Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 September 2022
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Lifestyle and Self-management of Chronic Pain across the Lifespan View all 5 articles

Electrophysiological indices of pain expectation abnormalities in fibromyalgia patients

  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Rey Juan Carlos University, Madrid, Spain

Fibromyalgia is a chronic pain syndrome characterized by dysfunctional processing of nociceptive stimulation. Neuroimaging studies have pointed out that pain-related network functioning seems to be altered in these patients. It is thought that this clinical symptomatology may be maintained or even strengthened because of an enhanced expectancy for painful stimuli or its forthcoming appearance. However, neural electrophysiological correlates associated with such attentional mechanisms have been scarcely explored. In the current study, expectancy processes of upcoming laser stimulation (painful and non-painful) and its further processing were explored by event-related potentials (ERPs). Nineteen fibromyalgia patients and twenty healthy control volunteers took part in the experiment. Behavioral measures (reaction times and subjective pain perception) were also collected. We manipulated the pain/no pain expectancy through an S1–S2 paradigm (cue-target). S1 (image: triangle or square) predicted the S2 appearance (laser stimulation: warmth or pinprick sensation). Laser stimuli were delivered using a CO2 laser device. Temporal and spatial principal component analyses were employed to define and quantify the ERP component reliability. Statistical analyses revealed the existence of an abnormal pattern of pain expectancy in patients with fibromyalgia. Specifically, our results showed attenuated amplitudes at posterior lCNV component in anticipation of painful stimulation that was not found in healthy participants. In contrast, although larger P2 amplitudes to painful compared to innocuous events were shown, patients did not show any amplitude change in this laser-evoked response as a function of pain predictive cues (as occurred in the healthy control group). Additionally, analyses of the subjective perception of pain and reaction time indicated that laser stimuli preceded by pain cues were rated as more painful than those signaling non-pain expectancy and were associated with faster responses. Differences between groups were not found. The present findings suggest the presence of dysfunction in pain expectation mechanisms in fibromyalgia that eventually may make it difficult for patients to correctly interpret signs that prevent pain symptoms. Furthermore, the abnormal pattern in pain expectancy displayed by fibromyalgia patients could result in ineffective pain coping strategies. Understanding the neural correlates of pain processing and its modulatory factors is crucial to identify treatments for chronic pain syndromes.

Introduction

Chronic pain is a complex pathological state influenced by numerous physiological, psychological and social factors. Fibromyalgia, as a chronic pain syndrome, has sparked interest in the scientific field due to its unknown etiology and unclear pathophysiological mechanisms. Nevertheless, there is no doubt that psychoneurobiological dysfunctions might have crucial roles in the multifactorial symptomatology of this clinical condition (Schweinhardt et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2011; Murga et al., 2017; Richard et al., 2019).

Beyond widespread pain, patients commonly have additional physical, affective and cognitive alterations (Aparicio et al., 2013; Bell et al., 2018; Ferrera et al., 2021; Mercado et al., 2022). Indeed, current evidence has confirmed the principal role of attentional and affective processes in influencing pain hypersensitivity in fibromyalgia (Peters et al., 2000; Gracely et al., 2002; Geisser et al., 2003; Giesecke et al., 2005; Bartley et al., 2009; Duschek et al., 2014; Ellingson et al., 2018). Of note, attention processing abnormalities, mainly consisting of attentional biases or hypervigilance to pain (Schoth et al., 2012; Crombez et al., 2013; Van Ryckeghem and Crombez, 2015; Todd et al., 2018; Van Ryckeghem et al., 2019), have been reported as a contributing factor to initiate, exacerbate and perpetuate augmented pain processing in chronic pain patients as a result of the perceived threat (Vlaeyen et al., 2016). Although increasing research supports the hypothesis that patients with fibromyalgia seem to manifest a preferential allocation of attention toward pain-related stimulation (González-Roldán et al., 2013; Lai et al., 2021; Fernandes-Magalhaes et al., 2022), other studies have failed to find such attentional bias in fibromyalgia patients (Sitges et al., 2018; Pidal-Miranda et al., 2019). It has been proposed that hypervigilance to pain is a dynamic mechanism that activates whenever patients anticipate or interpret pain as a threat, promoting escape and avoidance behaviors, and it is also involved in the increase in pain perception and severity of fibromyalgia (Van Damme et al., 2015).

The expectation of pain seems to be one of the most relevant attentional factors modulating pain experience (Rainville, 2002; Ploghaus et al., 2003; Apkarian et al., 2005; Wiech et al., 2008; Knudsen et al., 2011; Wiech, 2016). Experimental studies focused on pain expectation processing have usually employed predictive cues before presenting an impending somatosensory stimulation (cue-target or S1–S2 paradigms). Applying such paradigms, it has been demonstrated that pain expectancy enhances evaluative pain-related brain regions and subsequent pain perception of painful stimuli (Sawamoto et al., 2000; Koyama et al., 2005; Keltner et al., 2006; Fairhurst et al., 2007; Moseley and Arntz, 2007; Johnston et al., 2012; Kong et al., 2013). In patients with chronic pain, including those suffering from fibromyalgia, neuroimaging studies also found enhanced activation within cerebral regions linked to evaluative pain processing, including periaqueductal gray matter, parietal, insular, cingulate, prefrontal and parahippocampal cortices, when patients were waiting for the upcoming application of a nociceptive stimulation (Cook et al., 2004; Burgmer et al., 2011; González-Roldán et al., 2016). Evidence from some electrophysiological studies has also reported a clear alteration of neural expectancy mechanisms in patients with cervical dystonia or migraine (Rizzo et al., 1985; Kaji et al., 1995), indicating reduced neural anticipatory responses. In the same way, but in contrast to the abovementioned behavioral and neuroimaging findings, fibromyalgia patients also showed decreased amplitudes of the Contingent Negative Variation (CNV) component in anticipation of upcoming pain events (Brown et al., 2014).

Event-related potentials (ERPs) represent a high temporal resolution methodology and are considered a very useful tool to reliably explore the temporal dynamics of neural networks underlying attention and emotion processes (Legrain et al., 2012; Luck, 2014; Gupta et al., 2019). Slow and late negative components of the ERPs (elicited following a predictive cue and ending just before the appearance of an impending stimulus) have been considered the neural correlates of anticipatory attentional mechanisms (i.e., CNV- or stimulus preceding negativity -SPN-; Brunia and van Boxtel, 2001; Brunia et al., 2011). Specifically, two subcomponents of CNV (early and late) have been found in association with cognitive anticipation and motor/cognitive preparation processes, respectively (Rohrbaugh and Gaillard, 1983). While early CNV (mean latency approximately 600 ms) has a mainly frontocentral scalp distribution, late CNV (starting at 800 ms from cue onset) has shown maximal amplitudes in the parieto-occipital scalp regions (Rosahl and Knight, 1995; Cui et al., 2000; Carretié et al., 2001a; Gómez et al., 2003). Thus, it is thought that enhanced amplitudes of expectation-related waves preceding a painful stimulus represent the mobilization of attentional resources to prepare the organism to effectively process and react to it as a relevant and potential threat (Brown et al., 2008; Brown, 2017).

Moreover, pain expectations have the capacity to modulate neural networks involved in pain processing along with the subsequent pain experience (Sawamoto et al., 2000; Atlas and Wager, 2012; Jepma et al., 2018; Shih et al., 2019; Henderson et al., 2020). Electrical (Hird et al., 2018), thermal (Granovsky et al., 2008), or laser painful stimulation (de Tommaso et al., 2011) have all been used as different kinds of nociceptive signals for eliciting pain-related neural correlates. In particular, laser-evoked potentials (LEPs) have been repeatedly described as good neural indices of pain processing (Treede et al., 2003; Plaghki and Mouraux, 2005). For instance, the P2 component of LEPs, with a maximal amplitude at Cz and a mean peak latency of 300–360 ms (Kakigi et al., 2004), has accumulated considerable evidence about its involvement in attentional aspects of pain processing, since its amplitude increases when individuals are paying attention to painful stimulation (Beydoun et al., 1993; García-Larrea et al., 1997; Lorenz and García-Larrea, 2003; Boyle et al., 2008). Interestingly, an increase in P2 amplitudes in response to laser stimulation has also been shown in fibromyalgia (Gibson et al., 1994; Lorenz et al., 1996; Lorenz, 1998; de Tommaso et al., 2011, 2017), suggesting that it may be considered a reliable neural index of attentional influences on pain processing in this chronic syndrome.

