Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 05 July 2022
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic TMS application in both health and disease View all 13 articles

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Long-Term Smoking Cessation: Preliminary Examination of Delay Discounting as a Therapeutic Target and the Effects of Intensity and Duration

\r\nAlina Shevorykin*&#x;Alina Shevorykin1*†Ellen CarlEllen Carl1Martin C. MahoneyMartin C. Mahoney1Colleen A. HanlonColleen A. Hanlon2Amylynn LiskiewiczAmylynn Liskiewicz1Cheryl RivardCheryl Rivard1Ronald AlbericoRonald Alberico1Ahmed BelalAhmed Belal1Lindsey BenschLindsey Bensch1Darian VantucciDarian Vantucci1Hannah ThornerHannah Thorner1Matthew MarionMatthew Marion1Warren K. BickelWarren K. Bickel3Christine E. Sheffer&#x;Christine E. Sheffer1†
  • 1Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center, Buffalo, NY, United States
  • 2Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, United States
  • 3Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at Virginia Tech Carilion, Roanoke, VA, United States

Background: Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is a novel treatment for smoking cessation and delay discounting rate is novel therapeutic target. Research to determine optimal therapeutic targets and dosing parameters for long-term smoking cessation is needed. Due to potential biases and confounds introduced by the COVID-19 pandemic, we report preliminary results from an ongoing study among participants who reached study end prior to the pandemic.

Methods: In a 3 × 2 randomized factorial design, participants (n = 23) received 900 pulses of 20 Hz rTMS to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) in one of three Durations (8, 12, or 16 days of stimulation) and two Intensities (1 or 2 sessions per day). We examined direction and magnitude of the effect sizes on latency to relapse, 6-month point-prevalence abstinence rates, research burden, and delay discounting rates.

Results: A large effect size was found for Duration and a medium for Intensity for latency to relapse. Increasing Duration increased the odds of abstinence 7–8-fold while increasing Intensity doubled the odds of abstinence. A large effect size was found for Duration, a small for Intensity for delay discounting rate. Increasing Duration and Intensity had a small effect on participant burden.

Conclusion: Findings provide preliminary support for delay discounting as a therapeutic target and for increasing Duration and Intensity to achieve larger effect sizes for long-term smoking cessation and will provide a pre-pandemic comparison for data collected during the pandemic.

Clinical Trial Registration: [www.ClinicalTrials.gov], identifier [NCT03865472].

Introduction

“Making progress on longstanding challenges requires a different lens and a new approach.”

Ayanna Pressley.

Over one-half of individuals who smoke cigarettes in the US attempt to quit every year, but over 90% rapidly reverse this decision, choosing the immediate reward of smoking over the long-term benefits of quitting (Babb et al., 2017). Despite the increased availability of evidence-based behavioral and pharmacological treatments for cigarette smoking, less than one-third of cigarette smokers use them (Babb et al., 2017; Office on Smoking and United States Public Health Service Office of the Surgeon General, and National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (US) Office on Smoking and Health, 2020). Negative attitudes about taking medications for smoking cessation are a commonly endorsed barrier to using pharmacological treatments (Mooney et al., 2006; Gross et al., 2008; Morphett et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2015). Novel, non-pharmacological treatment approaches have the potential to provide cigarette smokers with more smoking cessation treatment options.

Tremendous progress has been made in the development of brain stimulation techniques to support smoking cessation (Ekhtiari et al., 2019). High frequency (HF) (rTMS) is a non-invasive brain stimulation technique that can selectively modulate neuronal plasticity (Ekhtiari et al., 2019). Using a variety of different coil configurations, rTMS generates brief focal electromagnetic pulses that penetrate the skull to stimulate brain regions via localized axonal depolarization (Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Thut and Pascual-Leone, 2010). rTMS coil selection is based on the need for stimulation depth and focality (Lu and Ueno, 2017). HF rTMS using an H4-coil was recently cleared by the Federal Drug Administration (K200957) for short-term smoking cessation (Zangen et al., 2021). The H4-coil stimulates broad swaths of the (PFC) and insula (Fiocchi et al., 2018). The conventional figure of 8 coil delivers stimulation with more focality than the H4 coil (i.e., targets with more specificity) (Lu and Ueno, 2017). HF rTMS of the left dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) using a figure of 8 coil is emerging as a novel non-pharmacological treatment approach for long-term smoking cessation (Ekhtiari et al., 2019).

The Competing Neurobehavioral Decisions Systems (CNDS) Model is a broad, fundamental framework grounded in neuroeconomics and dual processing theory (Mukherjee, 2010). The CNDS Model describes the general neurobiological underpinnings involved with making far-sighted decisions about one’s health (e.g., maintaining abstinence from smoking) in the context of immediately rewarding, though less healthy choices (e.g., continued smoking) (Bickel et al., 2007; McClure and Bickel, 2014). The Model posits that these decisions are broadly driven by the interaction between two functional neurobiological networks: the executive function network, embodied in the PFC; and the impulsive network, embodied in the limbic and paralimbic regions of the brain (Bickel et al., 2014, 2016; Hanlon et al., 2015). The balance of activity in these two networks shapes reward-related decision-making (Hanlon et al., 2015). Greater activity in the PFC is linked with a higher likelihood of more prudent decision-making, even in the context of temptation (McClure et al., 2007; MacKillop et al., 2012). However, chronic nicotine administration can significantly impact reward-related decision-making (Koob, 2008a,b) and over time, the balance and functioning of these networks can become dysregulated, resulting in significant deficits in executive function neural processing (Ernst et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2005; McClure and Bickel, 2014; Koob et al., 2014). Dysfunction or hypo-activation of the executive function network contributes to undervaluation of the long-term rewards from cessation (Hanlon et al., 2015), adding to the behavioral and psychosocial challenges of achieving long-term abstinence from cigarettes.

