Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 May 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic General Anesthesia: Neurobiological Underpinnings of Individual Cognitive Patient Outcome View all 7 articles

Influence of Previous General Anesthesia on Cognitive Impairment: An Observational Study Among 151 Patients

\nFederico Linassi
Federico Linassi1*Alessandro De LaurenzisAlessandro De Laurenzis2Eleonora MaranEleonora Maran2Alessandra GadaldiAlessandra Gadaldi2Leonardo Spano&#x;Leonardo Spano'2Gino GerosaGino Gerosa3Demetrio PittarelloDemetrio Pittarello2Paolo ZanattaPaolo Zanatta4Michele CarronMichele Carron2
  • 1Department of Pharmaceutical and Pharmacological Sciences, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Department of Medicine, Anaesthesiology and Intensive Care, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 3Department of Cardiac, Thoracic, Vascular Sciences and Public Health, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 4Department of Anesthesia and Intensive Care, Integrated University Hospital of Verona, Verona, Italy

Introduction: Preoperative neurocognitive disorder (preO-NCD) is a common condition affecting 14–51. 7% of the elderly population. General anesthesia has already been associated with the one-year post-operative neurocognitive disorder (PostO-NCD), specifically, a deficit in executive function, measured by the Trail Making Test B (TMT-B), but its long-term effects on cognitive function have not been investigated. We aimed to detect preO-NCD prevalence in patients scheduled for cardiac surgery and further investigate the possible role of previous general anesthesia (pGA) in general preoperative cognitive status [measured via the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)] and/or in executive functioning (measured via TMT-B).

Methods: In this observational, prospective study, 151 adult patients scheduled for elective cardiac surgery underwent MoCA and TMT-B. Data on age, education, pGA, comorbidities, and laboratory results were collected.

Results: We discovered a general cognitive function impairment of 79.5% and an executive function impairment of 22%. Aging is associated with an increased likelihood (OR 2.99, p = 0.047) and education with a decreased likelihood (OR 0.35, p = 0.0045) of general cognitive impairment, but only education was significantly associated with a decreased likelihood (OR 0.22, p = 0.021) of executive function impairment. While pGA did not significantly affect preO-NCD, a noteworthy interaction between aging and pGA was found, resulting in a synergistic effect, increasing the likelihood of executive function impairment (OR 9.740, p = 0.0174).

Conclusion: We found a high prevalence of preO-NCD in patients scheduled for cardiac surgery. General cognitive function impairment is highly associated with advancing age (not pGA). However, older patients with at least one pGA appeared to be at an increased risk of preO-NCD, especially executive function impairment, suggesting that TMT-B should be associated with MoCA in the preoperative cognitive evaluation in this population.

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) refers to a range of conditions that occur frequently in the general population; it is estimated that 14–51.7% (Scott et al., 2018) of elderly people suffer from MCI and that an additional 10% suffer from a major cognitive disorder or dementia. MCI is described as a mild cognitive and functional impairment that is greater than expected for a patient's age and education level but is not severe enough to warrant a diagnosis of dementia (DSM-5 defines MCI as a “mild neurocognitive disorder”), thus representing part of a declining cognitive trajectory between normal aging and overt dementia (Lonie et al., 2009; Crosby et al., 2011).

Individuals with MCI are at a greater risk of progressing to major cognitive disorders; however, studies have shown that up to 25% of patients with MCI can revert to normal cognition (Malek-Ahmadi, 2016). This makes MCI an important temporal “window” in which intervening or delaying progression to dementia may be possible (Anderson, 2019). The core clinical criteria for the diagnosis of MCI are as follows: concern about a change in cognition expressed by the patient or an informant, impairment in one or more cognitive domains, the preservation of independence in functional abilities, and absence of dementia (Albert et al., 2011).

The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), a test developed specifically for the detection of MCI (Nasreddine et al., 2005), has a sensitivity of 80–100% and a specificity of 50–76% using a cut-off point of 25 (Lin et al., 2013) to make a diagnosis, and it has been shown to be more accurate than Mini Mental Status Evaluation (MMSE) in accurately differentiating individuals with MCI from those with normal cognition (McKhann et al., 2011).

In addition to the MoCA, various neuropsychological tests can be administered for the diagnosis of MCI; among others, the Trail Making Test (TMT) is one of the most popular neuropsychological tests that provides information on visual search, scanning, speed of processing, mental flexibility, and executive function. It has been shown to be highly sensitive in detecting anesthesia-related impairment and post-operative neurocognitive disorder (PostO-NCD; Reitan, 1955; Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009). The TMT consists of two parts, Part A and Part B; measures of rote memory, visual scanning, graphomotor speed, and visuomotor processing speed were more related to the performance of the TMT-A score, while working memory and inhibition control were mainly associated with the TMT-B, which is an indicator of executive functioning (Arbuthnott and Frank, 2000; Llinàs-Reglà et al., 2017).

The etiology of MCI is complex, and the roles of multiple biological, social, and environmental factors have been investigated. Although they are non-modifiable, age, sex, family history of dementia, and genetics are considered to be the most important risk factors (Deckers et al., 2015). To develop more effective preventative and treatment interventions, researchers have paid increased attention to modifiable environmental and lifestyle risk factors; among these, lower educational level, vascular disease, obesity, and sleep-disordered breathing seem to have a major impact (Plassman et al., 2008; Iwashyna et al., 2010; Petersen et al., 2010; Yaffe et al., 2011).

