Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 September 2022
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Development and the Attention Spectrum: Volume II View all 9 articles

Neuroprotection in late life attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review of pharmacotherapy and phenotype across the lifespan

  • 1Biostatistics Group, Computer, Electrical and Mathematical Sciences and Engineering Division, King Abdullah University of Science and Technology, Thuwal, Saudi Arabia
  • 2School of Modeling, Simulation, and Training, and Computer Science, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States
  • 3Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States

For decades, psychostimulants have been the gold standard pharmaceutical treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In the United States, an astounding 9% of all boys and 4% of girls will be prescribed stimulant drugs at some point during their childhood. Recent meta-analyses have revealed that individuals with ADHD have reduced brain volume loss later in life (>60 y.o.) compared to the normal aging brain, which suggests that either ADHD or its treatment may be neuroprotective. Crucially, these neuroprotective effects were significant in brain regions (e.g., hippocampus, amygdala) where severe volume loss is linked to cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Historically, the ADHD diagnosis and its pharmacotherapy came about nearly simultaneously, making it difficult to evaluate their effects in isolation. Certain evidence suggests that psychostimulants may normalize structural brain changes typically observed in the ADHD brain. If ADHD itself is neuroprotective, perhaps exercising the brain, then psychostimulants may not be recommended across the lifespan. Alternatively, if stimulant drugs are neuroprotective, then this class of medications may warrant further investigation for their therapeutic effects. Here, we take a bottom-up holistic approach to review the psychopharmacology of ADHD in the context of recent models of attention. We suggest that future studies are greatly needed to better appreciate the interactions amongst an ADHD diagnosis, stimulant treatment across the lifespan, and structure-function alterations in the aging brain.

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is one of the most common psychiatric disorders in children (Biederman and Faraone, 2005; Simonoff et al., 2008; Banaschewski et al., 2017; Sayal et al., 2018). The psychiatric nosology of ADHD is primarily based on its clinical phenomenology and diagnosed with the use of standardized diagnostic rating scales (Thome et al., 2012). Classically, ADHD has been described as a highly heritable (60–75%) neurodevelopmental disorder, characterized along the domains of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (Wolraich et al., 1996; Swanson et al., 1998) with increased novelty seeking behavior recently noted to be a core feature of ADHD (Donfrancesco et al., 2015).

One of the defining characteristics of ADHD is the heterogeneity of behavioral presentations, yet the diagnosis replicates with reasonable success rates across practitioners (McGough, 2014; Faraone et al., 2015). Early descriptions of this disorder suggested that hyperkinetic and inattentive symptoms during childhood generally attenuated by late adolescence (Laufer and Denhoff, 1957), a description maintained through the DSM-IV. However, it is now recognized that 60–70% of pediatric cases continue to have functional impairments into adulthood (Fischer et al., 2002; Sibley et al., 2016), though in many cases only three to four symptoms (sub-threshold for diagnostic criteria) may continue after adolescence (Faraone et al., 2006).

It is evident that variations in the epidemiology and pharmacoepidemiology of ADHD are found across the globe. A meta-analysis of worldwide prevalence in 27 countries in children and adolescents with ADHD was reported at 3.4% (Confidence Interval 95% 2.6–4.5) (Polanczyk et al., 2015). For example, it was estimated that the prevalence rate of childhood ADHD in 2020 would be 1% (95% Confidence Interval: 0.875–1.125) in Slovenia (Štuhec et al., 2015), and less than 50% of patients in Slovenia were treated with medication for pediatric ADHD (Stuhec and Locatelli, 2017). In contrast, a systematic and meta-analysis report in China found a childhood ADHD prevalence rate of 6.26% (95% Confidence Interval: 5.36–7.22%) (Wang et al., 2017) and utilization of prescriptions for ADHD medication was more than 50% in patients aged 6–11 years (Wang et al., 2021). Variability of prevalence rates could be explained by methodological approaches, specifically in diagnostic criteria, source of information, and requirement of impairment for diagnosis (Polanczyk et al., 2014; Visser et al., 2014). Pharmacoepidemiology studies have revealed geographic variability both globally (Raman et al., 2018) and cross-nationally (Zito, 2000) with respect to diagnostic practices, clinical management, and societal differences toward pharmacotherapy in children. Differences in clinical decision-making and medication utilization are also time-varying, further complicating lifespan studies on ADHD populations (Chang et al., 2019).

A number of studies have examined the trajectory and stability of ADHD symptoms across the lifespan. In children, approximately 5% (predominantly male) prevalence rates are generally reported (e.g., Wolraich et al., 1996). In contrast, adult prevalence rates are more gender balanced and estimated at ∼3% (Fayyad et al., 2007). However, adult prevalence rates are higher if the DSM-5 requirement for a childhood-onset (before age 12) is removed (Caye et al., 2016). A recent meta-analysis reported 2.58% prevalence for adult ADHD with a childhood onset, with 6.76% of adults overall having ADHD symptoms (Song et al., 2021). A longitudinal study across four decades found that 90% of adult ADHD cases lacked a childhood history (Moffitt et al., 2015).

Taken together, these reports suggest that there may be at least two different disease trajectories, and the potential for the existence of multiple etiologies (Moffitt et al., 2015; Caye et al., 2016). There is some evidence that the neural substrates subtending the inattentive phenotype are distinct from other ADHD presentations. For example, when independent component analysis, a technique used to isolate statistically independent brain networks, was applied to resting state fMRI data, approximately double the number of components were computed in inattentive subjects compared to either typically developing or hyperactive ADHD subjects (Colby et al., 2012).

Despite copious research, a clear link between the clinical features of ADHD and its biological substrates remains at least somewhat elusive. However, neuropsychological, imaging and genetic studies have converged on a few central features with correlates across domains. Contemporary neurocognitive models of ADHD consider deficits in executive functioning, specifically response disinhibition, to be a core deficit (Curatolo et al., 2010; Purper-Ouakil et al., 2011). Neuropsychological and imaging studies have implicated abnormalities in prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region thought to be critical for many aspects of executive function such as sustaining and dividing attention as well as inhibiting distraction (Arnsten and Li, 2005; Seidman, 2006; Christakou et al., 2013). PFC lesions often produce a behavioral profile of distractibility, forgetfulness, impulsivity, poor planning, and locomotor hyperactivity similar to ADHD (Brennan and Arnsten, 2010).

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is mitigated by pharmacological medications that increase concentrations or residence time of dopamine and norepinephrine in the synapse. According to evidence-based guidelines and meta-analyses of pharmacological management of ADHD, stimulants are considered the first-line treatment in both children (Faraone and Buitelaar, 2010; Chan et al., 2016; Catalá-López et al., 2017; Wolraich et al., 2019) and adults (Rostain, 2008; Cunill et al., 2016; De Crescenzo et al., 2017; Stuhec et al., 2019), while non-stimulants remain second-line option treatments (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014; Chan et al., 2016; Caye et al., 2019).

Here, we suggest that more studies should focus on examining pharmacotherapy-based treatment for ADHD across the lifespan. However, isolating the effects of psychostimulant treatment is difficult for a number of reasons. First, the high rate of psychiatric comorbidity in adults (Kessler, 2006; Sobanski et al., 2007) and children (Kadesjö and Gillberg, 2001; Reale et al., 2017) with ADHD frequently necessitates combination of psychopharmacology, which may pose a risk of drug–drug interactions, and further complicate lifespan studies of pharmacotherapy. This is important in light of the differences in profiles of dopaminergic capacity and dopamine transporter availability in ADHD and comorbidities (Howes et al., 2012; Jauhar et al., 2017). Second, psychostimulants are known to carry the risk of abuse (Barkley et al., 2003; Weaver et al., 2005), which is pertinent given that youths with ADHD are more likely to experiment with psychoactive substances (Elkins et al., 2007; Wilens et al., 2011), and its increasing usage (Zuvekas and Vitiello, 2012; Olfson et al., 2013) may further complicate studies on psychostimulant treatment alone. Growing evidence also suggests an overlap in genetic susceptibility between substance abuse and ADHD (Goldman et al., 2005; Lesch et al., 2008; Wang, 2013; Demontis et al., 2019). Current guidelines recommend the non-stimulant atomoxetine as a first-choice treatment for adults with substance use disorder (SUD) and ADHD, but evidence of efficacy is still lacking (Kooij et al., 2010). Although evidence on the use of stimulants for patients with SUD and ADHD is mixed, Consensus Statements recommend the use of stimulants in adults (Klassen et al., 2012; Torgersen et al., 2013) and adolescents (Özgen et al., 2020). Therefore, there is a need to clarify if substance abuse patients with ADHD will abuse prescription drugs and if treatments will maintain their effectiveness in the presence of SUD (Mariani and Levin, 2007; Molina et al., 2013; Kooij et al., 2019; Wilens et al., 2003; MTA Cooperative Group, 2004). Third, there is still a need for guidelines on evidence-based hierarchies on the efficacy and tolerability of all pharmacological treatments for ADHD in children and adults. Evidence from a systematic and network meta-analysis study supports methylphenidate in children and adolescents, and amphetamines in adults, as the preferred first-choice medications for short-term treatment, but new research should assess long-term effects of these drugs (Cortese et al., 2018). On the other hand, methylphenidate was recommended as the first-line treatment for ADHD in adults in the UK (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014), and statistically significant response of lisdexamfetamine was found in children and adolescents with ADHD (Roskell et al., 2014) but safety data proved inconclusive due to low event rates. Fourth, genes (Bruxel et al., 2014, 2015; Myer et al., 2018) and gene-environment (Pagerols et al., 2018) factors influence the efficacy of pharmaceutical drugs for ADHD (Elsayed et al., 2020). Fifth, only stimulants and atomoxetine were found to reach Ia evidence levels (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014), thus requiring further evidence-based medicine research on other common and novel pharmacological treatments for individuals with ADHD of all ages.

Lastly, and most importantly, a delay in brain degeneration was observed in adults with ADHD regardless of medication status. Hoogman et al. (2017) found that in certain brain regions, age-related volumetric decline (age > 60) was less pronounced in the ADHD group compared to age-matched controls, notably in amygdala and hippocampal regions (Figure 1). Given that volumetric loss in these brain regions is a hallmark of cognitive impairment later in life, there is a clear need for studies that examine the relationships between neurodegeneration, ADHD, psychostimulant use, and their neuroimaging-genetic correlates.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Moving averages, corrected for age, sex, intracranial volume, and site for subcortical volumes. Error bars denote the standard error. As individuals with ADHD approach late adulthood (>60), volume remains relatively higher than controls in certain subcortical structures, such as the hippocampus, accumbens, putamen, and amygdala. Reproduced with permission (Hoogman et al., 2017).

Given these concerns, it is critically important to review how psychostimulant treatment for ADHD affects the structural integrity of the brain throughout the lifespan and how these treatments should best be administered to patients with ADHD, given that medication may resolve differences in the ADHD brain, and thus counteract the potential for ADHD itself to provide neuroprotection in late life. Bridging multiple levels of description from a molecular to a system levels of abstraction may improve our understanding of how these substances enhance or attenuate behavioral symptoms across ADHD presentations. It may also lead to quantitative metrics that aid in individualized treatment regimens during early and late stage titration periods across the lifespan.

In this paper, we first review the pharmacology of drugs commonly prescribed to children and adults with ADHD. We then discuss structural and functional neuroimaging differences in the ADHD brain. Interestingly, there is mixed evidence as to whether or not pharmaceutical treatment normalizes these structural differences. Here, we review this evidence in the context of recent findings which note a delay in late life neurodegeneration in the ADHD population, particularly in brain regions noted to decline in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). An important future line of inquiry may address the overlap or lack thereof between ADHD, neurodegenerative diseases, and their respective genetics. Finally, we suggest that improved models of ADHD and pharmaceutical treatment across the lifespan may help update clinical decision-making in light of recent evidence. Our method for identifying relevant literature for inclusion in this review is described in Supplementary Methods.

Principal drug targets: Catecholaminergic systems

One of the defining characteristics of ADHD is its heterogeneity of symptom presentation, and multiple genetic variations have been linked to increased risk for the ADHD (Faraone and Mick, 2010). A recent review found that about a third of ADHD’s heritability is due to polygenetic components, each contributing small effects to the overall presentation (Faraone and Larsson, 2019). In this sense, multiple genetic variants and combinations thereof may subtend a given cluster of behavioral attributes grouped under a given ADHD behavioral phenotype, thus creating an equifinality effect. Conversely, identical twin studies have demonstrated that a single genotype can lead to a diversity of presentations and a spectrum of severities. Thus, both equifinality and multifinality effects are evident with ADHD. A variety of neurocognitive endophenotypes that mediate pathways between the genotype and phenotype have been identified and have helped unravel the complexity of this disease across the lifespan (Boxhoorn et al., 2019).

Several genetic markers of ADHD identified to date involve alterations in dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline signaling (NA) (Pliszka et al., 1996). There are certain similarities between dopaminergic and noradrenergic signaling and disruptions thereof in the brain. For example, both DA and NA are primarily synthesized in localized nuclei: the substantia nigra and locus coeruleus, respectively (Beaulieu and Gainetdinov, 2011; Bhatia and Saadabadi, 2020). However, both the dopaminergic (Friston et al., 1994) and noradrenergic (Smythies, 2005) systems are thought to project diffusely across the brain (Stahl, 2003; Gerfen and Surmeier, 2011; Purves, 2012), thus having the potential to “broadcast” their responses to exert widespread effects in the brain. Both systems also exert varied effects across different time scales depending on whether response patterns are (sustained) or phasic (transient) and there is evidence that both exert varied effects depending on whether they modulate top-down or bottom-up processing (Manev and Uz, 2009; Badgaiyan et al., 2015). Noradrenaline is thought to modulate multiple functions in the brain from learning rates (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Sales et al., 2019) to attention (Sved et al., 2001). Tonic firing of the locus coeruleus is correlated with arousal levels and behavioral flexibility (Mefford and Potter, 1989). In contrast, salient stimuli are thought to evoke more brief, high frequency phasic responses (Sales et al., 2019).

Therefore, there are relatively few candidate pathways that are thought to be involved in the ADHD phenotype, in contrast to many other psychiatric disorders (Hauser et al., 2016). Here, we summarize the major classes of pharmaceuticals aimed at treating ADHD and their putative involvement with DA and NA signaling.

Stimulants

Stimulants are considered the gold standard pharmaceutical treatment and generally recommended as a first line of treatment for children and adults with severe or moderate ADHD (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2005; Pliszka and AACAP Work Group on Quality Issues, 2007; Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, Wolraich et al., 2011; Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). An estimated 70% of patients respond favorably to the initial stimulant selection (Elia et al., 1991; Schachter et al., 2001; Barbaresi et al., 2006). Methylphenidate and amphetamine treatment for ADHD reach level Ia in terms of effectiveness (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014) and as revealed by meta-analyses comparing stimulant drug and placebo interventions in both children (Faraone and Buitelaar, 2010; Cortese et al., 2018) and adults (De Crescenzo et al., 2017; Cortese et al., 2018) with ADHD. However, all stimulant formulations of methylphenidate (MPH) and amphetamine (AMPH) are classified as Schedule II drugs, denoting high risk for misuse, diversion, and potential neurotoxicity (Wilens et al., 2003, 2008; Casey et al., 2007; Clemow and Walker, 2014; McGough, 2014), as well as tolerance or sensitization if used at high doses. In the case of risk of misuse of psychostimulants or comorbidity to SUD, atomoxetine is recommended as a first-choice treatment, however, evidence of efficacy is limited (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Here, we briefly summarize the neuropharmacology and cellular pathway alterations induced via these key stimulant drugs while also noting its long-term effects.

Amphetamine

Amphetamine (AMPH; IUPAC name: 1-phenylpropane-2-amine), the active component of drugs like Adderall, Dexedrine, and Vyvanse, acts as an indirect agonist to catecholamines. The term amphetamine typically refers to the racemic mixture, or equal parts mixture, of dextro and levo amphetamine, though the drugs themselves typically have varying mixtures of these enantiomers, as well as derivations thereof. The cellular signaling pathways altered by AMPH are complex. Repeated exposure to psychostimulant drugs can lead to both acute and enduring effects on neurobiological substrates in the brain (Howell and Kimmel, 2008). The primary short-term effects of these drugs tend to increase the concentration and/or residence time of monoamines [e.g., dopamine (DA), norepinephrine (NE)] in the synapse by promoting efflux into the synaptic cleft, and inhibiting or competing with monoamines for reuptake.

Amphetamine is lipophilic, and can therefore enter neurons via direct diffusion across the cellular membrane (Gulaboski et al., 2007). However, the chemical structure of AMPH is similar to DA (Figure 2); it is therefore unsurprising that AMPH can also enter via these monoamine transporters (Mortensen and Amara, 2003; Sulzer et al., 2005; Howell and Kimmel, 2008) and compete with these endogenous ligands for their transport machinery (Mortensen and Amara, 2003; Zhu and Reith, 2008) thereby reducing monoamine clearance from the synapse. For example, the dopamine transporter (DAT), a monoamine transporter, is an integral transmembrane protein that typically functions to import or clear DA from the synapse, and represents an important target site for both MPH and AMPH (Spencer et al., 2005; Larisch et al., 2006).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mechanisms of action for stimulant drugs. (Box, Upper Left). Chemical structures for the (top) Amphetamine and (bottom) Methylphenidate. Stimulant mechanisms of action. Methylphenidate (MPH) acts as an antagonist drug by binding and inhibiting dopamine transporters (DATs) and norepinephrine transporters (NETs), thereby reducing reuptake of (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) from the synaptic cleft. Amphetamine (AMPH) can enter neurons either via direct diffusion across the plasma membrane or via competition with the endogenous transport machinery for DA (i.e., the dopamine transporter (DAT)). Once inside the cell, AMPH binds at distinct sites on the vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2), causing vesicular gradient collapse, which in turn induces DA release from the vesicles into the cytosol, increasing intracellular concentrations of DA, and reversing the concentration gradient across the cellular membrane, promoting efflux of DA into the synapse. AMPH is also strong trace amine associated receptor 1 (TAAR1) agonist. Activation of TAAR1, through protein kinase A (PKA), protein kinase C (PKC), and adenylyl cyclase signaling, causes phosphorylation of DAT, which has long N and C intracellular termini. Phosphorylation of the N-terminus can promote DA reverse efflux into the synapse. Phosphorlylation of DAT can also inhibit its function, thus diminishing the plasma membrane transporter from clearing DA from the synapse. The number of DATs localized to the plasma membrane is also dynamically regulated, and a reduction of DATs following AMPH treatment is thought to subtend many of the long-term effects of stimulant treatment.

Amphetamine can also increase efflux of monoamines into the synapse via multiple mechanisms. First AMPH can inhibit of a variety of vesicular monoamine transporters (VMATs), including VMAT1 and VMAT2, as well as interacting with excitatory amino acid transporters (e.g., SLC1A1), which are also biological substrate targets for non-stimulant drugs (e.g., atomoxetine) (Hammerness et al., 2009). In particular, VMAT2 is a transporter protein, which serves to import monoamines from the cytosol into vesicles for storage and later release into the synapse. AMPH can bind to VMAT2 and inhibit its normal vesicular import function, leading to a reduction of monoamines released during exocytosis. However, when AMPH enters synaptic vesicles expressing VMAT2, the vesicular pH gradient collapses, which in turn causes stored amines to be released into the cytosol, thus increasing their intracellular concentration (Sulzer and Rayport, 1990; Sulzer et al., 2005). The increased cytosolic concentration of monoamines, in turn, induces “reverse transport” through primary plasma membrane monoamine transporters (Sulzer et al., 1995; John and Jones, 2007) such as DAT. DAT is an Na+/Cl– symporter (Khelashvili et al., 2015), driven primarily via Na+ ion and amine concentration gradients (Wheeler et al., 1993; Bröer and Gether, 2012). Therefore, reverse efflux is more likely during neuronal depolarization.

Amphetamine is also a powerful agonist for trace amine-associated receptor 1 (TAAR1), a G-protein coupled receptor, which acts in conjunction with plasma membrane monoamine transporters to regulate the extracellular concentrations of these monoamine neurotransmitters (Kobayashi et al., 2010). By activating TAAR1, protein kinase A (PKA) and protein kinase C (PKC) are activated leading to increased cyclic AMP, which in turn inhibits the synaptic influx function of integral membrane monoamine transporters, again resulting in diminished clearance from the synapse (Kebabian, 1978; Johnson et al., 2005a; Miller, 2011). The short term intracellular mechanisms of action of AMPH: increasing monoamine release into the synapse, and diminish reuptake rates, are summarized in Figure 2.