Different cognitive mechanisms have been proposed to modulate the P2 component (Brown et al., 2014; de Tommaso et al., 2011; Peláez et al., 2019; Vecchio et al., 2022). Some data have reported that both P2 amplitudes and subjective pain perception could be modulated by the amplitude of expectancy ERP components (e.g., early SPN) elicited by pain-informative cues (Brown et al., 2008). Moreover, the manipulation of the predictive value of pain cues may affect cortical activation; thus, incongruent cue-target associations produce greater neural responses to somatosensory stimulation than congruent ones (Dowman, 2004, 2011; Lorenz et al., 2005; Huang et al., 2017). However, such a relationship between pain anticipatory mechanisms and pain processing neural indices was not confirmed in fibromyalgia patients (Brown et al., 2014). As mentioned, to date, only a few studies have focused on the electrophysiological neural indices of pain expectancy and their potential effect on pain processing in patients with fibromyalgia.

Therefore, the present study aimed to explore the neural mechanisms of pain expectancy and their influence on the further processing of painful events in patients with fibromyalgia through the analysis of ERPs. Whereas the detection and quantification of CNV allows for the study of anticipatory attention mechanisms toward upcoming stimulation (painful or non-painful), analysis of the P2 component of LEPs provides a reliable neural correlate of pain processing. According to prior data about hypervigilance to pain, we hypothesized neural abnormalities in anticipatory responses (CNV) to painful events in fibromyalgia. This alteration in pain expectancy mechanisms might influence brain processing of pain (P2), its subjective perception, and the behavioral outcome (reaction time) in these chronic pain patients.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty-three female fibromyalgia patients and 26 female healthy control participants with an age range from 35 to 65 years old participated in this study. However, data from only 19 patients and 20 controls were considered for further analysis, as will be fully explained later. Patients were diagnosed according to the American College of Rheumatology (ACR) criteria for fibromyalgia (Wolfe et al., 1990, 2016) and recruited from the “Pain Unit” belonging to the Alcorcón Foundation-Hospital (Madrid, Spain) and the association of fibromyalgia patients of Madrid “AFIBROM.” Healthy control participants were recruited by emailing the whole university community of Rey Juan Carlos University (Madrid, Spain) and placing advertisements along the university campus. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants with neurological (stroke, traumatic brain injury), ischemic or psychiatric disorders that might impair cognitive functioning were excluded from the study. The groups were matched by age [t(1,37) = 1.43, p = 0.161, d = 0.45] and educational level [χ2(1,4) = 5.46, p = 0.243, V = 0.37].

At the time of the study, 89% of the patients were taking medication (analgesics, antidepressants, anxiolytics, antiepileptics, or psycholeptics) to manage their clinical symptoms. Due to both ethical and medical considerations, the patients were allowed to continue taking medication, but their possible effects on their behavioral and brain electrical responses were analyzed through statistical contrasts. Only one control participant occasionally took analgesic medication on the days of the experimental protocol. The sociodemographic and psychological measures of the patients whose data were finally processed are shown in Table 1, along with the information about their medication.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical measures.

Participants gave written informed consent for their involvement in the experiment. The Rey Juan Carlos University Research Ethics Board approved this study according to the ethical principles for research conducted with human participants required by this committee. Just before starting the experimental session, different self-report instruments were administered to the participants to characterize their emotional and physical health conditions. The affective status of the participants was assessed by application of the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961) and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1982). Fibromyalgia patients had higher anxiety and depression scores than healthy control participants (see Table 1). Additionally, the patients were asked to provide information about the time elapsed since the onset of symptoms and their diagnosis. They also completed the West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (Kerns et al., 1985) to assess several characteristics of the experienced chronic pain (Table 1).

Stimuli and procedure

An S1–S2 (cue–target) paradigm was used to manipulate the pain expectations and analyze their influence on brain electrical responses to laser stimulation (see Figure 1). The subjective perception of laser stimulation was also measured. Thus, two types of stimuli were presented: a picture representing a geometric figure (S1: white triangle or square) followed by an upcoming laser stimulus (S2: painful or non-painful). Each S1 informed about the nature of the subsequent laser stimulation, configuring two types of cues (Pain cue -PC- or No Pain cue -NPC-). The visual cue stimuli (S1) were matched in size (5 cm height × 5 cm width). They were presented for 250 ms at the center of a 19′′ screen against a black background. Each of them predicted the appearance of a painful (P) or non-painful (NP) laser stimulation. The correspondence between them (S1 and S2) was counterbalanced (i.e., triangle-PC and square-NPC for 50% of the participants).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the experimental procedure. The S1–S2 paradigm is displayed, where S1 = cue (visual) correctly predicted the intensity of the upcoming S2 = target (laser stimulation) in 75% of the trials. (A) No Pain cue trial, and (B) pain cue trial representations. The interstimulus interval (ISI) was 1,500 ms. Intertrial interval (ITI) = 4,030 ms. Only responses within the first 2,000 ms of ITI were considered.

As the target stimulation (S2), brief laser pulses were delivered over the dorsum of the participants’ dominant hand through a CO2 laser (Neurolas, Electronic Engineering; wavelength of 10.6 μm) with a power of 6 watts. Two different laser intensities were set at infra- and supra-threshold pain levels: 15.3 mJ/mm2 (beam diameter of 2.5 mm) for eliciting a “pinprick” or painful sensation (P) and 10.9 mJ/mm2 (beam diameter of 5 mm) for a “warmth” or non-painful sensation (NP). The modification of the laser pulse intensities was made via the beam diameter; the smaller the beam diameter was, the more painful the stimuli. Participants were asked to inform, as soon as possible, about the perceived intensity associated with S2.

Before the experimental session, the two thresholds were determined using the method of limits. Participants experienced 6 ascending and descending series of 10 laser stimuli using the method of limits, as recommended in prior studies (Weiss et al., 2003; Peláez et al., 2016). Participants were asked to verbally inform about the perception of the stimuli (no sensation–sensation) and the intensity of the perceived stimuli (warmth or painful). All participants described the 5 mm diameter laser stimulation as an innocuous warmth sensation, while the 2,5 mm one was established as a tolerable painful pinprick sensation. To avoid tissue damage, habituation or nociceptor sensitization, the laser stimulation was shifted approximately 2 cm after each trial.

The stimuli sequence was controlled by the Gentask module of the STIM2 software (Compumedics Neuroscan). Participants and researchers wore protective goggles during all phases of the experimental procedure. After each laser stimulus, participants were instructed to report the perceived intensity of the laser stimulation through a four-button response pad (1 = “no pain,” 2 = “low pain,” 3 = “moderate pain,” and 4 = “extreme pain”). To prevent impulsive responses, a white dot was displayed on the screen simultaneously with the appearance of a laser stimulus, indicating to the participants the moment for giving their responses. Any response given above 2,000 ms was identified and removed from further analyses.

To explore the pain expectation influences on pain processing, the cue probability for correctly informing about the nature of the subsequent impending laser stimulation (P or NP) was manipulated. Thus, each kind of cue only correctly predicted each type of laser stimulation for 75% of the trials. This manipulation led to four experimental conditions: (1) PC-P, (2) PC-NP, (3) NPC-NP, and (4) NPC-P, among which two congruent (cues correctly predicted laser intensity) and two incongruent (cues incorrectly predicted laser intensity) conditions were established. Participants were told that the cue stimuli correctly predicted the intensity of the laser stimulation in most trials, but some trials contained invalid pain cues. The experimental session included a total of 320 pseudorandomized trials (160 PC/P trials and 160 NPC/NP; 50% each). Among them, 120 trials were congruent (75%), and 40 trials were incongruent (25%). The same experimental condition was not displayed in more than three consecutive trials. The total stimulation was distributed in 8 blocks of 40 trials, maintaining the mentioned proportions of congruent and incongruent trials. After each block, the participants were offered an optional short break (1–2 min) to minimize fatigue. Each trial lasted 5,800 ms with a fixed intertrial interval (ITI) of 4,030 ms (the experimental paradigm can be seen in Figure 1). The entire experimental paradigm lasted 32 min (4 min per block). Before the experimental procedure, the participants completed at least two practice sequences of ten trials to ensure their understanding of the task.

