Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 21 February 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Connectivity, Dynamics, and Complexity View all 11 articles

The Relationship Between Default Mode and Dorsal Attention Networks Is Associated With Depressive Disorder Diagnosis and the Strength of Memory Representations Acquired Prior to the Resting State Scan

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States

Previous research indicates that individuals with depressive disorders (DD) have aberrant resting state functional connectivity and may experience memory dysfunction. While resting state functional connectivity may be affected by experiences preceding the resting state scan, little is known about this relationship in individuals with DD. Our study examined this question in the context of object memory. 52 individuals with DD and 45 healthy controls (HC) completed clinical interviews, and a memory encoding task followed by a forced-choice recognition test. A 5-min resting state fMRI scan was administered immediately after the forced-choice task. Resting state networks were identified using group Independent Component Analysis across all participants. A network modeling analysis conducted on 22 networks using FSLNets examined the interaction effect of diagnostic status and memory accuracy on the between-network connectivity. We found that this interaction significantly affected the relationship between the network comprised of the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and hippocampal formation and the network comprised of the inferior temporal, parietal, and prefrontal cortices. A stronger positive correlation between these two networks was observed in individuals with DD who showed higher memory accuracy, while a stronger negative correlation (i.e., anticorrelation) was observed in individuals with DD who showed lower memory accuracy prior to resting state. No such effect was observed for HC. The former network cross-correlated with the default mode network (DMN), and the latter cross-correlated with the dorsal attention network (DAN). Considering that the DMN and DAN typically anticorrelate, we hypothesize that our findings indicate aberrant reactivation and consolidation processes that occur after the task is completed. Such aberrant processes may lead to continuous “replay” of previously learned, but currently irrelevant, information and underlie rumination in depression.

Introduction

Resting state neuroimaging techniques examine brain activation and functional connectivity in the absence of stimuli or tasks. Previous studies characterized a set of networks in which brain regions co-activate during resting state (Beckmann et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; De Luca et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2009; Biswal et al., 2010; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010; Yeo et al., 2011; Lv et al., 2018). Resting state functional connectivity may be related to sustained information processing (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001) as well as environment monitoring and internal thought processes (Buckner et al., 2008) that can dynamically change based on the experiences preceding the resting state scan (Daselaar et al., 2010; Sami et al., 2014; Cecchetto et al., 2019) as well as predict task performance that follows the resting state scan (Tambini et al., 2010; Sala-Llonch et al., 2012; López Zunini et al., 2013; Reineberg et al., 2015).

Resting state functional connectivity is altered in a variety of psychiatric illnesses (Greicius, 2008; Zhang and Raichle, 2010) including depressive disorders (Greicius et al., 2007; Woodward and Cascio, 2015; Zhang et al., 2021). Depressive disorders (DD) (e.g., major depressive and persistent depressive disorders) are characterized by feelings of sadness, loss of interest and motivation, fatigue, anhedonia, feelings of hopelessness, worthlessness and guilt, as well as changes in sleeping, eating, and overall cognitive and psychosocial functioning (First et al., 2015). In addition, individuals with DD may experience memory dysfunction (Burt et al., 1995; Williams et al., 2007; Hamilton and Gotlib, 2008; Vasic et al., 2009; Gotlib and Joormann, 2010; Dillon and Pizzagalli, 2018; Ge et al., 2019). Previous neuroimaging studies found elevated connectivity within the default mode network (DMN) as well as between the DMN and non-DMN regions, including the dorsal attention network (DAN), insula, thalamus, and subgenual cingulate in individuals with DD relative to healthy controls (HC) (Sheline et al., 2010; Scalabrini et al., 2020). Clinical features of depression, such as symptom severity, illness duration, medication status and, most notably, ruminative and self-referential thought, have been linked to disruption of the cognitive control, salience, and default mode networks (Greicius et al., 2007; Bluhm et al., 2009; Sheline et al., 2010; Zeng et al., 2012; Brakowski et al., 2017; Scalabrini et al., 2020).

Mind wandering (Kucyi and Davis, 2014; Chou et al., 2017) and experiencing self-referential thoughts (Sheline et al., 2009) and spontaneous rumination (Rosenbaum et al., 2017) might be related to intentional or unintentional reactivation of memory representations that participants encountered prior to the resting state scan and memory consolidation (Sami et al., 2014). During memory consolidation, recently formed memories change into stable memory representations through reorganization and transitioning of hippocampal-dependent to neocortex supported memories (Squire et al., 2015). Memory representations can be reactivated during stimulus re-study (Vilberg and Davachi, 2013), memory tests (Manelis et al., 2011), or spontaneously during rest following stimuli encoding (Deuker et al., 2013; Wittkuhn and Schuck, 2021). The latter is particularly relevant for the study of psychiatric disorders as it might explain such phenomena as depressive rumination, obsessive thoughts, and other aberrant forms of cognition. Given that the majority of previous studies focused on healthy individuals and administered the resting state scan following stimulus encoding and prior to a memory test (Tambini et al., 2010; Tambini and Davachi, 2013; Schlichting and Prestona, 2014; Tompary et al., 2015; Murty et al., 2017), little is known about how the accuracy of memories acquired prior to the resting state scan is related to functional connectivity within and between resting state networks in individuals with DD, compared to HC.