Other potential risk factors for MCI, especially in the population with high cardiovascular comorbidity risk factors, are previous transitory ischaemic attacks (TIAs; Nicolas et al., 2020): one-third of patients with TIA have a profile similar to that of vascular MCI (van Rooij et al., 2016).

The influence of anesthesia and surgery on cognitive functions has been extensively studied. Data from animal experiments, in particular, suggest that anesthetic exposure causes neuronal apoptosis and impairs normal synaptic development and conformation (Rizzi et al., 2008; Paule et al., 2011; Zou et al., 2011; Lei et al., 2012). This anesthetic-induced damage might contribute to the following aging-related cerebral changes: cerebral atrophy, especially in the temporal and prefrontal regions (Hafkemeijer et al., 2014), areas involved in TMT-B performance (Zakzanis et al., 2005), and the average rate of brain volume loss (Enzinger et al., 2005).

In line with the recommendations for the nomenclature of cognitive change associated with anesthesia and surgery, the term “perioperative neurocognitive disorder” refers to “a cognitive deficit or change detected in the preoperative or post-operative period” (Evered et al., 2018). Cognitive deficits identified during preoperative assessment [hereafter referred to as preoperative neurocognitive disorders (preO-NCD)] cannot be related to the scheduled surgery and anesthesia and follow the classification of disorders as observed in the general population: mild NCD (e.g., MCI) or major NCD (e.g., dementia).

It has been estimated that preO-NCD is itself a risk factor for dementia [it converts to dementia at a rate of 10% per year (Farias et al., 2009)], delirium (Marcantonio et al., 1994; Rudolph et al., 2009; Lloyd et al., 2012), and postO-NCD (Moller et al., 1998; Lloyd et al., 2012; Paredes et al., 2016), which is particularly frequent in cardiac surgery (Linassi et al., 2022). PreO-NCD has also been associated with a strong decline in long-term behavioral functional capacity 1 year after cardiac surgery (Patron et al., 2013).

Executive function, measured via TMT-B, has been associated with 12-month postO-NCD in cardiac surgery (Messerotti Benvenuti et al., 2014); however, to the best of our knowledge, no trials have been performed to investigate the association between previous general anesthesia (pGA) and preO-NCD.

Hence, this study aims to determine the prevalence of preO-NCD in cardiac surgery and examine the role of pGA as a possible risk factor for preoperative neurocognitive disorders, measured via MoCA (for general cognitive function impairment) and TMT-B (for executive function impairment).

Materials and Methods

This observational, prospective study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Azienda Ospedaliera di Padova, Italy (Protocol ID: 65408) and registered at clinicaltrials.gov (NCT04182477).

During the study period, from January 2020 to September 2021, 160 out of 168 adult patients were scheduled for cardiac surgery, coronary artery bypass grafting (CABG) or valve surgery (VS), at Padua University Hospital and provided written informed consent to participate in the study.

We excluded patients with any neurological or psychiatric disease and renal insufficiency in anamnesis. Among the neurological exclusion criteria and known strokes, patients with a history of TIA or other neurological diseases known at the time of administration of the tests were also excluded. Additionally, we excluded patients who were unable to understand the information contained in the informed consent form, patients affected by uncontrolled arterial hypertension (AHT) and diabetes mellitus (DM), patients with a history of drug and alcohol abuse, and patients without the preservation of independence in functional abilities [discovered with activity daily living (ADL) and instrumental activity daily living (IADL)] or who had significant impairment in social or occupational functioning.

Therefore, of the 160 patients who gave their consent to participate, five were excluded because they did not meet the inclusion criteria, and four were excluded because of incomplete data. Therefore, data from the remaining 151 patients were analyzed (Figure 1). These patients underwent two neurocognitive tests:

- The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was administered to assess for general cognitive function impairment (visuospatial, executive function, naming, memory, attention, language, abstraction, and orientation).

- The Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B) was administered to assess executive function impairment.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart illustrating patients' recruitment.

The tests were performed and interpreted in a silent room of the cardiac surgery unit and delivered by a neuropsychologist (ADL).

Clinical End-Points and Variables

The primary objective was to determine the prevalence of preO-NCD in patients enrolled for cardiac surgery, analyzing both the prevalence of general cognitive function impairment [measured by a MoCA score ≤ 25 after adding 1 point if the years of education were ≤ 12 (Nasreddine et al., 2005)] and that of executive function impairment [measured by a TMT-B score ≥ 283 after the correction for age and education or the inability to complete the test in 7 min (Giovagnoli et al., 1996)]. The secondary objective was to determine whether exposure to pGAs impacts the incidence of general cognitive function and executive function impairment. The variables collected were as follows:

- Demographic variables: sex, age (continuous and dichotomised into ≥ 65 years old), weight, height, body mass index (BMI), education years (also dichotomised into ≥ 10 years), right- or left-handedness.

- Anamnestic variables (investigated during the interview with the patient and confirmed by the hospital or personal archives): age at and number of pGAs (dichotomised into ≥ 1, pediatric pGA if at < 18 years old and greater than or equal to the mean of years passed from the last pGA), diagnosis of DM, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg·m−2), obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome (OSAS), thyropathy, internal carotid artery (ICA) stenosis (diagnosed via ecography performed within 6 months before the interview, expressed in percentage and dichotomised into ≥ 35%; Arntzen et al., 2012).

- Blood tests parameters: C-reactive protein (CPR), direct bilirubin, and total white blood cells (WBC), dichotomised into greater than our laboratory sensibility (respectively > 2.9 mg/L, > 5.1 umol/L, and > 11·109/L).