Differential functional brain activities are present in dexamphetamine (S-amphetamine, D-amphetamine) and levoamphetamine (R-amphetamine, lisdexamfetamine). Presently, the modern formulation of amphetamine for ADHD treatment is a combination of racemic amphetamine and d-amphetamine (Knackstedt, 2013). Lisdexamfetamine is metabolized by red blood cells to yield its active metabolite, D-amphetamine, and L-lysine (Pennick, 2010). D-amphetamine was shown to be 3–4 times more potent than l-amphetamine in the striatum, but not in the cortex, thus, D-amphetamine has a greater affinity for striatal DAT than l-amphetamine (Knackstedt, 2013). S-Amphetamine is more potent in CNS stimulation, while R-amphetamine is slightly more potent in its cardiovascular action (Varesio and Veuthey, 1995). Similar to dexamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine is posed to be an effective treatment of ADHD (evidence level Ia) in randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled trials in children (Biederman et al., 2007) and adults (Adler et al., 2008) with ADHD and in open-label long-term investigations in adults (Weisler et al., 2009). Side effect profiles are similar in both D-amphetamine and lisdexamfetamine, but recreational abuse may be substantially lower in lisdexamfetamine than that of immediate-release D-amphetamine (Jasinski and Krishnan, 2009).

Methylphenidate

Methylphenidate (MPH; IUPAC name: methyl 2-phenyl-2-piperidin-2-ylacetate), active in drugs such as Ritalin, Methylin, Concerta, and Focalin, blocks dopamine (DAT) and norepinephrine (NET) transporter (Kuczenski and Segal, 1997; Solanto, 1998; Heal and Pierce, 2006; Iversen, 2009), which leads to increased dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) concentrations and/or residence time within the synaptic cleft following impulse release (Volkow et al., 2001, 2005). MPH also shares part of its basic structure with catecholamines, which is putatively related to its antagonistic role as a DA/NE reuptake inhibitor (Figure 2). Systematic reviews and meta-analyses reveal differences in the efficacy profiles of amphetamine (AMPH) and methylphenidate (MPH) in adults and children with ADHD. In two meta-analyses, both AMPH and MPH showed comparable efficacy in adults (Faraone and Glatt, 2009) and children (Catalá-López et al., 2017) with ADHD. Other meta-analyses revealed amphetamine as having greater efficacy than methylphenidate and non-stimulants in children (Faraone and Buitelaar, 2010; Stuhec et al., 2015; Joseph et al., 2017) and adults (De Crescenzo et al., 2017; Stuhec et al., 2019) with ADHD. A reduction of adult ADHD symptoms was observed in lisdexamfetamine, whereas mixed amphetamine salts and methylphenidate reduced symptoms moderately compared to placebo (Stuhec et al., 2019). In children and adolescents with ADHD, efficacy in reduction of ADHD symptoms compared to placebo is small for bupropion, modest for atomoxetine and methylphenidate, and high efficacy for lisdexamfetamine (Stuhec et al., 2015). Similarly, lisdexamfetamine dimesylate had the highest efficacy than guanfacine extended release, atomoxetine, and methylphenidate extended release among children and adolescents with ADHD (Joseph et al., 2017). Tolerability of both treatments are relatively comparable (Faraone, 2018). It must be noted that current observations of methylphenidate treatment on dopaminergic signaling are based on the effects seen in normal versus pathological conditions. In healthy animals, there are long term changes in reduction of striatal dopamine transporters densities (Moll et al., 2001) and variability in firing rate among dopaminergic midbrain neurons (Brandon et al., 2003; Viggiano et al., 2003, 2004); although the neurotrophic effects may differ (Gill et al., 2012) based on age (Fukui et al., 2003) and quantity of dose (Volkow et al., 1998). In rat midbrain slices, methylphenidate inhibits dopamine transporters and this depression of firing is mediated by synthesized dopamine that increases intracellularly due to reuptake inhibition (Morón et al., 2002; Federici et al., 2005).

Converging evidence from in vitro to rodent to human studies suggest that the d enantiomer is thought to mediate therapeutic effects for the behavioral symptoms of ADHD (e.g., Patrick et al., 1987; Teo et al., 2003; Ding et al., 2004; Markowitz and Patrick, 2008; Markowitz et al., 2009), though clinical evidence suggests that the l-enantiomer is very effective at treating behavioral symptoms in children (Hubbard et al., 1989). Many studies suggest that the chirality is important and that the l enantiomer is non-specific, and may actually interfere with the specific targeting of the d version of this molecule.

Long term effects of stimulant treatment

The extent to which long-term exposure to stimulants induces persistent neuroplasticity has not been fully elucidated, partly due to the fact that an X-ray structure for DAT was only reported somewhat recently in Drosophila (Penmatsa et al., 2013). Phosphorylation of monoamine transporters induced via a range of kinases (e.g., protein kinase C) appears to alter transporter functionality (Johnson et al., 2005a). The DAT has long N and C intracellular termini, which provide multiple sites for phosphorylation and regulation (Foster et al., 2003; Giambalvo, 2003). For example, phosphorylation of the N-terminus of DAT appears to be tightly linked to DA efflux (e.g., Khoshbouei et al., 2004), and may be requisite for AMPH induced DA efflux (e.g., Kantor and Gnegy, 1998; Mortensen and Amara, 2003).

Stimulant treatment can also alter the quantity and plasmalemal expression of monoamine transporters (Krause et al., 2000; Kahlig and Galli, 2003; Johnson et al., 2005a; McGinty et al., 2008; Zhu and Reith, 2008). In vitro studies in rat striatal synaptosomes have demonstrated increased trafficking of DAT to the plasma membrane following rapid treatment (∼1 min) with AMPH (Furman et al., 2009). In contrast, numerous in vitro studies have demonstrated that longer exposure (>∼30 min) results in DAT internalization (e.g., Chi and Reith, 2003; Johnson et al., 2005b). In untreated adult humans with ADHD, SPECT imaging studies have shown an increase in DAT specific ligand binding (TRODAT-1) in striatum compared to controls, and a significant reduction in binding after 4 weeks of MPH treatment (Krause et al., 2000), consistent with longer exposure in vitro studies.

Overall, long-term effects of stimulant treatment may result from a number of factors including phosphorylation of monoamine transporters (Khelashvili et al., 2015), alterations in the dynamic expression and quantity of transporters at the plasma membrane (Zhu and Reith, 2008), and the induction of downstream gene expression (McGinty et al., 2008; Zhu and Reith, 2008). Alterations in brain structure and function revealed by neuroimaging may provide further insight into prolonged effects of pharmacotherapy for ADHD (see neuroimaging section below).

Non-stimulants

About 10–30% of ADHD patients respond poorly to stimulant medication (Greenhill et al., 2002; Spencer et al., 2004). For these patients, non-stimulant agents can be second-line treatment options, used when stimulants are ineffective, or used in combination with stimulants to enhance treatment response (Zito et al., 2008; Sallee et al., 2009; Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, Wolraich et al., 2011; Kornfield et al., 2013). The two most common FDA-approved non-stimulants for ADHD are Alpha-2-Adrenergic Agonists and Atomoxetine.

Alpha 2-adrenergic agonists

Alpha 2-adrenergic agonists typically bind to α 2 adrenergic receptors, causing vasodilation of the arteries (see Figure 3; Hossmann et al., 1980; Cinnamon Bidwell et al., 2010). Clonidine [brand names Kapvay and Catapres; IUPAC name: N-(2,6-dichlorophenyl)-4,5-dihydro-1H-imidazole-2-amine] is an imidazoline derivative and a non-selective α2 adrenergic agonist. It binds to all subtypes of α2-adrenergic receptors (Roth and Driscol, 2011): α2A, α2B, α2C. Alpha 2A-receptors have been found in the locus coeruleus, frontal cortex, cerebellum, septum, hypothalamus, and hippocampus (Guyenet et al., 1994; Grijalba et al., 1996; Talley et al., 1996; Saunders and Limbird, 1999; Offermans and Rosenthal, 2004). Alpha 2B-receptors are localized in the thalamus, hippocampus, and cerebellar Purkinje layer (Saunders and Limbird, 1999). Alpha 2C-receptors are expressed in several subcortical regions including the thalamus, amygdala, substancia nigra and ventral tegmentum area (Saunders and Limbird, 1999; Offermans and Rosenthal, 2004). Guanfacine [brand names Intuniv, Estulic, and Tenex; IUPAC name: N-(diaminomethylidene)-2-(2,6 dichlorophenyl)acetamide] is a selective agonist drug for α2A adrenergic receptors, with a 60x and 20x higher affinity to these receptors than to α2B- and α2C-receptors, respectively (Uhlén et al., 1995; Kawaura et al., 2014). Clonidine and guanfacine are level Ib in evidence-based medicine practice (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014), signifying level of evidence for individual RCTs (with narrow confidence intervals) (Burns et al., 2011). Combination of stimulants with alpha 2 agonists (clonidine, guanfacine) and monotherapy of alpha 2 agonists have not been extensively studied in adults (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Comorbidity of ADHD and other psychiatric conditions alter evidence levels to III and IV (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Guanfacine remains the preferential α2 adrenergic agonist with about 50–60% of children responding favorably as placed alongside relative efficacy with other non-stimulant drugs and effective in treating youths with symptoms of hyperactive/impulsive-inattentive (evidence level Ia) (Biederman et al., 2008; Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). In youths with ADHD, a medium effect size for efficacy and tolerability were reported, but these have to be weighed against possible adverse events of fatigue, somnolence/sedation, hypotension, bradycardia, and possible QTc prolongation (Hirota et al., 2014). Clarity of dose-response relationship for efficacy and tolerability outcomes are greatly needed using head-to-head controlled studies that compare α2 adrenergic agonists with stimulants and atomoxetine (Hirota et al., 2014).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Clonidine and Guanfacine alpha 2 adrenergic agonist drugs. Clonidine acts on various α2- adrenergic subtypes (α2A, α2B, and α2C), while guanfacine is a selective α2A adrenergic agonist. Presynaptic stimulation of these autoreceptors inhibits NE release. Post-synaptic stimulation of these α2-adrenergic receptors activates a series of steps: G-protein alpha is released and sent to adenylyl cyclase by guanosine triphosphate (GTP). Binding to adenylyl cyclase lowers adenosine triphosphate (ATP), which decreases the intracellular production of the secondary messenger cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP). This reduces phosphorylation and inhibits NE release. Both drugs act as sympatholytic drugs, decreasing sympathetic outflow. Atomoxetine (ATM) is a selective antagonist drug to NE and has a lower affinity to serotonin (SERT) and dopamine (DAT) transporters. ATM blocks NETs, thereby increasing NE in the synaptic cleft.

Atomoxetine

Atomoxetine [ATM; IUPAC name: (3R)-N-methyl-3-(2-methylphenoxy)-3-phenylpropane-1-amine; brand name Strattera] is a noradrenergic antagonist drug that inhibits reuptake of NE by blocking NETs (Witcher et al., 2003; Corman et al., 2004), leading to an increase in NE in the synaptic cleft (Figure 3). In addition, ATM has a low affinity for serotonin and DA transporters that also inhibit reuptake of these monoamines across certain brain regions, such as the PFC (Bymaster et al., 2002; Sauer et al., 2005; Ding et al., 2014). For example, in animal studies, atomoxetine was shown to selectively increase dopamine in the PFC, while not altering areas rich with dopamine innervations such as the striatum (Bymaster et al., 2002; Swanson et al., 2006). Atomoxetine is the main non-stimulant drug that is recommended for treatment of ADHD in adults (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Clinical efficacy of stimulants and atomoxetine are comparable (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014; Shang et al., 2016) in adult ADHD (Durell et al., 2013; Fredriksen et al., 2013) and pediatric ADHD (Cheng et al., 2007), although they cannot be considered similar because they have differences in their mechanisms of action and hazards (Spencer et al., 1998; Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Atomoxetine is preferred if there are any contra-indications to stimulant treatment, such as if methylphenidate is ineffective or not tolerated, presence of anxiety disorder or tics, and risk of misuse or diversion (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). The presence of family history of cardiac problems or significant cardiovascular concerns should be monitored carefully (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014), such as cases with prolonged QT interval (Štuhec and Švab, 2013) and hypertension (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). A systematic and expanded review has shown atomoxetine to be most effective in the treatment of ADHD symptoms in child, adolescent, and adults with comorbidity to anxiety and oppositional defiant disorder, while mixed or limited findings were found for individuals with ADHD and comorbid substance-use disorders, autism spectrum disorders, dyslexia or reading disorder, depression, bipolar disorder, and Tourette syndrome (Hutchison et al., 2016).

Bupropion

The antidepressant and smoking cessation agent Bupropion [brand names: Zyban, Wellbutrin, Elontril; IUPAC name: 2-(tert-butylamino)-1-(3-chlorophenyl)propan-1-one] is an off-label, non-FDA approved use for treating ADHD (Ng, 2017). Efficacy and tolerability for bupropion reaches a level Ia (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014), indicating small benefits for children (Stuhec et al., 2015; Ng, 2017) and adults (Verbeeck et al., 2017). Thus, more randomized controlled trials with larger sample sizes are warranted for a better clinical evaluation of bupropion. Bupropion is often used if methylphenidate, atomoxetine, and amphetamines have poor clinical response or if risk of stimulant abuse is high (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). Contraindications include seizure disorder and suicidal ideation (Huecker et al., 2021). Compared to classical tricyclic antidepressants, bupropion mechanism of action serves as a norepinephrine-dopamine disinhibitor (Khan et al., 2016) and as a non-competitive antagonism of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (Potter et al., 2006; Weiss et al., 2007; Verbeeck et al., 2017). Reuptake inhibition is high for dopamine, while norepinephrine uptake is less potent (Huecker et al., 2021).

Neuroimaging alterations in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Medicated versus medication naïve

Structural and functional neuroimaging techniques permit non-invasive imaging of the anatomy and physiology of the human brain in vivo. In this section, we will review structural and functional brain differences in youths and adults who are ADHD medication naive, ADHD medicated with pharmacotherapy, and typically developing (TD) controls, as measured by (1) PET, (2) structural MRI, and (3) DTI at drug naivety and pharmacotherapy intervention.

Positron emission tomography

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder drug naïve

Radioactive tracers used in PET imaging often compete with endogenous ligands (e.g., dopamine), and measurements therefore can be used to determine the number of available binding sites for specific receptors (Muehllehner and Karp, 2006). Radiotracers commonly used to study ADHD include those that bind with dopaminergic presynaptic terminals (i.e., radiotracers: [99MTc]TRODAT, [11C]PE2I, [11C]cocaine, [11C]-altropane), L-amino acid transporters (i.e., radiotracers: [18F]DOPA), and D2/D3 receptors (i.e., radiotracer [11C]raclopride) (Fusar-Poli et al., 2012). Compared to controls, there is increased radiotracer binding to striatal DATs in adults with ADHD (Dresel et al., 2000; Spencer et al., 2007), suggesting less DA in the synaptic cleft due to high levels of DA reuptake. Similarly, relative to controls, ADHD adult men had greater d-amphetamine-induced decrease in striatal [(11)C]raclopride binding, and the magnitude of this change was associated with poor response inhibition (Cherkasova et al., 2014). In addition, adults with ADHD have low D2/D3 receptor binding potential across the left hemisphere of the caudate, nucleus accumbens, midbrain, and hypothalamus (Volkow et al., 2007, 2009), suggesting low levels of D2/D3 receptors. Interestingly, low densities of D2/D3 receptors in the nucleus accumbens are also associated with greater risk for drug abuse (Dalley et al., 2007). In another PET study by Volkow et al. (2002), there was an attenuation of DAT and D3/D4 receptor availability in the nucleus accumbens and midbrain from adults with ADHD compared to individuals without ADHD, and this reduction was associated with low motivation. Adolescents with ADHD also have decreased DAT binding potential in the midbrain suggesting altered DA signaling, and DA 2 receptor binding in the right caudate correlated with greater motor hyperactivity (Jucaite et al., 2005).

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medicated

Positron emission tomography (PET) studies have generally found that stimulant treatment for ADHD tends to increase the rate of dopamine release into synapses from striatal brain regions. However, few studies have examined children due to concerns about PET radiation. A meta-analysis of 9 PET and 10 SPECT studies revealed that patients with ADHD had 14% greater striatal DAT density than in TD, but this was influenced by previous exposure to ADHD medication with lower DAT density in medication naïve ADHD subjects (Fusar-Poli et al., 2012). Methylphenidate (MPH) blocks DAT sites in the striatum of adults with ADHD compared to controls, leading to increased DA in the extracellular space (Dresel et al., 2000). Ludolph et al. (2008) found lower radiotracer binding for DATs in MPH-treated adults in the striatum, suggesting a down-regulation of DA turnover. In children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD, a reduction of binding potential is seen in striatal D2/D3 receptors after intake of MPH, which may reflect increased extracellular DA (Rosa-Neto et al., 2005; Volkow et al., 2007). The amount of extracellular DA regulated by psychostimulants depends on a combination of the DAT blockade and the rate of DA release, as well as individual differences in cell firing and stimulation (Volkow et al., 2005). For example, one PET study noted a possible physiological explanation for drug tolerance, where an increase in DATs was seen in adults with ADHD who had received less than 12 months of MPH treatment (Wang et al., 2013).

Structural magnetic resonance imaging

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder drug naïve

A range of structural brain alterations have been reported in structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) studies examining children and adults with ADHD at baseline (medication naïve). These abnormalities are often reported in the frontostriatal circuitry (Emond et al., 2009; Cubillo et al., 2012; Norman et al., 2016), but also affected regions include fronto-parieto-temporal, fronto-cerebellar, and fronto-limbic networks (Seidman et al., 2005; Rubia et al., 2014). These multiple systems are implicated in attention, cognitive control, and working memory (Arnsten and Rubia, 2012). Meta-analyses of ROI or whole brain sMRI volumetric studies of ADHD consistently report overall reduction of total or right cerebral volumes (Valera et al., 2007; Nakao et al., 2011), right (Valera et al., 2007) or bilateral caudate (Nakao et al., 2011; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012), right (Ellison-Wright et al., 2008; Norman et al., 2016) or bilateral putamen (Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012), right globus pallidus (Ellison-Wright et al., 2008; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012), right lentiform nucleus (Nakao et al., 2011), posterior and inferior cerebellar vermis (Valera et al., 2007), splenium of the corpus callosum (Valera et al., 2007), anterior cingulate cortex (Seidman et al., 2006; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012), whereas greater volume has been found in the left posterior cingulate cortex (Nakao et al., 2011). Other observed abnormalities in gray matter volume have been found in prefrontal and frontal areas, temporal, occipital, and parietal cortices (Mostofsky et al., 1998; Carmona et al., 2005; McAlonan et al., 2007; Valera et al., 2007; Ahrendts et al., 2011; Nakao et al., 2011; Castellanos and Proal, 2012; Lopez-Larson et al., 2012; Bralten et al., 2016; Norman et al., 2016; Gehricke et al., 2017; Lu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2019), with some studies indicating greater volumes than controls in frontal (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017; Lu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2019) and caudate regions (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017). In the largest cross-sectional study conducted to date on subcortical brain volumes of ADHD, Hoogman et al. (2017) found significantly smaller volumes for the accumbens, caudate, amygdala, hippocampus, and putamen bilaterally. This confirms other sMRI studies on morphology abnormalities of the amygdala (Plessen et al., 2006; Frodl et al., 2010; Douglas et al., 2018), hippocampus (Plessen et al., 2006; Douglas et al., 2018), caudate (Douglas et al., 2018), and thalamus (Douglas et al., 2018). Regardless of these significant findings, a recent meta-analysis using activation likelihood estimation reported no significant convergent structural MRI alterations in children and adolescents with ADHD, highlighting the need to explore homogenous clinical samples and analyses (Samea et al., 2019). In contrast, adults with ADHD are shown to have normal prefrontal, striatal, and parietal gray matter volumes (Ahrendts et al., 2011). Differences in ADHD presentation phenotypes have been found. The right inferior frontal gyrus in young adults (Depue et al., 2010) and cerebellar vermis lobes VIII–X (Bledsoe et al., 2009) in children with ADHD combined phenotype are found to be reduced in volume. Decreased morphology volumes in the right lateral and left posterior thalamic surfaces (associated with hyperactivity) and increased volumes in the right medial thalamic surfaces (associated with inattention) (Ivanov et al., 2010) are shown to differentiate ADHD behavioral profiles.