Our study examined whether the relationship between memory retrieval accuracy for recently encoded stimuli and resting state functional connectivity was moderated by diagnostic status. The experimental paradigm consisted of the encoding phase, during which participants encountered pictures of objects and tried to memorize them, the forced-choice recognition task, and the subsequent resting state scan in which the memorized stimuli were no longer relevant. Memory encoding, retrieval, and consolidation involve the hippocampus (Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997; Squire et al., 2015). Considering that the hippocampus is a part of the DMN (Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010b) that has aberrant functional connectivity in individuals with DD (Sheline et al., 2009; Marchetti et al., 2012; Scalabrini et al., 2020) and that individuals with DD have difficulty disengaging from previously learned information (Xia and Evans, 2020), we hypothesized that memory accuracy in individuals with DD would be associated with aberrant DMN connectivity relative to HC.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. Participants were recruited from the community, universities, and counseling and medical centers through referrals and advertisements. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were right-handed, fluent in English, and matched on age, sex, and IQ. HC had no personal or family history of psychiatric disorders. Symptomatic participants met DSM-5 criteria for a depressive disorder (DD) such as major depressive disorder (MDD) or persistent depressive disorder (PDD).

Neuroimaging data were collected from 114 participants (53 HC and 61 DD). A total of 17 participants were excluded from the analyses due to excessive motion (>2 mm translation; 7 HC and 5 DD), poor data quality due to scanner noise (1 DD) or falling asleep during the resting state scan (1 HC and 3 DD). The final sample included 45 HC and 52 DD for a total of 97 participants. Of the 52 individuals with DD, 35 were diagnosed with MDD and 17 were diagnosed with PDD. MDD and PDD differ in some characteristics, such as symptom duration and severity, yet display substantial overlap in symptomatology (First et al., 2015). Of the 17 individuals with PDD, 13 had intermittent major depressive episodes, a clinical feature making the symptom profiles of MDD and PDD difficult to differentiate (Schramm et al., 2020).

Clinical Assessment

All diagnoses were made by a trained clinician and confirmed by a psychiatrist according to DSM-5 criteria using the SCID-5 clinical interview (First et al., 2015). We also collected information about current depression symptoms using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS-25) (Hamilton, 1960) and lifetime dimensional symptoms of depression using the MOODS-SR (Dell’Osso et al., 2002). A total psychotropic medication load was calculated for each participant, with greater numbers and doses of medications corresponding to a greater medication load (Hassel et al., 2008; Manelis et al., 2016). Exclusion criteria included a history of head injury, metal in the body, pregnancy, claustrophobia, neurodevelopmental disorders, systemic medical illness, premorbid IQ < 85 per the National Adult Reading Test (Nelson, 1982), current alcohol/drug abuse, the YMRS (Young et al., 1978) scores >10 at scan, and meeting criteria for any psychotic-spectrum disorder. Table 1 reports group statistics for participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics.

Memory Encoding and Retrieval

The experimental paradigm is depicted on Figure 1. During encoding, participants were presented with 48 pictures of common objects and food items (Blechert et al., 2014). To ensure deep information processing, they were instructed to rate each stimulus as pleasant or unpleasant (Richardson-Klavehn, 2010; Schott et al., 2013), and to memorize the presented stimuli. Approximately 25 min after encoding, participants performed the forced-choice object recognition task in which they were presented with pictures of an old (seen previously) and a novel (not seen previously) stimulus side-by-side and were required to select the old stimulus by pressing a corresponding button. The task consisted of 48 trials. The set of the novel stimuli was obtained from the same database as the set of the old stimuli (Blechert et al., 2014) and consisted of the items that categorically matched and visually resembled old stimuli (i.e., were from subordinate categories of old items). The pairings of novel and old stimuli were assigned randomly so that the old stimulus could be paired with a similar novel stimulus (e.g., blue chair and yellow chair), or a novel stimulus from a different category (e.g., bread and tomatoes). Each old stimulus had a closely resembling novel stimulus; therefore, participants had to have a detailed memory representation of an old stimulus to correctly identify it as old. The memory accuracy was calculated as a percent of correct responses relative to a total number of responses. While participants were scanned during both the encoding and forced-choice tasks, the neuroimaging results of these tasks will be described in separate reports.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm.

The scans administered between encoding and forced choice object recognition tasks included spin-echo field maps in the posterior-to-anterior and anterior-to-posterior phase encoding directions (30 s each), a 7-min T1w anatomical scan, a 6.5-min T2w anatomical scan, and a forced-choice recognition task for face stimuli (approximately 10 min) that were encoded before the objects in the separate task.

Resting State Scan

Immediately following the forced-choice recognition task, participants completed a 5-min resting state scan in which they were asked to look at the fixation star presented in the center of the screen and to stay awake. We used an eye-tracking camera system to monitor the participants’ wakefulness during the scan and made a note if a participant fell asleep or kept their eyes closed for more than 20 s. Participants also self-reported their wakefulness after the resting state scan.

Neuroimaging Data Acquisition

The neuroimaging data were collected at the University of Pittsburgh/UPMC Magnetic Resonance Research Center (MRRC) using a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner with a 64-channel head coil. The acquisition series were named according to the ReproIn naming convention (Visconti di Oleggio Castello et al., 2020). The EPI data were collected in the anterior-to-posterior direction using a multi-band sequence (factor = 8, TR = 800 ms, resolution = 2 × 2 × 2 mm, FOV = 210 mm, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 52°, 72 slices, 375 volumes). High-resolution T1w images were collected using the MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2,400 ms, resolution = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm, 208 slices, FOV = 256 mm, TE = 2.22 ms, flip angle = 8°). Field maps were collected in the AP and PA directions using the spin echo sequence (TR = 8,000 ms, resolution = 2 × 2 × 2 mm, FOV = 210 mm, TE = 66 ms, flip angle = 90°, 72 slices).