- American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) Physical Status Classification and surgery type (VS or CABG) for enlisted patients.

Statistical Analysis

We based the sample size calculation on an MCI prevalence of 35% (Silbert et al., 2007) for the population of cardiac surgery candidates. We considered a confidence level of 95% and a margin of error of 7.5% (Daniel and Cross, 1999). Hence, we calculated a sample of 151 patients.

We created three databases, each of which was analyzed separately: one for all the patients, one for only those who had undergone at least one administration of general anesthesia, and one for only patients who had never undergone general anesthesia administration. To evaluate the impact of timing from the last pGA on cognitive function in the elderly, we further performed a subgroup analysis on the population mean of years passed from the last pGA.

We used the Shapiro–Wilk test to test for normality. We reported continuous, normally distributed variables as mean ± standard deviation (SD) and continuous, non-normally distributed variables as median [interquartile range, IQR] and minimum to maximum. The continuous variables were dichotomised according to the previous literature's cut-off findings where available or based on values higher or lower than the mean or median value (if normally or not normally distributed, respectively). We tested for differences between the groups using the two-tailed student t-test or the two-tailed Mann–Whitney U-test for normally and non-normally distributed variables, respectively. Using the chi-square test, we reported the categorical variables as numbers (percentages) and tested for differences between the groups.

Multiple linear regression analysis was also performed to explain the relationship between one dependent variable (MoCA and TMT-B scores) and the independent variables (sex, age, obesity, education years, past general anesthesia, COPD, AHT, DM, thyropathy, CPR, WBC count, bilirubin, and surgery type). The dependent variables were dichotomised as follows: MoCA (deficitary vs. not deficitary) and TMT-B (deficitary vs. not deficitary). The independent variables were dichotomised as follows: sex (male vs. female); age [younger patients (18–64 years) vs. older patients (≥65 years)]; obesity [no obesity (BMI < 30 kg·m−2) vs. obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg·m−2)]; education years (≥10 years vs. <10 years); WBC count; bilirubin; CPR according to the laboratory's normal range values (abnormal vs. normal value); surgery type (CABG vs. VS); and past general anesthesia (0 vs. ≥ 1). Other variables (COPD, AHT, DM, and thyropathy) were dichotomised as the presence or absence of pathology. The odds ratios (ORs) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) were determined. Multicollinearity was assessed using specific or generalized variance inflation factors. The Akaike information criterion and backward/forward stepwise regression analysis were used to choose the best model.

We used R version 3.4.0 (2017-04-21) for all statistical analyses and considered p < 0.05 statistically significant.

Results

The mean (±SD) age, weight, height, and BMI of 151 patients were as follows: 66.8 ± 10.2 years old, 76.0 ± 13.4 kg, 170.2 ± 9.2 cm, and 26.2 ± 4.1 kg m−2, respectively. Table 1 shows the patients' general characteristics subdivided according to the variables considered.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. General patient, drug, and monitoring characteristics based on baseline cognitive impairment.

A total of 120 patients (79.5%) had preoperative general cognitive function impairment, measured by a deficitary MoCA, whereas the remaining 33 patients (22%) had preoperative executive function impairment, measured by a deficitary TMT-B. No patient showed a deficitary TMT-B without a deficitary MoCA—i.e., no patients had deficits in executive function without general cognitive function impairment. Table 2 contains the patients' general characteristics subdivided according to the detection of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (measured by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. General patient, drug, and monitoring characteristics based on baseline cognitive impairment.

Among the 101 patients with at least one pGA, 79 (78.2%) had general cognitive function impairment, whereas the remaining 22 patients (21.8%) had executive function impairment. Supplementary Table 1 contains the characteristics of the patients with at least one pGA, subdivided according to the detection of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (detected by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

Among the 50 patients without any pGA, 41 (82%) had general cognitive function impairment, whereas the remaining 11 patients (22%) had executive function impairment. Supplementary Table 2 contains the characteristics of the patients without any pGA, subdivided according to the onset of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (measured by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

Table 1 contains the patients' general characteristics.

Table 2 contains the patients' general characteristics subdivided according to the onset of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (measured by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

Supplementary Table 1 contains the characteristics of patients with at least one pGA, subdivided according to the onset of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (measured by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

Supplementary Table 2 contains the characteristics of patients without any pGA, subdivided according to the onset of preoperative general cognitive function impairment (measured by a deficitary MoCA) and executive function impairment (measured by a deficitary TMT-B).

Table 3 contains the multiple linear regression analysis.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Logistic regression analysis to explain the relationship between MoCA and TMT-B tests and variables considered.

Supplementary Table 3 shows the role of age in patients with pGA performed better or worse than 16 years from the neurocognitive evaluation.

General Cognitive Function Impairment

Advancing age (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into age > 65 years old, p < 0.01, OR 3.6), increasing BMI (p < 0.05), DM (p < 0.05, OR 3.1), AHT (p < 0.01, OR 3.4), increasing left ICA stenosis (p < 0.01), and increasing contralateral-handed ICA stenosis (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into > 35%, p < 0.05, OR 3.6) are associated with preoperative general cognitive function impairment, while increasing education years are protective (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into > 10 years, p < 0.01, OR 0.3); pGA does not seem to have any effect on general cognitive function in the general population (Table 2).