Electroencephalography recording and pre-processing

Participants were seated in a light- and sound-attenuated and electrically isolated room, facing a screen placed 90 cm away from their face. Electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was recorded using a cap with 60 electrodes homogeneously distributed over the scalp (QuickCap Neuroscan). In addition, vertical and horizontal electrooculographic (EOG) activity was recorded from two electrodes located supra- and infra-orbitally on the left eye and two electrodes located on the right and left orbital rims. All electrodes were referenced to the mastoids, and impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. During the entire recording session, channels continuously digitized the data at a sampling rate of 250 Hz. An online bandpass filter was set from 0.1 to 50 Hz. A notch filter at 50 Hz was also applied. Offline pre-processing was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer software v.1.0 (Brain products). Data were digitally filtered using a 30 Hz low pass filter. Segmentation of the continuous recording was carried out on the two different time windows regarding the laser stimulation: the Expectation period (after S1 and before S2) and the Pain processing period (after S2). The expectation period was divided into 1,700 ms epochs for each trial, beginning 200 ms before S1 onset, and two experimental conditions were defined (PC and NPC). Epochs of 1,000 ms (−200 to 800 ms, from S2 onset) were selected for the pain processing period, where four experimental conditions were established (PC-P, PC-NP, NPC-NP, and NPC-P). The EOG-artifact removal procedure was carried out according to the procedure described by Gratton et al. (1983). The artifact detection threshold was set at ± 70 μV. After that, baseline correction and EEG visual inspection were also carried out to remove epochs with artifacts (in both the expectation and pain processing periods) for further analyses. This artifact rejection procedure led to an average epoch admission of 60.3% PC, 59.3% NPC, 73.2% PC-P, 75.3% PC-NP, 73.5% NPC-NP, and 69.6% PC-NP trials. ERP averages were categorized according to each type of stimulus (2 types of cues for the expectation period and 2 cues × 2 laser intensities for the pain processing period). Three participants (2 healthy controls and 1 fibromyalgia) were excluded from the study due to the lack of perception of innocuous stimuli during the discrimination series conducted prior to the experimental session. Furthermore, data from 4 healthy controls and 3 fibromyalgia participants were excluded from further analyses because of the high rate of artifacts in the EEG recording (over 50% epochs rejected). Finally, data from 39 participants (19 fibromyalgia patients and 20 healthy controls) were analyzed.

Statistical analyses

Detection and quantification of event-related potentials

Temporal principal component analysis (tPCA) using a covariance matrix was employed to reliably detect and define the ERP components explaining most of the brain electrical activity variance in response to the predictive cues (expectation period) and laser stimulation (pain processing period). This statistical method for dimension reduction has been strongly recommended when high-density montages are employed, since it avoids subjectivity and misinterpretation in the selection of the time windows based on visual inspection of the grand-averaged ERPs. Thus, the resulting ERP components are free of the influences of other adjacent or subjacent components [see, Dien and Santuzzi, 2005)] for a more detailed description of the tPCA procedure and advantages]. This technique has already demonstrated its ability to disentangle and characterize ERP components, and it has been previously used for the study of expectation (i.e., CNV; Carretié et al., 2001b, 2004) and LEPs (Peláez et al., 2019). In brief, tPCA computes the covariance between all ERP time points, which tends to be high between those time points involved in the same component and low between those belonging to different components. The solution provides a set of different temporal factors made up of highly covarying time points, each of them theoretically involved in the same ERP component. The temporal factor (TF) score, the tPCA-derived parameter in which the extracted TFs may be quantified, is linearly related to the amplitude value of the ERP components. In the present study, thirty-nine subjects, two stimulus categories (PC and NPC) and sixty electrode sites yielded a total of 4,680 averaged waveforms, which served as the database for the tPCA related to the expectation period. In addition, thirty-nine subjects, four stimulus categories (PC-P, PC-NP, NPC-NP, and NPC-P) and sixty electrode sites yielded a total of 9,360 averaged waveforms, which served as the database for the tPCA related to the pain processing period.

Considering that signal overlapping may also occur in the space domain, a spatial principal component analysis (sPCA) was applied. This method allows us to reliably define the topographical distribution of ERP components and decompose the TFs into their main spatial regions or spatial factors (SFs) by means of the detection of covarying scalp points. Likewise, different neural processes may concur at any given time point, corresponding to different electrical signals, and the recording at any scalp location at that moment represents the electrical balance of these distinct neural processes. While tPCA allows for representing the complex superposition of different overlapping ERP components in time, sPCA separates the ERP components along the electrodes located on the scalp, providing a set of SFs of highly covarying scalp regions. These resulting SFs ideally may represent the concurrent neural processes underlying each TF. This configuring and quantifying scalp regions system is preferable to an a priori subdivision into fixed scalp regions for ERP components, since sPCA demarcates scalp regions according to the real behavior of each scalp-point recording. The number of extracted factors in both the temporal and spatial PCA was determined by the scree test method (Cattell, 1966). Promax rotation was applied to select both temporal and spatial factors (Dien, 2010, 2012).

Experimental effects on event-related potentials and behavior

To explore the pain expectations and their influence on pain processing at both the neural and behavioral levels, a series of repeated-measures ANOVAs was computed, as described below:

1. Neural response to pain predictive cues (expectation period): 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs, where cue (PC and NPC) was introduced as a within-subject factor and group (fibromyalgia and healthy control) was used as a between-subjects factor, were carried out to explore the effects on the CNV component (early and late) of the ERPs.

2. Neural response to laser stimulation (pain processing period): 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs were computed to explore the effects of pain expectations on the P2 LEP component. Here, the selected within-subject factors were cue (PC and NPC) and type of laser stimulus (P and NP). The group of participants (fibromyalgia and healthy control) was entered as the between-subjects factor.

3. Behavioral measures: Reaction time and perceived pain intensity in response to laser stimulation were analyzed by a series of 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs, including the same factors previously described in the pain processing period for testing the effects of pain expectations on these two variables.

To determine the significance and direction of the mentioned contrasts, post-hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni’s test were carried out (alpha = 0.05). Greenhouse− Geisser (GG) epsilon correction was applied to adjust the degrees of freedom of the F statistic. Effect sizes were computed through the eta-square (η2p) method. Finally, with the aim of detecting potential influences of medications on attentional and pain-related processes, repeated-measures ANOVAs (Greenhouse−Geisser correction) were also computed, using the consumption of drugs as between-subjects factors and the ERP components of interest (i.e., CNV and P2) and behavioral measures as within-subject factors. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics 25 (IBM, Inc.).

Results

Event-related potential data

Pain expectation period

The tPCA application on the interval between S1 and S2 led to the extraction of four TFs. Considering both their latency (starting at 800 and 460 ms) and topographic distribution (showing a maximal amplitude at the parieto-occipital and frontal scalp sites), TF1 and TF2 were associated with the ERP components signaled in the grand averages as lCNV and eCNV, respectively. Figure 2 shows the correspondence between the TFs derived from the tPCA and the ERP components in the expectation period. Through the subsequent application of the sPCAs to TF scores, two SFs or scalp regions were established for lCNV: SF1 (the posterior scalp region) and SF2 (the frontal scalp region). The whole TF corresponding to the eCNV component was divided into four SFs or scalp regions: SF1 (the frontal scalp region), SF2 (the posterior scalp region), SF3 (the central scalp region), and SF4 (the left temporal-parietal scalp region).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Correspondence between TFs and ERP components derived from the tPCA in the expectation period: (A) topographical distribution of early (TF2) and late (TF1) CNV; (B) TF loads after the application of tPCA; and (C) the correspondence of each component in the grand average of Cz. (PC, pain cue; NPC, no pain cue).

Repeated measures ANOVAs showed significant differences in the posterior lCNV component for the interaction between Cue × Group [F(1,37) = 5.580, p = 0.024, η2p = 0.131]. Post-hoc comparisons revealed a lower amplitude of posterior lCNV in fibromyalgia patients than in healthy control participants after the pain prediction cues. Figure 3 displays the grand averages of the posterior lCNV, where the experimental effects can be observed. Other effects associated with Group or Cue factors did not reach statistical significance. Analyses conducted on the eCNV revealed that this ERP component was not sensitive to experimental manipulations related to pain expectation (p > 0.05 for all of the contrasts).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Grand averages of the expectation period: (A) ERP waveforms for fibromyalgia patients (red) and healthy control participants (black) in response to PC (solid lines) and NPC (dashed lines) at the posterior selected electrodes (Pz and POz). The time window (800–1,500 ms, gray rectangle) and topographic map of lCNV are also highlighted; and (B) means of the lCNV spatial factor scores (posterior region) for fibromyalgia (left) and healthy control participants (right) in PC (red) and NPC (gray) conditions. Asterisks show the comparison in which significant differences were found. Bars show the standard error (*p < 0.05). (PC, pain cue; NPC, no pain cue).

Pain-processing period

Grand averages of the electrical brain activity in response to laser stimulation and the correspondence between the TFs and LEP components for the pain-processing period are displayed in Figure 4. After tPCA application in this time window, six TFs were extracted. Among them, latency (approximately 400 ms) and topographical features of TF2 (maximal amplitude at the central scalp sites in response to painful laser stimuli) corresponded with the P2 LEP waveform. Three SFs or scalp regions were extracted from TF2 (P2 component) after the application of the sPCA (see Figure 4): SF1 (the posterior scalp region), SF2 (the frontal scalp region) and SF3 (the central scalp region).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correspondence between TFs and ERP components derived from the tPCA in the pain-processing period: (A) topographical distribution of the P2 of the LEPs; (B) TF loads after the application of tPCA; and (C) correspondence of each component of the LEPs in the grand average of Cz. (PC-P, pain cue - pain; NPC-P, no pain cue - pain; NPC-NP, no pain cue - no pain; PC-NP, pain cue - no pain).