Data Analyses

Demographic and Clinical Data Analysis

The demographic and clinical characteristics were compared between DD and HC groups using t- and chi-square tests.

Memory Retrieval Analysis

We used t-tests to compare recognition accuracy and response time (RT) in DD vs. HC groups. RT was calculated across trials with correct responses only.

Neuroimaging Data Analysis

Preprocessing

The DICOM images were converted to BIDS standard using heudiconv version 0.5.4 (Halchenko et al., 2019; RRID:SCR_017427) After that, the images were preprocessed using fMRIPrep version 20.1.2 (Esteban et al., 2019; RRID:SCR_016216). First, a reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom methodology of fMRIPrep. Head-motion parameters with respect to the BOLD reference (transformation matrices, and six corresponding rotation and translation parameters) are estimated before any spatiotemporal filtering using mcflirt (FSL 5.0.9, (Jenkinson et al., 2002; RRID:SCR_002823). BOLD runs were slice-time corrected using 3dTshift (Cox and Hyde, 1997), (AFNI 20160207; RRID:SCR_005927). A B0-non-uniformity map (or fieldmap) was estimated based on two echo-planar imaging (EPI) references with opposing phase-encoding directions, with 3dQwarp (Cox and Hyde, 1997) (AFNI 20160207). Based on the estimated susceptibility distortion, a corrected EPI (echo-planar imaging) reference was calculated for a more accurate co-registration with the anatomical reference. The BOLD reference was then co-registered to the T1w reference using bbregister (FreeSurfer) (Dale et al., 1999; RRID:SCR_001847) which implements boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009). Co-registration was configured with six degrees of freedom. The BOLD time-series were resampled onto the following surfaces (FreeSurfer reconstruction nomenclature): fsaverage. The BOLD time-series (including slice-timing correction) were resampled onto their original, native space by applying a single, composite transform to correct for head-motion and susceptibility distortions. These resampled BOLD time-series will be referred to as preprocessed BOLD in original space, or just preprocessed BOLD. The BOLD time-series were resampled into standard space, generating a preprocessed BOLD run in MNI152NLin2009cAsym space. Automatic removal of motion artifacts using independent component analysis (ICA-AROMA) (Pruim et al., 2015) was performed on the preprocessed BOLD on MNI space time-series after removal of non-steady state volumes and spatial smoothing with an isotropic, Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM). Corresponding “non-aggressively” denoised runs were produced after such smoothing. Additionally, the aggressive noise-regressors were collected and placed in the corresponding confounds file. All resamplings were performed with a single interpolation step by composing all the pertinent transformations (i.e., head-motion transform matrices, susceptibility distortion correction when available, and co-registrations to anatomical and output spaces). Gridded (volumetric) resamplings were performed using antsApplyTransforms (ANTs), configured with Lanczos interpolation to minimize the smoothing effects of other kernels (Lanczos, 1964). Non-gridded (surface) resamplings were performed using mri_vol2surf (FreeSurfer).

While fMRIPrep automatically extracts the three global signals within the CSF, the WM, and the whole-brain masks, they are extracted prior to removal of motion artifacts using ICA-AROMA (Pruim et al., 2015). Therefore, instead of using the automatically generated values, we extracted these signals after running the ICA-AROMA (from the files with the suffix “space-MNI152NLin6Asym_desc-smoothAROMAnonaggr_bold”) and regressed the CSF and WM from preprocessed resting state data. After that, the data were band-pass temporal filtered (0.01–0.1 Hz). The processing pipeline is depicted in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Processing pipeline for the resting state data analysis.

Group-Level Independent Component Analysis

The preprocessed, registered-to-MNI-space and band-pass filtered BOLD images described above served as input files to MELODIC group-ICA (Beckmann et al., 2005). We limited the number of components discovered by MELODIC to 30 to obtain large-scale networks. MELODIC uses a mixture model and the loss function to estimate the probability for a voxel to belong to the “active” or to the “background noise” classes. We implemented a default threshold of 0.5 that “places an equal loss on false positives and false negatives” (Beckmann and Smith, 2004) to obtain thresholded group-ICA components. Prior to performing any between-group statistical analyses, these thresholded group-ICA components (3D spatial maps) were cross-correlated with Yeo’s 7-network solution maps (Yeo et al., 2011) using fslcc, a tool that estimates spatial similarities between ICA outputs and/or other volumetric data. The group-ICA components whose cross-correlation values exceeded 0.2 were retained for the further analyses. The remaining components were visually examined for usability. The components that followed WM tracts, or resembled physiological noise were removed from the analyses.

Within- and Between-Network Analysis

All analyses were conducted on thresholded independent component maps. Within-network connectivity was assessed with a dual regression which generated both subject-specific component time courses and subject-specific spatial maps as output. Dual regression automatically utilizes randomize [with 5000 permutations and threshold free cluster enhancement (TFCE) to correct for multiple comparisons] for non-parametric permutation testing (Winkler et al., 2014) to perform a diagnostic status-by-memory accuracy interaction analysis in this study.

The time courses generated by dual regression were fed into FSLNets (v0.6) to estimate between-network connectivity (Smith et al., 2013). FSLNets was run using aggressive component denoising and a full correlation. FSLNets uses network modeling in which each variable (in our case, each ICA component) is a node in the model and each connection between any two nodes is an edge. FSLNets produces a node × node matrix of correlation coefficients that represent the strength of connections (edge strength) between any two selected nodes. The effect of the diagnostic status-by-memory accuracy interaction on edge strength was tested using randomize with 5000 permutations and TFCE to correct for multiple comparisons corrected-p < 0.05. To correct for the number of estimated contrasts (n = 2), we used a Bonferroni corrected p < 0.025.

