Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 06 January 2023
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Simultaneous EEG-fMRI Applications in Cognitive Neuroscience View all 9 articles

Probing fMRI brain connectivity and activity changes during emotion regulation by EEG neurofeedback

  • 1School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
  • 3Medical Image Analysis Lab, Department of Radiology and Research Administration, Henry Ford Health System, Detroit, MI, United States
  • 4School of Cognitive Sciences, Institute for Research in Fundamental Sciences (IPM), Tehran, Iran

Despite the existence of several emotion regulation studies using neurofeedback, interactions among a small number of regions were evaluated, and therefore, further investigation is needed to understand the interactions of the brain regions involved in emotion regulation. We implemented electroencephalography (EEG) neurofeedback with simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) using a modified happiness-inducing task through autobiographical memories to upregulate positive emotion. Then, an explorative analysis of whole brain regions was done to understand the effect of neurofeedback on brain activity and the interaction of whole brain regions involved in emotion regulation. The participants in the control and experimental groups were asked to do emotion regulation while viewing positive images of autobiographical memories and getting sham or real (based on alpha asymmetry) EEG neurofeedback, respectively. The proposed multimodal approach quantified the effects of EEG neurofeedback in changing EEG alpha power, fMRI blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activity of prefrontal, occipital, parietal, and limbic regions (up to 1.9% increase), and functional connectivity in/between prefrontal, parietal, limbic system, and insula in the experimental group. New connectivity links were identified by comparing the brain functional connectivity between experimental conditions (Upregulation and View blocks) and also by comparing the brain connectivity of the experimental and control groups. Psychometric assessments confirmed significant changes in positive and negative mood states in the experimental group by neurofeedback. Based on the exploratory analysis of activity and connectivity among all brain regions involved in emotion regions, we found significant BOLD and functional connectivity increases due to EEG neurofeedback in the experimental group, but no learning effect was observed in the control group. The results reveal several new connections among brain regions as a result of EEG neurofeedback which can be justified according to emotion regulation models and the role of those regions in emotion regulation and recalling positive autobiographical memories.

Introduction

Emotion regulation consists of control and management of emotional states, mood, and affect with specific strategies, e.g., situation selection, situation modification, attentional deployment (e.g., distraction and rumination), cognitive change (e.g., reappraisal and distancing), and response modulation (e.g., exercise) (Gross, 1998; Koole, 2009; Aldao, 2012). The common strategies for emotion regulation can be categorized into implicit and explicit strategies based on conscious awareness of the goal of the regulation strategy which can be explained to participants or patients (Balconi et al., 2018).

Neurofeedback is a non-invasive brain training procedure with various clinical and non-clinical applications especially for emotion regulation (Sitaram et al., 2017). In neurofeedback, feedback is provided to a subject based on his/her brain activity in order to self-regulate his/her brain function.

Electroencephalography and fMRI are the two main neuroimaging modalities used in the study of neurofeedback for emotion regulation. Simultaneous recording of EEG and fMRI provides complementary information and allows for a more comprehensive understanding and research in neurofeedback by exploring EEG and fMRI correlation (EEG-informed fMRI), fusion analysis, and validation of the effectiveness of the applied paradigm for a specific purpose (Ebrahimzadeh et al., 2019a,b, 2021, 2022; Mosayebi et al., 2022). EEG-based neurofeedback has applications in the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Lubar et al., 1995; Zuberer et al., 2018), schizophrenia (Bolea, 2010), insomnia (Hammer et al., 2011), drug addiction (Lackner et al., 2016), autism (Coben et al., 2010), epilepsy (Saxby and Peniston, 1995; Walker and Kozlowski, 2005; Kaur and Singh, 2017; Linhartová et al., 2019), anxiety (Mennella et al., 2017), pain (Kubik and Biedroń, 2013, eating disorders (Bartholdy et al., 2013), Parkinson disease (Rossi-Izquierdo et al., 2013), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Hammond, 2003), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Gapen et al., 2016) in addition to other psychological applications e.g., emotion regulation (Dennis and Solomon, 2010; Quaedflieg et al., 2015; Linhartová et al., 2019).

Most of the EEG neurofeedback protocols try to modulate the EEG signal amplitude or power in particular frequency bands (Sourina et al., 2012; Peeters et al., 2014b; Allen and Reznik, 2015; Kelley et al., 2017; Mennella et al., 2017; Meyer et al., 2018). Recent development in MRI pulse sequences has made it possible to use fMRI for neurofeedback which is named time-real fMRI (Subramanian et al., 2011; Koush et al., 2013; Sulzer et al., 2013; DeBettencourt et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2015; Megumi et al., 2015; Sarkheil et al., 2015; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2016; Sherwood et al., 2016). EEG neurofeedback with simultaneous fMRI has been proposed in previous studies (Zotev et al., 2016, 2020; Lioi et al., 2020a; Perronnet et al., 2020), Theta/Alpha power ratio was used by Kinreich et al. (2012) to enhance relaxation in subjects, which is useful in a range of clinical applications such as PTSD and ADHD.

EEG frontal asymmetry has been used in several studies for emotion regulation based on the approach – withdrawal model proposed by Davidson et al. (1990). According to this model, withdrawal emotion or negative affect (such as fear, sadness, and disgust) is associated with higher activity in the right hemisphere. Conversely, increasing activity in the left hemisphere is associated with emotional states such as joy or anger (Davidson et al., 1990; Quaedflieg et al., 2015). Several applications of EEG frontal asymmetry are listed in Coan and Allen (2004). As mentioned in several previous studies (Coan and Allen, 2004; Peeters et al., 2014a; Allen and Reznik, 2015; Mennella et al., 2017; Allen et al., 2018; Meyer et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2018; López-Castro et al., 2021; Trambaiolli et al., 2021; Weon et al., 2021), EEG frontal asymmetry can be used as an indication of different emotional states and a biomarker for PTSD, anxiety, and depression.

In the study by Peeters et al. (2014b), the EEG frontal asymmetry was used for both up/down emotion regulation in a single session of neurofeedback training. In this study, it was hypothesized that positive/negative affect would result in increased activity of the left/right frontal hemisphere and decreased activity of the right/left frontal hemisphere. The results revealed that participants were able to up/down-regulate their EEG frontal asymmetry using neurofeedback. Similar to most previous neurofeedback studies (Marxen et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016; Vourvopoulos et al., 2019; Klöbl et al., 2020; Lioi et al., 2020a), this study lacked a control group for receiving sham neurofeedback.

Simultaneous recording of EEG and fMRI during emotion regulation was used in several previous studies, especially when using EEG frontal asymmetry alone or both EEG frontal asymmetry and fMRI as neurofeedback. In Cavazza et al. (2014), EEG neurofeedback based on frontal alpha asymmetry with simultaneous fMRI was used based on interactive narrative paradigms. The success rate of emotion regulation and changing the frontal asymmetry in this study was low. In addition, this study did not include a control group.

In the studies by Zotev et al. (2014, 2018b), emotion regulation was done through retrieving positive autobiographical memories. The neurofeedback in these studies was based on both EEG frontal asymmetry and BOLD signal of the amygdala. The results demonstrated the effectiveness of simultaneous EEG-fMRI neurofeedback for emotion regulation but the redundancy of neurofeedback based on both modalities was not clarified in these studies.

According to a recently published review paper in emotion regulation using fMRI neurofeedback (Linhartová et al., 2019), several brain regions including the amygdala, insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and prefrontal/frontal regions (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)) are involved in emotion regulation and treatment of mental disorders such as depression, PTSD, and anxiety. In some previous studies (Zotev et al., 2011, 2013, 2018b; Koush et al., 2017; Young et al., 2018), interactions among a small number of regions e.g., the amygdala and other regions were evaluated. Notwithstanding several existing emotion regulation studies using neurofeedback or retrieval of autobiographical memories, understanding the interaction of the brain regions involved in emotion regulation through neurofeedback still needs further investigation by considering brain regions with key roles in emotion regulation. Therefore, we implement simultaneous fMRI–EEG recording during an EEG neurofeedback using an induced implicit happiness task for emotion regulation through positive autobiographical memories. Initially, we wanted to use both EEG and fMRI for neurofeedback, but because of technical problems, we used EEG as neurofeedback.

Therefore, this study aims to investigate how EEG neurofeedback changes EEG and fMRI BOLD signals, psychometric assessments, and connectivity of all activated brain regions during emotion regulation and separate the effect of paradigm and neurofeedback during emotion regulation. Using different approach from our previous study (Dehghani et al., 2020, 2021, 2022), we use general GLM as a model-based method to extract activated brain voxels/regions during emotion regulation. We test the hypothesis that EEG frontal asymmetry, activity and the connectivity in/between the prefrontal, limbic, and insular regions change through EEG neurofeedback. The connectivity results can be used for the design of new neurofeedback paradigms for emotion regulation, especially using real-time fMRI, as a therapeutic tool for the treatment of mental disorders.

Materials and methods

Task design

The research protocol is approved by the ethics committees of the Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran. 20 healthy subjects (age 26.7 ± 3.6 years, all male) as the experimental group, and 15 healthy subjects (age 27 ± 3.8 years, all male) as the control group participated in this study. Participants in the control group are provided (without their knowledge) with sham EEG neurofeedback. We had no drop out according to quality control of data, but we had three drop out before starting the experiments because of technical problems (two subjects of experimental group and one subject of control group). The exclusion criteria are a prior history of major psychiatric or neurological disorder, drug or alcohol abuse during the past year, brain surgery, and contraindications to MRI and all participants were right-handed with the normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Before the experiment, two psychometric tests including Beck’s Depression Inventory (Craven et al., 1989) and General Health Questionnaire – 28 (GHQ-28) (Nazifi et al., 2014) are completed by each participant. The mean ± standard deviations of Beck’s Depression Inventory and GHQ-28 for all participants are 6.8 ± 3 and 2.2 ± 2, respectively. Therefore, the participants are normal and non-psychiatric according to the scores of the Beck’s Depression Inventory (all participants have scores less than or equal to 12 and are classified as normal or minimum depression, 24 participants have scores less than 9) (Khademi et al., 2005; Smarr and Keefer, 2011) and GHQ-28 tests (all participants have scores less than or equal to 7 and classified as non-psychiatric, 28 participants have scores less than or equal to 3) (Goldberg et al., 1998; Hjelle et al., 2019). All participants are examined by a resident physician before the experiments (to check their blood pressure and inform them about the experimental environment) and fill out the consent form for participation in the experiments.