Among the patients with at least one pGA, advancing age (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into age > 65 years old, p < 0.05, OR 3.5), DM (p < 0.05, OR 4.3), increasing left ICA stenosis (p < 0.01), and increasing contralateral-handed ICA stenosis (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into >35%, p < 0.05, OR 4.5) are associated with general cognitive function impairment, whereas increasing education years (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into > 10 years, p < 0.01, OR 0.3) is protective (Supplementary Table 1). Among the patients without any pGA, advancing age (p = 0.01, only if considered as a continuous variable) and AHT (p < 0.05, OR 6.8) are associated with general cognitive function impairment (Supplementary Table 2).

When we analyzed the role of age in the subgroup of patients who underwent at least one pGA more than 16 years before cognitive evaluation, we found that age > 65 years old is associated with a general cognitive function deficit (p < 0.01, OR 11.4), whereas in the subgroup of patients who underwent general anesthesia less than 16 years before evaluation (55 patients), it is not (p = 0.4; Supplementary Table 3).

Executive Function Deficit

Advancing age (p < 0.01) and DM (p < 0.05, OR 2.4) are associated with executive function impairment, whereas increasing education years (p < 0.001, also dichotomised into >10 years, p < 0.05, OR 0.3) is protective; pGA does not seem to have any effect on executive function in the general population (Table 2).

Among the patients with at least one pGA, advancing age (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into > 5 years old, p < 0.05, OR 4.5) and increasing WBC count (p < 0.05, OR 19.4) are associated with an executive function deficit, whereas increasing education years (p < 0.01, also dichotomised into > 10 years, p < 0.05, OR 0.2) is protective (Supplementary Table 1). Among the patients without any pGA, no variables were associated with executive function impairment (Supplementary Table 2).

When we analyzed the role of age in the subgroup of patients who underwent at least one pGA more than 16 years before cognitive evaluation (Supplementary Table 3), we found that age > 65 years old is associated with an executive function deficit (p < 0.05, OR 13.8), whereas in the subgroup of patients who underwent pGA <16 years before cognitive evaluation, it is not (p = 0.2).

Logistic Regression Analysis

According to the logistic regression analysis results (Table 3), aging was associated with an increased likelihood (OR 2.99, p = 0.047) and education years with a decreased likelihood (OR 0.35, p = 0.0045) of MoCA impairment, whereas only education years were significantly associated with a decreased likelihood (OR 0.22, p = 0.021) of TMT-B impairment (Table 3). Past anesthesia was not shown to significantly affect MoCA and TMT-B, but a noteworthy interaction between aging and pGA was found, resulting in a synergistic effect with regard to increasing the likelihood of TMT-B impairment [OR 9.740 (1.49–63.50), p = 0.0174].

Discussion

Our results show a high prevalence of preO-NCD: 79.5% of the patients present a general cognitive function deficit, whereas 22% of them present an executive function deficit. This prevalence, when compared to the incidence of MCI in the general population, is high, possibly because the international MoCA cut off of 26 (also adopted in our trial) seems to underestimate the Italian performance, resulting in global results lower than those in other countries (Bosco et al., 2017). However, our MCI prevalence is in line with recent literature findings in cardiac surgery (Scott et al., 2018; Itagaki et al., 2020), and it might increase in the next few years, which may be due to the advancing age of patients scheduled for cardiac surgery and the refinement in cardiac surgery technique, which allows surgery in frailer patients with more comorbidities. Since frailty and preO-NCD have been recently linked to delirium (Itagaki et al., 2020), delayed neurocognitive recovery, and postO-NCD—and this is related to prolonged intensive care unit (ICU), hospital stay, morbidity, and mortality (Needham et al., 2017)—a preoperative neurocognitive screening appears to be mandatory in cardiac surgery, where these post-operative complications appear to be very high (Linassi et al., 2022).

Our results about the synergic association of aging with at least one pGA in executive function impairment (measured via TMT-B, OR 9.7) are intriguing because neurocognitive impairment shows better relationships with pGA exposure than a general cognitive screening test such as MoCA (Messerotti Benvenuti et al., 2014). The detection of a preoperative TMT-B deficit might help track down more patients who are susceptible to anesthetics and should be strictly monitored if undergoing another form of surgery. However, future studies should elucidate the role of a pre-existing deficit in executive function and its role in postO-NCD.

Impact of Age, Education Years, and pGA on PreO-NCD

Our linear regression analysis results show that aging is associated with general cognitive function impairment (OR 2.99), whereas it is not significant for an executive function deficit. Instead, a total of > 10 education years seems protective for both general cognitive function and executive function impairment (OR 0.35 and 0.22, respectively), confirming previous literature findings (Giovagnoli et al., 1996; Ivnik et al., 1996).

If we considered only patients with at least one pGA (Supplementary Table 1), we observed that age > 65 years old is strongly associated with executive function impairment (OR 4.5) rather than general cognitive function impairment (OR 3.5). Furthermore, our linear regression analysis confirms a synergic association between aging and pGA solely on the TMT-B (OR 9.7). This suggests that, in older patients scheduled for surgery with general anesthesia, the TMT-B should be performed together with the MoCA in the preoperative cognitive evaluation since it can better detect past neurocognitive impairment associated with anesthesia.