Exploratory Analysis

Because the effects identified in the analyses described above could be related to severity of current or lifetime depression symptoms as well as the use of medications, we conducted exploratory analyses to examine the associations between severity of current (HDRS-25) and lifetime depression (MOODS-SR depression scale), as well as the age of depressive disorder onset, illness duration, current mood episode duration, and a total medication load with all significant effects identified in the resting state analyses. The exploratory analyses were conducted only in the DD group.

To further examine the heterogeneity of our patient population, we compared clinical, behavioral, and neuroimaging characteristics in individuals with DD who are diagnosed with MDD vs. those diagnosed with PDD, as well as in individuals with DD with and without comorbid diagnoses.

Finally, since we administered the forced-choice recognition test for faces prior to the recognition test for objects, we conducted an exploratory analysis to examine the relationship between accuracy on the facial recognition task with accuracy on the object recognition task and on the overall functional connectivity model.

Results

Demographic and Clinical Data Analysis

The two groups did not significantly differ in age, sex, or IQ (Table 1). Individuals with DD, compared to HC, had significantly higher scores of current depression severity measured by HDRS-25, lifetime depression measured by MOODS-SR, and current mania symptoms measured by YMRS (p < 0.001).

Memory Retrieval Analysis

The two groups did not significantly differ in recognition accuracy [HC = 76.3 (1.8); UD 76.9 (1.9), t(95) = −0.22, p = 0.8] and recognition RT [HC = 968 (25) msec; UD = 1010 (21) msec, t(95) = −1.36, p = 0.18].

Resting State Data Analysis

Of the 30 independent components, 21 cross-correlated with Yeo’s 7-network solution (Yeo et al., 2011) with at least 0.2 (Figure 3). During further visual examination, we identified a component that was comprised of regions in the basal ganglia. Even though this component did not correlate with any of Yeo’s networks, we retained it for the further analyses. The remaining eight components followed white matter pathways, represented the cerebellum, or followed patterns of physiological noise and, therefore, were excluded from the further analyses.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Spatial maps of 22 selected network components. Network labels are based on cross-correlation with Yeo et al. (2011) and values in parentheses indicate the cross-correlation coefficient. ** denotes the two components whose connectivity was related to diagnostic status-by-memory accuracy interaction.

Dual regression did not reveal any significant effects of diagnosis-by-memory accuracy interaction on functional connectivity in any of 22 independent components.

FSLNets revealed that the strength of connectivity between the independent component (IC) 7 that cross-correlated with the DMN and the IC6 that cross-correlated with the DAN (Figure 4) was significantly associated with the interaction between diagnostic status and memory accuracy (p = 0.0156). A post hoc analysis showed that this effect was driven by the positive correlation between the connectivity strength and memory accuracy in individuals with DD (t = 4.1, p = 0.00016). Specifically, the individuals with DD who had more accurate memory for objects had a positive relationship between the DMN and DAN connectivity. However, those with less accurate memory had a negative relationship (anticorrelation) between the DMN and DAN. In HC, the DMN-DAN relationship was not significantly associated with memory accuracy (t = −1.8, p = 0.07) and, on average, was not different from 0 [one-sample t-test: t(44) = −0.66, p = 0.5].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The diagnosis-by-memory accuracy interaction effect on the edge strength between the DMN and DAN in individuals with DD and HC.

Exploratory Analyses

Exploratory analyses investigated whether recognition accuracy and the connection strength between the DMN and DAN were associated with clinical variables in individuals with DD. Neither accuracy nor connection strength between the DMN and DAN were related to current or lifetime depressive symptom severity, age of DD onset, medication load, or duration of current mood episode (all p-values > 0.05).

Analyses investigating difference between MDD and PDD revealed that the two groups did not significantly differ in their current or lifetime depressive symptom severity, age of illness onset, or medication load (all p-values > 0.05; Table 2). Further, the two disorders did not significantly differ in recognition accuracy [MDD: 77.3 (2.2)%; PDD: 76.2 (13.8)%, t(50) = 0.26, p = 0.8] and recognition RT [MDD: 1010 (22) msec; PDD: 1019 (48) msec, t(50) = −0.2, p = 0.84]. Importantly, the linear regression analysis that predicted the DMN-DAN connectivity strength from memory accuracy yielded significant results in the MDD only sample [F(1,33) = 4.5, p = 0.04], as well as in the PDD only sample [F(1,15) = 13.6, p = 0.002], which paralleled the findings for the whole DD sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of individuals diagnosed with major depressive (MDD) and persistent depressive (PDD) disorders.

Individuals with DD with comorbid disorders had significantly higher lifetime depression severity than those without comorbid disorders based on the MOODS (Dell’Osso et al., 2002) assessment [t(50) = 3.3, p < 0.005]. Other clinical, behavioral, and connectivity measures were not related to the presence of comorbid disorders.

Finally, although the memory accuracy for faces significantly correlated with memory for objects across all participants (r = 0.52, p < 0.001), it was significantly lower than the memory accuracy for objects [t(96) = −9.6, p < 0.001]. Memory accuracy for faces was not associated with the presence of DD diagnosis. Importantly, the connectivity values were not related to the interaction between diagnostic status and memory accuracy for faces [F(1,93) = 1.64, p = 0.2].