Repeated measures ANOVAs on P2 revealed a significant main effect of laser stimulation in all the scalp regions: posterior [F(1,37) = 36.903, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.499], frontal [F(1,37) = 23.181, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.385], and central P2 [F(1,37) = 36.088, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.494]. Post-hoc comparisons showed larger amplitudes for painful than non-painful laser stimulation. Interestingly, a significant interaction effect between Group × Cue × Laser stimulation was also found for all of the scalp regions of P2: posterior [F(1,37) = 4.771, p = 0.035, η2p = 0.114], frontal [F(1,37) = 4.696, p = 0.037, η2p 0.113], and central P2 [F(1,37) = 7.828, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.175]. Post-hoc comparisons revealed different pain expectancy influences on the neural response to the laser stimulation in each group of participants. Whereas both fibromyalgia patients and healthy control groups showed larger P2 amplitudes after painful than innocuous laser stimulation, the P2 wave did not reveal changes associated with the meaning conveyed by predictive cues in the fibromyalgia group. Only healthy control participants exhibited an electrophysiological response to laser stimulation modulated by the congruency between predictive cues (pain or no pain) and targets (painful or innocuous). Thus, incongruent conditions (PC-NP and NPC-P) elicited higher P2 amplitudes than congruent conditions. Specifically, the P2 amplitude at posterior scalp regions was greater for NP laser stimulation preceded by PC than NPC (PC-NP > NPC-NP). Both frontal and central P2 scalp regions showed greater amplitudes when the P laser stimulation was preceded by NPC compared to PC (NPC-P > PC-P). Taken together, these results suggest the presence of abnormal processing of pain predictive cues in fibromyalgia patients, who seem to be insensitive to the influences of pain expectations on P2, as evidenced by the group of healthy participants. A representation of the effects described on the P2 LEP component is displayed in Figure 5. No other significant differences involving the variables of interest were found.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Grand averages of LEP components (pain-processing period): The left side of the figure represents the P2 waveform for fibromyalgia and healthy control groups in response to PC-P (red), NPC-P (orange), PC-NP (gray) and NPC-NP (black) conditions. The means of the P2 spatial factor scores for each experimental condition are displayed in the middle. Asterisks represent significant differences (*p < 0.05). Bars indicate the standard error. The topographical maps of each scalp region of P2 are shown on the right side. (A) Pz for posterior P2, (B) Fz for frontal P2, and (C) Cz for the central P2 scalp regions. (PC-P, pain cue - pain; NPC-P, no pain cue - pain; NPC-NP, no pain cue - no pain; PC-NP, pain cue - no pain).

Behavioral data

The mean and standard deviations of the pain perception and reaction times (RTs) for each type of laser stimulus (as a function of predictive cues) are displayed in Figure 6. As expected, repeated-measures ANOVAs on subjective pain perception showed a significant main effect for laser stimulation [F(1,37) = 340.516, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.902]. Post-hoc analyses revealed a greater intensity of pain perception for painful (2.8 ± 0.08) compared to non-painful stimulation (1.2 ± 0.03). Additionally, a significant main effect was detected for the type of Cue [F(1,37) = 12.766, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.257]. Laser stimulation preceded by pain cues elicited a greater pain perception (2.0 ± 0.05) than no pain cues (1.9 ± 0.04). Although patients with fibromyalgia showed a higher pain perception than healthy participants, this difference did not reach statistical significance [F(1,37) = 3.573, p = 0.067, η2p = 0.088]. No other significant results involving potential interaction effects between cues and laser stimulation were found.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Behavioral data: Mean and standard deviation of (A) RTs (ms) and (B) subjective perception of pain (1–4) for healthy control (gray) and fibromyalgia (red) of the four experimental conditions (PC-P, pain cue-pain; NPC-NP, no pain cue-no pain; NPC-P, no pain cue-pain; PC-NP, pain cue-no pain). Bars represent the standard error.

With respect to RTs, the results pointed out the existence of a main significant effect for type of Cue [F(1,37) = 10.888, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.227]. Longer RTs were obtained in response to pain cue trials (1020 ± 35) compared to those informing of no pain expectancy (976 ± 31). A main effect on laser stimulation was also found [F(1,37) = 4.765, p = 0.035, η2p = 0.114], according to which painful laser stimuli were associated with shorter RTs (957 ± 29) than non-painful events (1038 ± 44). In addition, a significant interaction between Cue × Laser stimulation [F(1,37) = 23.927, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.393] showed augmented RTs for incongruent trials in all of the contrasts, except for the no pain cues conditions (NP-P 975 ± 29 > P-P 939 ± 32, P-NP 1101 ± 49 > NP-NP 976 ± 42, P-NP 1101 ± 49 > P-P 939 ± 32 and NP-P 975 ± 29 = NP-NP 976 ± 42). Finally, a Group × Laser stimulation interaction was also detected [F(1,37) = 5.980, p = 0.019, η2p = 0.139]. Patients with fibromyalgia had shorter RTs for painful (956 ± 42) compared to non-painful laser stimulation (1129 ± 63). Moreover, patients were slower to inform themselves about the presence of a non-painful stimulation compared to healthy participants (948 ± 62).

Analysis controlling the potential effect of medication

Since 89% of patients with fibromyalgia were taking drugs by physician prescription and such medication could have effects on central nervous system (CNS) functioning, repeated-measures ANOVAs were computed to detect such possible influences in the patient group. Antidepressants and anxiolytics were introduced as between-subjects factors (due to their potential capacity to alter cognitive functioning), whereas the neural response and behavioral outcomes were defined as dependent variables. A significant effect of antidepressants was found for laser stimulation on frontal P2 amplitudes [F(1,17) = 6.812, p = 0.018, η2p = 0.286]. The P2 amplitude was higher in response to painful stimulation for patients taking antidepressant drugs, but it does not seem to add information to the present results since significant differences between groups on P2 components were not detected. lCNV amplitudes were not significantly affected by medication taken by patients with fibromyalgia.

Additionally, the impact of drug consumption on behavioral measures was also explored. A significant effect of anxiolytics for laser stimulation in RTs [F(1,17) = 6.366, p = 0.022, η2p = 0.272] was found. Thus, lower RTs were associated with painful stimulus responses in patients not taking anxiolytic medication compared to those taking them. Finally, a significant effect of antidepressants for Cue on perceived pain [F(1,17) = 11.067, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.394] was also detected. In this regard, perceived pain intensity was lower in PC trials for patients who were taking antidepressants. Again, such effects might not refute the previous results regarding RTs and perceived pain intensities.

Discussion

The main aim of the present study was to explore the neural mechanisms of pain expectancy and its potential influence on the processing of painful and innocuous laser stimulation in fibromyalgia patients. The results could be summarized as follows: (1) patients with fibromyalgia exhibited decreased posterior lCNV amplitudes in anticipation of painful stimulation; (2) in contrast with findings about expectancy-related modulations detected in healthy participants, the P2 component was not sensitive to the expectations of pain in fibromyalgia, varying only in accordance with the intensity of the laser stimulation; and (3) behavioral measures (pain perception and RT) were, however, modulated as a function of pain anticipation signals in the whole sample of participants, so that pain-related cues were associated with higher pain perception and slower RTs than no pain cues. Implications derived from the present findings will be addressed in more detail below.

Contingent Negative Variation has been reported as an ERP component that represents anticipatory attention, comprising cognitive and motor preparation processes, before an impending event (Brunia et al., 2011). Several studies have established that the prediction of upcoming nociceptive stimuli involves the recruitment of attentional mechanisms for pain regulation (Van Damme et al., 2010; Legrain et al., 2012; Palermo et al., 2015). Thus, pain expectation would enhance the attentional resources directed to the upcoming painful event being reflected as higher amplitudes in anticipatory ERP deflections, such as CNV or analogous components (Stude et al., 2003; Babiloni et al., 2004; Brown et al., 2008; Seidel et al., 2015). According to theoretical proposals on hypervigilance to pain (Crombez et al., 2013), it would be expected that increased pain anticipation in chronic pain patients might lead to amplified pain processing responses. In this vein, several findings have highlighted the presence of augmented activation of pain processing brain regions during pain anticipation in patients with fibromyalgia (Cook et al., 2004; Crombez et al., 2004; Burgmer et al., 2011). However, in this study, fibromyalgia patients showed a pronounced attenuation of lCNV amplitudes at posterior scalp regions for trials involving pain expectations. In support of the current results, reduced frontoparietal and ventral tegmental activation after predictive pain cues has also been reported (Loggia et al., 2014). The observed discrepancies among different sources of experimental evidence could be derived from the variability in methodological and experimental procedures regarding the manipulations on pain expectancy (i.e., in Loggia et al., 2014, pain cues informed about the pain stimulation onset, while in Burgmer et al., 2011, the uncertainty of pain predictive cues was manipulated). On the other hand, electrophysiological measures would entail more suitable techniques than other neuroimaging methodologies to reliably detect the temporal dynamics of anticipatory attention to pain processing.