The fact that pGA, in the whole analyzed population, is not significant for executive function impairment (Table 2), but the association between pGA and advancing age is, whereas the association between no pGA and advancing age is not (Supplementary Tables 1, 2, respectively), suggests that anesthesia damage to executive function may be exacerbated by aging. Furthermore, the fact that, among the patients who underwent general anesthesia > 16 years before the neurocognitive evaluation, aging is significantly associated with general cognitive and executive function impairment (OR 11.4, p < 0.01, and OR 13.8, p < 0.05, respectively), while in patients with pGA <16 years before, it is not (p = 0.4 and p = 0.6, respectively, for general cognitive and executive function impairment), suggests a period of exposure-dependent damage.

Data from animal experiments strongly indicated that anesthetics commonly used in clinical practice can cause neuronal apoptosis and impair normal synaptic development and conformation, and this is particularly true in the developing brain (Rizzi et al., 2008; Paule et al., 2011; Zou et al., 2011; Lei et al., 2012). This anesthetic-induced damage adds up to the age-related loss in gray matter and to the age-related changes affecting the brain. Cerebral atrophy is particularly relevant and affects both white and gray matter, most prominent in the temporal and prefrontal regions (Hafkemeijer et al., 2014), areas involved in TMT-B performance (Zakzanis et al., 2005). Additionally, the average rate of brain volume decrease is −0.4 ± 0.29% per year and is two times faster in the elderly (age from 65 to 75 years) than in the younger population (age 50–54 years; −0.55 ± 0.29% vs −0.28 ± 0.23%; Enzinger et al., 2005).

This explains our results and strengthens the hypothesis that, in the preoperative evaluation, the TMT-B should be associated with the MoCA when evaluating every old patient who has undergone at least one pGA because it can be used to detect patients who are more susceptible to anesthetics and whose cognitive performance may worsen after another pGA. Future studies, however, should determine the association between aging, pGA, postO-NCD, and the utility of close neurocognitive follow-up in the post-operative period.

Another interpretation of the fact that the last general anesthesia administration > 16 years from the cognitive evaluation is associated with an increased risk of preO-NCD is that the conduction of general anesthesia has changed in the last few decades, with inhalational anesthesia being gradually replaced by total intravenous anesthesia (Al-Rifai and Mulvey, 2016). Some volatile anesthetics, such as halothane and sevoflurane, have been associated, in pre-clinical trials, with the increasing aggregation of Aβ amyloid (Eckenhoff et al., 2004), typical of Alzheimer's dementia (Tang and Eckenhoff, 2013), and neuronal apoptosis (Lu et al., 2010); on the contrary, it has been postulated that propofol can enhance cognitive function, reducing Aβ-induced mitochondrial dysfunction and caspase activation (Shao et al., 2014). Future studies should determine whether the last pGA administration of more than 16 years is a preO-NCD risk factor in and of itself or if it is associated with the type of anesthetic adopted.

In our results, education years is a very important protective factor in preventing preO-NCD in both the general population (p < 0.005, OR 0.3, for general cognitive impairment; p < 0.05, OR 0.3, for executive function impairment) and in patients with pGA (p < 0.005, OR 0.3 for general cognitive impairment and OR 0.2 for executive function impairment). Intriguingly, this protective effect appears to be diminished in patients without pGA and is not statistically significant. This can be explained from a statistical point of view (only 50 patients in the no-pGA group vs. 101 patients in the pGA group), but it may also have some neurocognitive basis.

Schooling years, in fact, alter the “cognitive reserve,” an innovative concept that explains why some people can tolerate more age- and comorbidity-related brain injury changes than others to maintain better cognitive function (Stern, 2012). Since aging and pGA have a synergistic effect on cognitive impairment, the beneficial impact of schooling on cognitive reserve may be higher in these patients than in those who did not receive these kinds of brain injuries. Future studies should, however, be conducted to confirm this theory.

Role of Comorbidities on PreO-NCD

Our results confirm the role of multiple risk factors affecting cognitive functions, particularly DM (Cheng et al., 2012), AHT (Reitz et al., 2007), obesity (Sanderlin et al., 2017), and inflammation [i.e., higher WBC (Kao et al., 2011)]. Further, the fact that in the subgroup of patients with at least one pGA, DM is associated with higher general cognitive impairment than that in the general population (OR 4.3 vs. OR 3.1), whereas in patients without pGA, it is not significant at all, which suggests that special attention should be paid to these patients, specifically monitoring the MoCA. This result may be due to a synergistic effect between neuronal apoptosis and Aβ accumulation related to aging, anesthetic exposure, hyperglycaemia, and inflammation (Ninomiya, 2014).

In our sample, AHT is related to general cognitive function impairment (OR 3.4), but it is not related to executive function deficit, and in our subgroup analysis, it reaches statistical significance only in patients without pGA. It has been proposed that AHT impacts general cognitive function impairment through cerebrovascular injury (Pires et al., 2013), which is associated with impairment in global cognitive functions and episodic memory, typical of vascular dementia and of so-called vascular MCI (Meguro and Dodge, 2019). Thus, our results confirm that AHT and pGA have no synergistic effects, likely because they affect different brain areas and that the MoCA (and not the TMT-B) is altered in vascular MCI.

Increased BMI is significantly associated with deficitary MoCA (p < 0.05), but we found no significant risk associated with the dichotomised variable (BMI > 30 kg/m2), suggesting that this cut-off may be reconsidered. The correlation of obesity with cognitive decline is multifactorial as hypertension, dyslipidemia, and diabetes are frequently associated (Gustafson, 2006). In addition, hormones, cytokines, and inflammation mediators secreted by adipose tissue directly affect neuronal structure and functions.