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the interaction effect of diagnostic status (DD vs. HC) and the strength of memory representations acquired prior to the resting state scan on within- and between-network resting state connectivity. The results indicated that diagnostic status moderated the relationship between memory accuracy and the DMN-DAN connectivity strength. Specifically, the DMN and DAN anticorrelated in the individuals with DD who had lower recognition memory accuracy, but positively correlated in those with DD who had higher recognition memory accuracy. In contrast, the DMN and DAN connectivity was not associated with memory accuracy in HC. We hypothesize that the distinct relationship between memory accuracy and the DMN-DAN connectivity in individuals with DD could indicate aberrant memory reactivation and consolidation processes in depressive disorders because the resting state scan was acquired immediately after the stimulus recognition and was conducted within a timeframe of continued memory consolidation, the results were correlated with memory performance, and one of the networks included bilateral hippocampus.

The DMN increases in activation during rest (Buckner et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2009; Raichle, 2015) and supports perception-independent (Smallwood et al., 2013) and internally oriented cognition, such as autobiographical memory (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010a), abstract representation of task details in ongoing thought processes (Sormaz et al., 2018), future-oriented thought (Xu et al., 2016), and spontaneous cognition (Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010a). In contrast, the DAN increases in activation during cognitive task performance and is implicated in top-down control of attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Vossel et al., 2014). While the DMN typically anticorrelates with the DAN (Fox et al., 2005; Buckner et al., 2008; Hampson et al., 2010), the degree of such anticorrelation may vary across cognitive states (Dixon et al., 2017) and environmental factors (Qian et al., 2020). For example, one study reported that the regions in these networks co-activated during task preparation but “anti-correlated” during task performance (Koshino et al., 2014). Another study found that the DMN and working memory network (that included the regions comprising the DAN in our study) co-activated during encoding and retrieval phases of a working memory task but anticorrelated during the maintenance phase of this task (Piccoli et al., 2015). Our study contributes to this line of research by showing that the relationship between the DMN and DAN is associated with psychopathology and memory accuracy in the task immediately preceding the resting state scan.

Resting state functional connectivity may reflect experiences acquired prior to the resting state scan (Daselaar et al., 2010; Sami et al., 2014; Cecchetto et al., 2019). Specifically, one study has demonstrated that the patterns of activation observed during encoding can spontaneously reactivate during a subsequent resting state scan (Deuker et al., 2013). Consistent with the memory consolidation theories (McClelland et al., 1995; Squire et al., 2015), consolidation starts in the hippocampus and continues in the neocortex, including the PFC. Given that more accurate memory in individuals with DD, but not HC, was associated with a stronger positive relationship between the DMN (that included the bilateral hippocampus) and DAN (that included the PFC and parietal regions) during the subsequent resting state scan, we hypothesize that these findings indicate aberrant spontaneous memory reactivation and consolidation in these individuals. This pattern of results may indicate that HC disengaged from the task when the task was over, while the individuals with DD who accurately memorized the stimuli in the task were not able to do so. The latter individuals might experience spontaneous reactivation and “replay” of memories even in the absence of direct stimulation. Individuals with more accurate memories might reactivate those memories more than individuals with less accurate memories. Future studies should directly test the spontaneous memory reactivation and consolidation hypothesis because these memory processes could be a neurobiological basis for depressive rumination, which is a repetitive internal thinking pattern focusing on negative experiences and affect (Holtzheimer and Mayberg, 2011). While we did not directly examine memory reactivation at rest or ruminative processes, the previous literature on rumination supports this idea and suggests a relationship between rumination, resting state functional connectivity, and depression. For example, in depressed individuals, this phenomenon was positively associated with functional connectivity in the anterior DMN (Zhu et al., 2012; Lois and Wessa, 2016), negatively associated with functional connectivity in the cortical DMN regions (Rosenbaum et al., 2017), and was associated with more variable functional connectivity between medial prefrontal cortex and insula (Kaiser et al., 2016). Future studies are needed to clarify and test the hypothesized relationship between rumination and memory reactivation and consolidation processes proposed here.

The findings from exploratory analyses in the individuals with DD revealed that the memory accuracy and DMN-DAN correlation were not significantly related to current or lifetime depressive symptom severity, the use of psychiatric medications, age of illness onset, comorbid diagnoses, or diagnosis of MDD vs. PDD in the individuals with DD. While these findings are indicative of general reorganization of connectivity between brain networks in individuals with DD, future studies need to examine other factors affecting the DMN-DAN connectivity in affected individuals.

One limitation of this study is the relatively short duration of the resting state scan. A longer scan time is preferable because it provides more reliable estimates of functional connectivity (Birn et al., 2013). However, longer scan times also increase the likelihood of a participant falling asleep during the scan, which would affect resting state functional connectivity (Soehner et al., 2019). Sleep disturbance is one of the clinical symptoms of DD; therefore, staying awake during a longer resting state scan may be especially challenging for individuals with DD. Another limitation of this study is the lack of a baseline resting state scan performed prior to stimulus encoding or between encoding and memory retrieval. Many studies that examined the relationship between resting state functional connectivity and task performance included an additional resting state scan prior to task performance to capture baseline functional connectivity to better distinguish “trait-like” from “task-related” changes. However, determining the baseline in individuals with mood disorders might be challenging because there is no way to eliminate bias or interference from previous experiences and thus, baseline resting state data may still be affected by rumination or thoughts about previous experiences. Although we hypothesized that our findings might be related to aberrant reactivation and consolidation processes in the individuals with DD, our study was not designed to directly measure consolidation processes. Future studies should modify the existing experimental paradigm by adding a baseline resting state scan between encoding and memory retrieval as well as the second memory test after the resting state scan to test the consolidation hypothesis. Future studies should also incorporate rumination surveys and inquire about the specific thought content that participants experience during rest to discern how ruminative or other thinking patterns are related to basic memory consolidation processes in depression. In this study, we used the pictures of neutral everyday objects. However, the notion of “neutral” stimuli should be interpreted with caution in mood disorders research because affected individuals tend to misinterpret neutral stimuli as emotional (Manelis et al., 2019). Future studies should specifically examine the relationship between resting state functional connectivity and memory for positive vs. negative information immediately preceding the resting state scan.