Most individuals prefer immediate rewards because reinforcement loses value the longer one waits to receive it, but individuals demonstrate considerable variability in these preferences. Delay discounting rate represents the degree to which individuals discount the value of a reward as a function of time to receipt (Kirby, 1997; Odum, 2011; Commons et al., 2013). Delay discounting rate is considered a transdiagnostic biological marker for the relative balance between the executive and impulsive networks consistent with the CNDS model (Bickel et al., 2012, 2019), and is a well-established prognostic factor for smoking cessation treatment outcomes (Sheffer et al., 2012, 2014; Coughlin et al., 2020). Importantly, delay discounting rates are malleable, with decreases associated with improved health behaviors (Koffarnus et al., 2013; Bickel et al., 2014; Rung and Madden, 2018).

The frontolimbic balance outlined by the CNDS Model and its application to cigarette smoking, however, must be viewed as a general framework within the context of the multiple complex neurobiological, psychological, affective, environmental, socio-cultural, and evolutionary factors that contribute to the development and maintenance of dysfunctional human decision-making, a review of which is outside the scope of this manuscript (Alcaro et al., 2021). For instance, the well-established role of classical and operant conditioning in decision-making is optimized by the mesolimbic dopaminergic (MS DA) system (Robinson and Berridge, 2001, 2003, 2008; Salamone and Correa, 2002; Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Alcaro et al., 2007) and contributes significantly to the development and maintenance of the imbalance described by the CNDS Model. A significant body of research also shows that the SEEKING or exploring drive is neurologically foundational to all appetitively motivated behaviors (Alcaro et al., 2021). Addiction likely reflects dysfunction of the SEEKING drive, linked with the MS DA system and consistent with results predicted by the CNDS Model (Alcaro et al., 2021). In addition, delay discounting is one of many potential transdiagnostic dimensions that are relevant to reward dysfunction. Anhedonia, defined as the inability to feel pleasure, is a transdiagnostic dimension present in a wide variety of mental health and substance use disorders (Spano et al., 2019). As a symptom of abstinence from many substances, anhedonia can prevent adequate reinforcement from non-substance related reinforcers (Garfield et al., 2014).

The dlPFC, a functional node in the PFC, has a significant role in executive function and the controlled response inhibition associated with drug-related craving and self-regulation (Ernst et al., 2001; Brody et al., 2002; McBride et al., 2006). The proposed mechanism by which HF rTMS of the left dlPFC supports smoking cessation is by increasing neuronal activity and plasticity in the left dlPFC, thereby improving executive functions mediated by the dlPFC. Preliminary evidence indicates that the approach is feasible and can reduce delay discounting rates and increase short-term latency to relapse, abstinence rates, and uptake of psychoeducational material (Sheffer et al., 2013, 2018; Ekhtiari et al., 2019). Prior to conducting a large-scale study of efficacy, however, research is needed to determine the optimal dosing strategies to achieve long-term abstinence (i.e., 6 months or more).

The parent project from which this study is derived is an ongoing 5-year study aimed to determine the optimal dosing strategies of rTMS of the left dlPFC for long-term smoking cessation (Carl et al., 2020). This study employs a fully crossed, 3 × 2 × 2 randomized double-blinded factorial design, where Duration is defined as 8, 12, and 16 days of stimulation, Intensity is a number of pulses per day (900 in one session vs. 1,800 in two sessions), and participants are randomized to active/sham conditions.

The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic paused all in-person study-related activities in the parent study in late March of 2020. Once the study resumed, multiple factors associated with COVID-19 introduced possible biases and confounds on recruitment, retention, and outcomes. These factors include the need to use different recruitment strategies (social media vs. flyers in the community), increased participant burden due to safety precautions, concerns about physical distancing, and COVID-19 stress-related effects on engagement and outcomes. Thus, we report preliminary results from participants who reached study end prior to the COVID-19 outbreak in Western New York.

Our primary goal was to examine the effects of increasing rTMS Duration and Intensity of active stimulation on latency to relapse, 6-month point prevalence abstinence rates, participant burden, and delay discounting rates among participants who reached study end. The hypotheses were consistent with the parent study (see above). Given the preliminary nature of this study, the focus on pre-COVID participation, the 3–1 active/sham randomization in the parent study, and the limited sample size, we included only active participants, limited comparisons, and focused on effect sizes. Statistical significance, while reported, must be viewed with caution in this context, however, multiple statistical approaches were employed to establish consistency among the findings.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited right-handed adults (age 18–65) who smoked 6–25 cigarettes daily and who were motivated to quit smoking. Participants were required to pass a 12-panel urine drug test, a pregnancy test, the TMS Adult Safety and Screening Questionnaire (TASS) (Rossi et al., 2011), claustrophobia screen to assess the ability to undergo a closed MRI of the head (Carl et al., 2020). Participants were recruited using flyers in the community, print advertisement, and social media.