The absence of an enhanced lCNV elicited by predictive pain cues in patients with fibromyalgia makes a straightforward interpretation difficult and deserves further consideration. The scarce experimental ERP evidence of pain expectative mechanisms in chronic pain patients suggests a decreased CNV in anticipation of pain events (Kaji et al., 1995) or during a pain episode (Rizzo et al., 1985). More recently, lower amplitudes of a late expectation wave have been found in fibromyalgia in anticipation of moderate intensity laser stimuli compared to healthy people and osteoarthritis patients (Brown et al., 2014). Other similar data indicating CNV alterations during pain expectation or hyperalgesia conditioning paradigms have been related to associative learning dysfunction in chronic pain patients (Schneider et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2019). Similarly, dysfunctions of associative learning mechanisms have also been described in fibromyalgia (Jenewein et al., 2013; Meulders et al., 2015; Sandström et al., 2020). Learning from past pain experiences may allow us to adjust our behavior and respond adaptively to new pain events. Therefore, deficiencies in associative learning could become the context unstable and unpredictable, making the anticipation of pain for adequate coping very difficult, and could play an important role in the development and maintenance of chronic pain syndromes (Jepma et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019). In accordance with that, the abnormal pain anticipation processing in our results would suggest a disability in fibromyalgia patients, preventing them from taking advantage of the contextual cues that indicate pain increase or pain relief. Moreover, considering that psychophysiological signs of motor and cognitive preparation (such as those represented by CNV) would reflect the effort in the deployment of cognitive regulation mechanisms (Jennings and van der Molen, 2005), it could be proposed that the decreased CNV in fibromyalgia might imply difficulty mobilizing attentional resources, cognitive regulation and coping processes to adequately deal with painful events.

Regarding the neural findings for the processing of painful stimulation, our results showed that the P2 amplitude was sensitive to the intensity of the CO2 laser stimulation, since painful stimuli generated larger P2 waves than innocuous stimuli. Furthermore, a distinguishable modulation pattern of P2 was found by predictive cues for each group of participants. In this sense, the pain expectancy manipulation led to P2 amplitude changes only for the healthy control group. Surprisingly, these variations in P2 were not found in fibromyalgia and were influenced only by the laser stimulation intensities (larger amplitudes for painful compared to non-painful conditions). Greater amplitudes of the P2 component for painful stimuli have been repeatedly reported in the general population (Beydoun et al., 1993; García-Larrea et al., 1997; Ohara et al., 2004; Iannetti et al., 2005, 2008; Dowman, 2007). Likewise, hypersensitivity in fibromyalgia, along with increased LEPs components to painful laser CO2 stimuli (Gibson et al., 1994; Lorenz, 1998; de Tommaso et al., 2011, 2017), have been consistently described. Nevertheless, the absence of significant differences between groups for the amplitude of P2 in the current results goes against the mentioned neural correlates of the augmented pain processing response in fibromyalgia. Brown et al. also failed to find significant differences in P2 among fibromyalgia, osteoarthritis and healthy control participants when predictive cues were used for signaling the onset of an upcoming pain stimulus. It is well established that pain expectation cues enhance the P2 component of LEPs (Miyazaki et al., 1994; Hauck et al., 2007; Brown et al., 2008). However, when the predictive value of the cues is manipulated, the effects of pain expectancy on P2 become controversial (Dowman, 2004, 2011; Lorenz et al., 2005). The lack of pain cue effects on the P2 amplitude in the fibromyalgia group appears to be consistent with some previous evidence. Similarly, cognitive and emotional manipulations have failed to modulate the brain response after nociceptive stimulation in fibromyalgia (Kamping et al., 2013; Loggia et al., 2014), indicating the existence of an endogenous pain modulation disturbance. From this point of view, decrements of neural correlates of pain expectation in fibromyalgia have been associated with an alteration of descending pain modulation mechanisms (de Souza et al., 2009; Goffaux et al., 2009; Burgmer et al., 2011; Fields, 2018; Ioachim et al., 2022), which could be related to the absence of expectative modulation of the neural response to pain.

Complementarily, behavioral outcomes involving subjective perceptions of pain and RTs were sensitive to the experimental manipulations. Regarding pain perception, the present results support the well-established ideas about their modulation as a consequence of the anticipatory processes (Moseley and Arntz, 2007; McGowan et al., 2009; Johnston et al., 2012). All of the participants reported greater pain perception during the pain cue trials. On the other hand, both pain cueing and incongruent trials led to longer RTs than no pain cueing and congruent trials. Longer RTs to pain cue incongruent trials (PC-NP) were especially remarkable among the patients, but the lack of significant differences in the interaction involving group, type of cue and laser stimulation prevents any clear interpretation. It could be suggested that attention disengagement difficulties from pain-related cues might at least partially explain the present findings (Van Damme et al., 2002, 2004; Fernandes-Magalhaes et al., 2022). According to previous data, incongruent trials might produce an interference effect (i.e., longer RTs) in the processing of somatosensory events and, thereby, might affect behavioral performance (Yiend and Mathews, 2001). Such an effect would be more interfering for patients presenting with pain attentional biases, as usually occurs in fibromyalgia (Broadbent et al., 2021). It should be noted that the behavioral outcomes and P2 amplitudes in fibromyalgia seem to be contradictory, as the RT and perceived pain intensity were modulated by pain cues and the P2 waves were not. The ERP technique allows us to reliably disentangle the temporal course of rapid and overlapped cognitive processes, and behavior outcomes are the final output. The present investigation focused on two specific ERP components (CNV and P2), but other ERP waves (in turn, other cognitive processes) could be sensitive to pain expectative manipulations, affecting behavioral measures.

Although the present investigation is one of the few to explore pain expectancy in fibromyalgia and its further influence on pain processing, several limitations should be considered. First, we applied two different (painful and innocuous) fixed (equal for all participants) laser stimulus intensities, but it is more common to individually determine the intensity of the applied painful stimuli due to the subjectivity and variability in the pain experience (Peláez et al., 2019). Nonetheless, both methods have been employed in fibromyalgia patients, and similar results have been obtained (i.e., de Tommaso et al., 2011; Brown et al., 2014). Even though our results did not indicate significant differences in perceived pain intensity between groups, we should not omit the possible influence of individual variability in the processing of painful stimulation. In addition, the four-button device used for collecting behavioral responses (perceived pain intensity and RT) may not be optimal to show the great variability in pain reports. Mainly regarding the non-painful stimulation, a succession of warmth sensations was not reflected in the rating scale (1 = no pain), while it was reproduced in the pain sensations scale (2 = low pain, 3 = moderate pain, 4 = extreme pain). Although this pain rating procedure is unable to achieve fine somatosensory discrimination, the experimental manipulations seemed to have been effective. Second, the medication taken by fibromyalgia patients had an effect on both the brain electrical response and the behavioral performance levels. Previous scientific evidence has demonstrated that this kind of drug does not affect cognition in fibromyalgia or chronic pain patients (Chapman et al., 2002; Mohs et al., 2012; Reyes del Paso et al., 2012). Nevertheless, it should be considered with caution due to its influence on pain perception.

To conclude, the present findings point out a dysfunction of the pain expectation mechanisms in fibromyalgia, probably concerning associative learning difficulties, which would lead to abnormal processing of painful stimulation that eventually could result in ineffective strategies for pain regulation. It has been suggested that a decrease in neural expectation resources underlies such unsuccessful pain coping efforts in these patients (Brown et al., 2014). Understanding neural correlates of pain processing and pain modulation factors is indispensable to achieve a better knowledge of chronic pain syndromes. In addition, the influence of current spontaneous and endogenous pain in chronic pain sufferers should be taken into account as a factor modulating pain processing. The present research represents a contribution to broadening the knowledge of pain expectation neural mechanisms and their influence on pain processing in fibromyalgia. Future research in this field would help design more effective coping strategies to improve the quality of life of chronic pain patients.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: Open Science Forum https://osf.io/bhjnc/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Rey Juan Carlos University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

PB and FM: conceptualization, software, and writing—original draft. PB, JG-G, CP-P, AL-L, LV, and FM: data curation. PB, RF-M, and FM: formal analysis. FM: funding acquisition, project administration, and resources. PB, JG-G, LV, IP, and FM: investigation. PB, LV, JG-G, and FM: methodology. CP-P and FM: supervision. PB, IP, DF, AL-L, RF-M, and FM: validation. PB: visualization. PB, IP, DF, RF-M, and FM: writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants PID2020-115463RB-I00 from the Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades of Spain and SAPIENTIA-CM H2019/HUM-5705 of the Comunidad de Madrid.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all participants for taking part in the experiment.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aparicio, V. A., Ortega, F. B., Carbonell-Baeza, A., Cuevas, A. M., Delgado-Fernández, M., and Jonatan, R. (2013). Anxiety, depression and fibromyalgia pain and severity. Psicol. Conductual 21, 381–392.