Retrieving positive autobiographical memories is a common strategy used for upregulating positive emotions (Linhartová et al., 2019). The experimental paradigm in this study is similar to the one published in Young et al. (2014), Zotev et al., 2014, 2016, 2018a but with minor differences and is based on retrieving autobiographical happy memories. Before the experiment, each participant was interviewed and asked to write several positive autobiographical memories. The experiment contains 10 runs, and each participant has 10 blocks of emotion regulation using various autobiographical memory retrievals. The duration of each run is 130 s and total duration of whole experiment is 1300 s. Each run contains three blocks including Rest, View, and Upregulation. The difference between our paradigm and similar paradigms (Young et al., 2014; Zotev et al., 2014, 2016) is that we select pictures based on what subjects announce during interviews and present them during the experiments to remind the autobiographical memories related to these pictures in each run. The duration of the Rest block is 20 s, the View block is 40 s, and the Upregulation block is 60 s. During the Rest block, no image is shown, and only the message “please rest” is displayed on the screen to ask the participants to relax with no specific tasks. In the View block, two related pictures were presented for the 40s and the participant is asked to see them without thinking about them or remembering anything. In the Upregulation block, two images similar to the View block are presented and the participant tries to increase the height of the bar of neurofeedback based on the brain activity. The selected images for View and Upregulation blocks have similar arousal and valence (without any significant difference) according to the rates given after the experiment by each participant to individual images of positive autobiographical memories. The whole neurofeedback protocol for one run is depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neurofeedback bar and timing of the tasks used in the one run of neurofeedback protocol, which contains Rest, View, and Upregulation blocks.

Before the experiment, we explain the paradigm to each participant by the visual presentation of a sample run of the task. Then, we ask them to try to increase the height of the bar of the neurofeedback during the Upregulation block by remembering the positive autobiographical memories (without showing the individual pictures). In each step, the bar is blue if the participant succeeds to increase or maintain the brain activity represented by neurofeedback and red if he does not succeed.

The neurofeedback in the experimental group is based on the approach–withdrawal hypothesis (Davidson et al., 1990), which is defined as the difference between the EEG power in the right and left hemispheres in the alpha frequency band and in the windows with a length of 2 s, which is updated every 1 s with 50% overlap between the consecutive windows (the height of the neurofeedback bar at each time is the average of the EEG asymmetry alpha power in the current and two previous windows). For the control group, the sham feedback is a randomly generated signal for each Upregulation block as proposed in Zotev et al. (2018a). Neurofeedback is presented only in the Upregulation block.

Data acquisition

The MRI data are acquired using a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Prisma MRI Scanner located in the National Brain Mapping Lab (NBML), Tehran, Iran. Functional MRI are acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo, echo-planar (EPI) pulse sequence (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 64 × 64 × 30, and voxel size = 3.8 × 3.8 × 4 mm). During the 10 runs of the experiment session, 650 volume images (1300 s) are acquired. Structural images are acquired using a gradient-echo, T1-weighted MPRAGE pulse sequence (TI = 1100 ms, TR = 1810 ms, TE = 3.47 ms, and voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm).

The EEG data are recorded simultaneously with fMRI using the Brain Products EEG system. The EEG cap has 64 electrodes (FCz as the reference and AFz as the ground electrode) according to the 10–20 system and one ECG electrode. Electrode impedances are maintained below 5K Ohms by cleaning the skin and removing dirt using alcohol and injecting suitable gel. The EEG signal is recorded at 5K samples/sec. For quality control of the EEG data and neurofeedback, several minutes of the recording are performed in the MRI control room just before the subject goes in the MRI scanner. The task is presented by the Psychtoolbox program through a coil mounted display, which allows the subjects to see the stimulus and different blocks of the experiment.

Real-time data processing

Due to practical limitations, neurofeedback is provided only based on the EEG signal. For the EEG neurofeedback, the RecView software is applied to remove MRI and ballistocardiogram artifacts from the EEG data in real time, using the moving average method (Allen et al., 1998, 2000). As the mean head displacement obtained in offline analysis is 0.41 ± 0.17 mm (in the range of [0.10 mm 0.90 mm], all participants except for one had head motion less than 0.60 mm), the result of the moving average template subtraction method will not differ significantly from the counterpart methods (Niazy et al., 2005; Moosmann et al., 2009). In addition, since the head movement and ballistocardiogram artifacts induce large peaks in the EEG signal, the remaining large peaks are excluded. Removing the ballistocardiogram residual from the EEG signal is very difficult. However, because of small head displacements and since the frequency range of this artifact is less than 7 Hz, our feedback which is in the alpha frequency band will not be affected by the artifact.

The denoised data is down-sampled to 250 samples/sec. Then, the powers of channels F3 and F4 are calculated every 1 s using a 2-s moving window. The relative EEG power asymmetry for F4 and F3 with respect to the baseline (by averaging the asymmetry values in the previous View block) is calculated and presented as a bar during Upregulation blocks ((Ln(F4)−Ln(F3))in each 2sec window−(Ln(F4)−Ln(F3))previous View block). For high-quality neurofeedback, the following steps are done. First, the power spectrum of the data recorded inside and outside of the MRI scanner is compared to evaluate the quality of the denoised data. Secondly, the spectrum of neurofeedback and its values obtained by RecView are compared with the results of the offline analysis to evaluate the neurofeedback quality. The results confirm the quality of the offline removal of the EEG artifacts and the reliability of the neurofeedback. They also prove that the neurofeedback provided to each participant is based on the actual brain activity, not the artifacts.

EEG data analysis

An offline analysis of the EEG data acquired simultaneously with fMRI, is performed using the FMRIB plug-in as a Matlab toolbox (Christov, 2004; Kim et al., 2004; Niazy et al., 2005). This software removes the fMRI gradient artifacts, detects the QRS complexes from an ECG channel, and removes the pulse (ballistocardiography/BCG) artifacts from the EEG signal. The MR artifact removal method includes 4 steps. In the first step, slice-timing triggers are adjusted to optimize the alignment of triggers. Second, a local artifact template subtraction method is performed. Third, the artifact residuals are estimated using basis functions derived from performing PCA on each channel’s artifact segments. The moving average subtraction performed in stage 2 is adaptive and removes more than 98% of the artifacts and PCA removes the residuals. Fourth, adaptive noise cancellation (ANC) is used. ANC removes any components in the data that are correlated with a reference. By using the subtracted noise as a reference (from the second and third step) artifact components not captured in the basis set are removed. More details are described in Niazy et al. (2005). For the BCG, a robust QRS complex detection algorithm is used (Niazy et al., 2005). Then, optimal basis sets method used in the third step of MR artifact removal is used to remove BCG artifact. The validation of these methods was demonstrated in Niazy et al. (2005). After removing the MRI and BCG artifacts, the EEG data are down-sampled to 250 samples/sec and low-pass filtered at 100 Hz. Next, the fMRI slice selection frequency and its harmonics are removed by band-stop filtering (15, 30, 45, 60, 75, and 90 Hz). Then, ICA is applied over the entire EEG data after excluding the noisy and motion-affected intervals. Next, independent components (ICs) corresponding to the artifacts, e.g., eye blinking, head movement, neck, head, and shoulder which included Scalp/face and muscle EMG, and cardioballistic or BCG residual, are identified and removed. This is done based on the time course spectral density (head movement in the range of 0.5–4.5 Hz, cardioballistic motion in the range of 2–7 Hz, eye blinking in the range of 0.5–3 Hz), topographic map (bipolar topography for BCG) (Mayeli et al., 2016), and kurtosis (rapid and random head movements have high kurtosis values) (Mognon et al., 2011; Zotev et al., 2012; Wong et al., 2016). Then, the average EEG power spectrum is calculated for each of the experimental blocks (Upregulation, View, and Rest). A moving window with a length of 2 s and 50% interval overlap is applied to the EEG data to calculate the EEG asymmetry of channels F3 and F4 in the alpha band for each block. To evaluate the effect of heart rate variability (HRV) on emotion regulation, HRV was extracted for all participants in three blocks of the experiment, using their ECG channel recorded during the experiment.

fMRI data analysis

An offline analysis of the whole brain fMRI data is performed in FSL. Pre-processing of a single-subject fMRI data includes slice-timing correction, motion correction, temporal high pass filtering (cut-off = 0.005 Hz), and spatially smoothing using an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. The standard GLM analysis is then applied to the fMRI time series. Three regressors for Upregulation, View, and Rest are convolved with the hemodynamic response function and six motion confounds are included in the GLM model. Finally, the whole brain is thresholded at p-value = 0.01 for voxels and for cluster correction at p-value = 0.01 in the cluster-level correction algorithm, which corrects for the multiple comparisons using the Gaussian Random Field (GRF) model (Smith et al., 2004; Fsl, 2006; Woolrich et al., 2009). To determine the active regions in the Upregulation blocks, the contrasts of the “Upregulation versus View” and the “Upregulation versus Rest” are calculated. A global measure of the signal change (mean(Upregualtion)-mean(View)mean(View) or mean(Upregualtion)-mean(Rest)mean(Rest)) in different activated regions of the preprocessed fMRI data registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) atlas is calculated using an anatomical mask. For this purpose, anatomical masks from “WFU_PickAtlas” and FSL were used to label the activation clusters. Then, the mean signal changes of the activated voxels in every active region for Upregulation versus View and Rest are calculated in the 118 anatomical masks extracted from the “WFU_PickAtlas” and FSL (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002; Maldjian et al., 2003, 2004; Desikan et al., 2006; Gorgolewski et al., 2015). WFU_PickAtlas provides a method for generating ROI masks based on the Talairach Daemon database. The atlases include Brodmann area, Lobar, Hemisphere, Anatomic Label and Tissue Type. WFU_PichAtlas has totally 116 regions and we used them to find the activated brain regions.