Similar to volumetric results, there were significant findings in cortical surface area and cortical thickness metrics. Lower surface area values were found in children with ADHD, localized to the frontal, cingulate, and temporal areas (Wolosin et al., 2009; Mous et al., 2014; Silk et al., 2016; Ambrosino et al., 2017; Hoogman et al., 2019), as well as less rightward asymmetry for total hemispheric and medial orbitalfrontal cortex surface area (Postema et al., 2021). However, studies on gyrification or intrinsic curvature of surface area showed inconsistent findings whereby controls and children/young adults with ADHD did not show significant differences (Shaw et al., 2012; Forde et al., 2017). This suggests that cortical abnormalities in development are related to differential brain expansion across subjects (Forde et al., 2017). Additionally, rather than thinning of the cortex, Ambrosino et al. (2017) suggested that cortical volume decreases were driven primarily by surface area reductions. On the other hand, decreases in cortical thickness (CT) were observed throughout the cortex (Narr et al., 2009) and the magnitude of CT decrease appears to correlate with disease severity (Almeida et al., 2010). These CT reductions are found across the cortex, particularly in frontal regions (Sowell et al., 2003; Makris et al., 2007; Almeida et al., 2010; Qiu et al., 2011; Shaw et al., 2011; Almeida Montes et al., 2013; Silk et al., 2016), temporal (Hoogman et al., 2019), parietal (Makris et al., 2007; Almeida Montes et al., 2013; Silk et al., 2016), and occipital (Hoogman et al., 2019) across children and adults with ADHD (Albajara Sáenz et al., 2019). In contrast, increased cortical thickness was also found in a group of children/adolescents with ADHD within the occipital lobe (Almeida Montes et al., 2013). These studies evidence the complexity of structural abnormalities in medication naïve ADHD subjects across all age groups.

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder neurodevelopment during drug naivety

Anatomical MRI studies of ADHD are often cross-sectional, but prospective longitudinal studies have enabled researchers to detect patterns of aberrant developmental trajectories in ADHD groups compared to healthy subjects. It has long been argued that ADHD children have a delay in brain maturation (Shaw et al., 2007, 2012) due to late developing fronto-striatal and fronto-cerebellar systems (Rubia, 2013) that diminish ADHD symptoms in later adulthood. Shaw et al. (2007, 2012) found delays in peak cortical thickness and surface area development by 2–5 years in children with ADHD, with the most prominent delay in the prefrontal region that controls cognitive processes of motor and attention planning. This pattern of persistent reductions in frontal cortices of volume, surface area, and gyrification among ADHD subjects aged 6–28 were replicated in a recent study (Ambrosino et al., 2017). Age-related changes in the frontal eye field (L-FEF) and left ventral frontal cortex (L-VFC) were detected in children with ADHD (Lu et al., 2019). A decrease in surface area of the L-VFC and an increase in volume of the L-FEF persists in children with ADHD (7–16 years) (Lu et al., 2019). Besides frontal cortices, growth is also seen to be stunted in other regions. Age related growth in gray matter of bilateral occipital lobe appears reversed in children with ADHD (Wu et al., 2019). Stable symptoms of ADHD over an average of 4.8 years in a cohort of 362 youths was associated with reduction of thalamic volume (Sudre et al., 2021). Progressive atypical contraction was found in ventral and dorsal striatal regions that persisted into adolescence for the ADHD group compared to surface area expansion with age in the typically developing group (Shaw et al., 2014). On the other hand, not all studies ascertain these findings. Volumetric abnormalities in ADHD have been shown to normalize or decrease in childhood (Wu et al., 2019), adolescence (Castellanos et al., 2002), and adulthood (Perlov et al., 2008; Greven et al., 2015; Hoogman et al., 2017) compared to controls in the hippocampus (Perlov et al., 2008; Hoogman et al., 2017), amygdala (Perlov et al., 2008; Hoogman et al., 2017), accumbens (Hoogman et al., 2017), putamen (Greven et al., 2015; Hoogman et al., 2017), caudate (Castellanos et al., 2002; Greven et al., 2015; Hoogman et al., 2017), and medial frontal regions (Wu et al., 2019).

Nevertheless, persistent brain abnormalities are found in adult ADHD and are elevated due to severity levels, behavioral profiles, environmental factors. Adults with childhood ADHD showed sustained dysfunctions in the lateral fronto-striatal and ventromedial orbitofrontal during attention- and reward-related tasks similar to pediatric ADHD (Cubillo et al., 2012). Rate of cortical thinning in the medial and dorsolateral PFC has been associated with persistent inattentive rather than hyperactive/impulsive symptoms in adult ADHD, whereas cortical thickening or minimal thinning was found among ADHD adult subjects who remitted (Shaw et al., 2013). Interestingly, inattentive symptoms in healthy children have previously been associated with decreased regional thickness and thinning rate in the right lateral and left medial PFC (Ducharme et al., 2012). These results are in consonant with another study done by Shaw et al. (2011) where typically developing children with high levels of hyperactivity/impulsivity showed slow rate of cortical thinning among prefrontal, premotor, and cingulate regions. Additionally, ADHD subjects show a non-progressive loss of volume in the superior cerebellar vermis from childhood to adolescent years regardless of clinical outcome (Mackie et al., 2007). However, a downward trajectory in volumes of bilateral inferior-posterior cerebellar lobes are exhibited in ADHD subjects who have worst clinical outcomes (Mackie et al., 2007; Leech and Sharp, 2014). Evidence of severe early-life deprivation (by institutionalization) from a cohort of children with ADHD results in reduced cortical thickness across the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, insula, inferior parietal cortex, precuneus, superior temporal cortex, lingual gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus (McLaughlin et al., 2014) compared to age-matched community control subjects. These regions were also found to be associated with inattention and impulsivity (McLaughlin et al., 2014). Future neurodevelopmental studies of anatomy may shed light on the clinical presentations of ADHD by considering longitudinal cohort designs that account for phenotypic heterogeneity. Besides these factors that modulate the structural abnormalities in the ADHD brain, exposure to stimulant medication also alternates these brain signatures.

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medicated

There is mounting evidence to suggest neural anatomical alterations in ADHD as a result of psychotherapeutic intervention (Spencer et al., 2013; Chou et al., 2015). Numerous cross-sectional studies have examined the effects of psychostimulant treatment on ADHD. We first discuss cross-sectional studies that quantified volumetric alterations in the ADHD brain over the course of pharmacotherapy, followed by a discussion of morphometry results. Although there are relatively fewer longitudinal studies, we summarize extant literature of studies examining pre/post medications effects on structural MRI measures.

Volumetric cross-sectional studies

Overall voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies have yielded mixed results when considering the effects of stimulant medication on brain structure. In general, studies that reported significant effects suggest that certain subcortical and cortical nuclei are normalized in medicated ADHD children compared to medication naïve ADHD children. A meta-regression analysis found no association between gray matter volume abnormalities and long-term stimulant medication use from ADHD patients across childhood and adulthood ages (Norman et al., 2016). Villemonteix et al. (2015) reported a volume reduction in middle frontal and precentral gyrus in treatment naïve ADHD children compared to both medicated children with ADHD and controls (Villemonteix et al., 2015). For subcortical areas, Semrud-Clikeman et al. (2006) found an association between duration of treatment and normalized GM volume in the caudate and left nucleus accumbens (ACC) in children with ADHD. Similarly, the right anterior cingulate cortex was found to be normalized in children and adolescents with a treatment history in ADHD-combined (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2014).

Contrary to results that showed brain normalization, Schweren et al. (2015) found that a combined treatment group (methylphenidate and antipsychotics) of adolescents with ADHD results in reduction of total cortical volume, bilateral ventral diencephalon, and left thalamus compared to healthy controls. This was not found in the group that were only medicated with methylphenidate (Schweren et al., 2015). Thus, the findings may indicate that antipsychotic treatment could counteract the normalizing effects of methylphenidate on brain structure, but the authors are quick to note that they did not have an untreated ADHD group which hinders interpretation of the results (Schweren et al., 2015). Furthermore, another cross-sectional study found that current stimulant use versus no current use was associated with lower surface area in two frontal cortex regions (Hoogman et al., 2019).

Interhemispheric laterality findings of stimulant medication have been mixed. Patterns of absolute asymmetry volumes appear to increase in the caudal anterior cingulate and isthmus cingulate for medicated ADHD youths compared to medication naïve ADHD youths (Douglas et al., 2018). In a longitudinal replication design, increase absolute asymmetry volume was greater in medication naïve than medicated ADHD youths in cortical regions of the frontal, occipital, parietal, and temporal (Dutta, 2020). Postema et al. (2021) found current medication use was associated with surface area asymmetries in the precuneus and transverse temporal, and with thickness asymmetries in the inferior parietal cortex and precentral. Lifetime psychostimulant medication use involved asymmetries of surface area insula, supramarginal gyrus, and rostral anterior cingulate cortex, and thickness asymmetry of the paracentral lobule (Postema et al., 2021). The asymmetry of precentral gyrus thickness was associated with an ADHD diagnosis across all age groups (Postema et al., 2021). Thus, it appears that stimulants both increase and decrease structural brain asymmetries.

In contrast, other cross-sectional studies have found no volumetric differences in the ADHD brain as a result of medication intake. Greater (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017) or smaller (Castellanos et al., 2002; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2006) volumes of the caudate were no different based on medication history/status in children and adolescents with ADHD. These also translated to other regional volumetric findings in the cerebellum (Castellanos et al., 2002), right prefrontal (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017), bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2006), insula and middle temporal gyrus (Villemonteix et al., 2015). A cross-sectional mega-analysis on subcortical nuclei found that stimulant medication for ADHD was not related to structural changes across the lifespan, noting that the effects may be too local to be picked up by volumetric analysis (Hoogman et al., 2017). Indeed, morphology analyses may provide more insight into regional differences.

Morphology studies

A variety of studies suggest that more localized morphological alterations appear to resolve or normalize after treatment with stimulants that enhance DA signaling. For example, Sobel et al. (2010) found attenuation of morphology deformations in specific basal ganglia regions in ADHD medicated children, suggesting that stimulants may normalize morphological differences in the caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus. Sub-regions of the cerebellum, such as the posterior inferior vermis, show reduced attenuation in ADHD medicated children compared to medication naïve subjects (Berquin et al., 1998; Bledsoe et al., 2009). Morphology analyses from youths (children and adolescents) with ADHD reveal increased volumes in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2006), anterior thalamic pulvinar (Ivanov et al., 2010), splenium of the corpus callosum (Schnoebelen et al., 2010), and left lateral cerebellar surface (Ivanov et al., 2014) associated with intake of stimulant treatment. On the other hand, follow-up analyses revealed atypical surface area morphology in the PFC for ADHD children prescribed with stimulant medication (Dirlikov et al., 2015).

Longitudinal studies

Relatively fewer studies have investigated longitudinal changes before and after medication onset prospectively. Some evidence suggests that the age of medication onset and treatment duration may play a role in predicting the magnitude of medication effects on brain structure and function. For example, Shaw et al. (2009b) found the rate of change between adolescents (12.5–16.4 years) with ADHD taking psychostimulants differed from those not taking psychostimulants in cortical thickness measures of the right motor strip, left middle/inferior frontal gyrus, and right parieto-occipital region, but importantly, this was not associated with clinical outcome. They found cortical thinning was more pronounced in the group not taking psychotherapy (0.16 mm/year) compared to the group taking psychotherapy (0.03 mm/year) (Shaw et al., 2009b). On the contrary, no association between psychostimulant medication and the development of the surface morphology for the basal ganglia (caudate, putamen, globus pallidus) was found (Shaw et al., 2014). Instead, the ADHD group (childhood to adolescence), regardless of medication status, showed progressive contraction of the ventral striatal surfaces (1.75 mm2 per year) compared to controls (rate of increase 0.54 mm2 per year) (Shaw et al., 2014). In adults with ADHD, before and after 3 years of psychostimulant treatment, left putamen GM volumes are similar to controls, but increased compared to non-medicated ADHD adults suggesting a normalizing effect (Pretus et al., 2017). Similarly, after 1–2 years of MPH treatment, adult ADHD patients revealed recovered nucleus accumbens (Nacc) gray matter volumes relative to controls (Hoekzema et al., 2014). van Elst et al. (2016) observed an increase in cerebellar GM volume for MPH-treated adults with ADHD after a year of treatment, but no change in cortical thinning. Two meta-regression analyses of VBM studies (Nakao et al., 2011; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012) and one qualitative review (Spencer et al., 2013) examined long-term psychostimulant effects. These studies found that long-term stimulant medication use was associated with normalized basal ganglia volumes (Nakao et al., 2011; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012; Spencer et al., 2013) thus suggesting no evidence that stimulant drugs cause abnormal brain development in ADHD (Spencer et al., 2013). Another longitudinal study found brain volume asymmetry decreases across the cortex (frontal, temporal, occipital, and parietal) with stimulant medication (8 weeks exposure to MPH and/or guanfacine) compared to medication naïve ADHD youths (Dutta, 2020). Thus, it is likely that the impact of psychostimulants on neurodevelopment is enabling anatomic normalization (Friedman and Rapoport, 2015). Figure 4 displays subcortical alterations seen in ADHD medication naïve subjects and ADHD-Rx (medicated), and Figure 5 images cortical changes seen in medication naïve and treated ADHD subjects.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Subcortical brain alterations in ADHD-free (medication naïve) and ADHD-Rx (medicated). These figures were created using ggseg library from R program (Mowinckel and Vidal-Piñeiro, 2020).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Cortical brain alterations in ADHD-free (medication naïve) and ADHD-Rx (medicated). These figures were created using ggseg library from R program (Mowinckel and Vidal-Piñeiro, 2020).

Diffusion tensor imaging

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder drug naïve

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) examines direction and displacement of water molecules in the brain in order to infer white matter (WM) architecture (Basser et al., 1994). Region of interest (ROI) studies have found asymmetry in WM integrity in ADHD individuals. Figure 6 displays each of these regions along with their asymmetries in fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity found in ADHD children and adults: the superior and inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Hamilton et al., 2008; Makris et al., 2008; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Silk et al., 2009b; Kobel et al., 2010; Liston et al., 2011; Nagel et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Cortese et al., 2013; Lawrence et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016); the anterior, posterior, and superior of the corona radiata (Pavuluri et al., 2009; Kobel et al., 2010; Nagel et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; Cortese et al., 2013); posterior and anterior thalamic radiation (Silk et al., 2009b; Cortese et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016); cerebellum and cellebellar peduncle (Ashtari et al., 2005; Makris et al., 2008; Bechtel et al., 2009; Kobel et al., 2010; Nagel et al., 2011); the splenium, isthmus, and genu of the corpus callosum (Chao et al., 2009; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Cao et al., 2010; Peterson et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; Dramsdahl et al., 2012) and the posterior and anterior limb of the internal capsule (Pavuluri et al., 2009; Silk et al., 2009b; Nagel et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Cortese et al., 2013). Voxel-based analyses (VBA) also confirm ADHD white matter deficits in: the unicinate fasciculus (Silk et al., 2009b; Shaw et al., 2015); forceps minor (Qiu et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Lawrence et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016); corticospinal tract (Hamilton et al., 2008; Svatkova et al., 2016); cingulum bundle (Makris et al., 2008; Konrad et al., 2010; Svatkova et al., 2016) and sagittal stratum (Peterson et al., 2011; Cortese et al., 2013). Abnormalities may depend on ADHD presentation, where inattention has been linked to impairments in frontostriatal circuits while hyperactivity has been linked to impairments in frontolimbic circuits (Konrad and Eickhoff, 2010; Lei et al., 2014; Svatkova et al., 2016). These results parallel the dual pathway model of ADHD, suggesting that frontostriatal pathways lead to executive dysfunction while frontolimbic pathways lead to rewarding response and motivation deficits (Sonuga-Barke, 2003, 2005; Castellanos et al., 2006).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Illustrative figure of tractography fibers showing significant differences across ADHD treatment naïve and treated individuals. In ADHD-Free (treatment naive), subjects had differences of fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD) in the following white matter regions: cingulum (decrease FA in adults and increase FA in children; Makris et al., 2008; Konrad et al., 2010; Svatkova et al., 2016), uncinate fasciculus (UF) (increase FA in children and decrease FA in adults; Silk et al., 2009b; Shaw et al., 2015), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) (increase and decrease of FA in children/adults), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) (increase and decrease in FA in children/adults; Hamilton et al., 2008; Makris et al., 2008; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Silk et al., 2009b; Kobel et al., 2010; Liston et al., 2011; Nagel et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Cortese et al., 2013; Lawrence et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016), corticospinal tract (CT) [decrease FA in adults and increase FA in children; increase MD in children; Hamilton et al., 2008; Luis-García et al., 2015; Svatkova et al., 2016; included are white matter regions of the superior, anterior, and posterior corona radiata (decrease FA for children/adolescents/adults; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Kobel et al., 2010; Nagel et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; Cortese et al., 2013), the anterior and superior limb of the internal capsule (decrease FA in children/adolescents/adults; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Silk et al., 2009b; Nagel et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Cortese et al., 2013), and the cerebellar peduncle (decrease/increase FA in children; increase FA in adolescents; Ashtari et al., 2005; Makris et al., 2008; Bechtel et al., 2009; Kobel et al., 2010; Nagel et al., 2011)], inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) [included is the sagittal stratum (decrease FA in adults; increase FA in children; Peterson et al., 2011; Cortese et al., 2013)], anterior and superior thalamic radiation (decrease FA in adults; increase FA in children/adolescents; increase MD in children/adolescents; Silk et al., 2009b; Cortese et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016), and corpus callosum [included white matter tracts include the splenium, genu, isthmus (decrease FA in children/adolescents/adults; increase FA in children/adolescents; increase MD in children/adolescents; Chao et al., 2009; Pavuluri et al., 2009; Cao et al., 2010; Peterson et al., 2011; Qiu et al., 2011; Dramsdahl et al., 2012; Luis-García et al., 2015)] and the forceps minors (decrease FA in children; Qiu et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Lawrence et al., 2013; Svatkova et al., 2016). ADHD-Rx (treated) children had decreases in MD for the following white matter tracts (Luis-García et al., 2015): unicinate fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, corticospinal tract, and corpus callosum (sector IV). All images were created using DTI data from DTI Studio (Jiang et al., 2006; Fedorov et al., 2012).

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medicated

The impact of ADHD medication on WM microstructure remains unclear. Ashtari et al. (2005) found no significant effects on FA values between ADHD-Rx (medicated) and ADHD-Free (medication naïve) groups in six brain regions. Luis-García et al. (2015) found MD was reduced in methylphenidate (MPH) medicated ADHD patients in fronto-striatal WM tracts (Luis-García et al., 2015). In their longitudinal study, de Zeeuw et al. (2012) found a decrease of FA for all ADHD groups regardless of MPH treatment duration. Asymmetry patterns for FA measures of the uncinate fasciculus and inferior lateral fasciculus appear to normalize with stimulant medication compared to ADHD-Free (medication naïve) subjects (Douglas et al., 2018). Figure 6 displays fiber regions that were found to differ in ADHD children and adults when under psychostimulant treatment and medication naive.

Neurocomputational theories of catecholamines and attention

Catecholaminergic brain systems, such as dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE), are important neuromodulators that control attention. In light of their shared biosynthesis, intracellular signaling, and innervation pathways, it is critically important to differentiate their functions within a unified paradigm to explain the pathophysiology of ADHD. Computational psychiatry attempts to develop neurocomputational models that can describe the cognitive deficits typical of ADHD as they relate to brain function. While extent attempts are scarce, they are promising (see Williams and Dayan, 2005; Frank et al., 2007a,b; Luman et al., 2010; Hauser et al., 2016; Ziegler et al., 2016). Behavioral profiles of ADHD have been differentiated given current neurocomputational models: A hyperactive/impulsivity presentation can be expressed as a behavioral switching between less valuable options, whereas an inattentive presentation is a shift between goal orientation and an inability to stay focus on one individual goal (Hauser et al., 2016). The catecholaminergic profiles of each ADHD presentation is still difficult to disentangle though the clear distinction is made between dopamine and norepinephrine action selection models that could explain the overarching diagnosis. A decrease in ‘dopamine’ precision leads to high entropy or to many surprising events for the brain to process, whereas ‘norepinephrine’ heightens learning to both typical and novel information thus increasing attentional variability. Here, we merge computational, algorithmic, and implementation Marrian levels of analysis (Marr, 2010) to demonstrate how catecholamines can be dissociated to explain ADHD phenotype.