Google Scholar

Apkarian, A. V., Bushnell, M. C., Treede, R.-D., and Zubieta, J.-K. (2005). Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur. J. Pain 9, 463–463. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atlas, L. Y., and Wager, T. D. (2012). How expectations shape pain. Neurosci. Lett. 520, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Rossini, P. M., Brancucci, A., Del Percio, C., et al. (2004). Cortical sensorimotor interactions during the expectancy of a Go/No-Go task: Effects of painful stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 925–935. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartley, E. J., Rhudy, J. L., and Williams, A. E. (2009). Experimental assessment of affective processing in fibromyalgia. J. Pain 10, 1151–1160. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571.

Google Scholar

Bell, T., Trost, Z., Buelow, M. T., Clay, O., Younger, J., Moore, D., et al. (2018). Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in fibromyalgia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 698–714. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1422699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beydoun, A., Morrow, T. J., Shen, J. F., and Casey, K. L. (1993). Variability of laser-evoked potentials: Attention, arousal and lateralized differences. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Evoked Potentials 88, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90002-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, Y., El-Deredy, W., Montes, E. M., Bentley, D. E., and Jones, A. K. P. (2008). Selective modulation of nociceptive processing due to noise distraction. Pain 138, 630–640. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, P., Liossi, C., and Schoth, D. E. (2021). Attentional bias to somatosensory stimuli in chronic pain patients: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 162, 332–352. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. (2017). “Anticipatory brain responses and expectancy effects on pain: Theory, research findings and functional networks,” in Neuroimaging of pain, (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 123–152. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-48046-6_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. A., El-Deredy, W., and Jones, A. K. P. (2014). When the brain expects pain: Common neural responses to pain anticipation are related to clinical pain and distress in fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 663–672. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. A., Seymour, B., Boyle, Y., El-Deredy, W., and Jones, A. K. P. (2008). Modulation of pain ratings by expectation and uncertainty: Behavioral characteristics and anticipatory neural correlates. Pain 135, 240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunia, C. H. M., Hackley, S. A., van Boxtel, G. J. M., Kotani, Y., and Ohgami, Y. (2011). Waiting to perceive: Reward or punishment? Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 858–868. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunia, C. H., and van Boxtel, G. J. (2001). Wait and see. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 43, 59–75. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00179-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgmer, M., Petzke, F., Giesecke, T., Gaubitz, M., Heuft, G., and Pfleiderer, B. (2011). Cerebral activation and catastrophizing during pain anticipation in patients with fibromyalgia. Psychosom. Med. 73, 751–759. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318236588a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carretié, L., Martín-Loeches, M., Hinojosa, J. A., and Mercado, F. (2001a). Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 1109–1128. doi: 10.1162/089892901753294400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carretié, L., Mercado, F., and Tapia, M. (2001b). Emotion, attention, and the ‘ negativity bias ’, studied through event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 41, 75–85.

Google Scholar

Carretié, L., Tapia, M., Mercado, F., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., and De La Serna, J. M. (2004). Voltage-based versus factor score-based source localization analyses of electrophysiological brain activity: A comparison. Brain Topogr. 17, 109–115. doi: 10.1007/s10548-004-1008-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattell, R. (1966). The Scree Test for the number of factors. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1, 116–141. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr0102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, S. L., Byas-Smith, M. G., and Reed, B. A. (2002). Effects of intermediate- and long-term use of opioids on cognition in patients with chronic pain. Clin. J. Pain 18, S83–S90. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200207001-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, D. B., Lange, G., Ciccone, D. S., Wen-Ching, L., Steffener, J., and Natelson, B. H. (2004). Functional Imaging of Pain in Patients with Primary Fibromyalgia. J. Rheumatoly 31, 364–378.

Google Scholar

Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van den Broeck, A., Goubert, L., and Van Houdenhove, B. (2004). Hypervigilance to Pain in Fibromyalgia. Clin. J. Pain 20, 98–102. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200403000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crombez, G., Van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Eccleston, C., and Van Damme, S. (2013). Attentional bias to pain-related information: A meta-analysis. Pain 154, 497–510. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, R., Egkher, A., Huter, D., Lang, W., Lindinger, G., and Deecke, L. (2000). High resolution spatiotemporal analysis of the contingent negative variation in simple or complex motor tasks and a non-motor task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1847–1859. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00388-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Souza, J. B., Potvin, S., Goffaux, P., Charest, J., and Marchand, S. (2009). The deficit of pain inhibition in fibromyalgia is more pronounced in patients with comorbid depressive symptoms. Clin. J. Pain 25, 123–127. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318183cfa4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Federici, A., Santostasi, R., Calabrese, R., Vecchio, E., Lapadula, G., et al. (2011). Laser-evoked potentials habituation in fibromyalgia. J. Pain 12, 116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Ricci, K., Libro, G., Vecchio, E., Delussi, M., Montemurno, A., et al. (2017). Pain processing and vegetative dysfunction in fibromyalgia: A study by sympathetic skin response and laser evoked potentials. Pain Res. Treat. 2017, 1–12. doi: 10.1155/2017/9747148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dien, J. (2010). The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data. J. Neurosci. Methods 187, 138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dien, J. (2012). Applying principal components analysis to event-related potentials: A Tutorial. Dev. Neuropsychol. 37, 497–517. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2012.697503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dien, J., and Santuzzi, A. M. (2005). Application of repeated measures ANOVA to high-density ERP datasets: A review and tutorial. Event Relat. Potentials A Methods Handb. 4, 57–82. doi: 10.5061/dryad.30dn3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowman, R. (2004). Electrophysiological indices of orienting attention toward pain. Psychophysiology 41, 749–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00207.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowman, R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of detecting and orienting attention toward unattended threatening somatosensory targets. I. Intermodal effects. Psychophysiology 44, 407–419. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00508.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowman, R. (2011). The role of somatic threat feature detectors in the attentional bias toward pain: Effects of spatial attention. Psychophysiology 48, 397–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01068.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duschek, S., Werner, N. S., Limbert, N., Winkelmann, A., and Montoya, P. (2014). Attentional bias toward negative information in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain Med. 15, 603–612. doi: 10.1111/pme.12360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellingson, L. D., Stegner, A. J., Schwabacher, I. J., Lindheimer, J. B., and Cook, D. B. (2018). Catastrophizing interferes with cognitive modulation of pain in women with fibromyalgia. Pain Med. 19, 2408–2422. doi: 10.1093/pm/pny008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairhurst, M., Wiech, K., Dunckley, P., and Tracey, I. (2007). Anticipatory brainstem activity predicts neural processing of pain in humans. Pain 128, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes-Magalhaes, R., Ferrera, D., Peláez, I., Martín-Buro, M. C., Carpio, A., De Lahoz, M. E., et al. (2022). Neural correlates of the attentional bias towards pain-related faces in fibromyalgia patients: An ERP study using a dot-probe task. Neuropsychologia 166:108141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrera, D., Mercado, F., Peláez, I., Martínez-Iñigo, D., Fernandes-Magalhaes, R., Barjola, P., et al. (2021). Fear of pain moderates the relationship between self-reported fatigue and methionine allele of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene in patients with fibromyalgia. PLoS One 16:e0250547. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, H. L. (2018). How expectations influence pain. Pain 159, S3–S10. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Larrea, L., Peyron, R., Laurent, B., and Mauguière, F. (1997). Association and dissociation between laser-evoked potentials and pain perception. Neuroreport 8, 3785–3789. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geisser, M. E., Casey, K. L., Brucksch, C. B., Ribbens, C. M., Appleton, B. B., and Crofford, L. J. (2003). Perception of noxious and innocuous heat stimulation among healthy women and women with fibromyalgia: Association with mood, somatic focus, and catastrophizing. Pain 102, 243–250. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00417-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, S., Littlejohn, G. O., Gorman, M., Helme, R., and Granges, G. (1994). Altered heat pain thresholds and cerebral event-related potentials following painful CO2 laser stimulation in subjects with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain 58, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90198-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giesecke, T., Gracely, R. H., Williams, D. A., Geisser, M. E., Petzke, F. W., and Clauw, D. J. (2005). The relationship between depression, clinical pain, and experimental pain in a chronic pain cohort. Arthritis Rheum. 52, 1577–1584. doi: 10.1002/art.21008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffaux, P., de Souza, J. B., Potvin, S., and Marchand, S. (2009). Pain relief through expectation supersedes descending inhibitory deficits in fibromyalgia patients. Pain 145, 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez, C., Marco, J., and Grau, C. (2003). Preparatory visuo-motor cortical network of the contingent negative variation estimated by current density. Neuroimage 20, 216–224. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00295-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Roldán, A. M., Bomba, I. C., Diesch, E., Montoya, P., Flor, H., and Kamping, S. (2016). Controllability and hippocampal activation during pain expectation in fibromyalgia syndrome. Biol. Psychol. 121, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Roldán, A. M., Muñoz, M. A., Cifre, I., Sitges, C., and Montoya, P. (2013). Altered psychophysiological responses to the view of others’. Pain and anger faces in fibromyalgia patients. J. Pain 14, 709–719. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.01.775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gracely, R. H., Petzke, F., Wolf, J. M., and Clauw, D. J. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 46, 1333–1343. doi: 10.1002/art.10225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granovsky, Y., Granot, M., Nir, R.-R., and Yarnitsky, D. (2008). Objective correlate of subjective pain perception by contact heat-evoked potentials. J. Pain 9, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, R. S., Kujawa, A., and Vago, D. R. (2019). The neural chronometry of threat-related attentional bias?: Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for early and late stages of selective attentional processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 146, 20–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauck, M., Lorenz, J., Zimmermann, R., Debener, S., Scharein, E., and Engel, A. K. (2007). Duration of the cue-to-pain delay increases pain intensity: A combined EEG and MEG study. Exp. Brain Res. 180, 205–215. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0863-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, L. A., Di Pietro, F., Youssef, A. M., Lee, S., Tam, S., Akhter, R., et al. (2020). Effect of expectation on pain processing: A psychophysics and functional MRI analysis. Front. Neurosci. 14:6. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hird, E. J., Jones, A. K. P., Talmi, D., and El-Deredy, W. (2018). A comparison between the neural correlates of laser and electric pain stimulation and their modulation by expectation. J. Neurosci. Methods 293, 117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Shang, Q., Dai, S., and Ma, Q. (2017). Dread of uncertain pain: An event-related potential study. PLoS One 12:e0182489. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Hughes, N. P., Lee, M. C., and Mouraux, A. (2008). Determinants of laser-evoked EEG responses: Pain perception or stimulus saliency? J. Neurophysiol. 100, 815–828. doi: 10.1152/jn.00097.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Zambreanu, L., Cruccu, G., and Tracey, I. (2005). Operculoinsular cortex encodes pain intensity at the earliest stages of cortical processing as indicated by amplitude of laser-evoked potentials in humans. Neuroscience 131, 199–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioachim, G., Warren, H. J. M., Powers, J. M., Staud, R., Pukall, C. F., and Stroman, P. W. (2022). Altered pain in the brainstem and spinal cord of fibromyalgia patients during the anticipation and experience of experimental pain. Front. Neurol. 13:862976. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.862976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenewein, J., Moergeli, H., Sprott, H., Honegger, D., Brunner, L., Ettlin, D., et al. (2013). Fear-learning deficits in subjects with fibromyalgia syndrome? Eur. J. Pain 17, 1374–1384. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00300.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, J. R., and van der Molen, M. W. (2005). Preparation for speeded action as a psychophysiological concept. Psychol. Bull. 131, 434–459. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jepma, M., Koban, L., van Doorn, J., Jones, M., and Wager, T. D. (2018). Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 838–855. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, N. E., Atlas, L. Y., and Wager, T. D. (2012). Opposing effects of expectancy and somatic focus on pain. PLoS One 7:e38854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaji, R., Ikeda, A., Ikeda, T., Kubori, T., Mezaki, T., Kohara, N., et al. (1995). Physiological study of cervical dystonia. Task-specific abnormality in contingent negative variation. Brain 118, 511–522. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.2.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakigi, R., Inui, K., Tran, D. T., Qiu, Y., Wang, X., Watanabe, S., et al. (2004). Human brain processing and central mechanisms of pain as observed by electro- and magneto-encephalography. J. Chin. Med. Assoc. 67, 377–386.