Design

This study is a fully crossed 3 × 2 randomized factorial design. The two factors were Duration (8, 12, or 16 days of stimulation) and Intensity (900 or 1,800 pulses per day). Although only those participants who received active stimulation were included in the analyses, all participants and technicians were blinded to active/sham condition. Participants were followed for 6 months after the quit date. Daily number of cigarettes smoked per day was collected every 2 weeks. Exhaled carbon monoxide (CO) level was assessed at each in-person outcome assessment (4-, 8-, 12-, 18-, and 24-weeks after the quit date). Given the preliminary nature of this study, the focus on pre-COVID participation, the 3–1 active/sham randomization in the parent study, and the limited sample size, only those participants who received active stimulation and reached study end prior to April 4, 2020 were included in this study.

Procedure

Participants were screened over the telephone and invited to an in-person interview during which urine drug and pregnancy tests were administered. After informed consent, participants completed baseline assessments and were scheduled for an MRI. Prior to the MRI, the International 10-10 Electrode System was used to place a vitamin E capsule at the AF3 electrode position as a fiducial marker on the image. AF3 was chosen because the cognitive functions of interest are located in the anterior region of the dlPFC (Cieslik et al., 2012). The MRI was uploaded into the neuronavigation system with the fiducial marker readily apparent on the image. The MRI was also used to identify brain abnormalities that might impact participant safety. Eligible participants were randomized and scheduled for quit counseling, a quit date, and rTMS sessions.

The quit day was the day immediately prior to the first rTMS session. Participants were provided 30 min of brief structured cognitive behavioral counseling over the telephone 2 days prior to the quit date. Participants were required to abstain from smoking for at least 24 h prior to the first stimulation session, as evidenced by an expired breath CO level of < 10 ppm (SRNT Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification, 2002). Immediately prior to initiating rTMS, participants were randomized using permuted block randomization stratified by high or low nicotine dependence level [FTND; high (≥5) or low (<5)] (Heatherton et al., 1991).

TMS power was tailored to the Motor Threshold (MT), which was defined as the minimum stimulation power required to elicit a motor evoked potential of 50 μV from the abductor pollicis brevis (APB) in 3 of 6 trials. Each rTMS session provided 900 pulses of 20 Hz rTMS to the left dlPFC at 110% of the MT. Magstim Super RAPID2 PLUS1 System with Magstim 70 mm Double Air Film Active Figure of 8 Coil was used. The Brainsight Neuronavigational system (Rouge Research, Inc.) was used to track the placement of the coil in real time with respect to an MRI-derived image. Pulses were delivered in 45 20-pulse trains of 1 s duration with an inter-train interval of 20 s. Stimulation time was 16 min. Participants read psychoeducational materials (Forever Free® self-help booklets) during the first 8 stimulation sessions. Participants were compensated $20 after each in-person visit, a weekly $50 bonus for completing all scheduled rTMS sessions, and $100 bonus for completing all five outcome assessments.

Bioethics

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center (#I-65718). Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Demographic information collected at baseline included age, sex, race, ethnicity, partnered status, education, and household income. Other measures included the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) (Heatherton et al., 1991; Fagerstrom, 2012) and other clinical factors such as impulsivity measured by Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Patton et al., 1995). Delay discounting and participant research burden were assessed at baseline and at each outcome assessment point. Primary outcomes included latency to relapse (number of days to relapse), 6-month point prevalence abstinence rates, participant research burden, and delay discounting rates.

Delay discounting rates were assessed using the 5-trial adjusting delay task for $100 and $1,000 magnitudes (Koffarnus and Bickel, 2014). During this task, participants were presented with a choice between two hypothetical monetary amounts ($100 vs. $50 in the $100 condition and $1,000 vs. $500 in the $1,000 condition). In each of the seven choice presentations, the smaller amount was available immediately, and the higher amount available at a discrete delay, beginning with 3 weeks. Based on the participant’s choice, delay either increased (when participants select the delayed option) or decreased (when participants select the immediate option). Participants made this choice for five trials, resulting in potential k-values which were subsequently log transformed into lnk.

Latency to relapse and point prevalence abstinence were assessed using the Timeline Follow Back procedure (TLFB) every 2 weeks (Sobell and Sobell, 1992; Brown et al., 1998) by telephone and during the in-person outcome assessments. Relapse was defined as 7 consecutive days of any cigarette smoking (Hughes et al., 2003). CO in exhaled breath of ≤ 5 ppm as measured by the Micro + Smokerlyzer (Covita, Inc.) was considered abstinent from cigarette smoking (SRNT Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification, 2002).