In summary, we have demonstrated that connectivity strength between the DMN and DAN during resting state was significantly associated with the interaction between participants’ diagnostic status (DD or HC) and memory accuracy in the task preceding the resting state scan. We hypothesize that these differences may relate to aberrant memory consolidation in depression, which may in turn be a basis for depressive rumination and an inability to disengage from thoughts and feelings associated with previous experiences. Understanding the relationship between resting state connectivity and previous experiences may inform the development of interventions strategies alleviating repetitive and intrusive thoughts in depression. However, future studies are needed to clarify the relationship between post-encoding and retrieval DMN-DAN resting state functional connectivity, memory consolidation, and rumination.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SS acquired the data, evaluated data quality, analyzed and interpreted the data, and drafted and critically evaluated the manuscript. YH curated the data organization and analyses and drafted and critically evaluated the manuscript. RR and ML acquired the data, evaluated data quality, and critically evaluated the manuscript. MP curated the study development, interpreted the data, and critically evaluated the manuscript. HS curated the study development, participants’ recruitment, and evaluation, and critically evaluated the manuscript. AM obtained funding, designed the study, acquired the data, evaluated data quality, analyzed and interpreted the data, and drafted and critically evaluated the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of this work.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health R01MH114870 to AM.

Conflict of Interest

HS receives royalties from Wolters Kluwer, royalties and an editorial stipend from APA Press, and served as a consultant for Intracellular Therapeutics and Medscape/WebMD.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank participants for taking part in this research study.