Google Scholar

Kamping, S., Bomba, I. C., Kanske, P., Diesch, E., and Flor, H. (2013). Deficient modulation of pain by a positive emotional context in fibromyalgia patients. Pain 154, 1846–1855. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, J. R., Furst, A., Fan, C., Redfern, R., Inglis, B., and Fields, H. L. (2006). Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 26, 4437–4443. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4463-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, R. D., Turk, D. C., and Rudy, T. E. (1985). The West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI). Pain 23, 345–356. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(85)90004-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knudsen, L., Petersen, G. L., Nørskov, K. N., Vase, L., Finnerup, N., Jensen, T. S., et al. (2011). Review of neuroimaging studies related to pain modulation. Scand. J. Pain 2, 108–120. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2011.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, J., Jensen, K., Loiotile, R., Cheetham, A., Wey, H. Y., Tan, Y., et al. (2013). Functional connectivity of the frontoparietal network predicts cognitive modulation of pain. Pain 154, 459–467. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koyama, T., McHaffie, J. G., Laurienti, P. J., and Coghill, R. C. (2005). The subjective experience of pain: Where expectations become reality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 12950–12955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408576102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, C., Ciacchella, C., Pellicano, G. R., Altavilla, D., Sambucini, D., Paolucci, T., et al. (2021). Different electrophysiological responses to pain-related visual stimuli between fibromyalgia and chronic low back pain women: A pilot case-control study. Chronic Stress 5:24705470211046881. doi: 10.1177/24705470211046881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. C., Nassikas, N. J., and Clauw, D. J. (2011). The role of the central nervous system in the generation and maintenance of chronic pain in rheumatoid arthritis, osteoarthritis and fibromyalgia. Arthritis Res. Ther. 13:211. doi: 10.1186/ar3306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Mancini, F., Sambo, C. F., Torta, D. M., Ronga, I., and Valentini, E. (2012). Cognitive aspects of nociception and pain. Bridging neurophysiology with cognitive psychology. Neurophysiol. Clin. 42, 325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loggia, M. L., Berna, C., Kim, J., Cahalan, C. M., Gollub, R. L., Wasan, A. D., et al. (2014). Disrupted brain circuitry for pain-related reward/punishment in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheumatol. 66, 203–212. doi: 10.1002/art.38191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, J. (1998). Hyperalgesia or hypervigilance? An evoked potential approach to the study of fibromyalgia syndrome. Z. Rheumatol. 57, Suppl 2 19–22.

Google Scholar

Lorenz, J., and García-Larrea, L. (2003). Contribution of attentional and cognitive factors to laser evoked brain potentials. Neurophysiol. Clin. 33, 293–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2003.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, J., Grasedyck, K., and Bromm, B. (1996). Middle and long latency somatosensory evoked potentials after painful laser stimulation in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Potentials Sect. 100, 165–168. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00259-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, J., Hauck, M., Paur, R. C., Nakamura, Y., Zimmermann, R., Bromm, B., et al. (2005). Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments–A possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions. Brain Behav. Immun. 19, 283–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J. (2014). “Overview of common ERP components,” in In an introduction to the event-related potential technique, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 71–117.

Google Scholar

McGowan, N., Sharpe, L., Refshauge, K., and Nicholas, M. K. (2009). The effect of attentional re-training and threat expectancy in response to acute pain. Pain 142, 101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercado, F., Ferrera, D., Fernandes-Magalhaes, R., Peláez, I., and Barjola, P. (2022). Altered subprocesses of working memory in patients with fibromyalgia: An event-related potential study using N -back task. Pain Med. 23, 475–487. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnab190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meulders, A., Jans, A., and Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2015). Differences in pain-related fear acquisition and generalization. Pain 156, 108–122. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyazaki, M., Shibasaki, H., Kanda, M., Xu, X., Shindo, K., Honda, M., et al. (1994). Generator mechanism of pain-related evoked potentials following CO2 laser stimulation of the hand: Scalp topography and effect of predictive warning signal. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 11, 242–254. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199403000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohs, R., Mease, P., Arnold, L. M., Wang, F., Ahl, J., Gaynor, P. J., et al. (2012). The effect of duloxetine treatment on cognition in patients with fibromyalgia. Psychosom. Med. 74, 628–634. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825b9855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, G. L., and Arntz, A. (2007). The context of a noxious stimulus affects the pain it evokes. Pain 133, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murga, I., Guillen, V., and Lafuente, J.-V. (2017). Cambios en la resonancia magnética cerebral asociados al síndrome de fibromialgia. Med. Clin (Barc). 148, 511–516. doi: 10.1016/j.medcli.2017.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohara, S., Crone, N. E., Weiss, N., Treede, R. D., and Lenz, F. A. (2004). Amplitudes of laser evoked potential recorded from primary somatosensory, parasylvian and medial frontal cortex are graded with stimulus intensity. Pain 110, 318–328. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Benedetti, F., Costa, T., and Amanzio, M. (2015). Pain anticipation: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1648–1661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peláez, I., Ferrera, D., Barjola, P., Fernandes, R., and Mercado, F. (2019). Subliminal emotional pictures are capable of modulating early cerebral responses to pain in fibromyalgia. PLoS One 14:e0217909. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peláez, I., Martínez-Iñigo, D., Barjola, P., Cardoso, S., and Mercado, F. (2016). Decreased pain perception by unconscious emotional pictures. Front. Psychol. 7:1636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, M. L., Vlaeyen, J. W. S., and van Drunen, C. (2000). Do fibromyalgia patients display hypervigilance for innocuous somatosensory stimuli? Application of a body scanning reaction time paradigm. Pain 86, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00259-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pidal-Miranda, M., González-Villar, A. J., and Carrillo-de-la-Peña, M. T. (2019). Pain expressions and inhibitory control in patients with fibromyalgia: Behavioral and neural correlates. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:323. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaghki, L., and Mouraux, A. (2005). EEG and laser stimulation as tools for pain research. Curr. Opin. Investig. Drugs 6, 58–64.