Participant research burden was assessed with the 21-item Perceived Research Burden Assessment (PeRBA) (Lingler et al., 2014). Total scores range from 21 to 105, with lower scores reflecting lower participant burden.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive analyses were used to characterize the sample. Because the aim was to examine the direction and strength of effect sizes, the primary tests were Cohen’s d, partial eta squared (η2), Cramer’s Phi squared (Φ2), and odds ratios (OR), as appropriate (Rhea, 2004; Le and Marcus, 2012; Tomczak and Tomczak, 2014; Enzmann, 2015; Kim, 2015). Cohen’s d provides a standardized difference between two means by expressing the difference in units of standard deviation. With Cohen’s d, a small effect size is ∼0.2, medium is ∼0.5, and large is ∼0.8 or greater. Partial η2 measures the proportion of variance explained by the dependent variable attributable to the independent variable. With η2, a small effect size is ∼ 0.01, medium is ∼0.06, and large is ∼0.14 or greater. Cramer’s Phi (Φ) reflects the strength of the association between two variables, and when squared, reflects how much variance is accounted for by the association. With Φ2, a small effect size is ∼0.01, medium is ∼0.09, and large is ∼0.25 or greater (Maher et al., 2013; Ialongo, 2016). OR reflect the direction and strength of the effect relative to the comparison group. Hazard ratios, chi square, confidence intervals, F-statistics, and p-values are reported as appropriate, but given small sample size, must be viewed with caution.

Cox proportional hazard (CPH) models were used to examine the effects of Duration and Intensity on latency to relapse. Days to relapse were right-censored. Right censoring was defined as participants who did not relapse while under observation, either because they maintained abstinence to the end of the study period or were lost to follow-up. Participants were considered abstinent at least as long as they were observed to have been abstinent. Cohen’s d was calculated for CPH results using the formula: d = ln(HR)x✓(6/π) (Azuero, 2016).

Binary logistic regression models were used to examine the effects of Duration and Intensity on 6-month/7-day point prevalence abstinence rates. Missing data was imputed as smoking for the Intention to treat analysis (ITT). Missing data was excluded for complete case analysis (CCA). For Duration, 8 days was used as the comparison group. For intensity, 1 session per day (900 pulses) was used as the comparison group. OR and confidence intervals are reported.

The analysis of delayed discounting rate and PeRBA was conducted in two ways: (1) Repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to examine main effects, and (2) Generalized estimating equations (GEE) was used to examine rate of change across time. Dependent variables were discounting (lnk) of $100 and $1,000 magnitudes, and the total score of the PeRBA. Time was entered as a within-subject factor, with six timepoints: baseline and 5 outcome assessments (4-, 8-, 12-, 16-, and 24-weeks after the first rTMS session). The Bonferroni adjustment was used to control for multiple comparisons. When Mauchly’s test statistic was significant, Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. Partial η2 was calculated for MANOVA results using the formula: η2 = SSeffect/SStotal (Lakens, 2013). Cramer’s Phi (Φ) was calculated for GEE results using the formula: Φ = ✓(χ2/N); Φ2 was calculated to show shared variance (Phi/Cramer’s Phi; Sharpe, 2015).

Results

Participant Characteristics

Prior to pausing the parent project, n = 23 participants in the active condition reached study end. Participants were primarily middle-aged (M = 50.78, SD = 10.96). About 30% identified as non-white and 70% as women. Participants included a high proportion of individuals of lower income and were diverse in terms of employment status. Nearly 80% were Medicaid and/or Medicare beneficiaries, over half had household incomes less than $25,000 per year, and 40% did not attend college. Participants were highly dependent on smoking. Most began smoking as adolescents, and over half had not made a quit attempt in the past year. Participants were moderately confident in their ability to quit smoking and maintained relatively high levels of motivation to quit throughout the study. Baseline levels of overall impulsivity were moderate and remained steady throughout the study (see Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant (n = 23) characteristics at baseline.

Engagement and retention were high; 98.35% of rTMS sessions were completed and 78.30% (n = 18) completed the final outcome assessment (see Supplementary Table 2).

Latency to Relapse

Although the standard deviations and interquartile ranges were large, the mean and median latency to relapse increased as Duration and Intensity increased, as hypothesized. Compared to 8 days of stimulation, 12 days showed a medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.310) and 16 days showed a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.741). Increasing Duration from 8 to 16 days significantly reduced the relative risk of relapse [HR 0.29 (0.09, 1.00); p = 0.049], such that with 16 days of stimulation the mean days to relapse increased from 17 days to 76 and the median from 2 to 31 days. Increasing Intensity from 900 to 1,800 pulses per day approached a medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.381). Increasing Intensity from 900 pulses per day to 1800 pulses reduced the relative risk of relapse [HR 0.53 (0.17, 1.66); p = 0.28], such that with 1,800 pulses the mean days to relapse increased from 26 to 64 and the median from 2 to 28 days (see Supplementary Table 3 and Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Probability of abstinence over 6 months by duration and intensity of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Point Prevalence Abstinence

Using both ITT and CCA analysis, the proportion of participants abstinent from smoking consistently increased as Duration and Intensity increased. Logistic regressions revealed that increasing Duration from 8 to 12 days and from 8 to 16 days increased the odds of abstinence 4 and 7–8 fold, respectively. Increasing Intensity doubled the odds of long-term abstinence. Nevertheless, the confidence intervals were quite large. Differences were not statistically significant (Duration ITT: χ2 = 3.260, p = 0.196, R2 = 0.187; CCA: χ2 = 2.885, p = 0.236, R2 = 0.178) (Intensity ITT: χ2 = 0.712, p = 0.399, R2 = 0.043; CCA: χ2 = 0.421, p = 0.516, R2 = 0.028) (see Supplementary Table 4).

