during movie viewing. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 175–191. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamberg, M. (1991). Narrative activity as perspective taking: the role of emotionals, negations and voice in the construction of the story realm. J. Cogn. Psychother. 5, 275–290. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.5.4.275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekkali, S., Youssef, G. J., Donaldson, P. H., Albein-Urios, N., Hyde, C., and Enticott, P. G. (2021). Is the putative mirror neuron system associated with empathy? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 31, 14–57. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09452-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bierzynska, M., Sobczak, P. A., Kozak, A., Bielecki, M., Strelau, J., and Kossut, M. M. (2019). No risk, no differences. Neural correlates of temperamental traits revealed using naturalistic fMRI method. Front. Psychol. 10:1757. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruneau, E. G., and Saxe, R. (2010). Attitudes towards the outgroup are predicted by activity in the precuneus in Arabs and Israelis. Neuroimage 52, 1704–1711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, D., Li, Y., and Niznikiewicz, M. A. (2020). Neural characteristics of cognitive reappraisal success and failure: an ERP study. Brain Behav. 10:e01584. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, D., Li, Y., and Tang, Y. (2021). Functional specificity of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in positive reappraisal: a single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 21, 793–804. doi: 10.3758/s13415-021-00881-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. J. (2023). Empathy and Mental Health: An Integral Model for Developing Therapeutic Skills in Counseling and Psychotherapy. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, A., Howells, K., Mohr, P., Schall, E., and Gerace, A. (2008). The development of CBT programmes for anger: The role of interventions to promote perspective-taking skills. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 36, 299–312. doi: 10.1017/S135246580800430X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, T. A., and Solomon, B. (2010). Frontal EEG and emotion regulation: electrocortical activity in response to emotional film clips is associated with reduced mood induction and attention interference effects. Biol. Psychol. 85, 456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Detre, G. J., Natarajan, A., Gershman, S. J., and Norman, K. A. (2013). Moderate levels of activation lead to forgetting in the think/no-think paradigm. Neuropsychologia 51, 2371–2388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dörfel, D., Lamke, J. P., Hummel, F., Wagner, U., Erk, S., and Walter, H. (2014). Common and differential neural networks of emotion regulation by detachment, reinterpretation, distraction and expressive suppression: a comparative fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 101, 298–309. doi: 10.1515/cclm-2022-1181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumontheil, I., Küster, O., Apperly, I. A., and Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Taking perspective into account in a communicative task. Neuroimage 52, 1574–1583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D., and Gierhan, S. M. E. (2013). The language network. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 250–254. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakonen, M., Ikäheimonen, A., Hultèn, A., Kauttonen, J., Koskinen, M., Lin, F. H., et al. (2022). Processing of an audiobook in the human brain is shaped by cultural family background. Brain Sci. 12:649. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12050649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, E., Porat, R., Tamir, M., and Gross, J. J. (2013). Can emotion regulation change political attitudes in intractable conflicts? From the laboratory to the field. Psychol. Sci. 24, 106–111. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Landesman, O., Knappmeyer, B., Vallines, I., Rubin, N., and Heeger, D. J. (2008). Neurocinematics: the neuroscience of film. Projections 2, 1–26. doi: 10.3167/proj.2008.020102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Malach, R., and Heeger, J. D. (2010). Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., and Malach, R. (2004). Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303, 1634–1640. doi: 10.1126/science.1089506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacoboni, M., Lieberman, M. D., Knowlton, B., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Moritz, M., Throop, C. J., et al. (2004). Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. Neuroimage 21, 1167–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jääskeläinen, I. P., Sams, M., Glerean, E., and Ahveninen, J. (2021). Movies and narratives as naturalistic stimuli in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 224:117445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage 24, 771–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, Y., Gilam, G., Lin, T., Raz, G., and Hendler, T. (2018). Anger modulates influence hierarchies within and between emotional reactivity and regulation networks. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:60. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, S., Walther, S., Nennig, E., Kronmüller, K., Mundt, C., Weisbrod, M., et al. (2008). Gender-specific strategy use and neural correlates in a spatial perspective taking task. Neuropsychologia 46, 2524–2531. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalokerinos, E. K., Résibois, M., Verduyn, P., and Kuppens, P. (2017). The temporal deployment of emotion regulation strategies during negative emotional episodes. Emotion 17, 450–458. doi: 10.1037/emo0000248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauppi, J.-P., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Sams, M., and Tohka, J. (2010). Inter-subject correlation of brain hemodynamic responses during watching a movie: localization in space and frequency. Front. Neuroinform. 4:5. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2010.