Dopamine

Dopamine was discovered as a neurotransmitter in 1957 by neuropharmacologist Arvid Carlsson during exploits of DA in the basal ganglia and motor function in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Carlsson, 1959, 1993). Interest in dopamine as a mechanism for reward-based learning and motivation gained momentum through use of electrophysiological recordings and pharmacological manipulations of DA in animals (Olds and Milner, 1954; Wise, 1982; Schultz, 1986; Schultz et al., 1993; Viggiano et al., 2003, 2004). These early experiments uncovered two patterns of DA firing: tonic and phasic activity (Schultz, 1986, 2001). Tonic firing patterns consist of slow and sustained extracellular DA neuron firing, while phasic activation consists of sudden firing rate change (50–110 ms; duration < 200 ms) of DA concentrations. Intermediary levels of firing reflect slow burst firing lasting seconds to minutes (Schultz, 2001). Current theoretical neurocomputational models examine phasic and tonic DA signaling in select brain regions. One such model, reinforcement learning theory (Sutton and Barto, 1998), proposes an adaptive decision-making framework based on optimizing behavior to obtain future reward or avoid punishments. This trial-and-error learning process is established under four main schemes (Sutton and Barto, 1998; Montague et al., 2004; Dayan and Daw, 2008; Samson et al., 2010; Daw and Tobler, 2014): (1) a reward function or value to a state, (2) a weighted running average update that accounts for all rewards received previously in the presence of the stimulus with the most recent reward weighted heavily than the prior rewards, (3) a value function updating the prediction based on the current reward/state and direction of the weight, and (4) reward prediction error (RPE) comparing what reward the subject experiences on a specific trial and what reward they expected based on previous learning. The RPE reflects the rate of firing of D1 and D2 neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the substancia nigra pars compacta (Schultz et al., 1997; Schultz, 2001; Lammel et al., 2012), striatum (Hernández-López et al., 1997, 2000; Daw et al., 2011; Watabe-Uchida and Uchida, 2018), lateral orbitofrontal cortex (Tobler et al., 2007), hippocampus and PFC (Gurden et al., 2000). Fast latency (50–110 ms) and duration (<200 ms) of phasic DA RPEs are observed during food/liquid rewards (animal experiments), conditioned reward-predicting stimuli (classical, simple choice RT, delayed go/no-go, visual discrimination tasks), and non-noxious stimuli that induce avoidance (Schultz, 2001). Dopamine depression or activation-depression responses are seen following stimuli that resemble the reward, following novel or intense stimuli, or during reward omission errors (Schultz, 2001).

Under some other accounts, dopamine neurons are postulated to signal ‘prediction error’ rather than reward. In this view, the brain makes inferences about the environment and tests these against sensory evidence in order to reduce free energy (i.e., prevent entropy or time average of surprise) (Friston, 2010; Friston et al., 2015, 2017; Parr and Friston, 2017). A Markovian decision process of a probabilistic generative model is considered: P(π) = σ(−γ⋅G(π)), whereby P prior distributions over policies π is equal to policies selected based on free energy G multiplied by an inverse temperature parameter −γ corresponding to precision of beliefs (i.e., dopamine firing) about policies over a softmax normalized exponential σ. Here, γ reflects the rate of DA firing. Encoding of dopamine as a ‘precision signal’ is illustrated in a number of empirical studies such as repetition suppression during learning (Bromberg-Martin and Hikosaka, 2009), hippocampal place cell activity during spatial tasks (Moser et al., 2015; Retailleau and Morris, 2018), and errors of omission and commission during oddball tasks (Bendixen et al., 2012). Besides its modulations in reward and precision, dopamine is ascribed to a number of other behaviors including belief and latent states (Rao, 2010), critic (ventral striatum) versus actor roles (dorsal striatum) (O’Doherty et al., 2004), hierarchical levels of abstraction learning when confronted with novelty (O’Reilly and Frank, 2006; Badre et al., 2010), judgment of time (Kurth-Nelson and Redish, 2009; Soares et al., 2016; Hamid et al., 2019), quality versus quantity stimuli attributes (de Berker et al., 2019), values for each effector (Gershman et al., 2009), and future events besides reward (Gardner et al., 2018).

Several behavioral markers of ADHD can be explained by theories of reinforcement learning and precision error. For example, the decision temperature parameter determines if a subject will choose the optimal response or a variable response amongst all other alternative options (Herrnstein, 1961). Increasing the levels of the temperature parameter elicits variable and exploratory behavior, which is the case for ADHD. Individuals with ADHD do not always choose or exploit the best option, rather they exhibit response variability and response inconsistency (Hauser et al., 2016). Patients with ADHD display increased reaction time (RT) variability (Castellanos and Tannock, 2002; Tamm et al., 2012; Kofler et al., 2013) in working memory, and go/no-go and stop tasks with increased activity in the frontal regions and default mode network (DMN) (Uddin et al., 2008; Fassbender et al., 2009; Mohan et al., 2016). During the continuous performance task (CPT), ADHD subjects respond more often to non-target related responses (errors of omission) than target-related responses (errors of commission) (Losier et al., 1996; Huang-Pollock et al., 2012) with implicated circuits of the PFC, insula, and parietal areas. In the context of decision-making, individuals diagnosed with ADHD are more likely to choose immediate and small rewards across delay of gratification tasks, delay discounting tasks (Patros et al., 2016) and probabilistic reversal learning task in the medial PFC (Hauser et al., 2014). This could suggest poor reward prediction error signals (Tripp and Wickens, 2008). On the other hand, ADHD subjects are suggested to have divergent learning patterns by choosing suboptimal choices through exploratory behaviors as indicated by an increased decision temperature (Friston et al., 2014; Schwartenbeck et al., 2015b). We illustrate low and high temperature parameters in the context of exploitation-exploration action selection for dopamine (Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. A softmax decision steepness parameter influences high and low temperatures for the action selection of dopamine. A high temperature renders variable behaviors as seen in ADHD, whereas a low temperature renders stable behaviors. Inspired by Hauser et al., 2016, Figure 1.

Norepinephrine/noradrenaline

Norepinephrine, or noradrenaline, has also been viewed from a computational neuroscience perspective. Similar to dopamine, tonic and phasic levels of firing are examined in the context of behavioral tasks. Within theory on precision (Yu and Dayan, 2005; Dayan and Yu, 2006; Parr and Friston, 2017), noradrenergic transmission is proposed as a signal of ‘unexpected uncertainty’ or volatility of state transitions. This theory stems from noradrenergic cell activity during task demands that induce unexpected changes, including novelty, the introduction of novel reinforcement agents, and reversing or eliminating prior contingencies (Bouret and Sara, 2005; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005; Bouret and Richmond, 2015). Drug treatment using idazoxan, an a2 receptor antagonist that activates noradrenergic system, was effective only when rats had to change from spatial to visual cues during a maze navigation rather than during these tasks, suggesting NA’s role in attentional shift (Curet et al., 1987; Devauges and Sara, 1990). In monkeys, non-target stimuli that resulted in false alarm responses elicited phasic locus coeruleus (LC) activation during a vigilance discriminative task (Rajkowski et al., 2004). Pupillometry measures have been linked to noradrenergic activity (for a review, see Koss, 1986; Larsen and Waters, 2018) of the LC, brainstem (e.g., areas of colliculi), and cingulate cortex as seen in extracellular recordings in rhesus macaques (Joshi et al., 2016), and human fMRI BOLD activity during rest and the oddball paradigm (Bush et al., 1999; Bush, 2011; Murphy et al., 2014). Other important roles denoted by noradrenergic activity include arousal modulations (Berridge and Waterhouse, 2003; Berridge, 2008; Pattij et al., 2012; de Gee et al., 2017), increased reaction time variability coinciding with tonic levels of NA firing, and ‘network reset’ behavioral planning through phasic NA levels (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Bouret and Sara, 2005; Dayan and Yu, 2006; Llorente et al., 2006; Frank et al., 2007a; de Gee et al., 2017), and exogenous attentional set shifting by phasic (exploitation) and tonic (exploration) NA levels (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Gabay et al., 2011; Pattij et al., 2012; Mathot et al., 2014; Tervo et al., 2014). A recent computational approach to norepinephrine has been linked to learning rates (Sales et al., 2019) under the active inference model (Schwartenbeck et al., 2015a; Friston et al., 2017). In brief, they explore noradrenergic activity as a state-action prediction error that can evoke both ‘explore-exploit’ and ‘network reset’ behavioral modifications. Phasic LC responses are linked to prediction of a reward in a Go/No-Go paradigm with reversal contingencies, and high tonic levels of LC activity were elicited during exploration and task disengagement (Sales et al., 2019). Prediction errors are enabled through LC innervations, while belief updates or responses to state-action prediction errors are broadcast via ascending projections from LC to the cortex (e.g., frontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, dorsal PFC) (Gläscher et al., 2010; Karlsson et al., 2012; Tervo et al., 2014; Ebitz and Platt, 2015; Sales et al., 2019).

Noradrenergic signaling influences attention through learning, saliency, and exploitative-exploratory behavioral avenues. NA action selection models, such as the decision temperature (Jepma and Nieuwenhuis, 2011; Eldar et al., 2016; Hauser et al., 2016) and learning rates (Sales et al., 2019), can describe ADHD phenotypes. Similar to dopamine, noradrenaline does affect response variability in ADHD, whereby high tonic but low phasic signaling is associated with reaction time variability (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Manev and Uz, 2009). The high tonic NA levels increase the representation of other stimuli cues in the environment, thus dissociating less strongly to high-valued options and preference for less optimal options (Servan-Schreiber et al., 1990; Eldar et al., 2013; Hauser et al., 2016). Evidence of NA dysfunction is reported in the prefrontal cortical region in rodent models of ADHD (Fan et al., 2011) and in human participants during response inhibition tasks (de Campo et al., 2011). The difference between noradrenaline learning and dopamine learning is that NA is associated with focus of relevant information and rate of learning, whereas dopamine is associated with precision of value that is ascribed to options. In an explore/exploit task, we illustrate total reward intake based on norepinephrine alpha parameters with slow, fast, and flexible model decays (Figure 8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Here we illustrate a simple explore/exploit task for behavioral output with fixed alpha (α) learning parameters. α=32 represents a slow model decay learning with hyperflexible behavior to change strategies after one single failed trial, whereas α=2 represents a fast model decay where behavior is inflexible and persistence in behavior is shown. A less stable environment reflects changes in reward for every 10–15 trials, whereas a more stable environment reflects changes after 50–60 trials. We also display flexible alpha parameter that is able to adapt, however, flexible 1 is a behavioral output that enables an agent to rapidly switch in task demands, whereas flexible 2 displays an agent that varies behavior, but is not able to attain learning over time. A possible model of ADHD can reflect a variation of these learning parameters. Inspired by Sales et al., 2019, Figure 7.

Discussion

Pharmaceutical treatment for ADHD often leads to regional, but not global, brain alterations as observed across numerous structural neuroimaging modalities (Castellanos et al., 2002, 2008; Nakao et al., 2011; van Ewijk et al., 2012; Svatkova et al., 2016; Hoogman et al., 2017). Stimulants also seem to attenuate brain volume decreases and regional morphology asymmetries in basal ganglia across children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2006; Bledsoe et al., 2009; Shaw et al., 2009a; Ivanov et al., 2010; Schnoebelen et al., 2010; Sobel et al., 2010; Nakao et al., 2011; Villemonteix et al., 2015). Less is known about ADHD treatment effects on white matter structure since few systematic DTI treatment studies have been conducted to date (Ashtari et al., 2005; de Zeeuw et al., 2012; Luis-García et al., 2015). Smaller white matter volumes and asymmetric patterns in white matter microstructure are seen in both medicated and non-medicated ADHD youths (Castellanos et al., 2002; Douglas et al., 2018; Dutta, 2020), although these effects are more pronounced in non-medicated ADHD youths. It is possible this asymmetry may subtend the behavioral features of ADHD; however, such asymmetries may also serve as advantageous later in life.

In longitudinal PET and SPECT studies (van Dyck et al., 2002), typically developing individuals experience a 6–8% decline in striatal DATs per decade. Longitudinal studies in PET have not been conducted in ADHD populations but, typically, there is low DAT availability in non-medicated patients, whereas there is a high DAT availability in medicated patients among striatal regions. It is possible that either of these extremes can provide benefits toward striatal dopamine transporter integrity later in life. In essence, drug treatment for ADHD does not always appear to increase global brain volume or attenuate morphology abnormalities across all white and gray matter. Conversely, stimulant drugs are not suggested to cause abnormal development in ADHD populations.

Interestingly, with increasing age, certain brain regions tend to normalize on their own without the help of psychostimulant treatment. For example, caudate volume seems to normalize by mid-adolescence to early-adulthood in ADHD participants, both stimulant-treated and non-treated populations (Castellanos et al., 1994, 2002). In addition, some studies report comparable brain volumes between treatment-naïve and treated ADHD populations. As mentioned earlier, pharmacotherapy has frequently been seen to normalize brain regions within the basal ganglia (Sobel et al., 2010). However, with or without treatment, children and adolescence with ADHD have relatively smaller global gray matter brain volumes compared to healthy age-matched participants. The growth of these brain volumes, although smaller, parallel age-matched controls, reflecting fixed neurobiological abnormalities (Castellanos et al., 2002; Nakao et al., 2011). In late adulthood such brain volumes tend to remain constant regardless of treatment and comorbidity, whereas aged-matched controls experience a decrease in brain volume.

Despite the importance of these findings, most ADHD studies are confounded by relatively small sample sizes, cross-sectional study design, and un-matched patient subgroups. These methodological issues currently hamper structural neuroimaging’s contribution to the diagnostic assessment of ADHD. Thus, standard clinical approaches to medication adjustment remain the best course when treatment ceases to provide positive effects (McGough, 2014; Barkley, 2015). Essential to good practice, clinicians must carefully document target symptoms, treatment responses, prescription information, dosage, and quantity of medication with each prescription, while also considering the possibility of abuse, misuse, and diversion of these drugs as the patient ages (McGough and McCracken, 2000; Craig et al., 2015; Rajeh et al., 2017). Patients with ADHD are prescribed about 3.8 times more amphetamine in 2005–2014 than methylphenidate (1.6 times from 2005 to 2014) (Moran et al., 2019). Moran et al. (2019) found that family medicine or internal medicine physicians (the most frequent prescribers) prescribed amphetamine about 72.5% of patients compared to pediatricians (51.6%) and psychiatrists (63.7%). This coincided with a rise of psychotic episodes based on prescription of these stimulants to ADHD cohorts (Moran et al., 2019). New onset psychosis was twice as high in ADHD patients that began amphetamine than among ADHD patients who began methylphenidate (Salo et al., 2013; Moran et al., 2019). Recall that methylphenidate acts as an inhibitor of dopamine transporters, whereas amphetamine releases four times as much dopamine in the synapse (Schiffer et al., 2006). The large heterogeneity of ADHD phenotypes also makes it difficult to access or appreciate the differences in catecholamine profiles. Medication selection should also be carefully considered based on comorbidities, co-medications, and pharmacokinetics such as patients who poorly metabolize pharmacological treatments (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014). This evidence suggests the need to cautiously screen for risk factors and to consistently monitor for reliability of the stimulant drug treatments after its administration to patients.

Neuroimaging studies have suggested that children with ADHD have diminished brain volume in certain subcortical structures compared to healthy controls (Castellanos et al., 2002; Qiu et al., 2009; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012; Hoogman et al., 2017). Recently, a cross-sectional mega-analysis across the lifespan revealed that ADHD in late adulthood may have a delay in brain degeneration. For example, in healthy individuals the volume of the hippocampus and the amygdala normally declines later in life (Hoogman et al., 2017), and this decline is more pronounced in people with prodomal dementia (or mild cognitive impairment) and AD (Nho et al., 2012; Fujishima et al., 2014; Kehoe et al., 2014; Matsuda, 2016; Femminella et al., 2018). Interestingly, an ADHD diagnosis is associated with reduced volumetric decline in these structures later in life. It is important, however, to consider that these results were not influenced by psychostimulant use and no differences in volumes were found when comparing ADHD subjects who had never taken medication (82 patients) and ADHD subjects who used stimulant medication for more than 4 weeks (637 patients) (Hoogman et al., 2017). Our review summarizes that pharmaceutical treatment either leads to no changes or normalizes brain alterations in select brain regions in ADHD as quantified across a variety of neuroimaging modalities ranging from PET to MRI. While many studies have demonstrated decrease in volume and morphology attenuations following medication treatment (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2006; Bledsoe et al., 2009; Ivanov et al., 2010; Sobel et al., 2010; van Elst et al., 2016; Douglas et al., 2018; Dutta, 2020), others did not find any measurable or quantifiable association of taking pharmaceutical drugs for ADHD on the brain. If ADHD poses a delay in brain degeneration later in life and if pharmaceutical treatment eliminates this neuroprotective element by normalizing structural changes associated with an ADHD diagnosis, then this benefit may be counteractive to neuroprotective volume into the geriatric years. On the other hand, pharmacological drugs for ADHD may potentially lead to neurodegenerative diseases. It is presumed that about 23% of cases with childhood ADHD will eventually develop MCI or dementia in older age, comparable to 21.5% of healthy subjects with no history of ADHD (Callahan et al., 2017). Whether there is a relationship between stimulant use during childhood or adult years from ADHD subjects and later MCI is largely unknown. It is important to note that screening methods for ADHD are often overlooked or confused with MCI diagnosis (Ivanchak et al., 2012), and there is a high frequency of antecedent ADHD symptoms to patients with dementia or AD (Ivanchak et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015; Fluegge and Fluegge, 2018). On the other hand, several lines of research in genetics (The Brainstorm Consortium et al., 2018; Pagoni et al., 2020) and neuroanatomy (Callahan et al., 2017) do not point to a direct link between ADHD and later MCI. There may be unrelated mediators that increase risk for later MCI or dementia, such as comorbidity (i.e., depression, anxiety, substance use) (Ivanchak et al., 2012) or stimulant use. Few neuroimaging and pharmacological studies have derived such relations as of yet, providing future opportunities to explore. Given that the evidence is inconclusive, it may be beneficial to act in a cautionary fashion when prescribing pharmaceutical treatment to ADHD. In this sense, one should weigh the benefits and limitations of medication treatment, including the severity of the disease presentation and comorbidities, against the potential risk of eliminating a benefit later in life.

The findings in this review paper have their limitations and their interpretations merit great caution. We did not use established methods of systematic review protocols and risk of bias (Higgins et al., 2011, 2019; Moher et al., 2015; Zeng et al., 2015), instead we tried to leverage a large number of existing and relevant studies. Therefore, the review may be flawed by selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias, reporting bias, and other bias (Higgins et al., 2011). Our review may have missed studies due to bias in eligibility criteria and coverage of the retrieved studies. Additionally, similar to other reviews and meta-analytic studies (Cunill et al., 2016; Cortese et al., 2018), there are methodological and clinical heterogeneity in the included studies, such as study design, patient samples, and outcome measurements. It should be noted that our review does not support any pharmacological treatment over another and does not reveal any significant differences between the variations of short- and long- acting formulations. Few head-to-head studies exist due to insufficient patient samples and events that can be conducted to account for differences in dosage and intensity of all types of pharmacological treatments. Risk of bias in other studies is also important and can be more apparent than what was reported in our review. Therefore, there is still room for improvement in future systematic reviews regarding the topic of lifespan considerations for the pharmacological treatment of adult and pediatric ADHD. It is a limitation that only few databases were used in this review paper, of which it is likely that important studies were not captured.