References

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., and Buckner, R. L. (2010a). Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 322–335. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010b). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2005:1634. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imag. 23, 137–152. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birn, R. M., Molloy, E. K., Patriat, R., Parker, T., Meier, T. B., Kirk, G. R., et al. (2013). The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. Neuroimage 83, 550–558. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X.-N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S. M., et al. (2010). Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 4734–4739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911855107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., and Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: An image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front. Psychol. 2014:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bluhm, R., Williamson, P., Lanius, R., Théberge, J., Densmore, M., Bartha, R., et al. (2009). Resting state default-mode network connectivity in early depression using a seed region-of-interest analysis: Decreased connectivity with caudate nucleus. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 63, 754–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.02030.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brakowski, J., Spinelli, S., Dörig, N., Bosch, O. G., Manoliu, A., Holtforth, M. G., et al. (2017). Resting state brain network function in major depression – Depression symptomatology, antidepressant treatment effects, future research. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2017:7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., and Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol. Bull. 117, 285. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cecchetto, C., Fischmeister, F. P. S., Reichert, J. L., Bagga, D., and Schöpf, V. (2019). When to collect resting-state data: The influence of odor on post-task resting-state connectivity. Neuroimage 191, 361–366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, Y. H., Sundman, M., Whitson, H. E., Gaur, P., Chu, M. L., Weingarten, C. P., et al. (2017). Maintenance and representation of mind wandering during resting-state fMRI. Sci. Rep. 7:722. doi: 10.1038/srep40722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W., and Hyde, J. S. (1997). Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data. NMR Biomed. 10, 171–178. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daselaar, S. M., Huijbers, W., de Jonge, M., Goltstein, P. M., and Pennartz, C. M. A. (2010). Experience-dependent alterations in conscious resting state activity following perceptuomotor learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 422–427. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Luca, M., Beckmann, C. F., De Stefano, N., Matthews, P. M., and Smith, S. M. (2006). fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29, 1359–1367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell’Osso, L., Armani, A., Rucci, P., Frank, E., Fagiolini, A., Corretti, G., et al. (2002). Measuring mood spectrum: Comparison of interview (SCI-MOODS) and self-report (MOODS-SR) instruments. Compr. Psychiatry 2002:29852. doi: 10.1053/comp.2002.29852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deuker, L., Olligs, J., Fell, J., Kranz, T. A., Mormann, F., Montag, C., et al. (2013). Memory consolidation by replay of stimulus-specific neural activity. J. Neurosci. 33, 19373–19383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0414-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillon, D. G., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2018). Mechanisms of memory disruption in depression. Trends Neurosci. 2018:6. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Spreng, R. N., Irving, Z. C., Mills, C., Girn, M., et al. (2017). Interactions between the default network and dorsal attention network vary across default subsystems, time, and cognitive states. Neuroimage 147, 632–649. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteban, O., Markiewicz, C. J., Blair, R. W., Moodie, C. A., Isik, A. I., Erramuzpe, A., et al. (2019). fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat. Methods 2019:2354. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., and Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—Research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, research version; SCID-5-RV). Washingt. D.C: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2005:102. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, R., Torres, I., Brown, J. J., Gregory, E., McLellan, E., Downar, J. H., et al. (2019). Functional disconnectivity of the hippocampal network and neural correlates of memory impairment in treatment-resistant depression. J. Affect. Disord. 253, 248–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., and Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol 2010:305. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. (2008). Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2008:5. doi: 10.1097/wco.0b013e328306f2c5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Flores, B. H., Menon, V., Glover, G. H., Solvason, H. B., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 429–437. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greve, D. N., and Fischl, B. (2009). Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gusnard, D. A., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685–694. doi: 10.1038/35094500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halchenko, Y., Goncalves, M., Castello, M. V., Ghosh, S., Hanke, M., DaeAmlien, I., et al. (2019). nipy/heudiconv: v0.5.4 [0.5.4]. San Francisco: GitHub. doi: 10.5281/ZENODO.2653784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, J. P., and Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Neural substrates of increased memory sensitivity for negative stimuli in major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 1155–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, M., Driesen, N., Roth, J. K., Gore, J. C., and Constable, R. T. (2010). Functional connectivity between task-positive and task-negative brain areas and its relation to working memory performance. Magn. Reson. Imag. 28, 1051–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2010.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassel, S., Almeida, J. R., Kerr, N., Nau, S., Ladouceur, C. D., Fissell, K., et al. (2008). Elevated striatal and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in response to emotional stimuli in euthymic bipolar disorder: no associations with psychotropic medication load. Bipolar. Disord. 10, 916–927. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00641.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtzheimer, P. E., and Mayberg, H. S. (2011). Stuck in a rut: Rethinking depression and its treatment. Trends Neurosci. 2011:4. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)91132-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, R. H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Dillon, D. G., Goer, F., Beltzer, M., Minkel, J., et al. (2016). Dynamic resting-state functional connectivity in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1822–1830. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koshino, H., Minamoto, T., Yaoi, K., Osaka, M., and Osaka, N. (2014). Coactivation of the default mode network regions and working memory network regions during task preparation. Sci. Rep. 4:5954. doi: 10.1038/srep05954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kucyi, A., and Davis, K. D. (2014). Dynamic functional connectivity of the default mode network tracks daydreaming. Neuroimage 100, 471–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanczos, C. (1964). Evaluation of noisy data. J. Soc. Ind. Appl. Math. Ser. B Numer. Anal. 1, 76–85. doi: 10.1137/0701007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lois, G., and Wessa, M. (2016). Differential association of default mode network connectivity and rumination in healthy individuals and remitted MDD patients. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11:85. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López Zunini, R. A., Thivierge, J. P., Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., and Taler, V. (2013). Alterations in resting-state activity relate to performance in a verbal recognition task. PLoS One 2013:8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, H., Wang, Z., Tong, E., Williams, L. M., Zaharchuk, G., Zeineh, M., et al. (2018). Resting-state functional MRI: Everything that nonexperts have always wanted to know. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 39, 1390–1399. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A5527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manelis, A., Almeida, J. R. C., Stiffler, R., Lockovich, J. C., Aslam, H. A., and Phillips, M. L. (2016). Anticipation-related brain connectivity in bipolar and unipolar depression: A graph theory approach. Brain 139, 2554–2566. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manelis, A., Hanson, C., and Hanson, S. J. (2011). Implicit memory for object locations depends on reactivation of encoding-related brain regions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 32–50. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manelis, A., Huppert, T. J., Rodgers, E., Swartz, H. A., and Phillips, M. L. (2019). The role of the right prefrontal cortex in recognition of facial emotional expressions in depressed individuals: fNIRS study. J. Affect. Disord. 2019:6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and De Raedt, R. (2012). The Default Mode Network and recurrent depression: A neurobiological model of cognitive risk factors. Neuropsychol. Rev. 22, 229–251. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9199-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., and O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murty, V. P., Tompary, A., Adcock, R. A., and Davachi, L. (2017). Selectivity in postencoding connectivity with high-level visual cortex is associated with reward-motivated memory. J. Neurosci. 37, 537–545. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4032-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, L., and Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 217–227. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test manual. Florida: Academica.