For further evaluation, Pearson’s correlation is used to estimate and compare the functional connectivity in the different blocks of the experiment. To this end, the anatomical masks of various brain regions are used to extract the mean BOLD signals of the activated voxels in the regions and calculate the functional connectivity between the regions. The results are used to reveal the neural mechanisms of the neurofeedback protocol (functional connectivity) in the brain. To remove the effect of the hemodynamic response of the View block on the Upregulation block, the first 4 s (two samples) of the BOLD signal of each Upregulation block is removed for signal change calculation and functional connectivity analysis.

Psychometric testing

To measure changes in the mood state, each participant completes the Persian version of Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Positive-Negative affect scale (PANAS), and short Persian version of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) before and after the neurofeedback test. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) contains 21 questions and measures the severity of anxiety (Beck et al., 1988). The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) includes 20 items for state anxiety and 20 items for trait anxiety (Azizi et al., 2013). The POMS includes a self-rating of six different aspects of the mood, e.g., Tension or Anxiety, Anger or Hostility, Vigor or Activity, Fatigue or Inertia, Depression or Dejection, and Confusion or Bewilderment, with a five-point scale rating from “not at all” to “extremely” (Spielberger, 1972). The PANAS also contains a self-rating of the positive and negative affects with a five-point scale rating from “not at all” to “very much” (Watson et al., 1988).

Results

EEG results

The EEG power in the alpha band is used for neurofeedback. The average change of the alpha power asymmetry (LnP(F4)-LnP(F3)) in the Upregulation, View, and Rest blocks for the experimental and control groups are illustrated in Figure 2. Since the feedback for the experimental group is based on the powers of channels F4 and F3, and the subjects try to increase LnP(F4)-LnP(F3) in the alpha frequency band versus the View period, we expect the mean power of “LnP(F4)-LnP(F3)” in the Upregulation block to be higher than the Rest and View blocks for the experimental group as illustrated in Figures 2A, B. The changes in the EEG asymmetry for the Upregulation versus View/Rest periods are significant for the experimental group (10 runs ×18 participants = 180 data for each Upregulation, View and Rest blocks of experimental group, using the “Kolmogorov–Smirnov test to confirm normality of data, Repeated Measures ANOVA with Upregulation, View, and Rest as independent variable, F(2,537) = 12.08, p-value < 0.001, Repeated Measures ANOVA with Upregulation and Rest as independent variable, F(1,358) = 21.85, p-value < 0.001, Repeated Measures ANOVA with Upregulation and View as independent variable, F(1,358) = 14.51, p-value < 0.001). Also, the changes in the EEG frontal asymmetry for the Upregulation versus View and Rest in the experimental group are significantly different from those in the control group (two sample t-test; t(318) = 3.62, p-valueUpregulation–View  (Experimental  group–Control group) = 1.7 ×10–4; and t(318) = 2.68; p-valueUpregulation–Rest (Experimental group–Control group) = 3.9 ×10–3, FDR-corrected for multiple comparison, q = 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Scatter plot of frontal asymmetry in blocks of View and Upregulation versus Rest for the experimental group. (B) Boxplot of frontal asymmetry in blocks of Rest, View, and Upregulation for the experimental and control groups. (C) Boxplot of frontal asymmetry for Upregulation versus View and Rest in the experimental and control groups.

Figures 2B, C show the effectiveness of the neurofeedback protocol as the difference between the frontal asymmetry of Upregulation and View/Rest in the experimental group are higher than in the control group. Consistent with the approach–withdrawal model that states during happiness, the difference between the power of the right and left hemispheres increases, the difference between the EEG power of channels F4 and F3 in the Upregulation blocks is higher than the View and Rest blocks.

To evaluate the effect of heart rate variability (HRV) on emotion regulation, HRV was extracted for all participants in three blocks of the experiment, using their ECG channel recorded during the experiment. The results of HRV analysis using the recorded ECG channel show that HRV in the Upregulation block is significantly different from those of the View and Rest blocks in only 3 of the 32 participants. This indicates that emotion regulation is not associated with HRV.

fMRI results

Activation detection in the whole brain using GLM with different contrasts of Upregulation versus View and Rest illustrate several regions for the experimental group during emotion regulation. As mentioned earlier, to have a global evaluation, we use an anatomical mask for each region to compare the mean signal change of the activated regions between the Upregulation and Rest/View blocks. The results of significant signal changes for the Upregulation versus View and Rest (Repeated Measures ANOVA with Upregulation, View, and Rest as independent variable for every one of the 118 ROIs used in this study, FDR-corrected for multiple comparison; q = 0.05) and the corresponding t-value and Cohen’s D effect size for the experimental group are listed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Percentages of signal change, t-score, and Cohen’s D effect size for brain regions with significant signal change between Upregulation versus View and Rest in the experimental group (FDR-corrected for multiple comparison, q = 0.05).

They illustrate several brain regions with large signal change and effect size for emotion regulation, which is consistent with the literature (listed in the last column of Table 1). The changes in the BOLD signal of brain regions in Table 1 for the Upregulation versus View and Rest in the experimental group are significantly different from those of the control group (two-sample t-test; df = 318; FDR-corrected for multiple comparison; q = 0.05).

Multiple brain regions especially in the subcortical regions play key roles in emotion regulation. The percentage of the signal change in the brain regions listed in Table 1 is higher than those of the previous studies. This may be due to the fact that the proposed emotion regulation paradigm (neurofeedback) is based on the recall of the positive autobiographical memory using the stimulus pictures related to the issues announced by the participants to induce happiness. However, other parameters e.g., protocol length and subject variability could be other sources of variability and higher signal changes. Figure 3 presents the activation map for the Upregulation versus View in the experimental group and the activation map masked by amygdala.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Activation map for Upregulation versus View in the experimental group (pcorrected = 0.01), (B) activation map masked by amygdala.

Figure 4 provides the box plot of the signal changes in the most important regions for the Rest, View, and Upregulation states for all subjects in the experimental and control groups. In Figure 4, the signal change of each experimental/control group participant for each ROI is computed by subtracting a global minimum from the average intensity of that ROI and normalizing the result by the “difference between the global minimum and the global maximum.” Figure 4 shows higher signal changes for the Upregulation relative to the View and Rest for the experimental group. Also, these changes for the Upregulation versus View and Rest in the experimental group are significantly different from those in the control group (two sample t-test; p-value Upregulation–View (Experimental group–Control group) for left amygdala = 2.4 × 10–3; p-value Upregulation−Rest(Experimentalgroup−Controlgroup)forleftamygdala = 1.3 × 10–4, p-value Upregulation−View (Experimentalgroup−Controlgroup) for left Insula = 3.6 × 10–5; p-valueUpregulation–Rest (Experimental group–Control group) for left Insula = 5.9 × 10–4, p-value Upregulation–View (Experimental group–Control group) for left OFC = 6.8 × 10–3; p-valueUpregulation–Rest (Experimental group–Control group) for left OFC = 1.5 × 10–5; FDR-corrected for multiple comparison; q = 0.05).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Normalized fMRI BOLD signal in the Rest, View, and Upregulation blocks for contrast of Upregulation versus View and Rest for the experimental and experimental groups in panel (A) left amygdala and p-value Upregulation–View(Experimental group–Control group) for left amygdala = 2.4 × 10– 3; p-value Upregulation–Rest(Experimental group–Control group) for left amygdala = 1.3 × 10– 4, (B) left insula and p-value Upregulation–View(Experimental group–Control group) for left Insula = 3.6 × 10– 5; p-value Upregulation–Rest(Experimental group–Control group) for left Insula = 5.9 × 10– 4, and (C) left orbitofrontal cortex and p-value Upregulation–View(Experimental group–Control group) for left OFC = 6.8 × 10– 3; p-value Upregulation–Rest(Experimental group–Control group) for left OFC = 1.5 × 10– 5 (*indicates p < 0.05 and n.s. indicates not significant).

The results of the experimental group in Figure 4 reveal the effect of neurofeedback during emotion regulation. To have a better understanding of the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation by the EEG neurofeedback and separate the effect of the neurofeedback and the recall of the autobiographical memories on Upregulation, the functional connectivity of the thirty-eight brain regions (including left/right amygdala, thalamus, insula, DMPFC, caudate, cuneus, hippocampus, posterior cingulate cortex, OFC, middle temporal gyrus, lingual gyrus, ventral striatum, DLPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), superior parietal, inferior parietal, supramarginal, postcentral and ACC are calculated during each block of the paradigm. Then, the significant connections between the Upregulation and View (Repeated Measures ANOVA for 38 × (38–1)/2 = 703 different connections; df = 179; FDR-corrected for multiple comparison; q = 0.05) for the experimental group are obtained. Functional connectivity is estimated by cross-correlation. Next, to reveal the effect of the neurofeedback, the significant links extracted from the experimental group are compared with those of the control group (two-sample t-test; df = 318; FDR-corrected for multiple comparison; q = 0.05). Connections significantly different between the two groups are due to the neurofeedback. Each illustrated edge in Figure 5 shows a connection with a significant change between the Upregulation and View blocks (and between the experimental and control groups) as a result of the neurofeedback. A thicker line between the regions corresponds to a higher correlation value.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Significant edges of differential connectivity networks which show network links with significant changes between the Upregulation and View blocks for panel (A) sagittal, (B) axial, and (C) coronal layout. L, left; R, right; Amyg, amygdala; ThaL, thalamus; Ins, insula; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; VS, ventral striatum; DMPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; VLPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; PC, postcentral.