In summary, subjects with ADHD may present differential trajectories of brain structure across the lifespan, with early abnormalities followed by a delay in brain degeneration later in life relative to controls. Drug treatment for ADHD does not always appear to increase global brain volume or attenuate morphology abnormalities across all white and gray matter. Rather, treatment seems to increase brain volume in a region-specific manner that normalizes ADHD brain structure, though these effects may vary throughout the lifespan. Indeed, additional studies are needed to determine the extent to which an ADHD diagnosis may be neuroprotective later in life. If so, determining the mechanisms that subtend this neuroprotective effect is essential. For example, it is reasonable to suggest that ADHD individuals switch more rapidly between stimuli to which they attend either externally (in hyperactive) or internally (in inattentive presentations). In this sense, the switching may in some sense exercise the brain. The delay in maturation hypothesis has continued to be a predominant theory in the field. Future studies may therefore focus on linking this maturation delay with delay in degeneration later in life. Unsupervised techniques (e.g., non-negative matrix factorization) (Anderson et al., 2014) may also be useful for identifying subgroups within or across the presentation domains that preferentially respond to pharmaceutical treatment throughout life, and the extent to which these subgroups overlap with those who may receive benefit from the behavioral manifestations of ADHD. Does the potential benefit of pharmaceutical treatment early in life (i.e., children, adolescents) outweigh the possibility that pharmaceutical treatment early in life may have detrimental effects in later adulthood? These questions must be examined in future longitudinal work in order to minimize the risk and maximize the utility of currently available ADHD medications.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The authors acknowledge the support provided in part by the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation NARSAD Young Investigator Grant, the K01 NIH Grant (1MH110645), and baseline funds from King Abdullah University of Science and Technology.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the help given by the Biostatistics Group at King Abdullah University of Science and Technology, University of Central Florida at the Institute for Simulation and Training (IST), UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Medicine, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, NARSAD Young Investigator Award, and a K01 Award from the NIMH (K01NIMH and 1MH110645), without which this work would not have been possible. This material is also based upon work supported by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research under award number FA9550-22-1-0000. We also thank Farzad V. Farahani for his contribution to Figure 1 and Aritra Dutta for his contribution to Figures 7, 8. Inclusion of text and images from Cintya N. Dutta’s dissertation was used for this review manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.938501/full#supplementary-material

References

Adler, L. A., Goodman, D. W., Kollins, S. H., Weisler, R. H., Krishnan, S., Zhang, Y., et al. (2008). Double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the efficacy and safety of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 69:1364. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahrendts, J., Rüsch, N., Wilke, M., Philipsen, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Glauche, V., et al. (2011). Visual cortex abnormalities in adults with ADHD: A structural MRI study. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 12, 260–270. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2010.518624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albajara Sáenz, A., Villemonteix, T., and Massat, I. (2019). Structural and functional neuroimaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 61, 399–405. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almeida Montes, L. G., Prado Alcántara, H., Martínez García, R. B., Torre, L. B., Avila Acosta, D., and Duarte, M. G. (2013). Brain cortical thickness in ADHD: Age, sex, and clinical correlations. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 641–654. doi: 10.1177/1087054711434351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almeida, L. G., Ricardo-Garcell, J., Prado, H., Barajas, L., Fernández-Bouzas, A., and Avila, D. (2010). Reduced right frontal cortical thickness in children, adolescents and adults with ADHD and its correlation to clinical variables: A cross-sectional study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 1214–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.04.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrosino, S., Zeeuw, P., Wierenga, L. M., Dijk, S., and Durston, S. (2017). What can cortical development in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder teach us about the early developmental mechanisms involved? Cereb. Cortex 27, 4624–4634. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, A., Douglas, P. K., Kerr, W. T., Haynes, V. S., Yuille, A. L., and Xie, J. (2014). Non-negative matrix factorization of multimodal MRI, fMRI and phenotypic data reveals differential changes in default mode subnetworks in ADHD. Neuroimage 102, 207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. T., and Li, B.-M. (2005). Neurobiology of executive functions: Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1377–1384. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. T., and Rubia, K. (2012). Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: Disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. J. Am Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 356–367. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashtari, M., Kumra, S., Bhaskar, S. L., Clarke, T., Thaden, E., and Cervellione, K. L. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 448–455. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badgaiyan, R. D., Sinha, S., Sajjad, M., and Wack, D. S. (2015). Attenuated tonic and enhanced phasic release of dopamine in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One 10:0137326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badre, D., Kayser, A. S., and D’Esposito, M. (2010). Frontal cortex and the discovery of abstract action rules. Neuron 66, 315–326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banaschewski, T., Becker, K., Döpfner, M., Holtmann, M., Rösler, M., and Romanos, M. (2017). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A current overview. Deutsch. Ärzteb. Int. 114:149. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2017.0149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., Leibson, C. L., and Jacobsen, S. J. (2006). Long-term stimulant medication treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from a population-based study. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 27, 1–10. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200602000-00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2015). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., and Fletcher, K. (2003). Does the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with stimulants contribute to drug use/abuse? A 13-year prospective study. Pediatrics 111, 97–109. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.1.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., and LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys. J. 66, 259–267. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, J., and Gainetdinov, R. (2011). The physiology, signaling, and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacol. Rev. 63, 182–217. doi: 10.1124/pr.110.002642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechtel, N., Kobel, M., Penner, I.-K., Klarhöfer, M., Scheffler, K., and Opwis, K. (2009). Decreased fractional anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncle in children with epilepsy and/or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary study. Epilepsy Behav. 15, 294–298. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendixen, A., SanMiguel, I., and Schröger, E. (2012). Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition: A review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 120–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Berker, A. O., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Rutledge, R. B., Bestmann, S., and Dolan, R. J. (2019). Computing value from quality and quantity in human decision-making. J. Neurosci. 39, 163–176. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0706-18.2018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berquin, P. C., Giedd, J. N., Jacobsen, L. K., Hamburger, S. D., Krain, A. L., and Rapoport, J. L. (1998). Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A morphometric MRI study. Neurology 50, 1087–1093. doi: 10.1212/WNL.50.4.1087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, C. W. (2008). Noradrenergic modulation of arousal. Brain Res. Rev. 58, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, C. W., and Waterhouse, B. D. (2003). The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: Modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42, 33–84. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00143-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhatia, A., and Saadabadi, A. (2020). Biochemistry, Dopamine Receptors. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing.

Google Scholar

Biederman, J., and Faraone, S. V. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 366, 237–248. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66915-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Krishnan, S., Zhang, Y., McGough, J. J., and Findling, R. L. (2007). Efficacy and tolerability of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate (NRP-104) in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A phase III, multicenter, randomized, double-blind, forced-dose, parallel-group study. Clin. Ther. 29, 450–463. doi: 10.1016/S0149-2918(07)80083-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Melmed, R. D., Patel, A., McBurnett, K., Konow, J., and Lyne, A. (2008). A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of guanfacine extended release in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 121, e73–e84. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bledsoe, J., Semrud-Clikeman, M., and Pliszka, S. R. (2009). A Magnetic resonance imaging study of the cerebellar vermis in chronically treated and treatment-naïve children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder combined type. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 620–624. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolea-Alamañac, B., Nutt, D. J., Adamou, M., Asherson, P., Bazire, S., Coghill, D., et al. (2014). Evidence-based guidelines for the pharmacological management of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Update on recommendations from the British association for psychopharmacology. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 179–203. doi: 10.1177/0269881113519509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouret, S., and Richmond, B. J. (2015). Sensitivity of locus ceruleus neurons to reward value for goal-directed actions. J. Neurosci. 35, 4005–4014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4553-14.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouret, S., and Sara, S. J. (2005). Network reset: A simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 28, 574–582. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boxhoorn, S., Lopez, E., Schmidt, C., Schulze, D., Hänig, S., Cholemkery, H., et al. (2019). Attention as neurocognitive endophenotype of ADHD across the life span: A family study. Eur Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 269, 627–644. doi: 10.1007/s00406-019-00993-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bralten, J., Greven, C. U., Franke, B., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., Rommelse, N. N., et al. (2016). Voxel-based morphometry analysis reveals frontal brain differences in participants with ADHD and their unaffected siblings. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41:272. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandon, C. L., Marinelli, M., and White, F. J. (2003). Adolescent exposure to methylphenidate alters the activity of rat midbrain dopamine neurons. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1338–1344. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00787-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, A. R., and Arnsten, A. F. T. (2010). Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 236–245. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bröer, S., and Gether, U. (2012). The solute carrier 6 family of transporters. Br. J. Pharmacol. 167, 256–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2012.01975.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bromberg-Martin, E. S., and Hikosaka, O. (2009). Midbrain dopamine neurons signal preference for advance information about upcoming rewards. Neuron 63, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruxel, E. M., Akutagava-Martins, G. C., Salatino-Oliveira, A., Contini, V., Kieling, C., Hutz, M. H., et al. (2014). ADHD pharmacogenetics across the life cycle: New findings and perspectives. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 165, 263–282. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruxel, E. M., Salatino-Oliveira, A., Akutagava-Martins, G. C., Tovo-Rodrigues, L., Genro, J. P., Zeni, C. P., et al. (2015). LPHN 3 and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A susceptibility and pharmacogenetic study. Genes Brain Behav. 14, 419–427. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, P. B., Rohrich, R. J., and Chung, K. C. (2011). The levels of evidence and their role in evidence-based medicine. Plast. Reconstruct. Surg. 128:305. doi: 10.1097/PRS.0b013e318219c171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G. (2011). Cingulate, frontal, and parietal cortical dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 1160–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G., Frazier, J. A., Rauch, S. L., Seidman, L. J., Whalen, P. J., and Jenike, M. A. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting stroop. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 1542–1552. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00083-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bymaster, F. P., Katner, J. S., Nelson, D. L., Hemrick-Luecke, S. K., Threlkeld, P. G., and Heiligenstein, J. H. (2002). Atomoxetine increases extracellular levels of norepinephrine and dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: A potential mechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 699–711. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00346-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callahan, B. L., Bierstone, D., Stuss, D. T., and Black, S. E. (2017). Adult ADHD: Risk factor for dementia or phenotypic mimic? Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:260. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Campo, N., Chamberlain, S. R., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 145–157. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Q., Sun, L., Gong, G., Lv, Y., Cao, X., and Shuai, L. (2010). The macrostructural and microstructural abnormalities of corpus callosum in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A combined morphometric and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain Res. 1310, 172–180. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlsson, A. (1959). The occurrence, distribution and physiological role of catecholamines in the nervous system. Pharmacol. Rev. 11, 490–493.

Google Scholar

Carlsson, A. (1993). Thirty years of dopamine research. Adv. Neurol. 60, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Carmona, S., Vilarroya, O., Bielsa, A., Tremols, V., Soliva, J. C., Rovira, M., et al. (2005). Global and regional gray matter reductions in ADHD: A voxel-based morphometric study. Neurosci. Lett. 389, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Nigg, J. T., and Durston, S. (2007). New potential leads in the biology and treatment of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 20, 119–124. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e3280a02f78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., and Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: Beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., and Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 617–628. doi: 10.1038/nrn896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Eckburg, P., Marsh, W. L., Vaituzis, A. C., and Kaysen, D. (1994). Quantitative morpholoy of the caudate nucleus in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 151, 1791–1796. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.12.1791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greenstein, D. K., and Clasen, L. S. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA 288, 1740–1748. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.14.1740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Margulies, D. S., Kelly, C., Uddin, L. Q., Ghaffari, M., and Kirsch, A. (2008). Cingulate-precuneus interactions: A new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 332–337. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Milham, M. P., and Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalá-López, F., Hutton, B., Núñez-Beltrán, A., Page, M. J., Ridao, M., Macías Saint-Gerons, D., et al. (2017). The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review with network meta-analyses of randomised trials. PLoS One 12:0180355. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caye, A., Rocha, T. B. M., Anselmi, L., Murray, J., Menezes, A. M., Barros, F. C., et al. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: Evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 705–712. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caye, A., Swanson, J. M., Coghill, D., and Rohde, L. A. (2019). Treatment strategies for ADHD: An evidence-based guide to select optimal treatment. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 390–408. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0116-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] (2005). Mental health in the United States. Prevalence of diagnosis and medication treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder–United States, 2003. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 54, 842–847.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chan, E., Fogler, J. M., and Hammerness, P. G. (2016). Treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents: A systematic review. JAMA 315, 1997–2008. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.5453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Z., Ghirardi, L., Quinn, P. D., Asherson, P., D’Onofrio, B. M., and Larsson, H. (2019). Risks and benefits of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medication on behavioral and neuropsychiatric outcomes: A qualitative review of pharmacoepidemiology studies using linked prescription databases. Biol. Psychiatry 86, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, T.-C., Chou, M.-C., Yang, P., Chung, H.-W., and Wu, M.-T. (2009). Effects of interpolation methods in spatial normalization of diffusion tensor imaging data on group comparison of fractional anisotropy. Magn. Reson. Imaging 27, 681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2008.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, J. Y., Chen, R. Y., Ko, J. S., and Ng, E. M. (2007). Efficacy and safety of atomoxetine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents—meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Psychopharmacology 194, 197–209. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0840-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherkasova, M. V., Faridi, N., Casey, K. F., O’driscoll, G. A., Hechtman, L., Joober, R., et al. (2014). Amphetamine-induced dopamine release and neurocognitive function in treatment-naive adults with ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 1498–1507. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, L., and Reith, M. E. (2003). Substrate-induced trafficking of the dopamine transporter in heterologously expressing cells and in rat striatal synaptosomal preparations. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 307, 729–736. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.055095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, T. L., Chia, S., Shang, C. Y., and Gau, S. S. F. (2015). Differential therapeutic effects of 12-week treatment of atomoxetine and methylphenidate on drug-naive children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A counting Stroop functional MRI study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 2300–2310. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christakou, A., Murphy, C. M., Chantiluke, K., Cubillo, A. I., and Smith, A. B. (2013). Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during sustained attention in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD. and with autism. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 236–244. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cinnamon Bidwell, L., Dew, R. E., and Kollins, S. H. (2010). Alpha-2 adrenergic receptors and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 12, 366–373. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0136-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemow, D. B., and Walker, D. J. (2014). The potential for misuse and abuse of medications in ADHD: A review. Postgrad. Med. 126, 64–81. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2014.09.2801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colby, J. B., Rudie, J. D., Brown, J. A., Douglas, P. K., Cohen, M. S., and Shehzad, Z. (2012). Insights into multimodal imaging classification of ADHD. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:59. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Consortium, T. B., Anttila, V., Bulik-Sullivan, B., Finucane, H. K., Walters, R. K., Bras, J., et al. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360:8757. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corman, S. L., Fedutes, B. A., and Culley, C. M. (2004). Atomoxetine: The first nonstimulant for the management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Health Syst. Pharm. 61, 2391–2399. doi: 10.1093/ajhp/61.22.2391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Adamo, N., Del Giovane, C., Mohr-Jensen, C., Hayes, A. J., Carucci, S., et al. (2018). Comparative efficacy and tolerability of medications for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 727–738. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30269-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Imperati, D., Zhou, J., Proal, E., Klein, R. G., and Mannuzza, S. (2013). White matter alterations at 33-year follow-up in adults with childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 591–598. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, S. G., Davies, G., Schibuk, L., Weiss, M. D., and Hechtman, L. (2015). Long-term effects of stimulant treatment for ADHD: What can we tell our patients? Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s40474-015-0039-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Smith, A., Taylor, E., and Rubia, K. (2012). A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex 48, 194–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunill, R., Castells, X., Tobias, A., and Capellà, D. (2016). Efficacy, safety and variability in pharmacotherapy for adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis and meta-regression in over 9000 patients. Psychopharmacology 233, 187–197. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4099-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curatolo, P., D’Agati, E., and Moavero, R. (2010). The neurobiological basis of ADHD. Ital. J. Pediatr. 36:79. doi: 10.1186/1824-7288-36-79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curet, O., Dennis, T., and Scatton, B. (1987). Evidence for the involvement of presynaptic alpha-2 adrenoceptors in the regulation of norepinephrine metabolism in the rat brain. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 240, 327–336.

Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Fryer, T. D., Brichard, L., Robinson, E. S. J., Theobald, D. E. H., and Laane, K. (2007). Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315, 1267–1270. doi: 10.1126/science.1137073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daw, N. D., and Tobler, P. N. (2014). “Value learning through reinforcement,” in Neuroeconomics, eds P. W. Glimcher and E. Fehr (London: Elsevier), 283–298. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-416008-8.00015-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., and Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans’. Choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69, 1204–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., and Daw, N. D. (2008). Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 429–453. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.4.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., and Yu, A. J. (2006). Phasic norepinephrine: A neural interrupt signal for unexpected events. Netw. Comput. Neural Syst. 17, 335–350. doi: 10.1080/09548980601004024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Crescenzo, F., Cortese, S., Adamo, N., and Janiri, L. (2017). Pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of adults with ADHD: A meta-review. Evid. Based Ment. Health 20, 4–11. doi: 10.1136/eb-2016-102415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., et al. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 51, 63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depue, B. E., Burgess, G. C., Bidwell, L. C., Willcutt, E. G., and Banich, M. T. (2010). Behavioral performance predicts grey matter reductions in the right inferior frontal gyrus in young adults with combined type ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 182, 231–237. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devauges, V., and Sara, S. J. (1990). Activation of the noradrenergic system facilitates an attentional shift in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 39, 19–28. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90118-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, Y.-S., Gatley, S. J., Thanos, P. K., Shea, C., Garza, V., Xu, Y., et al. (2004). Brain kinetics of methylphenidate (Ritalin) enantiomers after oral administration. Synapse 53, 168–175. doi: 10.1002/syn.20046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, Y.-S., Naganawa, M., Gallezot, J.-D., Nabulsi, N., Lin, S.-F., and Ropchan, J. (2014). Clinical doses of atomoxetine significantly occupy both norepinephrine and serotonin transports: Implications on treatment of depression and ADHD. Neuroimage 86, 164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dirlikov, B., Rosch, K. S., Crocetti, D., Denckla, M. B., Mahone, E. M., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Distinct frontal lobe morphology in girls and boys with ADHD. Neuroimage Clin. 7, 222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donfrancesco, R., Trani, M., Porfirio, M. C., Giana, G., Miano, S., and Andriola, E. (2015). Might the temperament be a bias in clinical study on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)?: Novelty seeking dimension as a core feature of ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 227, 333–338. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, P. K., Gutman, B., Anderson, A., Larios, C., Lawrence, K. E., and Narr, K. (2018). Hemispheric brain asymmetry differences in youths with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 18, 744–752. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dramsdahl, M., Westerhausen, R., Haavik, J., Hugdahl, K., and Plessen, K. J. (2012). Adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder — A diffusion-tensor imaging study of the corpus callosum. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 201, 168–173. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dresel, S., Krause, J., Krause, K. H., LaFougere, C., Brinkbäumer, K., and Kung, H. F. (2000). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Binding of [99mTc]TRODAT-1 to the dopamine transporter before and after methylphenidate treatment. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. 27, 1518–1524. doi: 10.1007/s002590000330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ducharme, S., Hudziak, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Albaugh, M. D., Nguyen, T. V., Karama, S., et al. (2012). Decreased regional cortical thickness and thinning rate are associated with inattention symptoms in healthy children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durell, T. M., Adler, L. A., Williams, D. W., Deldar, A., McGough, J. J., Glaser, P. E., et al. (2013). Atomoxetine treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adults with assessment of functional outcomes: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 33, 45–54. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31827d8a23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutta, C. (2020). Brain interhemispheric alterations in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder using structural neuroimaging features. Ph.D. thesis. University of Central Florida, 497.