Participant Burden

MANOVAs revealed small effect sizes for both Duration and Intensity on the total PeRBA score (Duration: F = 0.376, p = 695, η2 = 0.059 and Intensity: F = 0.008, p = 0.930, η2 = 0.001). Similarly, GEE revealed small effect sizes for Duration and Intensity on total PeRBA scores (Duration: χ2 = 1.921, p = 0.383, Φ = 0.289, Φ2 = 0.084 and Intensity: χ2 = 0.901, p = 0.343, Φ = 0.198, Φ2 = 0.039). Increasing Duration or Intensity did not increase research burden and the scores were in the lower range (possible range is 21–105) (see Supplementary Table 5).

Delay Discounting

MANOVAs revealed large effect sizes for Duration (between 8, 12, and 16 days) for the $100 and $1000 magnitudes and these differences were statistically significant ($100: F = 4.500, p = 0.035, η2 = 0.429; and $1,000 F = 5.657, p = 0.019, η2 = 0.485). See Figure 2A the difference for Intensity (between 900 and 1,800 pulses per day) was in the expected direction, with small effect size, and not statistically significant ($100 F = 0.083, p = 0.779, η2 = 0.007; and $1000 F = 0.023, p = 0.883, η2 = 0.002) (see Figure 2B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Repeated measures analysis of variance shows that increasing duration decreases delay discounting rates of $100 and $1000 overall over 6 months. (B) Repeated measures analysis of variance shows that increasing intensity decreases delay discounting rates of $100 and $1000 overall over 6 months.

GEE revealed an overall decrease in delay discounting rate over time (see Figure 3) for Duration but not Intensity. Overall, large effect sizes were found for Duration ($100: χ2 = 16.008, p < 0.001, Φ = 0.834, Φ2 = 0.696; and $1,000: χ2 = 19.042, p < 0.001, Φ = 0.909, Φ2 = 0.827). Participants who received 16 days of rTMS had a more robust change in $100 and $1,000 magnitudes across time compared to those receiving 8 days of rTMS. The difference between 900 and 1,800 pulses per day was in the expected direction, with small effects size, and not statistically significant ($100: χ2 = 1.187, p = 0.28, Φ = 0.227, Φ2 = 0.051; and $1,000: χ2 = 2.161, p = 0.14, Φ = 0.307, Φ2 = 0.094) (see Supplementary Table 5).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Generalized estimating equation show an overall decrease in delay discounting rate over 6 months of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Discussion

Using multiple outcomes and statistical approaches, these findings provide consistent preliminary support for the primary smoking cessation hypotheses. Greater Duration and Intensity had greater effects on increasing latency to relapse, improving abstinence rates, and decreasing delay discounting rates. Findings also provided support, though weak, for the hypothesis that increased Duration and Intensity also increase participant research burden. Finally, these findings suggest that the therapeutic target, delay discounting, was robustly engaged and demonstrated the predicted concurrent effects on delay discounting and efficacy outcomes.

These findings suggest that prior to the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, engagement was sufficiently high among participants who received higher doses of rTMS in terms of Duration and Intensity to support larger efficacy trials. In this sample, 98% of the rTMS sessions were completed, 78% completed the final outcome assessment, and at least some daily cigarette use was collected for all participants. Most participants were of lower income, were diverse in terms of employment, and over 30% identified as racial and/or ethnic minorities suggesting that rTMS studies are able to attract racially and socioeconomically diverse cigarette smokers.

Although increasing both Duration and Intensity increased effect sizes across multiple outcomes, it appears that the number of days of stimulation might have a larger impact on outcomes than the number of pulses per day. This suggests that effects of rTMS on smoking cessation are cumulative and might require time to produce changes in behavior. Future studies with larger sample sizes, should examine whether Duration and Intensity interact to produce higher effect sizes.

Although clearly not conclusive, the impact of increasing Duration and Intensity on perceived research burden was less than expected. We speculate these findings might be an artifact of including only participants who received active stimulation because they were more likely to receive benefits of cessation, which might have outweighed the greater requirements. Future research will need to examine differences in perceived research burden between participants who received active and sham stimulation. Nonetheless, these findings suggest that the burden of participating in this study was not strongly linked with the actual number of rTMS sessions required. Future research will examine whether participants who reached study end after the onset of the pandemic experienced greater participant burden.

The parent study is ongoing and expected to meet modified accrual objectives in 2022. Findings from this study will provide a pre-pandemic comparison for the data collected during the pandemic. Reporting on pre-pandemic findings is important because the pandemic created an environment in which possible biases and confounds potentially impact outcomes. Pre- and post-pandemic comparisons can inform interpretations about biases and confounds should pre- and post-pandemic findings differ.

Finally, future research needs to examine the potential long-term neural adaptations from multiple sessions of rTMS. Although an isolated finding, one study reported a reflection effect, whereby one session of rTMS decreased DD of monetary gains, but also increased DD of monetary losses, a potentially negative finding (Sheffer et al., 2013). Therefore, future research should incorporate the examination of long-term paradoxical or counter therapeutic effects.