TIAs are among the cardiovascular comorbidities that may potentially have an impact on cognitive function (van Rooij et al., 2016; Nicolas et al., 2020). TIAs are an exclusion criterion in this study; however, a population with misdiagnosed or underdiagnosed TIA is known to exist, and some studies [(Kessler C, Thomas KE. An examination of economic outcomes associated with misdiagnosis or undertreatment of TIA. Am J Manag Care. 2009 Jun;15(6 Suppl): S170-6. PMID: 19601692)] report that half of the patients who experience TIA are undiagnosed. Since TIAs share similar risk factors as those faced by enrolled patients, it is not possible to know the impact of undiagnosed TIAs in this study.

Our data show that left ICA stenosis and contralateral-handed ICA stenosis are associated with general cognitive dysfunction but not with executive function impairment. In the literature, two possible mechanisms of cognitive decline in patients with ICA disease have been suggested, cerebral emboli and hypoperfusion, which may lead to silent brain infarction and the loss of cerebral circulation autoregulation (Demarin et al., 2012). Intriguingly, the OR for general cognitive function impairment is higher in patients with contralateral ICA stenosis > 35%: 4.5 vs. 3.6 for left ICA stenosis. It is known that, in patients with MCI, the hippocampus, the parahippocampal gyrus, and the entorhinal cortex present more severe atrophy in the gray matter in the left hemisphere than in the right hemisphere (Thompson et al., 1998); however, future studies should elucidate the role of laterality in ICA stenosis and preO-NCD detection.

Limitations

This study has some noteworthy limitations. First, our monocentric trial enrolled only patients scheduled for cardiac surgery, with important comorbidities that justify our higher preO-NCD incidence, but our results should be confirmed with other multi-centric studies in a non-cardiologic population.

Second, other studies with higher sample sizes should confirm our results. Further studies are also necessary to investigate the impact of pediatric pGA and the influence of different anesthetic drugs and techniques on cognitive impairment development, as well as the roles of ICA stenosis and hemisphere dominance on cognitive impairment. Another limitation is that we have not performed cerebral diagnostic radiology to detect previous undiagnosed ministrokes; further studies should elucidate their impact on preO-NCD and the association with pGA.

Conclusion

Our study found a high incidence of preO-NCD among patients scheduled for cardiac surgery, confirming that this population should be strictly monitored for cognitive impairment, particularly before undergoing general anesthesia. Furthermore, in our linear regression analysis, advancing age is associated with general cognitive function impairment (OR 2.99) rather than executive function impairment, but there is a synergic association between aging and at least one pGA in executive function impairment (OR 9.7). This suggests that among the elderly, the TMT-B should be associated with the MoCA in preoperative cognitive evaluation.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://sendgb.com/ZMQN8KRDT1u.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of Azienda Ospedaliera di Padova, Italy (Protocol ID: 65408). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FL, AD, EM, LS, and AG conceived of the study, acquired, collected and analyzed data, and drafted and revised the final manuscript. FL, LS, GG, DP, PZ, and MC collected data, performed the statistical analysis, analyzed data, and revised the final manuscript. AD, EM, AG, GG, DP, PZ, and MC participated in the conceiving of the study, analyzed data, participated in the discussion of the results, and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.810046/full#supplementary-material

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Rifai, Z., and Mulvey, D. (2016). Principles of total intravenous anesthesia: practical aspects of using total intravenous anesthesia. BJA Educ. 16, 276–280. doi: 10.1093/bjaed/mkv074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, N. D. (2019). State of the science on mild cognitive impairment (MCI). CNS Spectr. 24, 78–87. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuthnott, K., and Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arntzen, K. A., Schirmer, H., Johnsen, S. H., Wilsgaard, T., and Mathiesen, E. B. (2012). Carotid artery plaque progression and cognitive decline: the Tromsø Study 1994-2008. Eur. J. Neurol. 19, 1318–1324. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03728.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosco, A., Spano, G., Caffò, A. O., Lopez, A., Grattagliano, I., Saracino, G., et al. (2017). Italians do it worse. Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) optimal cut-off scores for people with probable Alzheimer's disease and with probable cognitive impairment. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 1113–1120. doi: 10.1007/s40520-017-0727-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, G., Huang, C., Deng, H., and Wang, H. (2012). Diabetes as a risk factor for dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Internal Med. J. 42, 484–491. doi: 10.1111/j.1445-5994.2012.02758.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crosby, G., Culley, D. J., and Hyman, B. T. (2011). Preoperative cognitive assessment of the elderly surgical patient: a call for action. Anesthesiology 114, 1265–1268. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31821b1bc8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniel, W. W., and Cross, C. L. (1999). Biostatistics: A Foundation for Analysis in the Health Sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Deckers, K., van Boxtel, M. P., Schiepers, O. J., de Vugt, M., Muñoz Sánchez, J. L., Anstey, K. J., et al. (2015). Target risk factors for dementia prevention: a systematic review and Delphi consensus study on the evidence from observational studies. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 30, 234–246. doi: 10.1002/gps.4245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demarin, V., Zavoreo, I., and Kes, V. B. (2012). Carotid artery disease and cognitive impairment. J. Neurol. Sci. 322, 107–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckenhoff, R. G., Johansson, J. S., Wei, H., Carnini, A., Kang, B., Wei, W., et al. (2004). Inhaled anesthetic enhancement of amyloid-beta oligomerization and cytotoxicity. Anesthesiology 101, 703–709. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200409000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enzinger, C., Fazekas, F., Matthews, P. M., Ropele, S., Schmidt, H., Smith, S., et al. (2005). Risk factors for progression of brain atrophy in aging: six-year follow-up of normal subjects. Neurology 64, 1704–1711. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000161871.83614.BB