Google Scholar

van Dyck, C. H., Seibyl, J. P., Malison, R. T., Laruelle, M., Zoghbi, S. S., and Baldwin, R. M. (2002). Age-related decline in dopamine transporters: Analysis of striatal subregions, nonlinear effects, and hemispheric asymmetries. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 10, 36–43. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200201000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebitz, R. B., and Platt, M. L. (2015). Neuronal activity in primate dorsal anterior cingulate cortex signals task conflict and predicts adjustments in pupil-linked arousal. Neuron 85, 628–640. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eldar, E., Cohen, J. D., and Niv, Y. (2013). The effects of neural gain on attention and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1146–1153. doi: 10.1038/nn.3428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eldar, E., Niv, Y., and Cohen, J. D. (2016). Do you see the forest or the tree? Neural gain and breadth versus focus in perceptual processing. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1632–1643. doi: 10.1177/0956797616665578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elia, J., Borcherding, B. G., Rapoport, J. L., and Keysor, C. S. (1991). Methylphenidate and dextroamphetamine treatments of hyperactivity: Are there true nonresponders? Psychiatry Res. 36, 141–155. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90126-A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elkins, I. J., McGue, M., and Iacono, W. G. (2007). Prospective effects of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, and sex on adolescent substance use and abuse. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:1145. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison-Wright, I., Ellison-Wright, Z., and Bullmore, E. (2008). Structural brain change in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder identified by meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 8:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsayed, N. A., Yamamoto, K. M., and Froehlich, T. E. (2020). Genetic influence on efficacy of pharmacotherapy for pediatric attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Overview and current status of research. CNS Drugs 34, 389–414. doi: 10.1007/s40263-020-00702-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emond, V., Joyal, C., and Poissant, H. (2009). Structural and functional neuroanatomy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD. Encephale 35, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2008.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ewijk, H., Heslenfeld, D. J., Zwiers, M. P., Buitelaar, J. K., and Oosterlaan, J. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1093–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Bruno, K. J., and Hess, E. J. (2011). “Rodent models of ADHD,” in Behavioral neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment, eds C. Stanford and R. Tannock (Berlin: Springer), 273–300. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V. (2018). The pharmacology of amphetamine and methylphenidate: Relevance to the neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87, 255–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Buitelaar, J. (2010). Comparing the efficacy of stimulants for ADHD in children and adolescents using meta-analysis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 19, 353–364. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0054-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Glatt, S. J. (2009). A comparison of the efficacy of medications for adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using meta-analysis of effect sizes. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71:3475. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04902pur

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Larsson, H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 562–575. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Mick, E. (2010). Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 33, 159–180. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2009.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J. K., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., et al. (2015). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 1, 1–23.

Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., and Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol. Med. 36, 159–165. doi: 10.1017/S003329170500471X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fassbender, C., Zhang, H., Buzy, W. M., Cortes, C. R., Mizuiri, D., and Beckett, L. (2009). A lack of default network suppession is linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain Res. 1273, 114–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fayyad, J., Graaf, R., Kessler, R., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Demyttenaere, K., et al. (2007). Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 190, 402–409. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federici, M., Geracitano, R., Bernardi, G., and Mercuri, N. B. (2005). Actions of methylphenidate on dopaminergic neurons of the ventral midbrain. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 361–365. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedorov, A., Beichel, R., Kalpathy-Cramer, J., Finet, J., Fillion-Robin, J.-C., and Pujol, S. (2012). 3D Slicer as an image computing platform for the quantitative imaging network. Magn. Reson. Imaging 30, 1323–1341. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2012.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Femminella, G. D., Thayanandan, T., Calsolaro, V., Komici, K., Rengo, G., Corbi, G., et al. (2018). Imaging and molecular mechanisms of Alzheimer’s disease: A review. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 19:3702. doi: 10.3390/ijms19123702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., and Fletcher, K. (2002). Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: Self-reported psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of childhood conduct problems and teen CD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 30, 463–475. doi: 10.1023/A:1019864813776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fluegge, K., and Fluegge, K. (2018). Antecedent ADHD, dementia, and metabolic dysregulation: A U.S. based cohort analysis. Neurochem. Int. 112, 255–258. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2017.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forde, N. J., Ronan, L., and Zwiers, M. P. (2017). No association between cortical gyrification or intrinsic curvature and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents and young adults. Front. Neurosci. 11:218. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. D., Pananusorn, B., Cervinski, M. A., Holden, H. E., and Vaughan, R. A. (2003). Dopamine transporters are dephosphorylated in striatal homogenates and in vitro by protein phosphatase 1. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 110, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/S0169-328X(02)00645-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, M. J., Santamaria, A., O’Reilly, R. C., and Willcutt, E. (2007a). Testing computational models of dopamine and noradrenaline dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 1583–1599. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, M. J., Scheres, A., and Sherman, S. J. (2007b). Understanding decision-making deficits in neurological conditions: Insights from models of natural action selection. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 362, 1641–1654. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredriksen, M., Halmøy, A., Faraone, S. V., and Haavik, J. (2013). Long-term efficacy and safety of treatment with stimulants and atomoxetine in adult ADHD: A review of controlled and naturalistic studies. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 508–527. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, L. A., and Rapoport, J. L. (2015). Brain development in ADHD. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 30, 106–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Tononi, G., Reeke, G. N. Jr., Sporns, O., and Edelman, G. M. (1994). Value-dependent selection in the brain: Simulation in a synthetic neural model. Neuroscience 59, 229–243. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90592-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., FitzGerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P., and Pezzulo, G. (2017). Active inference: A process theory. Neural Comput. 29, 1–49. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Rigoli, F., Ognibene, D., Mathys, C., Fitzgerald, T., and Pezzulo, G. (2015). Active inference and epistemic value. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 187–214. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1020053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Moutoussis, M., Behrens, T., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). The anatomy of choice: Dopamine and decision-making. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130481. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodl, T., and Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 125, 114–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodl, T., Stauber, J., Schaaff, N., Koutsouleris, N., Scheuerecker, J., and Ewers, M. (2010). Amygdala reduction in patients with ADHD compared with major depression and healthy volunteers. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 121, 111–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01489.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujishima, M., Maikusa, N., Nakamura, K., Nakatsuka, M., Matsuda, H., and Meguro, K. (2014). Mild cognitive impairment, poor episodic memory, and late-life depression are associated with cerebral cortical thinning and increased white matter hyperintensities. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:306. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukui, R., Svenningsson, P., Matsuishi, T., Higashi, H., Nairn, A. C., Greengard, P., et al. (2003). Effect of methylphenidate on dopamine/DARPP signalling in adult, but not young, mice: Effect of methylphenidate on DARPP-32. J. Neurochem. 87, 1391–1401. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.02101.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furman, C. A., Chen, R., Guptaroy, B., Zhang, M., Holz, R. W., and Gnegy, M. (2009). Dopamine and amphetamine rapidly increase dopamine transporter trafficking to the surface: Live-cell imaging using total internal reflection fluorescence microscopy. J. Neurosci. 29, 3328–3336. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5386-08.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusar-Poli, P., Rubia, K., Rossi, G., Sartori, G., and Balottin, U. (2012). Striatal dopamine transporter alterations in ADHD: Pathophysiology or adaptation to psychostimulants? A meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 264–272. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11060940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabay, S., Pertzov, Y., and Henik, A. (2011). Orienting of attention, pupil size, and the norepinephrine system. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 123–129. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0015-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, M. P. H., Schoenbaum, G., and Gershman, S. J. (2018). Rethinking dopamine as generalized prediction error. Proc. Biol. Sci. 285:20181645. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.1645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Gee, J. W., Colizoli, O., Kloosterman, N. A., Knapen, T., Nieuwenhuis, S., and Donner, T. H. (2017). Dynamic modulation of decision biases by brainstem arousal systems. Elife 6:23232. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehricke, J. G., Kruggel, F., Thampipop, T., Alejo, S. D., Tatos, E., Fallon, J., et al. (2017). The brain anatomy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adults–a magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One 12:e0175433. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerfen, C. R., and Surmeier, D. J. (2011). Modulation of striatal projection systems by Dopamine. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 441–466. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113641

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gershman, S. J., Pesaran, B., and Daw, N. D. (2009). Human reinforcement learning subdivides structured action spaces by learning effector-specific values. J. Neurosci. 29, 13524–13531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2469-09.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giambalvo, C. T. (2003). Differential effects of amphetamine transport vs. dopamine reverse transport on particulate PKC activity in striatal synaptoneurosomes. Synapse 49, 125–133. doi: 10.1002/syn.10223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gill, K. E., Pierre, P. J., Daunais, J., Bennett, A. J., Martelle, S., Gage, H. D., et al. (2012). Chronic treatment with extended release methylphenidate does not alter dopamine systems or increase vulnerability for cocaine self-administration: A study in nonhuman primates. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2555–2565. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gläscher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron 66, 585–595. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, D., Oroszi, G., and Ducci, F. (2005). The genetics of addictions: Uncovering the genes. Nat. Rev. Genet. 6, 521–532. doi: 10.1038/nrg1635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhill, L. L., Pliszka, S., Dulcan, M. K., Bernet, W., Arnold, V., and Beitchman, J. (2002). Practice parameter for the use of stimulant medications in the treatment of children, adolescents, and adults. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 26–49. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200202001-00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greven, C. U., Bralten, J., Mennes, M., O’Dwyer, L., Hulzen, K. J., Rommelse, N., et al. (2015). Developmentally stable whole-brain volume reductions and developmentally sensitive caudate and putamen volume alterations in those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 490–499. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grijalba, B., Callado, L. F., Javier Meana, J., García-Sevilla, J. A., and Pazos, A. (1996). Alpha 2-adrenoceptor subtypes in the human brain: A pharmacological delineation of [3H]RX-821002 binding to membranes and tissue sections. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 310, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(96)00381-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Group, M. T. A. C. (2004). National institute of mental health multimodal treatment study of ADHD follow-up: 24-month outcomes of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 113, 754–761. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulaboski, R., Cordeiro, M. N. D., Milhazes, N., Garrido, J., Borges, F., Jorge, M., et al. (2007). Evaluation of the lipophilic properties of opioids, amphetamine-like drugs, and metabolites through electrochemical studies at the interface between two immiscible solutions. Anal. Biochem. 361, 236–243. doi: 10.1016/j.ab.2006.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurden, H., Takita, M., and Jay, T. M. (2000). Essential role of D1 but not D2 receptors in the NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation at hippocampal-prefrontal cortex synapses in vivo. J. Neurosci. 20:106. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-j0003.2000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyenet, P. G., Stornetta, R. L., Riley, T., Norton, F. R., Rosin, D. L., and Lynch, K. R. (1994). Alpha2A-adrenergic receptors are present in lower brainstem catecholaminergic and serotonergic neurons innervating spinal cord. Brain Res. 638, 285–294. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90661-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamid, A. A., Frank, M. J., and Moore, C. I. (2019). Dopamine waves as a mechanism for spatiotemporal credit assignment. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/729640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, L. S., Levitt, J. G., O’Neill, J., Alger, J. R., Luders, E., and Phillips, O. R. (2008). Reduced white matter integrity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroreport 19, 1705–1708. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283174415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammerness, P., McCarthy, K., Mancuso, E., Gendron, C., and Geller, D. (2009). Atomoxetine for the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: A review. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 5:215. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S3896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauser, T. U., Fiore, V. G., Moutoussis, M., and Dolan, R. J. (2016). Computational psychiatry of ADHD: Neural gain impairments across marrian levels of analysis. Trends Neurosci. 39, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Ball, J., Mathys, C., Brandeis, D., Walitza, S., et al. (2014). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in impaired decision making in juvenile attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 71:1165. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heal, D. J., and Pierce, D. M. (2006). Methylphenidate and its isomers: Their role in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder using a transdermal delivery system. CNS Drugs 20, 713–738. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200620090-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-López, S., Bargas, J., Surmeier, D. J., Reyes, A., and Galarraga, E. (1997). D1 receptor activation enhances evoked discharge in neostriatal medium spiny neurons by modulating an L-type Ca2+ conductance. J. Neurosci. 17, 3334–3342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-09-03334.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-López, S., Tkatch, T., Perez-Garci, E., Galarraga, E., Bargas, J., Hamm, H., et al. (2000). D2 dopamine receptors in striatal medium spiny neurons reduce L-type Ca2+ currents and excitability via a novel PLC[beta]1-IP3-calcineurin-signaling cascade. J. Neurosci. 20, 8987–8995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-08987.2000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 4, 267–272. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The cochrane collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2019). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/9781119536604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirota, T., Schwartz, S., and Correll, C. U. (2014). Alpha-2 agonists for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis of monotherapy and add-on trials to stimulant therapy. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 153–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoekzema, E., Carmona, S., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Canals, C., Moreno, A., Fernández, V. R., et al. (2014). Stimulant drugs trigger transient volumetric changes in the human ventral striatum. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 23–34. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0481-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D. P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., and Schweren, L. S. J. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: A cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 4, 310–319. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogman, M., Muetzel, R., Guimaraes, J. P., Shumskaya, E., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., et al. (2019). Brain imaging of the cortex in ADHD: A coordinated analysis of large-scale clinical and population-based samples. Am. J. Psychiatry 176, 531–542. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18091033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hossmann, V., Maling, T. J., Hamilton, C. A., Reid, J. L., and Dollery, C. T. (1980). Sedative and cardiovascular effects of clonidine and nitrazepam. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 28, 167–176. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1980.146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, L. L., and Kimmel, H. L. (2008). Monoamine transporters and psychostimulant addiction. Biochem. Pharmacol. 75, 196–217. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howes, O. D., Kambeitz, J., Kim, E., Stahl, D., Slifstein, M., Abi-Dargham, A., et al. (2012). The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment: Meta-analysis of imaging studies. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 776–786. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., and Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 360–371. doi: 10.1037/a0027205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubbard, J. W., Srinivas, N. R., Quinn, D., and Midha, K. K. (1989). Enantioselective aspects of the disposition of dl-threo-methylphenidate after the administration of a sustained-release formulation to children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. J. Pharm. Sci. 78, 944–947. doi: 10.1002/jps.2600781113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huecker, M. R., Smiley, A., and Saadabadi, A. (2021). “Bupropion,” in StatPearls, (Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing).

Google Scholar

Hutchison, S. L., Ghuman, J. K., Ghuman, H. S., Karpov, I., and Schuster, J. M. (2016). Efficacy of atomoxetine in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in patients with common comorbidities in children, adolescents and adults: A review. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 6, 317–334. doi: 10.1177/2045125316647686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nho, K., Risacher, S. L., Crane, P. K., DeCarli, C., Glymour, M. M., Habeck, C., et al. (2012). Voxel and surface-based topography of memory and executive deficits in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav. 6, 551–567. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9203-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanchak, N., Fletcher, K., and Jicha, G. A. (2012). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in older adults: Prevalence and possible connections to mild cognitive impairment. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 14, 552–560. doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0305-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanov, I., Bansal, R., Hao, X., Zhu, H., Kellendonk, C., and Miller, L. (2010). Morphological abnormalities of the thalamus in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 397–408. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09030398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanov, I., Murrough, J. W., Bansal, R., Hao, X., and Peterson, B. S. (2014). Cerebellar morphology and the effects of stimulant medications in youths with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 718–726. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iversen, L. (2009). Neurotransmitter transporters and their impact on the development of psychopharmacology: Neurotransmitter transporters. Br. J. Pharmacol. 147, 82–88. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0706428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasinski, D. R., and Krishnan, S. (2009). Abuse liability and safety of oral lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in individuals with a history of stimulant abuse. J. Psychopharmacol. 23, 419–427. doi: 10.1177/0269881109103113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauhar, S., Nour, M. M., Veronese, M., Rogdaki, M., Bonoldi, I., Azis, M., et al. (2017). A test of the transdiagnostic dopamine hypothesis of psychosis using positron emission tomographic imaging in bipolar affective disorder and schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 74:1206. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jepma, M., and Nieuwenhuis, S. (2011). Pupil diameter predicts changes in the exploration–exploitation trade-off: Evidence for the adaptive gain theory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1587–1596. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., Van Zijl, P. C., Kim, J., Pearlson, G. D., and Mori, S. (2006). DtiStudio: Resource program for diffusion tensor computation and fiber bundle tracking. Comput. Methods Progr. Biomed. 81, 106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2005.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, C. E., and Jones, S. R. (2007). Voltammetric characterization of the effect of monoamine uptake inhibitors and releasers on dopamine and serotonin uptake in mouse caudate-putamen and substantia nigra slices. Neuropharmacology 52, 1596–1605. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, L. A., Guptaroy, B., Lund, D., Shamban, S., and Gnegy, M. E. (2005a). Regulation of amphetamine-stimulated dopamine efflux by protein kinase C. J. Biol. Chem. 280, 10914–10919. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M413887200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, L. A., Furman, C. A., Zhang, M., Guptaroy, B., and Gnegy, M. E. (2005b). Rapid delivery of the dopamine transporter to the plasmalemmal membrane upon amphetamine stimulation. Neuropharmacology 49, 750–758. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2005.08.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, A., Ayyagari, R., Xie, M., Cai, S., Xie, J., Huss, M., et al. (2017). Comparative efficacy and safety of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder pharmacotherapies, including guanfacine extended release: A mixed treatment comparison. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 875–897. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0962-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, S., Li, Y., Kalwani, R. M., and Gold, J. I. (2016). Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron 89, 221–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jucaite, A., Fernell, E., Halldin, C., Forssberg, H., and Farde, L. (2005). Reduced midbrain dopamine transporter binding in male adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Association between striatal dopamine markers and motor hyperactivity. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadesjö, B., and Gillberg, C. (2001). The comorbidity of ADHD in the general population of Swedish school-age children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 42, 487–492. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahlig, K. M., and Galli, A. (2003). Regulation of dopamine transporter function and plasma membrane expression by dopamine, amphetamine, and cocaine. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 479, 153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2003.08.065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantor, L., and Gnegy, M. E. (1998). Protein kinase C inhibitors block amphetamine-mediated dopamine release in rat striatal slices. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 284, 592–598.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Karlsson, M. P., Tervo, D. G. R., and Karpova, A. Y. (2012). Network resets in medial prefrontal cortex mark the onset of behavioral uncertainty. Science 338, 135–139. doi: 10.1126/science.1226518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawaura, K., Karasawa, J., Chaki, S., and Hikichi, H. (2014). Stimulation of postsynapse adrenergic α2A receptor improves attention/cognition performance in an animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 270, 349–356. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kebabian, J. W. (1978). Multiple classes of dopamine receptors in mammalian central nervous system: The involvement of dopamine-sensitive adenylyl cyclase. Life Sci. 23, 479–483. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(78)90157-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kehoe, E. G., McNulty, J. P., Mullins, P. G., and Bokde, A. L. W. (2014). Advances in MRI biomarkers for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Biomark. Med. 8, 1151–1169. doi: 10.2217/bmm.14.42

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Am. J. Psychiatry 163:716. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, S. R., Berendt, R. T., Ellison, C. D., Ciavarella, A. B., Asafu-Adjaye, E., Khan, M. A., et al. (2016). Bupropion hydrochloride. Profiles Drug Substances Excipients Relat. Methodol. 41, 1–30. doi: 10.1016/bs.podrm.2015.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khelashvili, G., Doktorova, M., Sahai, M. A., Johner, N., Shi, L., and Weinstein, H. (2015). Computational modeling of the N-terminus of the human dopamine transporter and its interaction with PIP 2 -containing membranes: Modeling of DAT N-Terminus/PIP 2 Interactions. Proteins Struct. Funct. Bioinf. 83, 952–969. doi: 10.1002/prot.24792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khoshbouei, H., Sen, N., Guptaroy, B., Johnson, L., Lund, D., Gnegy, M. E., et al. (2004). N-terminal phosphorylation of the dopamine transporter is required for amphetamine-induced efflux. PLoS Biol. 2:78. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, L. J., Bilkey, T. S., Katzman, M. A., and Chokka, P. (2012). Comorbid attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorder: Treatment considerations. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 5, 190–198. doi: 10.2174/1874473711205030190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knackstedt, L. A. (2013). Biological research on addiction: Chapter 56. Neuropharmacology of cocaine and amphetamine. London: Elsevier Inc. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-398335-0.00056-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobayashi, T., Washiyama, K., and Ikeda, K. (2010). Inhibition of G-protein-activated inwardly rectifying K+ channels by the selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors atomoxetine and reboxetine. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1560–1569. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobel, M., Bechtel, N., Specht, K., Klarhöfer, M., Weber, P., and Scheffler, K. (2010). Structural and functional imaging approaches in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Does the temporal lobe play a key role? Psychiatry Res. 183, 230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.03.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., et al. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 795–811. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konrad, A., Dielentheis, T. F., El Masri, D., Bayerl, M., Fehr, C., and Gesierich, T. (2010). Disturbed structural connectivity is related to inattention and impulsivity in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 912–919. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07110.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konrad, K., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 904–916. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kooij, J. J. S., Bijlenga, D., Salerno, L., Jaeschke, R., Bitter, I., Balazs, J., et al. (2019). Updated European Consensus Statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD. Eur. Psychiatry 56, 14–34. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kooij, S. J., Bejerot, S., Blackwell, A., Caci, H., Casas-Brugué, M., Carpentier, P. J., et al. (2010). European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European network adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry 10:67. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-67