Google Scholar

Ploghaus, A., Becerra, L., Borras, C., and Borsook, D. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying pain modulation: Expectation, hypnosis, placebo. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 197–200. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00061-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainville, P. (2002). Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 195–204. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00313-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes del Paso, G. A., Pulgar, Á, Duschek, S., and Garrido, S. (2012). Cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia syndrome: The impact of cardiovascular regulation, pain, emotional disorders and medication. Eur. J. Pain 16, 421–429. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2011.00032.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, J. Y., Hurley, R. A., and Taber, K. H. (2019). Fibromyalgia: Centralized Pain Processing and Neuroimaging. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 31, A6–A187. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19050107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzo, P. A., Pierelli, F., Pozzessere, G., Fattapposta, F., Sanarelli, L., and Morocutti, C. (1985). Pain, anxiety, and contingent negative variation: A clinical and pharmacological study. Biol. Psychiatry 20, 1297–1302. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(85)90114-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohrbaugh, J. W., and Gaillard, A. W. K. (1983). “Sensory and motor aspects of the contingent negative variation,” in Advances in psychology, eds A. W. K. Gaillard and W. Ritter (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 269–310. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62044-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosahl, S. K., and Knight, R. T. (1995). Role of prefrontal cortex in generation of the contingent negative variation. Cereb. Cortex 5, 123–134. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.2.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandström, A., Ellerbrock, I., Tour, J., Kadetoff, D., Jensen, K. B., and Kosek, E. (2020). Neural correlates of conditioned pain responses in fibromyalgia subjects indicate preferential formation of new pain associations rather than extinction of irrelevant ones. Pain 161, 2079–2088. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawamoto, N., Honda, M., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Kanda, M., Fukuyama, H., et al. (2000). Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 20, 7438–7445. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.20-19-07438.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, C., Palomba, D., and Flor, H. (2004). Pavlovian conditioning of muscular responses in chronic pain patients: Central and peripheral correlates. Pain 112, 239–247. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoth, D. E., Nunes, V. D., and Liossi, C. (2012). Attentional bias towards pain-related information in chronic pain; a meta-analysis of visual-probe investigations. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweinhardt, P., Kalk, N., Wartolowska, K., Chessell, I., Wordsworth, P., and Tracey, I. (2008). Investigation into the neural correlates of emotional augmentation of clinical pain. Neuroimage 40, 759–766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidel, E.-M., Pfabigan, D. M., Hahn, A., Sladky, R., Grahl, A., Paul, K., et al. (2015). Uncertainty during pain anticipation: The adaptive value of preparatory processes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 744–755. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, Y.-W., Tsai, H.-Y., Lin, F.-S., Lin, Y.-H., Chiang, C.-Y., Lu, Z.-L., et al. (2019). Effects of positive and negative expectations on human pain perception engage separate but interrelated and dependently regulated cerebral mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 39, 1261–1274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2154-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sitges, C., González-Roldán, A. M., Duschek, S., and Montoya, P. (2018). Emotional influences on cognitive processing in fibromyalgia patients with different depression levels: An event-related potential study. Clin. J. Pain 34, 1106–1113. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., and Lushene, R. (1982). Manual del Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado/Rasgo (STAI). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Google Scholar

Stude, P., Wischniewski, C., Thümler, P., Lehmenkühler, A., Richter, F., Wiemann, M., et al. (2003). Scalp-recorded contingent negative variation (CNV) increases during experimentally induced sustained ischemic pain in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 348, 9–12. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00642-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, J., van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Sharpe, L., and Crombez, G. (2018). Attentional bias to pain-related information: A meta-analysis of dot-probe studies. Health Psychol. Rev. 12, 419–436. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1521729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treede, R.-D., Lorenz, J., and Baumgärtner, U. (2003). Clinical usefulness of laser-evoked potentials. Neurophysiol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 33, 303–314. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Damme, S., Crombez, G., and Eccleston, C. (2002). Retarded disengagement from pain cues: The effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy. Pain 100, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00290-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Damme, S., Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., and Goubert, L. (2004). Impaired disengagement from threatening cues of impending pain in a crossmodal cueing paradigm. Eur. J. Pain 8, 227–236. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Damme, S., Legrain, V., Vogt, J., and Crombez, G. (2010). Keeping pain in mind: A motivational account of attention to pain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Damme, S., Van Hulle, L., Spence, C., Devulder, J., Brusselmans, G., and Crombez, G. (2015). Hypervigilance for innocuous tactile stimuli in patients with fibromyalgia: An experimental approach. Eur. J. Pain 19, 706–714. doi: 10.1002/ejp.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Noel, M., Sharpe, L., Pincus, T., and Van Damme, S. (2019). Cognitive biases in pain: An integrated functional–contextual framework. Pain 160, 1489–1493. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ryckeghem, D., and Crombez, G. (2015). General hypervigilance in fibromyalgia: One swallow does not make a summer. Eur. J. Pain 19, 447–448. doi: 10.1002/ejp.658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, E., Quitadamo, S. G., Ricci, K., Libro, G., Delussi, M., Lombardi, R., et al. (2022). Laser evoked potentials in fibromyalgia with peripheral small fiber involvement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 135, 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2022.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Morley, S., and Crombez, G. (2016). The experimental analysis of the interruptive, interfering, and identity-distorting effects of chronic pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 86, 23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, T., Miltner, W. H. R., Dillmann, J., Miltner, W. H. R., Weiss, T., Miltner, W. H. R., et al. (2003). The influence of semantic priming on event-related potentials to painful laser-heat stimuli in migraine patients. Neurosci. Lett. 340, 135–138. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00103-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiech, K. (2016). Deconstructing the sensation of pain: The influence of cognitive processes on pain perception. Science 354, 584–587. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf8934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiech, K., Ploner, M., and Tracey, I. (2008). Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, F., Clauw, D. J., Fitzcharles, M.-A., Goldenberg, D. L., Häuser, W., Katz, R. L., et al. (2016). 2016 Revisions to the 2010/2011 fibromyalgia diagnostic criteria. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 46, 319–329. doi: 10.1016/j.semarthrit.2016.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, F., Smythe, H. A., Yunus, M. B., Bennett, R. M., Bombardier, C., Goldenberg, D. L., et al. (1990). The american college of rheumatology 1990 criteria for the classification of fibromyalgia. Report of the Multicenter Criteria Committee. Arthritis Rheum. 33, 160–172. doi: 10.1002/art.1780330203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiend, J., and Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 54, 665–681. doi: 10.1080/713755991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Lu, X., Bi, Y., and Hu, L. (2019). Pavlov’s Pain: The effect of classical conditioning on pain perception and its clinical implications. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 23:19. doi: 10.1007/s11916-019-0766-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain expectations, CNV, LEPs, fibromyalgia, pain processing, P2

Citation: Barjola P, Peláez I, Ferrera D, González-Gutiérrez JL, Velasco L, Peñacoba-Puente C, López-López A, Fernandes-Magalhaes R and Mercado F (2022) Electrophysiological indices of pain expectation abnormalities in fibromyalgia patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:943976. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.943976

Received: 14 May 2022; Accepted: 12 September 2022;
Published: 30 September 2022.

Edited by:

Marian van der Meulen, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg

Reviewed by:

Marianna Delussi, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy
Panpan Zheng, Southwest University, China
Daniela Altavilla, Roma Tre University, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Barjola, Peláez, Ferrera, González-Gutiérrez, Velasco, Peñacoba-Puente, López-López, Fernandes-Magalhaes and Mercado. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francisco Mercado, francisco.mercado@urjc.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.