Figure 5 illustrates the functional connectivity of the brain regions. Functional connectivity between several emotion-related regions increases in the Upregulation block, e.g., left amygdala and left thalamus. The roles of regions such as the amygdala, insula, thalamus, left ACC, hippocampus, OFC, and VLPFC in emotion regulation and recalling autobiographical happy memories are described in the Discussion Section.

To demonstrate the distinction between the functional connectivity of the Upregulation and View blocks, the functional connectivity distribution of the significant connections in the experimental group is shown in Figure 6. The distributions of the Upregulation and View blocks show higher functional connectivity in the Upregulation blocks and therefore more synchronization among the brain regions during emotion regulation.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Functional connectivity distribution of Upregulation and View blocks for significant connections in the experimental group.

Mood assessment

The emotional state test results obtained before and after the neurofeedback for the experimental and control groups are reported in Table 2. The results confirm the effectiveness of the neurofeedback in the experimental group. For the experimental group, the average scores for PANAS do not change significantly due to the neurofeedback but for the positive and negative mood states of PANAS, POMS, and Total Mood distribution (TMD), the changes are significant (paired t-test; t(17) = 4.01, ppositive mood states of PANAS = 4.5 × 10–4; t(17) = 3.32, pnegative mood states of PANAS = 2 × 10–3; t(17) = 3.61, pPOMS = 1.1 × 10–3; t(17) = 3.31, pTMD = 2.1 × 10–4). This demonstrates that neurofeedback is effective in increasing the positive mood state and decreasing the negative mood state through recalling positive autobiographical memories. Changing the mean value of TMD from 7.5 to −4.7 may be interpreted as an increase in the positive mood and a decrease in the negative mood as a result of the neurofeedback.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Psychometric test results before and after neurofeedback for the experimental and control groups.

For the control group, the scores of PANAS negative mood states show a change toward significance from before to after the neurofeedback (paired t-test; df = 13; p-value = 0.064) but for the positive mood states, they do not change significantly. This demonstrates that the recalling of the positive autobiographical memory even with sham neurofeedback is effective in decreasing (increasing) the negative (positive) mood state of the control group. The scores of POMS and Total Mood distribution (TMD) do not change significantly from before to after the neurofeedback. Changing the mean value of TMD from 6.6 to 3.6 for the control group may be interpreted as increasing the positive mood states and decreasing the negative mood states by recalling of the autobiographical memories even with sham neurofeedback. There are significant differences between the changes of the positive mood states of PANAS and TMD due to the neurofeedback in the experimental and control groups (two-sample t-test; df = 30; ppositive mood states of PANAS (Experimental group–Control group) = 0.02 and pTMD (Experimental group–Control group) = 0.004).

Before and after the neurofeedback experiment, the Persian version of the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) were completed by each participant to measure and evaluate the anxiety that might be induced by the MRI scanner. The results showed that the anxiety scores of all participants before and after the neurofeedback experiment were less than 16 and 33 for BAI and State Anxiety Inventory of STAI (in the range of minimal and mild anxiety), and after the neurofeedback test, the level of anxiety slightly decreased. Therefore, not only the anxiety did not increase during the experiment but also decreased as a result of the neurofeedback and emotion regulation. The mean ± standard deviation of the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) and State Anxiety Inventory for “before neurofeedback” was 8 ± 4.4 and 27 ± 2.5 and for “after neurofeedback” were 6.6 ± 4.7 and 26.3 ± 2.8.

Discussion

A The role of activated brain regions in neurofeedback paradigm

In this study, the participants were able to upregulate positive emotion using the EEG neurofeedback based on the frontal asymmetry in the alpha frequency band and happiness induced task through retrieving their positive autobiographical memories. Comparison of the results of the experimental and control groups reveals the increased activity of the prefrontal, insular, and limbic regions and increased functional connectivity in/between the prefrontal, limbic, and insular regions because of the EEG neurofeedback in the experimental group. The psychometric tests confirm an increased positive emotion and a decreased negative emotion because of the neurofeedback. In our previous study (Zotev et al., 2013), we used the fMRI dataset used in this study for functional brain connectivity analysis. For this purposes, independent component analysis (ICA) was used to extract spatially independent functional networks of fMRI data without the need for prior model of the data or paradigm. Then, the temporal correlation between the time courses of these networks was calculated as functional network connectivity (FNC) of the independent component. Each network (IC) composed of several sub-regions and each region involved in different networks. ICA extracts and identifies networks rather than single voxels and each network composes of several regions. One of the weaknesses of ICA is that it is an exploratory data-analytic technique, and it provides no straightforward method of testing specific a priori hypotheses about any components. General linear model (GLM) is a model-based method to extract the activated brain voxels/regions according to the task model. Therefore, despite the ICA as a data driven method, GLM uses the model of the paradigm to extract activated brain voxels during the task. Therefore, in different study from our previous published study (Dehghani et al., 2020), first we quantified changes of EEG frontal asymmetry in the alpha frequency band during different blocks of experiment in both experimental and control groups. Then using the model of task and GLM, activated brain regions were extracted and functional connectivity between different activated brain regions in different blocks of experiments were calculated to extract the effect of neurofeedback. Finally, psychometric assessments before and after neurofeedback were compared to understand the effect of neurofeedback on emotion regulation.

As mentioned in the previous studies, several brain areas including the subcortical and limbic regions are involved in emotion regulation. Consistent with the previous studies, during emotion regulation, amygdala, insula, ACC, cuneus, caudate, OFC, VLPFC, ventral striatum, and temporal gyrus are activated (Table 1; Pelletier et al., 2003; Kim and Hamann, 2007; Burianova et al., 2010; Johnston et al., 2010, 2011; Ino et al., 2011; Bado et al., 2014; Zotev et al., 2014, 2016; Li et al., 2016; Lempert et al., 2017). As shown in Figure 5, the functional connectivity between several emotional-related regions changes during the Upregulation blocks relative to the View blocks.

According to the Table 3, comparison of the results of this study with those of previous studies on emotion regulation, revealed several new connectivity links including thalamus - DMPFC, thalamus – insula, thalamus - OFC, thalamus - VLPFC, thalamus – postcentral, DMPFC – ventral striatum, OFC - VLPFC. The extracted new connections can be justified according to emotion regulation models and role of those regions in emotion regulation and recalling positive autobiographical memories. For the functional connectivity analysis, in our previous study (Dehghani et al., 2020), ICA was used to extract the spatially independent functional networks. Then, the correlation between these networks was calculated as functional network connectivity (FNC) to extract the effect of neurofeedback. Using the ICA methods, two pairs of networks had significant connectivity differences between View and Upregulation blocks. Each network was composed of voxels from different regions, and each voxel was involved in several ICs, and the rationale behind using ICA was to see if there is any connection between independent networks without considering the task model. For the ICA analysis, the connectivity between network 9 (cuneus, precuneus, PCC, VLPFC, DLPFC) and network 22 (amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, insula, putamen, thalamus) and between network 26 (cuneus, middle occipital, fusiform, lingual gyrus) and network 44 (ventral striatum, VLPFC, DLPFC, OFC, inferior parietal, middle temporal gyrus) increased and most of connections are between occipital regions and limbic/parietal regions. However, the results of functional connectivity analysis using the activated brain regions by GLM revealed that increased connectivity are between limbic regions, limbic and prefrontal/parietal, and between prefrontal regions and the only common connectivity link between this study and our previous study is increased connectivity between thalamus and VLPFC.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Significant connectivity obtained in previous emotion regulation researches and this study as a result of neurofeedback.

The limbic system and subcortical include several regions such as the hypothalamus, thalamus, amygdala, and hippocampus with key roles in emotion regulation. The amygdala plays a key role in emotion regulation/generation and connects with several emotion regulatory regions such as medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortices, ACC, and DLPFC through ventral and dorsal pathways (Phelps, 2004; Ochsner and Gross, 2008; Bracht et al., 2009). Thalamus activates a wide range of positive and negative emotional stimuli e.g., happiness, sadness, and disgust (Maddock et al., 2001; Zotev et al., 2018a). Thalamus is considered as a relay center for most of the sensory information. The sensory information first stops in the thalamus and then goes to destinations in the cortex (Cerqueira et al., 2008). The insula plays an important role in emotion processing and monitors internal emotional states e.g., disgust, happiness, and sadness (Chen et al., 2009; Pohl et al., 2013). Therefore, the increased activity of the thalamus, amygdala, insula and other regions e.g., the hippocampus, ACC, and ventral striatum is according to the role of these regions in emotion regulation and retrieving positive autobiographical memories (Bush et al., 2000; Hare et al., 2005; Marci et al., 2007Mitterschiffthaler et al., 2007; Suardi et al., 2016).

The lingual gyrus and cuneus from the occipital lobe are involved in visual processing and increased activity of these two regions was reported in previous studies of working memory, cognitive functions, visual processing, and memory retrieval (Gilboa et al., 2004; Burianova and Grady, 2007; Burianova et al., 2010; Vrticka et al., 2013; Deak et al., 2017). Therefore, according to the paradigm used in this study and visual processing of positive autobiographical memory images, the activity of these regions increased in this study.