Google Scholar

Piccoli, T., Valente, G., Linden, D. E. J., Re, M., Esposito, F., Sack, A. T., et al. (2015). The default mode network and the working memory network are not anti-correlated during all phases of a working memory task. PLoS One 10:123354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruim, R. H. R., Mennes, M., van Rooij, D., Llera, A., Buitelaar, J. K., and Beckmann, C. F. (2015). ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage 2015:64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qian, S., Zhang, J., Yan, S., Shi, Z., Wang, Z., and Zhou, Y. (2020). Disrupted anti-correlation between the default and dorsal attention networks during hyperthermia exposure: an fMRI Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci 14:564271. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.564272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 2015:14030. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reineberg, A. E., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Depue, B. E., Friedman, N. P., and Banich, M. T. (2015). Resting-state networks predict individual differences in common and specific aspects of executive function. Neuroimage 104, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2010). Priming, automatic recollection, and control of retrieval: toward an integrative retrieval architecture, in: the act of remembering: toward an understanding of how we recall the past. 2010:202. doi: 10.1002/9781444328202.ch7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D., Haipt, A., Fuhr, K., Haeussinger, F. B., Metzger, F. G., Nuerk, H. C., et al. (2017). Aberrant functional connectivity in depression as an index of state and trait rumination. Sci. Rep. 7:2211. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02277-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala-Llonch, R., Pena-Gomez, C., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Vidal-Pineiro, D., Bargallo, N., Junque, C., et al. (2012). Brain connectivity during resting state and subsequent working memory task predicts behavioural performance. Cortex 48, 1187–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sami, S., Robertson, E. M., and Chris Miall, R. (2014). The time course of task-specific memory consolidation effects in resting state networks. J. Neurosci. 34, 3982–3992. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4341-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scalabrini, A., Vai, B., Poletti, S., Damiani, S., Mucci, C., Colombo, C., et al. (2020). All roads lead to the default-mode network—global source of DMN abnormalities in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 2058–2069. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0785-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlichting, M. L., and Prestona, A. R. (2014). Memory reactivation during rest supports upcoming earning of related content. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 15845–15850. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1404396111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schott, B. H., Wüstenberg, T., Wimber, M., Fenker, D. B., Zierhut, K. C., Seidenbecher, C. I., et al. (2013). The relationship between level of processing and hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity during episodic memory formation in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013:21435. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schramm, E., Klein, D. N., Elsaesser, M., Furukawa, T. A., and Domschke, K. (2020). Review of dysthymia and persistent depressive disorder: history, correlates, and clinical implications. Lancet Psychiatry 2020:997. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30099-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2009). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 1942–1947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812686106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Price, J. L., Yan, Z., and Mintun, M. A. (2010). Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 11020–11025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000446107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., Tipper, C., Brown, K., Baird, B., Engen, H., Michaels, J. R., et al. (2013). Escaping the here and now: Evidence for a role of the default mode network in perceptually decoupled thought. Neuroimage 69, 120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Vidaurre, D., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Jenkinson, M., Miller, K. L., et al. (2013). Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013:16. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soehner, A. M., Chase, H. W., Bertocci, M. A., Greenberg, T., Stiffler, R., Lockovich, J. C., et al. (2019). Unstable wakefulness during resting-state fMRI and its associations with network connectivity and affective psychopathology in young adults. J. Affect. Disord. 2019:66. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.07.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sormaz, M., Murphy, C., Wang, H., Hymers, M., Karapanagiotidis, T., Poerio, G., et al. (2018). Correction: Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, E11198. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1817966115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., Genzel, L., Wixted, J. T., and Morris, R. G. (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:21766. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tambini, A., and Davachi, L. (2013). Persistence of hippocampal multivoxel patterns into postencoding rest is related to memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 19591–19596. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308499110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tambini, A., Ketz, N., and Davachi, L. (2010). Enhanced brain correlations during rest are related to memory for recent experiences. Neuron 65, 280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tompary, A., Duncan, K., and Davachi, L. (2015). Consolidation of associative and item memory is related to post-encoding functional connectivity between the ventral tegmental area and different medial temporal lobe subregions during an unrelated task. J. Neurosci. 35, 7326–7331. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4816-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020:8. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasic, N., Walter, H., Sambataro, F., and Wolf, R. C. (2009). Aberrant functional connectivity of dorsolateral prefrontal and cingulate networks in patients with major depression during working memory processing. Psychol. Med. 39, 977–987. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilberg, K. L., and Davachi, L. (2013). Perirhinal-hippocampal connectivity during reactivation is a marker for object-based memory consolidation. Neuron 79, 1232–1242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visconti di Oleggio Castello, M., Dobson, J. E., Sackett, T., Kodiweera, C., Haxby, J. V., Goncalves, M., et al. (2020). ReproNim/reproin 0.6.0. San Francisco: GitHub. doi: 10.5281/ZENODO.3625000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossel, S., Geng, J. J., and Fink, G. R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems: Distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist 20, 150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., et al. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol. Bull. 2007:122. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 2014:60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittkuhn, L., and Schuck, N. W. (2021). Dynamics of fMRI patterns reflect sub-second activation sequences and reveal replay in human visual cortex. Nat. Commun 12:2. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21970-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, N. D., and Cascio, C. J. (2015). Resting-state functional connectivity in psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 2015:484. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, J., and Evans, L. H. (2020). Neural evidence that disengaging memory retrieval is modulated by stimulus valence and rumination. Sci. Rep. 101, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-64404-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Yuan, H., and Lei, X. (2016). Activation and connectivity within the default mode network contribute independently to future-oriented thought. Sci. Rep. 2016:21001. doi: 10.1038/srep21001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, R. C., Biggs, J. T., Ziegler, V. E., and Meyer, D. A. (1978). A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry 133, 429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, L. L., Shen, H., Liu, L., Wang, L., Li, B., Fang, P., et al. (2012). Identifying major depression using whole-brain functional connectivity: A multivariate pattern analysis. Brain 135, 1498–1507. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., and Raichle, M. E. (2010). Disease and the brain’s dark energy. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2010:198. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Wu, W., Toll, R. T., Naparstek, S., Maron-Katz, A., Watts, M., et al. (2021). Identification of psychiatric disorder subtypes from functional connectivity patterns in resting-state electroencephalography. Nat. Biomed. Eng. 5, 309–323. doi: 10.1038/s41551-020-00614-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Wang, X., Xiao, J., Liao, J., Zhong, M., Wang, W., et al. (2012). Evidence of a dissociation pattern in resting-state default mode network connectivity in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 611–617. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resting state, fMRI, functional connectivity, depression, memory, default mode network, dorsal attention network

Citation: Satz S, Halchenko YO, Ragozzino R, Lucero MM, Phillips ML, Swartz HA and Manelis A (2022) The Relationship Between Default Mode and Dorsal Attention Networks Is Associated With Depressive Disorder Diagnosis and the Strength of Memory Representations Acquired Prior to the Resting State Scan. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:749767. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.749767

Received: 29 July 2021; Accepted: 01 February 2022;
Published: 21 February 2022.

Edited by:

Ruben Sanchez-Romero, Rutgers University, United States

Reviewed by:

Ruiyang Ge, University of British Columbia, Canada
Kristen Ellard, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, United States

Copyright © 2022 Satz, Halchenko, Ragozzino, Lucero, Phillips, Swartz and Manelis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Manelis, YW5uYS5tYW5lbGlzQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.