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornfield, R., Watson, S., Higashi, A. S., Conti, R. M., Dusetzina, S. B., and Garfield, C. F. (2013). Effects of FDA advisories on the pharmacologic treatment of ADHD, 2004–2008. Psychiatr. Serv. 64, 339–346. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201200147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koss, M. C. (1986). Pupillary dilation as an index of central nervous system α2-adrenoceptor activation. J. Pharmacol. Methods 15, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0160-5402(86)90002-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, K. H., Dresel, S. H., Krause, J., Kung, H. F., and Tatsch, K. (2000). Increased striatal dopamine transporter in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Effects of methylphenidate as measured by single photon emission computed tomography. Neurosci. Lett. 285, 107–110. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01040-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuczenski, R., and Segal, D. S. (1997). Effects of methylphenidate on extracellular dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine: Comparison with amphetamine. J. Neurochem. 68, 2032–2037. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1997.68052032.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurth-Nelson, Z., and Redish, A. D. (2009). Temporal-difference reinforcement learning with distributed representations. PLoS One 4:7362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammel, S., Lim, B. K., Ran, C., Huang, K. W., Betley, M. J., Tye, K. M., et al. (2012). Input-specific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental area. Nature 491, 212–217. doi: 10.1038/nature11527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larisch, R., Sitte, W., Antke, C., Nikolaus, S., Franz, M., and Tress, W. (2006). Striatal dopamine transporter density in drug naive patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nucl. Med. Commun. 27, 267–270. doi: 10.1097/00006231-200603000-00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, R. S., and Waters, J. (2018). Neuromodulatory correlates of pupil dilation. Front. Neural Circ. 12:21. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufer, M. W., and Denhoff, E. (1957). Hyperkinetic behavior syndrome in children. J. Pediatr. 50, 463–474. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(57)80257-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, K. E., Levitt, J. G., Loo, S. K., Ly, R., Yee, V., and O’Neill, J. (2013). White matter microstructure in subjects with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their siblings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 431–440.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leech, R., and Sharp, D. J. (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137, 12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, D., Ma, J., Du, X., Shen, G., Jin, X., and Gong, Q. (2014). Microstructural abnormalities in the combined and inattentive subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Sci. Rep. 4, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Lesch, K.-P., Timmesfeld, N., Renner, T. J., Halperin, R., Röser, C., and Nguyen, T. T. (2008). Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: Converging evidence from genome-wide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. J. Neural Transm. 115, 1573–1585. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0119-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liston, C., Cohen, M. M., Teslovich, T., Levenson, D., and Casey, B. J. (2011). Atypical prefrontal connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Pathway to disease or pathological end point? Biol. Psychiatry 69, 1168–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llorente, A. M., Voigt, R. G., Jensen, C. L., Berretta, M. C., Kennard Fraley, J., and Heird, W. C. (2006). Performance on a visual sustained attention and discrimination task is associated with urinary excretion of norepineprhine metabolite in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 133–144. doi: 10.1080/13854040490888495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Larson, M. P., King, J. B., Terry, J., McGlade, E. C., and YurgelunTodd, D. (2012). Reduced insular volume in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Res. 204, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.09.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losier, B. J., McGrath, P. J., and Klein, R. M. (1996). Error patterns on the continuous performance test in non-medicated and medicated samples of children with and without ADHD: A meta-analytic review. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 971–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01494.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., Zhang, L., Tang, S., Bu, X., Chen, Y., Hu, X., et al. (2019). Characterization of cortical and subcortical abnormalities in drug-naive boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 250, 397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludolph, A. G., Kassubek, J., Schmeck, K., Glaser, C., Wunderlich, A., and Buck, A. K. (2008). Dopaminergic dysfunction in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), differences between pharmacologically treated and never treated young adults: A 3,4-dihdroxy-6-[18F]fluorophenyl-l-alanine PET study. Neuroimage 41, 718–727. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luis-García, R., Cabús-Piñol, G., Imaz-Roncero, C., Argibay-Quiñones, D., Barrio-Arranz, G., and Aja-Fernández, S. (2015). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and medication with stimulants in young children: A DTI study. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luman, M., Tripp, G., and Scheres, A. (2010). Identifying the neurobiology of altered reinforcement sensitivity in ADHD: A review and research agenda. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 744–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackie, S., Shaw, P., Lenroot, R., Pierson, R., Greenstein, D. K., and Nugent, T. F. (2007). Cerebellar development and clinical outcome in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 647–655. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Biederman, J., Valera, E. M., Bush, G., Kaiser, J., Kennedy, D. N., et al. (2007). Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1364–1375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Buka, S. L., Biederman, J., Papadimitriou, G. M., Hodge, S. M., and Valera, E. M. (2008). Attention and executive systems abnormalities in adults with childhood ADHD: A DT-MRI study of connections. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1210–1220. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manev, H., and Uz, T. (2009). Dosing time-dependent actions of psychostimulants. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 88, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(09)88002-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariani, J. J., and Levin, F. R. (2007). Treatment strategies for co-occurring ADHD and substance use disorders. Am. J. Addict. 16(Suppl. 1), 45–56. doi: 10.1080/10550490601082783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, J. S., and Patrick, K. S. (2008). Differential Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of methylphenidate enantiomers: Does chirality matter? J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 28, 54–61. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181733560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, J. S., DeVane, C. L., Ramamoorthy, S., and Zhu, H.-J. (2009). The psychostimulant d-threo-(R,R)-methylphenidate binds as an agonist to the 5HT(1A. receptor. Pharmazie 64, 123–125.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marr, D. (2010). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Cambridge, MA: MIT press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262514620.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathot, S., Dalmaijer, E., Grainger, J., and Stigchel, S. (2014). The pupillary light response reflects exogenous attention and inhibition of return. J. Vis. 14, 7–7. doi: 10.1167/14.14.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H. (2016). MRI morphometry in Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 30, 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAlonan, G. M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Chua, S. E., Murphy, D. G., Suckling, J., et al. (2007). Mapping brain structure in attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: A voxel-based MRI study of regional grey and white matter volume. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 154, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinty, J. F., Shi, X. D., Schwendt, M., Saylor, A., and Toda, S. (2008). Regulation of psychostimulant-induced signaling and gene expression in the striatum: Regulation of psychostimulant-induced signaling in the striatum. J. Neurochem. 104, 1440–1449. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05240.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGough, J. (2014). ADHD. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

McGough, J. J., and McCracken, J. T. (2000). Assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review of recent literature. Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 12, 319–324. doi: 10.1097/00008480-200008000-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Winter, W., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2014). Widespread reductions in cortical thickness following severe early-life deprivation: A neurodevelopmental pathway to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 629–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mefford, I. N., and Potter, W. Z. (1989). A neuroanatomical and biochemical basis for attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity in children: A defect in tonic adrenaline mediated inhibition of locus coeruleus stimulation. Med. Hypotheses 29, 33–42. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(89)90164-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, G. M. (2011). The emerging role of trace amine-associated receptor 1 in the functional regulation of monoamine transporters and dopaminergic activity: TAAR1 regulation of monoaminergic activity. J. Neurochem. 116, 164–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07109.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Houts, R., Asherson, P., Belsky, D. W., Corcoran, D. L., Hammerle, M., et al. (2015). Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 967–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohan, A., Roberto, A. J., Mohan, A., Lorenzo, A., Jones, K., Carney, M. J., et al. (2016). The significance of the default mode network (DMN. neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders: A review. Yale J. Biol. Med. 89, 49–57.

Google Scholar

Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molina, B. S. G., Hinshaw, S. P., Eugene Arnold, L., Swanson, J. M., Pelham, W. E., and Hechtman, L. (2013). Adolescent substance use in the multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (MTA) as a function of childhood ADHD, random assignment to childhood treatments, and subsequent medication. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 250–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, G. H., Hause, S., Rüther, E., Rothenberger, A., and Huether, G. (2001). Early methylphenidate administration to young rats causes a persistent reduction in the density of striatal dopamine transporters. J. Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 11, 15–24. doi: 10.1089/104454601750143366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montague, P. R., Hyman, S. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Computational roles for dopamine in behavioural control. Nature 431, 760–767. doi: 10.1038/nature03015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, L. V., Ongur, D., Hsu, J., Castro, V. M., Perlis, R. H., and Schneeweiss, S. (2019). Psychosis with methylphenidate or amphetamine in patients with ADHD. N. Engl. J. Med. 380, 1128–1138. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1813751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morón, J. A., Brockington, A., Wise, R. A., Rocha, B. A., and Hope, B. T. (2002). Dopamine uptake through the norepinephrine transporter in brain regions with low levels of the dopamine transporter: Evidence from knock-out mouse lines. J. Neurosci. 22, 389–395. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-02-00389.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortensen, O. V., and Amara, S. G. (2003). Dynamic regulation of the dopamine transporter. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 479, 159–170. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2003.08.066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moser, M.-B., Rowland, D. C., and Moser, E. I. (2015). Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:021808. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mostofsky, S. H., Reiss, A. L., Lockhart, P., and Denckla, M. B. (1998). Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Neurol. 13, 434–439. doi: 10.1177/088307389801300904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mous, S. E., Karatekin, C., Kao, C. Y., Gottesman, I. I., Posthuma, D., and White, T. (2014). Gyrification differences in children and adolescents with velocardiofacial syndrome and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 221, 169–171. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mowinckel, A. M., and Vidal-Piñeiro, D. (2020). Visualization of brain statistics with R packages ggseg and ggseg3d. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 3, 466–483. doi: 10.1177/2515245920928009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muehllehner, G., and Karp, J. S. (2006). Positron emission tomography. Phys. Med. Biol. 51, 117–137. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/13/R08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, P. R., O’Connell, R. G., O’Sullivan, M., Robertson, I. H., and Balsters, J. H. (2014). Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4140–4154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myer, N. M., Boland, J. R., and Faraone, S. V. (2018). Pharmacogenetics predictors of methylphenidate efficacy in childhood ADHD. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1929–1936. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagel, B. J., Bathula, D., Herting, M., Schmitt, C., Kroenke, C. D., and Fair, D. (2011). Altered white matter microstructure in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 50, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakao, T., Radua, J., Rubia, K., and Mataix-Cols, D. (2011). Gray matter volume abnormalities in ADHD: Voxel-based meta-analysis exploring the effects of age and stimulant medication. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 1154–1163. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narr, K. L., Woods, R. P., Lin, J., Kim, J., Phillips, O. R., and Del’Homme, M. (2009). Widespread cortical thinning is a robust anatomical marker for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48, 1014–1022. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b395c0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, Q. X. (2017). A systematic review of the use of bupropion for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 27, 112–116. doi: 10.1089/cap.2016.0124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus—norepinephrine system. Psychol. Bull. 131, 510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C., Lukito, S., Hart, H., Mataix-Cols, D., and Radua, J. (2016). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73:815. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304, 452–454. doi: 10.1126/science.1094285

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Reilly, R. C., and Frank, M. J. (2006). Making working memory work: A computational model of learning in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Neural Comput. 18, 283–328. doi: 10.1162/089976606775093909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Offermans, S., and Rosenthal, W. (2004). Encyclopedic reference of molecular pharmacology. Berlin: Springer.

Google Scholar

Olds, J., and Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47, 419–427. doi: 10.1037/h0058775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olfson, M., Blanco, C., Wang, S., and Greenhill, L. L. (2013). Trends in office-based treatment of adults with stimulants in the United States. J. Clin. Psychiatry 74, 43–50. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m07975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özgen, H., Spijkerman, R., Noack, M., Holtmann, M., Schellekens, A. S., Van De Glind, G., et al. (2020). International consensus statement for the screening, diagnosis, and treatment of adolescents with concurrent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorder. Eur. Addict. Res. 26, 223–232. doi: 10.1159/000508385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagerols, M., Richarte, V., Sánchez-Mora, C., Rovira, P., SOLER Artigas, M., Garcia-Martínez, I., et al. (2018). Integrative genomic analysis of methylphenidate response in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20194-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagoni, P., Dardani, C., Leppert, B., Korologou-Linden, R., Smith, G. D., Howe, L. D., et al. (2020). Exploring the causal effects of genetic liability to ADHD and autism on Alzheimer’s disease. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.04.15.043380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, T., and Friston, K. J. (2017). Uncertainty, epistemics and active inference. J. R. Soc. Interface 14:20170376. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, K. S., Caldwell, R. W., Ferris, R. M., and Breese, G. R. (1987). Pharmacology of the enantiomers of threo-methylphenidate. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 241, 152–158.

Google Scholar

Patros, C. H. G., Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Tarle, S. J., Lea, S. E., and Hudec, K. L. (2016). Choice-impulsivity in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 43, 162–174. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattij, T., Schetters, D., Schoffelmeer, A. N. M., and Gaalen, M. M. (2012). On the improvement of inhibitory response control and visuospatial attention by indirect and direct adrenoceptor agonists. Psychopharmacology 219, 327–340. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2405-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavuluri, M. N., Yang, S., Kamineni, K., Passarotti, A. M., Srinivasan, G., and Harral, E. M. (2009). Diffusion tensor imaging study of white matter fiber tracts in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 586–593. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penmatsa, A., Wang, K. H., and Gouaux, E. (2013). X-ray structure of dopamine transporter elucidates antidepressant mechanism. Nature 503, 85–90. doi: 10.1038/nature12533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennick, M. (2010). Absorption of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate and its enzymatic conversion to d-amphetamine. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 6:317. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S9749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlov, E., Philipsen, A., Elst, L., Ebert, D., Henning, J., and Maier, S. (2008). Hippocampus and amygdala morphology in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 33, 509–515.

Google Scholar

Peterson, D. J., Ryan, M., Rimrodt, S. L., Cutting, L. E., Denckla, M. B., and Kaufmann, W. E. (2011). Increased regional fractional anisotropy in highly screened attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD. J. Child Neurol. 26, 1296–1302. doi: 10.1177/0883073811405662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plessen, K. J., Bansal, R., Zhu, H., Whiteman, R., Amat, J., and Quackenbush, G. A. (2006). Hippocampus and amygdala morphlogy in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 795–807. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pliszka, S. R., McCRACKEN, J. T., and Maas, J. W. (1996). Catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Current perspectives. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35, 264–272. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199603000-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pliszka, S. AACAP Work Group on Quality Issues. (2007). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46, 894–921. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318054e724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanczyk, G. V., Salum, G. A., Sugaya, L. S., Caye, A., and Rohde, L. A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 345–365. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanczyk, G. V., Willcutt, E. G., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., and Rohde, L. A. (2014). ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: An updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 43, 434–442. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Postema, M. C., Hoogman, M., Ambrosino, S., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Bandeira, C. E., et al. (2021). Analysis of structural brain asymmetries in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 39 datasets. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 1202–1219. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, A. S., Newhouse, P. A., and Bucci, D. J. (2006). Central nicotinic cholinergic systems: A role in the cognitive dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behav. Brain Res. 175, 201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pretus, C., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Richarte, V., Corrales, M., Picado, M., Carmona, S., et al. (2017). Time and psychostimulants: Opposing long-term structural effects in the adult ADHD brain. A longitudinal MR study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 1238–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purper-Ouakil, D., Ramoz, N., Lepagnol-Bestel, A.-M., Gorwood, P., and Simonneau, M. (2011). Neurobiology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr. Res. 69, 69–76. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212b40f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purves, D.,(2012). Neuroscience, 5th Edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

Google Scholar

Qiu, A., Crocetti, D., Adler, M., Mahone, E. M., Denckla, M. B., Miller, M. I., et al. (2009). Basal ganglia volume and shape in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 74–82. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, M., Ye, Z., Li, Q., Liu, G., Xie, B., and Wang, J. (2011). Changes of brain structure and function in ADHD children. Brain Topogr. 24, 243–252. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0168-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajeh, A., Amanullah, S., Shivakumar, K., and Cole, J. (2017). Interventions in ADHD: A comparative review of stimulant medications and behavioral therapies. Asian J. Psychiatry 25, 131–135. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2016.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajkowski, J., Majczynski, H., Clayton, E., and Aston-Jones, G. (2004). Activation of monkey locus coeruleus neurons varies with difficulty and performance in a target detection task. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 361–371. doi: 10.1152/jn.00673.2003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raman, S. R., Man, K. K. C., Bahmanyar, S., Berard, A., Bilder, S., Boukhris, T., et al. (2018). Trends in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder medication use: A retrospective observational study using population-based databases. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 824–835. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30293-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, R. P. N. (2010). Decision making under uncertainty: A neural model based on partially observable markov decision processes. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4:146. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reale, L., Bartoli, B., Cartabia, M., Zanetti, M., Costantino, M. A., Canevini, M. P., et al. (2017). Comorbidity prevalence and treatment outcome in children and adolescents with ADHD. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 1443–1457. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1005-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Retailleau, A., and Morris, G. (2018). Spatial rule learning and corresponding CA1 place cell reorientation depend on local dopamine release. Curr. Biol. 28, 836–846.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Neto, P., Lou, H. C., Cumming, P., Pryds, O., Karrebaek, H., and Lunding, J. (2005). Methylphenidate-evoked changes I striatal dopamine correlate with inattention and impulsivity in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage 25, 868–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roskell, N. S., Setyawan, J., Zimovetz, E. A., and Hodgkins, P. (2014). Systematic evidence synthesis of treatments for ADHD in children and adolescents: Indirect treatment comparisons of lisdexamfetamine with methylphenidate and atomoxetine. Curr. Med. Res. Opin. 30, 1673–1685. doi: 10.1185/03007995.2014.904772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rostain, A. L. (2008). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: Evidence-based recommendations for management. Postgrad. Med. 120, 27–38. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2008.09.1905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, B. L., and Driscol, J. (2011). Psychoactive drug screening program (PDSP). Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Google Scholar

Rubia, K. (2013). Functional neuroimaging across development: A review. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 22, 719–731. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0291-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Alegría, A. A., and Brinson, H. (2014). Brain abnormalities in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review. Rev. Neurol. 58, 3–16.