Prefrontal/frontal cortex regions (OFC, VLPFC, and DLPFC) play key roles in emotion generation/regulation, self-monitoring, cognitive functions, working memory, recalling autobiographical memory, decision making, and pleasant emotional stimulus (Herrington et al., 2005; Beer et al., 2006; Schutter and van Honk, 2006; Svoboda et al., 2006; He et al., 2018). These regions have reciprocal connections with the other regions related to emotion regulation such as the amygdala and ACC. Therefore, the activity of the prefrontal/frontal cortex increased according to the role of these regions in emotion regulation, as described by several previous studies.

The increased activity of the parietal regions (superior parietal, inferior parietal, supramarginal, and postcentral) is related to the role of these regions in attention deployment to the presented images of positive autobiographical memories and their involvement in the integration of the sensory and behavioral information (Andersen, 1997; Bullier, 2001; Aday et al., 2017).

B Network interpretation of functional connectivity analysis

According to the emotion regulation model described in Kohn et al. (2014), emotion regulation is modeled as a process with three steps. The first step is an appraisal of the stimulus. Subcortical regions such as the amygdala, basal ganglia, and ventral striatum play a key role in emotion generation. In this step, the affective arousal is relayed and projected to VLPFC via subcortical regions (amygdala, basal ganglia, and ventral striatum). The next step is “detecting and start of the regulation process” and the need for regulation is signaled by VLPFC and insula. The final step is regulation and change of the emotional state and moving to a new state. Thalamus directs the sensory information to different cortical and subcortical regions. Therefore, increasing the functional connectivity between amygdala and thalamus and between ventral striatum and thalamus are the results of passing visual stimulus information processed by the thalamus and occipital regions to the amygdala and ventral striatum for emotion generation and also the transmission of the somatosensory information as a result of emotion generation to the cortical regions by the thalamus. Decreased resting-state functional connectivity between the amygdala and thalamus was reported in a major depressive disorder study (Tang et al., 2018). Therefore, enhanced connectivity between the amygdala and thalamus is related to increased happiness and decreased sadness (based on psychometric assessment) as a result of emotion regulation and may be associated with decreased severity of the major depressive disorder.

The increased functional connectivity between thalamus and insula can be interpreted as the role of the insula in the second step of emotion regulation for sending the need for regulation to the cortical regions and its involvement in the third step of emotion regulation and the role of the thalamus as the relay center for sending sensory information to cortical and subcortical regions. The higher functional connection between the thalamus and insula was reported during meditation to reduce negative emotion (Jang et al., 2018) and seems to be similar to the effects of the neurofeedback paradigm used in this study. The increased functional connectivity between the thalamus and VLPFC may be interpreted as similar to those of the thalamus and insula. According to the proposed model in Kohn et al. (2014), VLPFC and insula have similar roles in emotion regulation. OFC receives input and sensory information from various brain regions such as the amygdala, hypothalamus, and insular cortex. Therefore, the rise of connectivity between OFC and thalamus originates from relaying sensory information by the thalamus and receiving it by OFC. There are indirect connections between the subcortical and prefrontal regions involved in emotion regulation. The functional connectivity between OFC and VLPFC, DMPFC and ventral striatum, DMPFC and amygdala, DMPFC and insula, thalamus and DMPFC are supported by extensive and reciprocal anatomical connections in the prefrontal cortex, between the prefrontal cortex and the limbic/paralimbic regions, and through indirect connection according to the steps of the emotion regulation models. The higher co-activation pattern between the limbic system (amygdala, insula, ventral striatum, and thalamus) and prefrontal and frontal cortex (DMPFC, OFC, and VLPFC) was associated with the effectiveness of emotion regulation and less intensity of negative affect and lower level of anxiety, especially lower functional connectivity was reported between thalamus/caudate and OFC among major depressive participants (Banks et al., 2007; Lui et al., 2011; Cheng et al., 2016; Lithari et al., 2016; Kong et al., 2018).

The increased functional connectivity between the thalamus and postcentral (as a part of the parietal lobe) is related to the role of the parietal to receive and process sensory information (e.g., visual information) from the thalamus as a relay center for sending sensory information.

The functional connectivity results of this study are consistent with the previous emotion regulation studies and models. There are some new connections identified in this study that are not mentioned in the previous studies. The new connections are between the thalamus and DMPFC, thalamus and insula, thalamus and OFC, thalamus and VLPFC, thalamus and postcentral (parietal), DMPFC and ventral striatum, and VLPFC and OFC. In Table 3, the connectivity identified in this study and those of the previous emotion regulation studies (using different strategies and not limited to neurofeedback and recalling autobiographical memories) is presented. We reported them because of the concept of emotion regulation used in those studies and the existence of some brain circuits activated regardless of the type of stimulus. We also wanted to illustrate that some of the connections identified in this study were not reported in the previous emotion regulation studies (even with different paradigms).

C Limitations

Due to the high cost, high time consumption, and practical limitation in the simultaneous recording of EEG and fMRI, e.g., installation of EEG electrodes, the sample sizes of such studies are small (Kinreich et al., 2012; Zotev et al., 2014, 2016; Zich et al., 2015; Perronnet et al., 2017; Cury et al., 2020; Dehghani et al., 2020; Lioi et al., 2020b; Ebrahimzadeh et al., 2021; Ogawa et al., 2021). However, comparing the number of participants in this study (n = 32) with those in a recent literature review on emotion regulation-based fMRI neurofeedback (Linhartová et al., 2019), our study has a sample size similar to or higher than 50 of the 55 studies reviewed. A limitation of this study was using only males. It was difficult to enroll females in this study. Therefore, generalization of the results is difficult based on the previous reports of gender-specific differences in emotion regulation (McRae et al., 2008; Mak et al., 2009a,b; Whittle et al., 2017). Future studies can be done on the female subjects using the same emotion regulation paradigm.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates the effectiveness of the EEG neurofeedback in changing EEG and fMRI signals. It also shows changes in the functional connectivity of the brain regions involved in emotion regulation, especially between the frontal, parietal, limbic, and insular regions with some new connectivity links not mentioned in previous studies. The EEG neurofeedback using the proposed happy autobiographical memory paradigm changes the fMRI BOLD signal of various brain regions and the EEG frontal asymmetry more than those observed in the previous studies, even when they used both modalities for neurofeedback (Johnston et al., 2010; Young et al., 2014; Zotev et al., 2014, 2016; Li et al., 2016). Comparison of the psychometric test results obtained before and after the neurofeedback experiment, confirms the effectiveness of the neurofeedback paradigm in changing the negative and positive mood states of the participants.

The increased functional connectivity between prefrontal/frontal and other brain regions may provide valuable information, particularly in the EEG neurofeedback studies, where the prefrontal activity can be measured by EEG and modulation/activity of other regions can be predicted. The results may suggest the use of the proposed paradigm for the treatment of mental disorders in a small number of treatment sessions because of the larger changes and effect size in the EEG and fMRI signals and the psychometric assessments.

The proposed connectivity analysis may be used in neurofeedback studies in addition to the powers of EEG and the fMRI BOLD signals. The brain is a complex network, and the function of each region affects the others. In most brain functions and mental disorders, multiple brain regions are involved. Therefore, using feedback based on the connectivity of the involved regions for changing the whole network for a specific paradigm or mental disorder may be more effective than the alternative methods and would be an avenue of further investigation (Sulzer et al., 2013; Linhartová et al., 2019).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank National Brain Mapping Lab (NBML), Tehran, Iran for assistance with the experiments and providing technical support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.988890/full#supplementary-material

References

Aday, J., Rizer, W., and Carlson, J. M. (2017). “Neural mechanisms of emotions and affect,” in Emotions and affect in human factors and human-computer interaction, ed. M. Jeon (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 27–87. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801851-4.00002-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldao, S. (2012). Nolen-Hoeksema, The influence of context on the implementation of adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Behav. Res. Ther. 50, 493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, J. J. B., and Reznik, S. J. (2015). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a promising marker of depression vulnerability: Summary and methodological considerations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 4, 93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, J. J., Keune, P. M., Schönenberg, M., and Nusslock, R. (2018). Frontal EEG alpha asymmetry and emotion: From neural underpinnings and methodological considerations to psychopathology and social cognition. Psychophysiology 55:e13028. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, P. J., Josephs, O., and Turner, R. (2000). A method for removing imaging artifact from continuous EEG recorded during functional MRI. Neuroimage 12, 230–239. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, P. J., Polizzi, G., Krakow, K., Fish, D. R., and Lemieux, L. (1998). Identification of EEG events in the MR scanner: The problem of pulse artifact and a method for its subtraction. Neuroimage 8, 229–239. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, R. A. (1997). Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 352, 1421–1428. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azizi, P., Mohammadkhani, A., Foroughi, A., Lotfi, S., and Bahramkhani, M. (2013). The validity and reliability of the iranian version of the self-compassion scale. Pract. Clin. Psychol. 1, 149–155.

Google Scholar

Bado, P., Engel, A., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., Paiva, F. F., Basilio, R., et al. (2014). Functional dissociation of ventral frontal and dorsomedial default mode network components during resting state and emotional autobiographical recall. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3302–3313. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Frezza, A., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2018). Emotion regulation in schizophrenia: A pilot clinical intervention as assessed by EEG and optical imaging (Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy). Front. Hum. Neurosci 12:395.