The strengths of this study include factorial design in which each participant is exposed to a level of each factor, allowing for the efficient examination of the main effects for Duration and Intensity in one study, eliminating confounds associated with systematic differences among pilot studies using different doses. This design also provides an estimate of the main effects of each factor in the presence of the other factor. Nonetheless, these findings are limited by a small sample size and lack of sham control comparisons. We did not include participants who received sham in this preliminary analysis for multiple reasons. Including sham participants would have doubled the number of cells and comparisons. In addition to the small number of participants who reached study end prior to the onset of the pandemic, the parent study randomized participants to active or sham in a 3–1 ratio. Many of the cells were simply too small to feasibly compare Duration and Intensity when the sham was included. Although including participants who smoke from 6 to 25 cigarettes per day might introduce uncontrolled variability, this limitation is tempered by permuted block randomization stratified by nicotine dependence level. Finally, all participants were motivated to quit based on the inclusion criteria, which limits generalizability of the results to treatment seeking individuals.

Conclusion

These findings provide preliminary support for targeting delay discounting as a therapeutic target for smoking cessation with rTMS. Greater Duration and Intensity of rTMS appear to have greater effects on delay discounting rates and multiple indicators of abstinence, with a small effect on participant burden. Findings provide a pre-pandemic comparison for the data collected during the pandemic and a basis to examine possible biases and confounds created by the COVID-19 pandemic in the parent study.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: All data, and research materials will be available upon completion of the associated clinical trial with the appropriate permissions (clinical trial identifier: NCT03865472). Requests to access these datasets should be directed to the corresponding author or Clinicaltrials.gov.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center (#I-65718). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AS, EC, MM, CH, WB, and CS: conceptualization. AS, AL, CR, RA, AB, LB, DV, HT, MM, and CS: methodology. AS, EC, and CS: formal analysis. MM, AL, CR, RA, AB, LB, DV, HT, MM, and CS: investigation. CS: resources, supervision, and funding acquisition. AS, EC, and AL: data curation. AS and CS: writing—original draft preparation. AS, EC, MM, CH, AL, CR, RA, AB, LB, DV, HT, MM, WB, and CE: writing—review and editing. AS: visualization. AL and CS: project administration. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Cancer Institute (R01 CA229415 PI: Sheffer). The content was solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Cancer Institute or the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of Interest

MM had provided expert testimony on the health effects of smoking in lawsuits filed against the tobacco industry. He has also received research support from Pizer, Inc., for an on-going clinical trial of smoking cessation, and has previously served on external advisory panels sponsored by Pfizer to promote smoking cessation in clinical settings.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We want to acknowledge all our participants who devoted their time and effort to our scientific community over many years and made advances in science possible. We also would like to acknowledge all the lab members, students, and trainees who helped with this project.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.920383/full#supplementary-material

References

Alcaro, A., Brennan, A., and Conversi, D. (2021). The SEEKING drive and its fixation: a neuro-psycho-evolutionary approach to the pathology of addiction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:635932. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.635932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., Huber, R., and Panksepp, J. (2007). Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system: an affective neuroethological perspective. Brain Res. Rev. 56, 283–321. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azuero, A. (2016). A note on the magnitude of hazard ratios. Cancer 122, 1298–1299. doi: 10.1002/cncr.29924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babb, S., Malarcher, A., Schauer, G., Asman, K., and Jamal, A. (2017). Quitting smoking among adults — united states, 2000–2015. MMWR. Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 65, 1457–1464. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6552a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Athamneh, L. N., Basso, J. C., Mellis, A. M., DeHart, W. B., Craft, W. H., et al. (2019). Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process: update on the state of the science. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 30, 59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Terry Mueller, E., Koffarnus, M. N., and Gatchalian, K. M. (2012). Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence. Pharmacol. Ther. 134, 287–297. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Koffarnus, M. N., Moody, L., and Wilson, A. G. (2014). The behavioral- and neuro-economic process of temporal discounting: a candidate behavioral marker of addiction. Neuropharmacology 76, 518–527. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Miller, M. L., Yi, R., Kowal, B. P., Lindquist, D. M., and Pitcock, J. A. (2007). Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 90, S85–S91. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Snider, S. E., Quisenberry, A. J., Stein, J. S., and Hanlon, C. A. (2016). Competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory of cocaine addiction: from mechanisms to therapeutic opportunities. Prog. Brain Res. 223, 269–293. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, A. L., Mandelkern, M. A., London, E. D., Childress, A. R., Lee, G. S., Bota, R. G., et al. (2002). Brain metabolic changes during cigarette craving. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 1162–1172. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. A., Burgess, E. S., Sales, S. D., Whiteley, J. A., Evans, D. M., and Miller, I. W. (1998). Reliability and validity of a smoking timeline follow-back interview. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 12, 101–112. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.12.2.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carl, E., Liskiewicz, A., Rivard, C., Alberico, R., Belal, A., Mahoney, M. C., et al. (2020). Dosing parameters for the effects of high-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on smoking cessation: study protocol for a randomized factorial sham-controlled clinical trial. BMC Psychol. 8:42. doi: 10.1186/s40359-020-00403-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieslik, E. C., Zilles, K., Caspers, S., Roski, C., Kellermann, T. S., Jakobs, O., et al. (2012). Is There “One” DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2677–2689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commons, M. L., Mazur, J. E., Nevin, J. A., and Rachlin, H. (2013). The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Vol. V. London: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9781315825502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coughlin, L. N., Tegge, A. N., Sheffer, C. E., and Bickel, W. K. (2020). A machine-learning approach to predicting smoking cessation treatment outcomes. Nicotine Tob. Res. 22, 415–422. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nty259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekhtiari, H., Tavakoli, H., Addolorato, G., Baeken, C., Bonci, A., Campanella, S., et al. (2019). Transcranial electrical and magnetic stimulation (tES and TMS) for addiction medicine: a consensus paper on the present state of the science and the road ahead. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 104, 118–140. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enzmann, D. (2015). Notes on effect size measures for the difference of means from two independent groups: the case of Cohen’sd and Hedges’g. January 12:2015.