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenigsberg, H. W., Fan, J., Ochsner, K. N., Liu, X., Guise, K., Pizzarello, S., et al. (2010). Neural correlates of using distancing to regulate emotional responses to social situations. Neuropsychologia 48, 1813–1822. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahnakoski, J. M., Glerean, E., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Hyönä, J., Hari, R., Sams, M., et al. (2014). Synchronous brain activity across individuals underlies shared psychological perspectives. Neuroimage 100, 316–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langeslag, S. J., and Surti, K. (2017). The effect of arousal on regulation of negative emotions using cognitive reappraisal: an ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 118, 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Mano, Y., Harada, T., Sugiura, M., Saito, D. N., and Sadato, N. (2009). Perspective-taking as part of narrative comprehension: a functional MRI study. Neuropsychologia 47, 813–824. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzi, T., Regina, A., and Righi, S. (2014). Emotions shape memory suppression in trait anxiety. Front. Psychol. 4:1001. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., and Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion 20, 1–9. doi: 10.1037/emo0000703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menahem, S., and Love, M. (2013). Forgiveness in psychotherapy: the key to healing. J. Clin. Psychol. 69, 829–835. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50, 655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammed, A. R., Kosonogov, V., and Lyusin, D. (2021). Expressive suppression versus cognitive reappraisal: effects on self-report and peripheral psychophysiology. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 167, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, M., Vanderwal, T., and Hasson, U. (2019). Shared understanding of narratives is correlated with shared neural responses. Neuroimage 184, 161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowicka, A., Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K., Tacikowski, P., and Brechmann, A. (2011). Forgetting of emotional information is hard: an fMRI study of directed forgetting. Cereb. Cortex 21, 539–549. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation: insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 153–158. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00566.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierguidi, L., Righi, S., Gronchi, G., Marzi, T., Caharel, S., Giovannelli, F., et al. (2016). Emotional contexts modulate intentional memory suppression of neutral faces: insights from ERPs. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 106, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: from reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron 72, 692–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. P., Graner, J. L., and LaBar, K. S. (2020). Multivariate patterns of posterior cortical activity differentiate forms of emotional distancing. Cereb. Cortex 30, 2766–2776. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G. (2005). The mirror neuron system and its function in humans. Anat. Embryol. (Berl) 210, 419–421. doi: 10.1007/s00429-005-0039-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2015). Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction-imitation inhibition, visual perspective-taking and theory of mind. Eur. J. Neurosci. 42, 2527–2533. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonkusare, S., Nguyen, V. T., Moran, R., van der Meer, J., Ren, Y., Koussis, N., et al. (2020). Intracranial-EEG evidence for medial temporal pole driving amygdala activity induced by multi-modal emotional stimuli. Cortex 130, 32–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulpizio, S., Grecucci, A., and Job, R. (2021). Tune in to the right frequency: theta changes when distancing from emotions elicited by unpleasant images and words. Eur. J. Neurosci. 53, 916–928. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Heiden, L., Scherpiet, S., Konicar, L., Birbaumer, N., and Veit, R. (2013). Inter-individual differences in successful perspective taking during pain perception mediates emotional responsiveness in self and others: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 65, 387–394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., and Fink, G. R. (2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 817–827. doi: 10.1162/089892904970799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Zhang, Z., and Jia, M. (2017). Understanding how leader humility enhances employee creativity: the roles of perspective taking and cognitive reappraisal. J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 53, 5–31. doi: 10.1177/0021886316678907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeshurun, Y., Nguyen, M., and Hasson, U. (2021). The default mode network: where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social world. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 181–192. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-00420-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeshurun, Y., Swanson, S., Simony, E., Chen, J., Lazaridi, C., Honey, C. J., et al. (2017). Same story, different story: the neural representation of interpretive frameworks. Psychol. Sci. 28, 307–319. doi: 10.1177/0956797616682029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: perspective taking, human brain, neuroimaging, precuneus, temporoparietal junction, prefrontal cortex

Citation: Jääskeläinen IP and Kosonogov V (2023) Perspective taking in the human brain: complementary evidence from neuroimaging studies with media-based naturalistic stimuli and artificial controlled paradigms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1051934. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1051934

Received: 23 September 2022; Accepted: 27 January 2023;
Published: 15 February 2023.

Edited by:

Maria Pia Viggiano, University of Florence, Italy

Reviewed by:

Kathleen Lyons, Western University, Canada
Stefania Righi, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Jääskeläinen and Kosonogov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vladimir Kosonogov, dmtvc29ub2dvdkBoc2UucnU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.