Google Scholar

Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., and Phan, K. Luan (2007). Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 303–312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholdy, S., Musiat, P., Campbell, I. C., and Schmidt, U. (2013). The potential of neurofeedback in the treatment of eating disorders: A review of the literature. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 21, 456–463. doi: 10.1002/erv.2250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., and Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. J. Cogn. Neurosci 18, 871–879. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolea, A. S. (2010). Neurofeedback treatment of chronic inpatient schizophrenia. J. Neurother. 14, 47–54. doi: 10.1080/10874200903543971

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bracht, T., Tüscher, O., Schnell, S., Kreher, B., Rüsch, N., Glauche, V., et al. (2009). Extraction of prefronto-amygdalar pathways by combining probability maps. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 174, 217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullier, J. (2001). Integrated model of visual processing. Brain Res. Rev. 36, 96–107. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00085-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burianova, H., and Grady, C. L. (2007). Common and unique neural activations in autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1520–1534. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burianova, H., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2010). A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. Neuroimage 49, 865–874. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavazza, M., Aranyi, G., Charles, F., Porteous, J., Gilroy, S., Klovatch, I., et al. (2014). Towards empathic neurofeedback for interactive storytelling, Proc. 5th Work. Comput. Model. Narrat. 14:304. doi: 10.4230/OASIcs.CMN.2014.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerqueira, C. T., Almeida, J. R. C., Gorenstein, C., Gentil, V., Leite, C. C., Sato, J. R., et al. (2008). Engagement of multifocal neural circuits during recall of autobiographical happy events. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 41, 1076–1085. doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X2008001200006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. H., Dammers, J., Boers, F., Leiberg, S., Edgar, J. C., Roberts, T. P. L., et al. (2009). The temporal dynamics of insula activity to disgust and happy facial expressions: A magnetoencephalography study. Neuroimage 47, 1921–1928. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, W., Rolls, E. T., Qiu, J., Liu, W., Tang, Y., Huang, C. C., et al. (2016). Medial reward and lateral non-reward orbitofrontal cortex circuits change in opposite directions in depression. Brain 139, 3296–3309. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christov, I. I. (2004). Real time electrocardiogram QRS detection using combined adaptive threshold. Biomed. Eng. Online 3:28. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-3-28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coan, J. A., and Allen, J. J. B. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol. Psychol. 67, 7–49. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coben, R., Linden, M., and Myers, T. E. (2010). Neurofeedback for autistic spectrum disorder: A review of the literature. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 35, 83–105. doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9117-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen Kadosh, K., Luo, Q., de Burca, C., Sokunbi, M. O., Feng, J., Linden, D. E. J., et al. (2016). Using real-time fMRI to influence effective connectivity in the developing emotion regulation network. Neuroimage 125, 616–626. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craven, J. L., Rodin, G. M., and Littlefield, C. (1989). The beck depression inventory as a screening device for major depression in renal dialysis patients. Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 18, 365–374.

Google Scholar

Cury, C., Maurel, P., Gribonval, R., and Barillot, C. (2020). A sparse EEG-informed fMRI model for hybrid EEG-fMRI neurofeedback prediction. Front. Neurosci. 13:1451. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., and Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 330–341. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deak, A., Bodrogi, B., Biro, B., Perlaki, G., Orsi, G., and Bereczkei, T. (2017). Machiavellian emotion regulation in a cognitive reappraisal task: An fMRI study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 528–541. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0495-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeBettencourt, M. T., Cohen, J. D., Lee, R. F., Norman, K. A., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2015). Closed-loop training of attention with real-time brain imaging. Nat. Neurosci 18, 470–475. doi: 10.1038/nn.3940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehghani, A., Soltanian-Zadeh, H., and Hossein-Zadeh, G. A. (2020). Global Data-Driven Analysis of Brain Connectivity during Emotion Regulation by Electroencephalography Neurofeedback. Brain Connect 10, 302–315. doi: 10.1089/brain.2019.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehghani, A., Soltanian-Zadeh, H., and Hossein-Zadeh, G. A. (2021). “EEG coherence pattern through recalling positive autobiographical memories and neurofeedback,” in Proceedings of the 2021 28th national and 6th international iranian conference on biomedical engineering (ICBME) (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 131–134.

Google Scholar

Dehghani, A., Soltanian-Zadeh, H., and Hossein-Zadeh, G. A. (2022). Increased neuromodulation ability through EEG connectivity neurofeedback with simultaneous fMRI for emotion regulation. arXiv [Preprint] arXiv: 2204.01087

Google Scholar

Dennis, T. A., and Solomon, B. (2010). Frontal EEG and emotion regulation: Electrocortical activity in response to emotional film clips is associated with reduced mood induction and attention interference effects. Biol. Psychol. 85, 456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebrahimzadeh, E., Saharkhiz, S., Rajabion, L., Oskouei, H., Seraji, M., Fayaz, F., et al. (2022). Simultaneous electroencephalography-functional magnetic resonance imaging for assessment of human brain function. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 16:934266. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.934266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebrahimzadeh, E., Shams, M., Fayaz, F., Rajabion, L., Mirbagheri, M., Nadjar Araabi, B., et al. (2019a). Quantitative determination of concordance in localizing epileptic focus by component-based EEG-fMRI, Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 177, 231–241. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2019.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebrahimzadeh, E., Shams, M., Rahimpour Jopungha, A., Fayaz, F., Mirbagheri, M., Hakimi, N., et al. (2019b). Epilepsy presurgical evaluation of patients with complex source localization by a novel component-based EEG-fMRI approach, Iran. J. Radiol. 16, 18–19. doi: 10.5812/iranjradiol.99134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebrahimzadeh, E., Shams, M., Rahimpour Jounghani, A., Fayaz, F., Mirbagheri, M., and Hakimi, N. (2021). Localizing confined epileptic foci in patients with an unclear focus or presumed multifocality using a component-based EEG-fMRI method. Cogn. Neurodyn. 15, 207–222. doi: 10.1007/s11571-020-09614-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fsl, F. (2006). Software library. Oxford: University of Oxford.

Google Scholar

Gapen, M., van der Kolk, B. A., Hamlin, E., Hirshberg, L., Suvak, M., and Spinazzola, J. (2016). A pilot study of neurofeedback for chronic PTSD. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 41, 251–261. doi: 10.1007/s10484-015-9326-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilboa, A., Winocur, G., Grady, C. L., Hevenor, S. J., and Moscovitch, M. (2004). Remembering our past: Functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1214–1225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, D. P., Oldehinkel, T., and Ormel, J. (1998). Why GHQ threshold varies from one place to another. Psychol. Med. 28, 915–921. doi: 10.1017/S0033291798006874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgolewski, K. J., Varoquaux, G., Rivera, G., Schwarz, Y., Ghosh, S. S., Maumet, C., et al. (2015). NeuroVault.org: A web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain. Front. Neuroinform. 9:8. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, B. U., Colbert, A. P., Brown, K. A., and Ilioi, E. C. (2011). Neurofeedback for insomnia: A pilot study of Z-score SMR and individualized protocols. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 36, 251–264. doi: 10.1007/s10484-011-9165-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, D. C. (2003). QGuided neurofeedback in the treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Neurother. 7, 25–52. doi: 10.1300/J184v07n02_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Davidson, M. C., Glover, G. H., and Casey, B. J. (2005). Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 624–632. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, W., Bu, H., Gao, H., Tong, L., Wang, L., Li, Z., et al. (2017). “Altered amygdala information flow during rt-fMRI neurofeedback training of emotion regulation,” in Proceedings of the DEStech transactions on computer science and engineering, 261–267. doi: 10.12783/dtcse/smce2017/12436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Z., Lin, Y., Xia, L., Liu, Z., Zhang, D., and Elliott, R. (2018). Critical role of the right VLPFC in emotional regulation of social exclusion: A tDCS study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 357–366. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrington, J. D., Mohanty, A., Koven, N. S., Fisher, J. E., Stewart, J. L., Banich, M. T., et al. (2005). Emotion-modulated performance and activity in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Emotion 5, 200–207. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herwig, U., Lutz, J., Scherpiet, S., Scheerer, H., Kohlberg, J., Opialla, S., et al. (2019). Training emotion regulation through real-time fMRI neurofeedback of amygdala activity. Neuroimage 184, 687–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hjelle, E. G., Bragstad, L. K., Zucknick, M., Kirkevold, M., Thommessen, B., and Sveen, U. (2019). The General Health Questionnaire-28 (GHQ-28) as an outcome measurement in a randomized controlled trial in a Norwegian stroke population. BMC Psychol. 7:18. doi: 10.1186/s40359-019-0293-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ino, T., Nakai, R., Azuma, T., Kimura, T., and Fukuyama, H. (2011). Brain activation during autobiographical memory retrieval with special reference to default mode network. Open Neuroimag J. 5, 14–23.