Google Scholar

Ernst, M., Heishman, S. J., Spurgeon, L., and London, E. D. (2001). Smoking history and nicotine effects on cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 313–319. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00257-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagerstrom, K. (2012). Determinants of tobacco use and renaming the FTND to the fagerstrom test for cigarette dependence. Nicotine Tob. Res. 14, 75–78. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiocchi, S., Chiaramello, E., Luzi, L., Ferrulli, A., Bonato, M., Roth, Y., et al. (2018). Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation for the addiction treatment: electric field distribution modeling. IEEE J. Electromagn. RF Microw. Med. Biol. 2, 242–248. doi: 10.1109/JERM.2018.2874528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, P. B., Fountain, S., and Daskalakis, Z. J. (2006). A comprehensive review of the effects of rTMS on motor cortical excitability and inhibition. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2584–2596. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.06.712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfield, J. B. B., Lubman, D. I., and Yücel, M. (2014). Anhedonia in substance use disorders: a systematic review of its nature, course and clinical correlates. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 48, 36–51. doi: 10.1177/0004867413508455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, B., Brose, L., Schumann, A., Ulbricht, S., Meyer, C., Völzke, H., et al. (2008). Reasons for not using smoking cessation aids. BMC Public Health 8:129. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., Austelle, C. W., DeVries, W., Mithoefer, O., Badran, B. W., et al. (2015). What goes up, can come down: novel brain stimulation paradigms may attenuate craving and craving-related neural circuitry in substance dependent individuals. Brain Res. 1628, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerström, K. O. (1991). The fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerström tolerance questionnaire. Br. J. Addict. 86, 1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, J. R., Keely, J. P., Niaura, R. S., Ossip-Klein, D. J., Richmond, R. L., and Swan, G. E. (2003). Measures of abstinence in clinical trials: issues and recommendations. Nicotine Tob. Res. 5, 13–25. doi: 10.1080/1462220031000070552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ialongo, C. (2016). Understanding the effect size and its measures. Biochem. Med. 26, 150–163. doi: 10.11613/BM.2016.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H.-Y. (2015). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: effect size. Restor. Dent. Endod. 40, 328–331. doi: 10.5395/rde.2015.40.4.328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 126, 54–70. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.126.1.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koffarnus, M. N., and Bickel, W. K. (2014). A 5-trial adjusting delay discounting task: accurate discount rates in less than one minute. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 22, 222–228. doi: 10.1037/a0035973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., and Bickel, W. K. (2013). Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: a review. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 99, 32–57. doi: 10.1002/jeab.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F. (2008a). A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron 59, 11–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F. (2008b). Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation as a driver of drug-seeking behavior. Drug Discov. Today Dis. Models 5, 207–215. doi: 10.1016/j.ddmod.2009.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., Arends, M. A., and Le Moal, M. (2014). Drugs, Addiction, and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 4:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, H., and Marcus, J. (2012). The overall odds ratio as an intuitive effect size index for multiple logistic regression. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 72, 1001–1014. doi: 10.1177/0013164412445298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lingler, J. H., Schmidt, K. L., Gentry, A. L., Hu, L., and Terhorst, L. A. (2014). A new measure of research participant burden: brief report. J. Empir. Res. Hum. Res. Ethics 9, 46–49. doi: 10.1177/1556264614545037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, M., and Ueno, S. (2017). Comparison of the induced fields using different coil configurations during deep transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS One 12:e0178422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Wier, L. M., David, S. P., Ray, L. A., Bickel, W. K., et al. (2012). The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: a preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging study of delay discounting of monetary and cigarette rewards in smokers. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 202, 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maher, J. M., Markey, J. C., and Ebert-May, D. (2013). The other half of the story: effect size analysis in quantitative research. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 12, 345–351. doi: 10.1187/cbe.13-04-0082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, D., Barrett, S. P., Kelly, J. T., Aw, A., and Dagher, A. (2006). Effects of expectancy and abstinence on the neural response to smoking cues in cigarette smokers: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2728–2738. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S. M., and Bickel, W. K. (2014). A dual-systems perspective on addiction: contributions from neuroimaging and cognitive training. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1327, 62–78. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. J. Neurosci. 27, 5796–5804. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mooney, M., Leventhal, A., and Hatsukami, D. (2006). Attitudes and knowledge about nicotine and nicotine replacement therapy. Nicotine Tob. Res. 8, 435–446. doi: 10.1080/14622200600670397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morphett, K., Partridge, B., Gartner, C., Carter, A., and Hall, W. (2015). Why don’t smokers want help to quit? A qualitative study of smokers’ attitudes towards assisted vs. unassisted quitting. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 6591–6607. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120606591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, K. (2010). A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychol. Rev. 117, 243–255. doi: 10.1037/a0017884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: i’m a k. you’re a k. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 96, 427–439. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhea, M. R. (2004). Determining the magnitude of treatment effects in strength training research through the use of the effect size. J. Strength Cond. Res. 18, 918–920. doi: 10.1519/00124278-200411000-00040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2001). Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96, 103–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2003). Addiction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2008). Review. the incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2011). Screening questionnaire before TMS: an update. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122:1686. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rung, J. M., and Madden, G. J. (2018). Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147, 1349–1381. doi: 10.1037/xge0000462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., and Correa, M. (2002). Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav. Brain Res. 137, 3–25. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00282-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharpe, D. (2015). Chi-Square test is statistically significant: now what? Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 20:8.