Google Scholar

Sales, A. C., Friston, K. J., Jones, M. W., Pickering, A. E., and Moran, R. J. (2019). Locus coeruleus tracking of prediction errors optimises cognitive flexibility: An active inference model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 15:1006267. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sallee, F. R., Lyne, A., Wigal, T., and McGough, J. J. (2009). Long-term safety and efficacy of guanfacine extended release in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 19, 215–226. doi: 10.1089/cap.2008.0080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salo, R., Fassbender, C., Iosif, A.-M., Ursu, S., Leamon, M. H., and Carter, C. (2013). Predictors of methamphetamine psychosis: History of ADHD-relevant childhood behaviors and drug exposure. Psychiatry Res. 210, 529–535. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samea, F., Soluki, S., Nejati, V., Zarei, M., Cortese, S., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2019). Brain alterations in children/adolescents with ADHD revisited: A neuroimaging meta-analysis of 96 structural and functional studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 100, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samson, R. D., Frank, M. J., and Fellous, J.-M. (2010). Computational models of reinforcement learning: The role of dopamine as a reward signal. Cogn. Neurodyn. 4, 91–105. doi: 10.1007/s11571-010-9109-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauer, J.-M., Ring, B. J., and Witcher, J. W. (2005). Clinical pharmacokinetics of atomoxetine. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 44, 571–590. doi: 10.2165/00003088-200544060-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, C., and Limbird, L. E. (1999). Localization and trafficking of alpha2-adrenergic receptor subtypes in cells and tissues. Pharmacol. Ther. 84, 193–205. doi: 10.1016/S0163-7258(99)00032-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayal, K., Prasad, V., Daley, D., Ford, T., and Coghill, D. (2018). ADHD in children and young people: Prevalence, care pathways, and service provision. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 175–186. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30167-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schachter, H. M., King, J., Langford, S., and Moher, D. (2001). How efficacious and safe is short-acting methylphenidate for the treatment of attention-deficit disorder in children and adolescents? A meta-analysis. CMAJ 165, 1475–1488.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schiffer, W. K., Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Alexoff, D. L., Logan, J., and Dewey, S. L. (2006). Therapeutic doses of amphetamine or methylphenidate differentially increase synaptic and extracellular dopamine. Synapse 59, 243–251. doi: 10.1002/syn.20235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnoebelen, S., Semrud-Clikeman, M., and Pliszka, S. R. (2010). Corpus callosum anatomy in chronically treated and stimulant naïve ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 14, 256–266. doi: 10.1177/1087054709356406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (1986). Responses of midbrain dopamine neurons to behavioral trigger stimuli in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 56, 1439–1461. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.5.1439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (2001). Book review: Reward signaling by dopamine neurons. Neuroscientist 7, 293–302. doi: 10.1177/107385840100700406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W., Apicella, P., and Ljungberg, T. (1993). Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. J. Neurosci. 13, 900–913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-00900.1993

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W., Dayan, P., and Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T. H. B., Mathys, C., Dolan, R., and Friston, K. (2015a). The dopaminergic midbrain encodes the expected certainty about desired outcomes. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3434–3445. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T. H. B., Mathys, C., Dolan, R., Kronbichler, M., and Friston, K. (2015b). Evidence for surprise minimization over value maximization in choice behavior. Sci. Rep. 5:16575. doi: 10.1038/srep16575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweren, L. J. S., Hartman, C. A., Zwiers, M. P., Heslenfeld, D. J., van der Meer, D., Franke, B., et al. (2015). Combined stimulant and antipsychotic treatment in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A cross-sectional observational structural MRI study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 959–968. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0645-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, L. J. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 466–485. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., and Makris, N. (2005). Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1263–1272. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Makris, N., Monuteaux, M. C., Boriel, D. L., and Kelkar, K. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1071–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Fine, J. G., Bledsoe, J., and Zhu, D. C. (2017). Regional volumetric differences based on structural MRI in children with two subtypes of ADHD and controls. J. Atten. Disord. 21, 1040–1049. doi: 10.1177/1087054714559642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Pliszka, S. R., Bledsoe, J., and Lancaster, J. (2014). Volumetric MRI differences in treatment naive and chronically treated adolescents with ADHD-combined type. J. Atten. Disord. 18, 511–520. doi: 10.1177/1087054712443158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Pliśzka, S. R., Lancaster, J., and Liotti, M. (2006). Volumetric MRI differences in treatment-naïve vs chronically treated children with ADHD. Neurology 67, 1023–1027. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000237385.84037.3c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servan-Schreiber, D., Printz, H., and Cohen, J. (1990). A network model of catecholamine effects: Gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 249, 892–895. doi: 10.1126/science.2392679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, C. Y., Yan, C. G., Lin, H. Y., Tseng, W. Y., Castellanos, F. X., and Gau, S. S. (2016). Differential effects of methylphenidate and atomoxetine on intrinsic brain activity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol. Med. 46, 3173–3185. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D. E. E. A., et al. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 19649–19654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Gilliam, M., Liverpool, M., Weddle, C., Malek, M., and Sharp, W. (2011). Cortical development in typically developing children with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: Support for a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 143–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Lalonde, F., Lepage, C., Rabin, C., Eckstrand, K., and Sharp, W. (2009a). Development of cortical asymmetry in typically developing children and its disruption in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 888–896. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Sharp, W. S., Morrison, M., Eckstrand, K., Greenstein, D. K., and Clasen, L. S. (2009b). Psychostimulant treatment and the developing cortex in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 58–63. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Greenstein, D., De Rossi, P., and Sharp, W. (2013). Trajectories of cerebral cortical development in childhood and adolescence and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Sharp, W., Evans, A., and Greenstein, D. (2012). Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Rossi, P., Watson, B., Wharton, A., Greenstein, D., Raznahan, A., et al. (2014). Mapping the development of the basal ganglia in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 780–789. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Sudre, G., Wharton, A., Weingart, D., Sharp, W., and Sarlls, J. (2015). White matter microstructure and the variable adult outcome of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 746–754. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, M. H., Mitchell, J. T., and Becker, S. P. (2016). Method of adult diagnosis influences estimated persistence of childhood ADHD: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 1157–1165. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30190-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silk, T. J., Beare, R., and Malpas, C. (2016). Cortical morphometry in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Contribution of thickness and surface area to volume. Cortex 82, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silk, T. J., Vance, A., Rinehart, N., Bradshaw, J. L., and Cunnington, R. (2009b). White-matter abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2757–2765. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silk, T. J., Vance, A., Rinehart, N., Bradshaw, J. L., and Cunnington, R. (2009a). Structural development of the basal ganglia in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res. 172, 220–225. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., and Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 921–929. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smythies, J. (2005). Section II. The dopamine system. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 64, 123–172. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(05)64002-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, S., Atallah, B. V., and Paton, J. J. (2016). Midbrain dopamine neurons control judgment of time. Science 354, 1273–1277. doi: 10.1126/science.aah5234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobanski, E., Brüggemann, D., Alm, B., Kern, S., Deschner, M., Schubert, T., et al. (2007). Psychiatric comorbidity and functional impairment in a clinically referred sample of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 257, 371–377. doi: 10.1007/s00406-007-0712-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobel, L. J., Bansal, R., Maia, T. V., Sanchez, J., Mazzone, L., and Durkin, K. (2010). Basal ganglia surface morphology and the effects of stimulant medications in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 977–986. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solanto, M. V. (1998). Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms of stimulant drug action in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review and integration. Behav. Brain Res. 94, 127–152. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00175-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, P., Zha, M., Yang, Q., Zhang, Y., Li, X., and Rudan, I. (2021). The prevalence of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Glob. Health 11:04009. doi: 10.7189/jogh.11.04009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 593–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: From common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1231–1238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., Toga, A. W., and Peterson, B. S. (2003). Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 362, 1699–1707. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14842-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., Madras, B. K., Dougherty, D. D., Bonab, A. A., and Livni, E. (2007). Further evidence of dopamine transporter dysregulation in ADHD: A controlled PET imaging study using altropane. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1059–1061. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., Madras, B. K., Faraone, S. V., Dougherty, D. D., and Bonab, A. A. (2005). In vivo neuroreceptor imaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A focus on the dopamine transporter. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1293–1300. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T. J., Brown, A., Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Makris, N., and Lomedico, A. (2013). Effect of psychostimulants on brain structure and function in ADHD: A qualitative literature review of magnetic resonance imaging-based neuroimaging studies. J. Clin. Psychiatry 74, 902–917. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12r08287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T., Biederman, J., and Wilens, T. (2004). Nonstimulant treatment of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 27, 373–383. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2003.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Prince, J., Hatch, M., and Jones, J. (1998). Effectiveness and tolerability of tomoxetine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 693–695. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.5.693

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahl, S. M. (2003). Neurotransmission of cognition, part 3. Mechanism of action of selective NRIs: Both dopamine and norepinephrine increase in prefrontal cortex. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 230–231. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuhec, M., and Locatelli, I. (2017). Age-related pharmacotherapy of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Slovenia in children and adolescents: A population-based study. Eur. Psychiatry 42, 129–133. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Štuhec, M., and Švab, V. (2013). Atomoxetine-induced life-threatening long QT syndrome. Irish J. Med. Sci. 182, 535–537. doi: 10.1007/s11845-013-0925-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuhec, M., Lukić, P., and Locatelli, I. (2019). Efficacy, acceptability, and tolerability of lisdexamfetamine, mixed amphetamine salts, methylphenidate, and modafinil in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann. Pharmacother. 53, 121–133. doi: 10.1177/1060028018795703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuhec, M., Munda, B., Svab, V., and Locatelli, I. (2015). Comparative efficacy and acceptability of atomoxetine, lisdexamfetamine, bupropion and methylphenidate in treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis with focus on bupropion. J. Affect. Disord. 178, 149–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Štuhec, M., Švab, V., and Locatelli, I. (2015). Prevalence and incidence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Slovenian children and adolescents: A database study from a national perspective. Croat. Med. J. 56, 159–165. doi: 10.3325/cmj.2015.56.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management Wolraich, M., Brown, L., Brown, R. T., DuPaul, G., et al. (2011). ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 128, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudre, G., Sharp, W., Kundzicz, P., Bouyssi-Kobar, M., Norman, L., Choudhury, S., et al. (2021). Predicting the course of ADHD symptoms through the integration of childhood genomic, neural, and cognitive features. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 4046–4054.

Google Scholar

Sulzer, D., and Rayport, S. (1990). Amphetamine and other psychostimulants reduce pH gradients in midbrain dopaminergic neurons and chromaffin granules: A mechanism of action. Neuron 5, 797–808. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90339-H

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulzer, D., Chen, T., Lau, Y., Kristensen, H., Rayport, S., and Ewing, A. (1995). Amphetamine redistributes dopamine from synaptic vesicles to the cytosol and promotes reverse transport. J. Neurosci. 15, 4102–4108. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-04102.1995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulzer, D., Sonders, M., Poulsen, N., and Galli, A. (2005). Mechanisms of neurotransmitter release by amphetamines: A review. Prog. Neurobiol. 75, 406–433. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, R. S., and Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.1109/TNN.1998.712192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svatkova, A., Nestrasil, I., Rudser, K., Goldenring Fine, J., Bledsoe, J., and Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2016). Unique white matter microstructural patterns in ADHD presentations-a diffusion tensor imaging study: Microstructural WM differences between ADHD presentations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3323–3336. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sved, A. F., Cano, G., and Card, J. P. (2001). Neuroanatomical specificity of the circuits controlling sympathetic outflow to different targets. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 28, 115–119. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1681.2001.03403.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, C. J., Perry, K. W., Koch-Krueger, S., Katner, J., Svensson, K. A., and Bymaster, F. P. (2006). Effect of the attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder drug atomoxetine on extracellular concentrations of norepinephrine and dopamine in several brain regions of the rat. Neuropharmacology 50, 755–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2005.11.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, J. M., Sunohara, G. A., Kennedy, J. L., Regino, R., Fineberg, E., and Wigal, T. (1998). Association of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene with a refined phenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A family-based approach. Mol. Psychiatry 3, 38–41. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talley, E. M., Rosin, D. L., Lee, A., Guyenet, P. G., and Lynch, K. R. (1996). Distribution of??2A-adrenergic receptor-like immunoreactivity in the rat central nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol. 372, 111–134. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960812)372:1<111::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamm, L., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., O’Brien, K. M., Hawk, L. W., and Epstein, J. N. (2012). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A review. Neurotherapeutics 9, 500–508. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0138-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teo, S. K., San, R. H., Wagner, V. O., Gudi, R., Stirling, D. I., Thomas, S. D., et al. (2003). D-Methylphenidate is non-genotoxic in in vitro and in vivo assays. Mutat. Res. 537, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/S1383-5718(03)00053-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tervo, D. G. R., Proskurin, M., Manakov, M., Kabra, M., Vollmer, A., Branson, K., et al. (2014). Behavioral variability through stochastic choice and its gating by anterior cingulate cortex. Cell 159, 21–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.08.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thome, J., Ehlis, A. C., Fallgatter, A. J., Krauel, K., Lange, K. W., Riederer, P., et al. (2012). Biomarkers for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A consensus report of the WFSBP task force on biological markers and the World Federation of ADHD. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 379–400. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2012.690535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobler, P. N., O’Doherty, J. P., Dolan, R. J., and Schultz, W. (2007). Reward value coding distinct from risk attitude-related uncertainty coding in human reward systems. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1621–1632. doi: 10.1152/jn.00745.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torgersen, T., Gjervan, B., Rasmussen, K., Vaaler, A., and Nordahl, H. M. (2013). Prevalence of comorbid substance use disorder during long-term central stimulant treatment in adult ADHD. ADHD Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 5, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12402-012-0094-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripp, G., and Wickens, J. R. (2008). Research review: Dopamine transfer deficit: A neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 691–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01851.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q., Kelly, A. M. C., Biswal, B. B., Margulies, D. S., Shehzad, Z., Shaw, D., et al. (2008). Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD. J. Neurosci. Methods 169, 249–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.11.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlén, S., Muceniece, R., Rangel, N., Tiger, G., and Wikberg, J. E. (1995). Comparison of the binding activities of some drugs on alpha 2A, alpha 2B and alpha 2C-adrenoceptors and non-adrenergic imidazoline sites in the guinea pig. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 76, 353–364. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0773.1995.tb00161.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., and Seidman, L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 1361–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elst, L. T., Maier, S., Klöppel, S., Graf, E., Killius, C., Rump, M., et al. (2016). The effect of methylphenidate intake on brain structure in adults with ADHD in a placebo-controlled randomized trial. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41, 422–430. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varesio, E., and Veuthey, J. L. (1995). Chiral separation of amphetamines by high-performance capillary electrophoresis. J. Chromatogr. A 717, 219–228. doi: 10.1016/0021-9673(95)00483-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbeeck, W., Bekkering, G. E., Van den Noortgate, W., and Kramers, C. (2017). Bupropion for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 10:CD009504. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009504.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viggiano, D., Vallone, D., and Sadile, A. (2004). Dysfunctions in dopamine systems and ADHD: Evidence from animals and modeling. Neural Plast. 11, 97–114. doi: 10.1155/NP.2004.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viggiano, D., Vallone, D., Ruocco, L. A., and Sadile, A. G. (2003). Behavioural, pharmacological, morpho-functional molecular studies reveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine system in an animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 683–689. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villemonteix, T., Brito, S. A., Kavec, M., Balériaux, D., Metens, T., and Slama, H. (2015). Grey matter volumes in treatment naïve vs. chronically treated children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A combined approach. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 1118–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.04.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, S. N., Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., and Ghandour, R. M. (2014). Trends in the parent-report of health care provider-diagnosed and medicated attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: United States, 2003–2011. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 34–46.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G., Ding, Y., and Gatley, S. J. (2002). Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: Insights from PET imaging studies. J. Atten. Disord. 6(Suppl. 1), 31–43. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601S05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G., Fowler, J. S., Logan, J., Gerasimov, M., Maynard, L., et al. (2001). Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 21:121. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-j0001.2001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Fowler, J. S., and Ding, Y.-S. (2005). Imaging the effects of methylphenidate on brain dopamine: New model on its therapeutic actions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1410–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Fowler, J. S., Gatley, S. J., Logan, J., Ding, Y.-S., et al. (1998). Dopamine transporter occupancies in the human brain induced by therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 1325–1331. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Newcorn, J. H., and Telang, F. (2009). Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in ADHD: Clinical implications. JAMA 302, 1084–1091. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Newcorn, J., Telang, F., Solanto, M. V., and Fowler, J. S. (2007). Depressed dopamine activity in caudate and preliminary evidence of limbic involvement in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:932. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.932

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G.-J., Volkow, N. D., Wigal, T., Kollins, S. H., Newcorn, J. H., and Telang, F. (2013). Long-term stimulant treatment affects brain dopamine transporter level in patients with attention deficit hyperactive disorder. PLoS One 8:e63023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K.-S. (2013). Relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and alcohol dependence: A genetic view. Int. J. High Risk Behav. Addict. 1, 192–193. doi: 10.5812/ijhrba.9629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, T., Liu, K., Li, Z., Xu, Y., Liu, Y., Shi, W., et al. (2017). Prevalence of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children and adolescents in China: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 17:32. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1187-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Wu, X., Yu, Z., and Yu, L. (2021). Utilization of drugs for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder among young patients in China, 2010–2019. Front. Psychiatry 12:802489. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.802489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watabe-Uchida, M., and Uchida, N. (2018). Multiple dopamine systems: Weal and woe of dopamine. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 83, 83–95. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2018.83.037648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, A. L., Leibson, C. L., Ph, D., Jacobsen, S. J., and Ph, D. (2005). Psychostimulant treatment and risk for substance abuse among young adults with a history of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A population-based. Birth Cohort Study. 15, 764–776. doi: 10.1089/cap.2005.15.764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisler, R., Young, J., Mattingly, G., Gao, J., Squires, L., and Adler, L. (2009). Long-term safety and effectiveness of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. CNS Spect. 14, 573–586. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900024056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., Tzavara, E. T., Davis, R. J., Nomikos, G. G., Michael, McIntosh, J., et al. (2007). Functional alterations of nicotinic neurotransmission in dopamine transporter knock-out mice. Neuropharmacology 52, 1496–1508. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, D. D., Edwards, A. M., Chapman, B. M., and Ondo, J. G. (1993). A model of the sodium dependence of dopamine uptake in rat striatal synaptosomes. Neurochem. Res. 18, 927–936. doi: 10.1007/BF00998279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Adler, L. A., Adams, J., Sgambati, S., Rotrosen, J., and Sawtelle, R. (2008). Misuse and diversion of stimuants prescribed for ADHD: A systematic review of the literature. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 21–31. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a56f1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., and Gunawardene, S. (2003). Does stimulant therapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder beget later substance abuse? A meta-analytic review of the literature. Pediatrics 111, 179–185. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.1.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Martelon, M., Joshi, G., Bateman, C., Fried, R., and Petty, C. (2011). Does ADHD predict substance-use disorders? A 10-year follow-up study of young adults with ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 50, 543–553. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J., and Dayan, P. (2005). Dopamine, learning, and impulsivity: A biological account of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 15, 160–179. doi: 10.1089/cap.2005.15.160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, R. A. (1982). Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 39–53. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00010372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witcher, J. W., Long, A., Smith, B., Sauer, J.-M., Heilgenstein, J., and Wilens, T. (2003). Atomoxetine pharmacokinetics in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 13, 53–63. doi: 10.1089/104454603321666199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolosin, S. M., Richardson, M. E., Hennessey, J. G., Denckla, M. B., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Abnormal cerebral cortex structure in children with ADHD. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 175–184. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20496

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolraich, M. L., Hagan, J. F., Allan, C., Chan, E., Davison, D., Earls, M., et al. (2019). Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 144:e20192528.

Google Scholar

Wolraich, M. L., Hannah, J. N., Pinnock, T. Y., Baumgaertel, A., and Brown, J. (1996). Comparison of diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a county-wide sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35, 319–324. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199603000-00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Z. M., Llera, A., Hoogman, M., Cao, Q. J., Zwiers, M. P., Bralten, J., et al. (2019). Linked anatomical and functional brain alterations in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage 23:101851. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, A. J., and Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron 46, 681–692. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Zeeuw, P., Mandl, R. C. W., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Engeland, H., and Durston, S. (2012). Decreased frontostriatal microstructural organization in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1941–1951. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, X., Zhang, Y., Kwong, J. S., Zhang, C., Li, S., Sun, F., et al. (2015). The methodological quality assessment tools for preclinical and clinical studies, systematic review and meta-analysis, and clinical practice guideline: A systematic review. J. Evid. Based Med. 8, 2–10. doi: 10.1111/jebm.12141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Q., Du, G., John, V., Kapahi, P., and Bredesen, D. E. (2015). Alzheimer’s model develops early ADHD syndrome. J. Neurol. Neurophysiol. 6, 1–6. doi: 10.4172/2155-9562.1000329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, J., and Reith, M. E. A. (2008). Role of the dopamine transporter in the action of psychostimulants, nicotine, and other drugs of abuse. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 7, 393–409. doi: 10.2174/187152708786927877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, S., Pedersen, M. L., Mowinckel, A. M., and Biele, G. (2016). Modelling ADHD: A review of ADHD theories through their predictions for computational models of decision-making and reinforcement learning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 633–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zito, J. M. (2000). “Pharmacoepidemiology of methylphenidate and other medications for the treatment of ADHD,” in Title mental health, united states, 2000, Vol. 20, eds R. W. Manderscheid and M. J. Henderson (Rockville, MD: Institution Substance Abuse And Mental Health Services Administration), 128.

Google Scholar

Zito, J. M., Derivan, A. T., Kratochvil, C. J., Safer, D. J., Fegert, J. M., and Greenhill, L. L. (2008). Off-label psychopharmacologic prescribing for children: History supports close clinical monitoring. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2:24. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-2-24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuvekas, S. H., and Vitiello, B. (2012). Stimulant medication use in children: A 12-year perspective. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 160–166. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ADHD, MRI, psychostimulants, cognitive impairment, neurodegeneration

Citation: Dutta CN, Christov-Moore L, Ombao H and Douglas PK (2022) Neuroprotection in late life attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review of pharmacotherapy and phenotype across the lifespan. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:938501. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.938501

Received: 07 May 2022; Accepted: 16 August 2022;
Published: 26 September 2022.

Edited by:

Yael Leitner, Dana-Dwek Children’s Hospital, Israel

Reviewed by:

David W. Goodman, Johns Hopkins Medicine, United States
Matej Stuhec, University of Maribor, Slovenia

Copyright © 2022 Dutta, Christov-Moore, Ombao and Douglas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cintya Nirvana Dutta, bGFyaW9zLmNuQGtuaWdodHMudWNmLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.