Google Scholar

Jang, J. H., Kim, J. H., Yun, J. Y., Choi, S. H., An, S. C., and Kang, D. H. (2018). Differences in functional connectivity of the insula between brain wave vibration in meditators and non-meditators. Mindfulness (N. Y) 9, 1857–1866. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0928-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, S. J., Boehm, S. G., Healy, D., Goebel, R., and Linden, D. E. J. (2010). Neurofeedback: A promising tool for the self-regulation of emotion networks. Neuroimage 49, 1066–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, S., Linden, D. E. J., Healy, D., Goebel, R., Habes, I., and Boehm, S. G. (2011). Upregulation of emotion areas through neurofeedback with a focus on positive mood. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 44–51. doi: 10.3758/s13415-010-0010-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaur, C., and Singh, P. (2017). Toward EEG spectral analysis of tomographic neurofeedback for depression. Adv. Intell. Syst. Comput. 479, 97–103. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-1708-7_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, N. J., Hortensius, R., Schutter, D. J. L. G., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2017). The relationship of approach/avoidance motivation and asymmetric frontal cortical activity: A review of studies manipulating frontal asymmetry. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 119, 19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khademi, A., Alleyassin, M., AghaHosseini, M., Ramezanzadeh, F., and Abhari, A. A. (2005). Pretreatment Beck depression inventory score is an important predictor for post-treatment score in infertile patients: A before-after study. BMC Psychiatry 5:25. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-5-25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D. Y., Yoo, S. S., Tegethoff, M., Meinlschmidt, G., and Lee, J. H. (2015). The inclusion of functional connectivity information into fmri-based neurofeedback improves its efficacy in the reduction of cigarette cravings. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1552–1572. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H., Yoon, H. W., and Park, H. W. (2004). Improved ballistocardiac artifact removal from the electroencephalogram recorded in fMRI. J. Neurosci. Methods 135, 193–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., and Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 776–798. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinreich, S., Podlipsky, I., Intrator, N., and Hendler, T. (2012). “Categorized EEG neurofeedback performance unveils simultaneous fMRI deep brain activation,” in Machine learning and interpretation in neuroimaging. Lecture notes in computer science, Vol. 7263, eds G. Langs, I. Rish, M. Grosse-Wentrup, and B. Murphy (Berlin: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-34713-9_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klöbl, M., Michenthaler, P., Godbersen, G. M., Robinson, S., Hahn, A., and Lanzenberger, R. (2020). Reinforcement and punishment shape the learning dynamics in fMRI neurofeedback. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:304. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohn, N., Eickhoff, S. B., Scheller, M., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., and Habel, U. (2014). Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation - An ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Neuroimage 87, 345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, Q. M., Qiao, H., Liu, C. Z., Zhang, P., Li, K., Wang, L., et al. (2018). Aberrant intrinsic functional connectivity in thalamo-cortical networks in major depressive disorder. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 24, 1063–1072. doi: 10.1111/cns.12831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koole, S. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cogn. Emot. 23, 4–41. doi: 10.1080/02699930802619031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koush, Y., Meskaldji, D. E., Pichon, S., Rey, G., Rieger, S. W., Linden, D. E. J., et al. (2017). Learning control over emotion networks through connectivity-based neurofeedback. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1193–1202. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koush, Y., Pichon, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Van De Ville, D., Vuilleumier, P., and Scharnowski, F. (2019). Brain networks for engaging oneself in positive-social emotion regulation. Neuroimage 189, 106–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koush, Y., Rosa, M. J., Robineau, F., Heinen, K., Rieger, S. W., Weiskopf, N., et al. (2013). Connectivity-based neurofeedback: Dynamic causal modeling for real-time fMRI. Neuroimage 81, 422–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubik, A., and Biedroń, A. (2013). Neurofeedback therapy in patients with acute and chronic pain syndromes–literature review and own experience. Przeglaa̧d Lek 70, 440–442.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lackner, N., Unterrainer, H., Skliris, D., Wood, G., Wallner-Liebmann, S., Neuper, C., et al. (2016). The Effectiveness of visual short-time neurofeedback on brain activity and clinical characteristics in alcohol use disorders: Practical issues and results. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 47, 188–195. doi: 10.1177/1550059415605686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lempert, K. M., Speer, M. E., Delgado, M. R., and Phelps, E. A. (2017). Positive autobiographical memory retrieval reduces temporal discounting. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1584–1593. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Tong, L., Wang, L., Li, Y., He, W., Guan, M., et al. (2016). Self-regulating positive emotion networks by feedback of multiple emotional brain states using real-time fMRI. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 3575–3586. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4744-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linhartová, P., Látalová, A., Kóša, B., Kašpárek, T., Schmahl, C., and Paret, C. (2019). fMRI neurofeedback in emotion regulation: A literature review. Neuroimage 193, 75–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lioi, G., Butet, S., Fleury, M., Bannier, E., Lécuyer, A., Bonan, I., et al. (2020a). A multi-target motor imagery training using bimodal EEG-fMRI neurofeedback: A pilot study in chronic stroke patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:37. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lioi, G., Cury, C., Perronnet, L., Mano, M., Bannier, E., Lécuyer, A., et al. (2020b). Simultaneous EEG-fMRI during a neurofeedback task, a brain imaging dataset for multimodal data integration. Sci. Data 7:173. doi: 10.1038/s41597-020-0498-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lithari, C., Moratti, S., and Weisz, N. (2016). Limbic areas are functionally decoupled and visual cortex takes a more central role during fear conditioning in humans. Sci. Rep 6:29220. doi: 10.1038/srep29220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Castro, T., Martin, L., Nickley, S., Saraiya, T. C., and Melara, R. D. (2021). Frontal alpha asymmetry in posttraumatic stress disorder: Group differences among individuals with and without PTSD during an inhibitory control task. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 15500594211046703. doi: 10.1177/15500594211046703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., and O’Donnell, P. H. (1995). Evaluation of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changes in T.O.V.A. scores, behavioral ratings, and WISC-R performance. Biofeedback Self. Regul. 20, 83–99. doi: 10.1007/BF01712768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lui, S., Wu, Q., Qiu, L., Yang, X., Kuang, W., Chan, R. C. K., et al. (2011). Resting-state functional connectivity in treatment-resistant depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 642–648. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10101419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., and Buonocore, M. H. (2001). Remembering familiar people: The posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroscience 104, 667–676. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00108-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, A. K. Y., Hu, Z. G., Zhang, J. X. X., Xiao, Z., and Lee, T. M. C. (2009a). Sex-related differences in neural activity during emotion regulation. Neuropsychologia 47, 2900–2908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, A. K. Y., Hu, Z. G., Zhang, J. X., Xiao, Z. W., and Lee, T. M. C. (2009b). Neural correlates of regulation of positive and negative emotions: An fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 457, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., and Burdette, J. H. (2004). Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas. Neuroimage 21, 450–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., and Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00169-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marci, C. D., Glick, D. M., Loh, R., and Dougherty, D. D. (2007). Autonomic and prefrontal cortex responses to autobiographical recall of emotions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 243–250. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.3.243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marxen, M., Jacob, M. J., Müller, D. K., Posse, S., Ackley, E., Hellrung, L., et al. (2016). Amygdala regulation following fMRI-neurofeedback without instructed strategies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayeli, A., Zotev, V., Refai, H., and Bodurka, J. (2016). Real-time EEG artifact correction during fMRI using ICA. J. Neurosci. Methods 274, 27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J. D., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal, Gr. Process. Intergr. Relat. 11, 143–162. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Megumi, F., Yamashita, A., Kawato, M., and Imamizu, H. (2015). Functional MRI neurofeedback training on connectivity between two regions induces long-lasting changes in intrinsic functional network. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:160. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mennella, R., Patron, E., and Palomba, D. (2017). Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for the reduction of negative affect and anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 92, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, T., Quaedflieg, C., Weijland, K., Schruers, K., Merckelbach, H., and Smeets, T. (2018). Frontal EEG asymmetry during symptom provocation predicts subjective responses to intrusions in survivors with and without PTSD. Psychophysiology 55:e12779. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Fu, C. H. Y., Dalton, J. A., Andrew, C. M., and Williams, S. C. R. (2007). A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1150–1162. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mognon, A., Jovicich, J., Bruzzone, L., and Buiatti, M. (2011). ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology 48, 229–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01061.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moosmann, M., Schönfelder, V. H., Specht, K., Scheeringa, R., Nordby, H., and Hugdahl, K. (2009). Realignment parameter-informed artefact correction for simultaneous EEG-fMRI recordings. Neuroimage 45, 1144–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morawetz, C., Bode, S., Baudewig, J., and Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Effective amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts individual differences in successful emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 569–585. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosayebi, R., Dehghani, A., and Hossein-Zadeh, G.-A. (2022). Dynamic functional connectivity estimation for neurofeedback emotion regulation paradigm with simultaneous EEG-fMRI analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:933538. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.933538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nazifi, M., Mokarami, H. R., Akbaritabar, A. K., Faraji Kujerdi, M., Tabrizi, R., and Rahi, A. (2014). Reliability, validity and factor structure of the persian translation of general health questionnire (GHQ-28) in hospitals of kerman university of medical sciences. J. Fasa Univ. Med. Sci. 3, 336–342.