Google Scholar

Sheffer, C. E., Bickel, W. K., Brandon, T. H., Franck, C. T., Deen, D., Panissidi, L., et al. (2018). Preventing relapse to smoking with transcranial magnetic stimulation: feasibility and potential efficacy. Drug Alcohol Depend. 182, 8–18. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.09.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheffer, C. E., Christensen, D. R., Landes, R., Carter, L. P., Jackson, L., and Bickel, W. K. (2014). Delay discounting rates: a strong prognostic indicator of smoking relapse. Addict. Behav. 39, 1682–1689. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheffer, C. E., Mennemeier, M., Landes, R. D., Bickel, W. K., Brackman, S., Dornhoffer, J., et al. (2013). Neuromodulation of delay discounting, the reflection effect, and cigarette consumption. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 45, 206–214. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2013.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheffer, C., Mackillop, J., McGeary, J., Landes, R., Carter, L., Yi, R., et al. (2012). Delay discounting, locus of control, and cognitive impulsiveness independently predict tobacco dependence treatment outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. Am. J. Addict. 21, 221–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00224.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. L., Carter, S. M., Chapman, S., Dunlop, S. M., and Freeman, B. (2015). Why do smokers try to quit without medication or counselling? A qualitative study with ex-smokers. BMJ Open 5:e007301. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobell, L. C., and Sobell, M. B. (1992). “Timeline Follow-Back,” in Measuring Alcohol Consumption: Psychosocial and Biochemical Methods, eds R. Z. Litten and J. P. Allen (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press), 41–72. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-0357-5_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spano, M. C., Lorusso, M., Pettorruso, M., Zoratto, F., Di Giuda, D., Martinotti, G., et al. (2019). Anhedonia across borders: transdiagnostic relevance of reward dysfunction for noninvasive brain stimulation endophenotypes. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 25, 1229–1236. doi: 10.1111/cns.13230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

SRNT Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification (2002). Biochemical verification of tobacco use and cessation. Nicotine Tob. Res. 4, 149–159. doi: 10.1080/14622200210123581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). A review of combined TMS-EEG studies to characterize lasting effects of repetitive TMS and assess their usefulness in cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Brain Topogr. 22, 219–232. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0115-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomczak, M., and Tomczak, E. (2014). The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends Sport Sci. 1, 19–25.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

United States Public Health Service Office of the Surgeon General, and National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (US) Office on Smoking and Health (2020). Smoking Cessation: A Report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.

Google Scholar

Xu, J., Mendrek, A., Cohen, M. S., Monterosso, J., Rodriguez, P., Simon, S. L., et al. (2005). Brain activity in cigarette smokers performing a working memory task: effect of smoking abstinence. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zangen, A., Moshe, H., Martinez, D., Barnea-Ygael, N., Vapnik, T., Bystritsky, A., et al. (2021). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for smoking cessation: a pivotal multicenter double-blind randomized controlled trial. World Psychiatry 20, 397–404. doi: 10.1002/wps.20905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial magnetic stimulation, smoking cessation, tobacco dependence treatment, delay discounting, self-regulation, brain stimulation

Citation: Shevorykin A, Carl E, Mahoney MC, Hanlon CA, Liskiewicz A, Rivard C, Alberico R, Belal A, Bensch L, Vantucci D, Thorner H, Marion M, Bickel WK and Sheffer CE (2022) Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Long-Term Smoking Cessation: Preliminary Examination of Delay Discounting as a Therapeutic Target and the Effects of Intensity and Duration. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:920383. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.920383

Received: 14 April 2022; Accepted: 20 June 2022;
Published: 05 July 2022.

Edited by:

Joao Miguel Castelhano, University of Coimbra, Portugal

Reviewed by:

David Conversi, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Mauro Pettorruso, University of Studies G. d’Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Shevorykin, Carl, Mahoney, Hanlon, Liskiewicz, Rivard, Alberico, Belal, Bensch, Vantucci, Thorner, Marion, Bickel and Sheffer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alina Shevorykin, alina.shevorykin@roswellpark.org

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.