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evered, L., Silbert, B., Knopman, D. S., Scott, D. A., DeKosky, S. T., Rasmussen, L. S., et al. (2018). Recommendations for the nomenclature of cognitive change associated with anaesthesia and surgery-2018. Anesthesiology 129, 872–879. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Harvey, D., and DeCarli, C. (2009). Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs. community-based cohorts. Archiv. Neurol. 66, 1151–1157. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovagnoli, A. R., Del Pesce, M., Mascheroni, S., Simoncelli, M., Laiacona, M., and Capitani, E. (1996). Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 17, 305–309. doi: 10.1007/BF01997792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafson, D. (2006). Adiposity indices and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 5, 713–720. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70526-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hafkemeijer, A., Altmann-Schneider, I., de Craen, A. J., Slagboom, P. E., van der Grond, J., and Rombouts, S. A. (2014). Associations between age and gray matter volume in anatomical brain networks in middle-aged to older adults. Aging Cell 13, 1068–1074. doi: 10.1111/acel.12271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itagaki, A., Sakurada, K., Matsuhama, M., Yajima, J., Yamashita, T., and Kohzuki, M. (2020). Impact of frailty and mild cognitive impairment on delirium after cardiac surgery in older patients. J. Cardiol. 76, 147–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jjcc.2020.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivnik, J. R., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G., and Petersen, R. C. (1996). Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clin. Neuropsychol. 10, 262–278. doi: 10.1080/13854049608406689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwashyna, T. J., Ely, E. W., Smith, D. M., and Langa, K. M. (2010). Long-term cognitive impairment and functional disability among survivors of severe sepsis. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 304, 1787–1794. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kao, T. W., Chang, Y. W., Chou, C. C., Hu, J., Yu, Y. H., and Kuo, H. K. (2011). White blood cell count and psychomotor cognitive performance in the elderly. Eur. J. Clin. Investig. 41, 513–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2362.2010.02438.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, X., Guo, Q., and Zhang, J. (2012). Mechanistic insights into neurotoxicity induced by anesthetics in the developing brain. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 13, 6772–6799. doi: 10.3390/ijms13066772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J. S., O'Connor, E., Rossom, R. C., Perdue, L. A., Burda, B. U., Thompson, M., et al. (2013). Screening for cognitive impairment in older adults: an evidence update for the U.S. Prev. Serv. Task Force. 2013:730. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-159-9-201311050-00730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linassi, F., Maran, E., De Laurenzis, A., Tellaroli, P., Kreuzer, M., Schneider, G., et al. (2022). Targeted temperature management in cardiac surgery: a systematic review and meta-analysis on post-operative cognitive outcomes. Br. J. Anaesthesia 128, 11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2021.09.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinàs-Reglà, J., Vilalta-Franch, J., López-Pousa, S., Calvó-Perxas, L., Torrents Rodas, D., and Garre-Olmo, J. (2017). The trail making test. Assessment 24, 183–196. doi: 10.1177/1073191115602552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, D. G., Daqing, M., and Marcela, P. V. (2012). Cognitive decline after anaesthesia and critical care. Continuing Educ. Anaesthesia Crit. Care Pain 12, 105–109. doi: 10.1093/bjaceaccp/mks004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lonie, J. A., Tierney, K. M., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). Screening for mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 24, 902–915. doi: 10.1002/gps.2208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, Y., Wu, X., Dong, Y., Xu, Z., Zhang, Y., and Xie, Z. (2010). Anesthetic sevoflurane causes neurotoxicity differently in neonatal naïve and Alzheimer disease transgenic mice. Anesthesiology 112, 1404–1416. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181d94de1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malek-Ahmadi, M. (2016). Reversion from mild cognitive impairment to normal cognition: a meta-analysis. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 30, 324–330. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcantonio, E. R., Goldman, L., Mangione, C. M., Ludwig, L. E., Muraca, B., Haslauer, C. M., et al. (1994). A clinical prediction rule for delirium after elective noncardiac surgery. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 271, 134–139. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03510260066030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., and Kawas, C. H. (2011), The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meguro, K., and Dodge, H. H. (2019). Vascular mild cognitive impairment: identifying disease in community-dwelling older adults, reducing risk factors, and providing support. The Osaki-Tajiri and Kurihara Projects. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 70, S293–S302. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Patron, E., Zanatta, P., Polesel, E., and Palomba, D. (2014). Pre-existing cognitive status is associated with reduced behavioral functional capacity in patients 3 months after cardiac surgery: an extension study. General Hosp. Psychiatr. 36, 368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2014.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moller, J. T., Cluitmans, P., Rasmussen, L. S., Houx, P., Rasmussen, H., Canet, J., et al. (1998). Long-term post-operative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. Int. Study Post Operat. Cogn. Dysfunct. Lancet 351, 857–861. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)07382-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Needham, M. J., Webb, C. E., and Bryden, D. C. (2017). Postoperative cognitive dysfunction and dementia: what we need to know and do. Br. J. Anaesthesia 119(Suppl.1), i115–i125. doi: 10.1093/bja/aex354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolas, K., Levi, C., Evans, T. J., Michie, P. T., Magin, P., Quain, D., et al. (2020). Cognition in the first year after a minor stroke, transient ischemic attack, or mimic event and the role of vascular risk factors. Front. Neurol. 11:216. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ninomiya, T. (2014). Diabetes mellitus and dementia. Curr. Diabet. Rep. 14:487. doi: 10.1007/s11892-014-0487-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paredes, S., Cortínez, L., Contreras, V., and Silbert, B. (2016). Post-operative cognitive dysfunction at 3 months in adults after non-cardiac surgery: a qualitative systematic review. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 60, 1043–1058. doi: 10.1111/aas.12724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patron, E., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Zanatta, P., Polesel, E., and Palomba, D. (2013). Pre-existing depressive symptoms are associated with long-term cognitive decline in patients after cardiac surgery. General Hosp. Psychiatr. 35, 472–479. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paule, M. G., Li, M., Allen, R. R., Liu, F., Zou, X., Hotchkiss, C., et al. (2011). Ketamine anesthesia during the first week of life can cause long-lasting cognitive deficits in rhesus monkeys. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 220–230. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Roberts, R. O., Knopman, D. S., Geda, Y. E., Cha, R. H., Pankratz, V. S., et al. (2010). Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment is higher in men. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology 75, 889–897. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f11d85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pires, P. W., Dams Ramos, C. M., Matin, N., and Dorrance, A. M. (2013). The effects of hypertension on the cerebral circulation. Am. J. Physiol. 304, H1598–H1614. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00490.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plassman, B. L., Langa, K. M., Fisher, G. G., Heeringa, S. G., Weir, D. R., Ofstedal, M. B., et al. (2008). Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Ann. Internal Med. 148, 427–434. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-148-6-200803180-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1955). The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 19, 393–394. doi: 10.1037/h0044509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitz, C., Tang, M. X., Manly, J., Mayeux, R., and Luchsinger, J. A. (2007). Hypertension and the risk of mild cognitive impairment. Archiv. Neurol. 64, 1734–1740. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.12.1734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzi, S., Carter, L. B., Ori, C., and Jevtovic-Todorovic, V. (2008). Clinical anesthesia causes permanent damage to the fetal guinea pig brain. Brain Pathol. 18, 198–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudolph, J. L., Jones, R. N., Levkoff, S. E., Rockett, C., Inouye, S. K., Sellke, F. W., et al. (2009). Derivation and validation of a preoperative prediction rule for delirium after cardiac surgery. Circulation 119, 229–236. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.795260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Cubillo, I., Periáñez, J. A., Adrover-Roig, D., Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Ríos-Lago, M., Tirapu, J., et al. (2009). Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 438–450. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanderlin, A. H., Todem, D., and Bozoki, A. C. (2017). Obesity and co-morbid conditions are associated with specific neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:164. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, D. A., Evered, L., Maruff, P., MacIsaac, A., Maher, S., and Silbert, B. S. (2018). Cognitive function before and after left heart catheterization. J. Am. Heart Asso. 7:e008004. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.117.008004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shao, H., Zhang, Y., Dong, Y., Yu, B., Xia, W., and Xie, Z. (2014). Chronic treatment with anesthetic propofol improves cognitive function and attenuates caspase activation in both aged and Alzheimer's disease transgenic mice. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 41, 499–513. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silbert, B. S., Scott, D. A., Evered, L. A., Lewis, M. S., and Maruff, P. T. (2007). Pre-existing cognitive impairment in patients scheduled for elective coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Anesthesia Analgesia 104:3. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0000263285.03361.3a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, J. X., and Eckenhoff, M. F. (2013). Anesthetic effects in Alzheimer transgenic mouse models. Progr. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatr. 47, 167–171. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, P. M., Moussai, J., Zohoori, S., Goldkorn, A., Khan, A. A., Mega, M. S., et al. (1998). Cortical variability and asymmetry in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Cerebr. Cortex 8, 492–509. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.6.492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rooij, F. G., Kessels, R. P., Richard, E., De Leeuw, F. E., and van Dijk, E. J. (2016). Cognitive impairment in transient ischemic attack patients: a systematic review. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 42, 1–9. doi: 10.1159/000444282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaffe, K., Laffan, A. M., Harrison, S. L., Redline, S., Spira, A. P., Ensrud, K. E., et al. (2011). Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 306, 613–619. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakzanis, K. K., Mraz, R., and Graham, S. J. (2005). An fMRI study of the Trail Making Test. Neuropsychologia 43, 1878–1886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, X., Liu, F., Zhang, X., Patterson, T. A., Callicott, R., Liu, S., et al. (2011). Inhalation anaesthetic-induced neuronal damage in the developing rhesus monkey. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 592–597. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: general anesthesia, age, cardiac surgery, comorbidities, pre-operative neurocognitive disorder

Citation: Linassi F, De Laurenzis A, Maran E, Gadaldi A, Spano' L, Gerosa G, Pittarello D, Zanatta P and Carron M (2022) Influence of Previous General Anesthesia on Cognitive Impairment: An Observational Study Among 151 Patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:810046. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.810046

Received: 05 November 2021; Accepted: 14 April 2022;
Published: 16 May 2022.

Edited by:

Stephan Kratzer, Technical University of Munich, Germany

Reviewed by:

José Manuel Reales, National University of Distance Education (UNED), Spain
Chariklia Tziraki-Segal, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel

Copyright © 2022 Linassi, De Laurenzis, Maran, Gadaldi, Spano', Gerosa, Pittarello, Zanatta and Carron. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Federico Linassi, ZmVkZXJpY28ubGluYXNzaSYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.