Google Scholar

Niazy, R. K., Beckmann, C. F., Iannetti, G. D., Brady, J. M., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Removal of FMRI environment artifacts from EEG data using optimal basis sets. Neuroimage 28, 720–737. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholson, A. A., Rabellino, D., Densmore, M., Frewen, P. A., Paret, C., Kluetsch, R., et al. (2017). The neurobiology of emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: Amygdala downregulation via real-time fMRI neurofeedback. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 541–560. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 153–158. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00566.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa, T., Moriya, H., Hiroe, N., and Kawanabe, M. (2021). EEG-based neurofeedback with network components extraction:data-driven approach by multilayer ICA extension and simultaneous EEG-fMRI measurements. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2021.06.20.449196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paret, C., Kluetsch, R., Zaehringer, J., Ruf, M., Demirakca, T., Bohus, M., et al. (2016a). Alterations of amygdala-prefrontal connectivity with real-time fMRI neurofeedback in BPD patients. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 952–960. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paret, C., Ruf, M., Gerchen, M.F., Kluetsch, R., Demirakca, T., Jungkunz, M., Bertsch, K., et al. (2016b). FMRI neurofeedback of amygdala response to aversive stimuli enhances prefrontal-limbic brain connectivity. Neuroimage 125, 182–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, F., Oehlen, M., Ronner, J., Van Os, J., and Lousberg, R. (2014a). Neurofeedback as a treatment for major depressive disorder -a pilot study. PLoS One 9:e91837. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, F., Ronner, J., Bodar, L., van Os, J., and Lousberg, R. (2014b). Validation of a neurofeedback paradigm: Manipulating frontal EEG alpha-activity and its impact on mood. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 116–120. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelletier, M., Bouthillier, A., Lévesque, J., Carrier, S., Breault, C., Paquette, V., et al. (2003). Separate neural circuits for primary emotions? Brain activity during self-induced sadness and happiness in professional actors. Neuroreport 14, 1111–1116. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200306110-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perronnet, L., Lécuyer, A., Mano, M., Bannier, E., Lotte, F., Clerc, M., et al. (2017). Unimodal versus bimodal EEG-fMRI neurofeedback of a motor imagery task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:193. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perronnet, L., Lécuyer, A., Mano, M., Fleury, M., Cury, C., Clerc, M., et al. (2020). Learning 2-in-1: Towards Integrated EEG-fMRI-Neurofeedback. BioRxiv 397729. doi: 10.1101/397729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohl, A., Anders, S., Schulte-Rüther, M., Mathiak, K., and Kircher, T. (2013). Positive facial affect - an fMRI study on the involvement of insula and amygdala. PLoS One 8:e69886. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Smulders, F. T. Y., Meyer, T., Peeters, F., Merckelbach, H., and Smeets, T. (2015). The validity of individual frontal alpha asymmetry EEG neurofeedback. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 33–43. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ros, T., Théberge, J., Frewen, P. A., Kluetsch, R., Densmore, M., Calhoun, V. D., et al. (2013). Mind over chatter: Plastic up-regulation of the fMRI salience network directly after EEG neurofeedback. Neuroimage 65, 324–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi-Izquierdo, M., Ernst, A., Soto-Varela, A., Santos-Perez, S., Faraldo-Garcia, A., Sesar-Ignacio, A., et al. (2013). Vibrotactile neurofeedback balance training in patients with Parkinson’s disease: Reducing the number of falls. Gait Posture 37, 195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarkheil, P., Zilverstand, A., Kilian-Hütten, N., Schneider, F., Goebel, R., and Mathiak, K. (2015). fMRI feedback enhances emotion regulation as evidenced by a reduced amygdala response. Behav. Brain Res. 281, 326–332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxby, E., and Peniston, E. G. (1995). Alpha-theta brainwave neurofeedback training: An effective treatment for male and female alcoholics with depressive symptoms. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 685–693. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199509)51:5<685::AID-JCLP2270510514<3.0.CO;2-K

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutter, D. J., and van Honk, J. (2006). Increased positive emotional memory after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the orbitofrontal cortex. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 31, 101–104.

Google Scholar

Sherwood, M. S., Kane, J. H., Weisend, M. P., and Parker, J. G. (2016). Enhanced control of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurophysiology with real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (rt-fMRI) neurofeedback training and working memory practice. Neuroimage 124, 214–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sitaram, R., Ros, T., Stoeckel, L., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Lewis-Peacock, J., et al. (2017). Closed-loop brain training: The science of neurofeedback. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 86–100. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smarr, K. L., and Keefer, A. L. (2011). Measures of depression and depressive symptoms: Beck depression inventory-II (BDI-II), Center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D), Geriatric depression scale (GDS), Hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS), and patient health questionna. Arthritis Care Res. 63, S454–S466. doi: 10.1002/acr.20556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Matthews, Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sourina, O., Liu, Y., and Nguyen, M. K. (2012). Real-time EEG-based emotion recognition for music therapy. J. Multimodal User Interfaces 5, 27–35. doi: 10.1007/s12193-011-0080-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Profile of mood states. Prof. Psychol. 3, 387–388. doi: 10.1037/h0020742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suardi, A., Sotgiu, I., Costa, T., Cauda, F., and Rusconi, M. (2016). The neural correlates of happiness: A review of PET and fMRI studies using autobiographical recall methods. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 383–392. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0414-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subramanian, L., Hindle, J. V., Johnston, S., Roberts, M. V., Husain, M., Goebel, R., et al. (2011). Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback for treatment of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurosci. 31, 16309–16317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3498-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulzer, J., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Weiskopf, N., Birbaumer, N., Blefari, M. L., et al. (2013). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback: Progress and challenges. Neuroimage 76, 386–399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., and Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, S., Lu, L., Zhang, L., Hu, X., Bu, X., Li, H., et al. (2018). Abnormal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in adults and adolescents with major depressive disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. EBioMedicine 36, 436–445. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2018.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trambaiolli, L. R., Kohl, S. H., Linden, D. E. J., and Mehler, D. M. A. (2021). Neurofeedback training in major depressive disorder: A systematic review of clinical efficacy, study quality and reporting practices. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 125, 33–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, R., Singh, V., Sitaram, R., Caria, A., Rauss, K., and Birbaumer, N. (2012). Using real-time fmri to learn voluntary regulation of the anterior insula in the presence of threat-related stimuli. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 623–634. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vourvopoulos, A., Pardo, O. M., Lefebvre, S., Neureither, M., Saldana, D., Jahng, E., et al. (2019). Effects of a brain-computer interface with virtual reality (VR) neurofeedback: A pilot study in chronic stroke patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:210. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrticka, P., Simioni, S., Fornari, E., Schluep, M., Vuilleumier, P., and Sander, D. (2013). Neural substrates of social emotion regulation: A fMRI study on imitation and expressive suppression to dynamic facial signals. Front. Psychol. 4:95. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, J. E., and Kozlowski, G. P. (2005). Neurofeedback treatment of epilepsy. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am 14, 163–76,viii. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2004.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Sokhadze, E. M., El-Baz, A. S., Li, X., Sears, L., Casanova, M. F., et al. (2016). Relative power of specific eeg bands and their ratios during neurofeedback training in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:723. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weon, H., Yoo, J., Yu, J., Park, M., and Son, H. (2021). Effects of cognicise-neurofeedback on health locus of control, depression, and quantitative electroencephalography alpha asymmetry in elderly women. Brain Sci. 11:899. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11070899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittle, S., Simmons, J. G., and Allen, N. B. (2017). “Emotion and gender-specific neural processing in men and women,” in Principles of gender-specific medicine: Gender in the genomic era, 3rd edition, (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press Ltd-Elsevier Science Ltd), doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-803506-1.00013-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, C. K., Zotev, V., Misaki, M., Phillips, R., Luo, Q., and Bodurka, J. (2016). Automatic EEG-assisted retrospective motion correction for fMRI (aE-REMCOR). Neuroimage 129, 133–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T., et al. (2009). Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45, S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y. Y., Feng, Z. Q., Xie, Y. J., Zhang, J., Peng, S. H., Yu, Y. J., et al. (2018). Frontal alpha EEG asymmetry before and after positive psychological interventions for medical students. Front. Psychiatry 9:432. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. D., Siegle, G. J., Misaki, M., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Drevets, W. C., et al. (2018). Altered task-based and resting-state amygdala functional connectivity following real-time fMRI amygdala neurofeedback training in major depressive disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 17, 691–703. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. D., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Yuan, H., Drevets, W. C., et al. (2014). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One 9:e88785. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zich, C., Debener, S., Kranczioch, C., Bleichner, M. G., Gutberlet, I., and De Vos, M. (2015). Real-time EEG feedback during simultaneous EEG-fMRI identifies the cortical signature of motor imagery. Neuroimage 114, 438–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Krueger, F., Phillips, R., Alvarez, R. P., Simmons, W. K., Bellgowan, P., et al. (2011). Self-regulation of amygdala activation using real-time FMRI neurofeedback. PLoS One 6:e24522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Mayeli, A., Misaki, M., and Bodurka, J. (2020). Emotion self-regulation training in major depressive disorder using simultaneous real-time fMRI and EEG neurofeedback. Neuroimage Clin. 27:102331. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Misaki, M., Phillips, R., Wong, C. K., and Bodurka, J. (2018a). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback of the mediodorsal and anterior thalamus enhances correlation between thalamic BOLD activity and alpha EEG rhythm. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1024–1042. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Wong, C. K., Wurfel, B. E., Krueger, F., et al. (2018b). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of the amygdala activity with simultaneous EEG in veterans with combat-related PTSD. Neuroimage Clin. 19, 106–121. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Young, K. D., Drevets, W. C., and Bodurka, J. (2013). Prefrontal control of the amygdala during real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of emotion regulation. PLoS One 8:e79184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Yuan, H., Misaki, M., and Bodurka, J. (2014). Self-regulation of human brain activity using simultaneous real-time fMRI and EEG neurofeedback. Neuroimage 85 Pt 3, 985–995. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Yuan, H., Misaki, M., Phillips, R., Young, K. D., Feldner, M. T., et al. (2016). Correlation between amygdala BOLD activity and frontal EEG asymmetry during real-time fMRI neurofeedback training in patients with depression. Neuroimage Clin. 11, 224–238. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Yuan, H., Phillips, R., and Bodurka, J. (2012). EEG-assisted retrospective motion correction for fMRI: E-REMCOR. Neuroimage 63, 698–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuberer, A., Minder, F., Brandeis, D., and Drechsler, R. (2018). Mixed-effects modeling of neurofeedback self-regulation performance: Moderators for learning in children with ADHD. Neural Plast. 2018:2464310. doi: 10.1155/2018/2464310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurofeedback, simultaneous recording of EEG and fMRI, emotion regulation, frontal asymmetry, functional connectivity, autobiographical memory

Citation: Dehghani A, Soltanian-Zadeh H and Hossein-Zadeh G-A (2023) Probing fMRI brain connectivity and activity changes during emotion regulation by EEG neurofeedback. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:988890. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.988890

Received: 07 July 2022; Accepted: 13 December 2022;
Published: 06 January 2023.

Edited by:

Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Ahmad Mayeli, University of Pittsburgh, United States
Mohammad Rasool Izadi, University of Notre Dame, United States

Copyright © 2023 Dehghani, Soltanian-Zadeh and Hossein-Zadeh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amin Dehghani, www.frontiersin.org amin.dehghani@dartmouth.edu, www.frontiersin.org dehghani@ut.ac.ir

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.