Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 January 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Time Interaction With Two Spatial Dimensions: From Left/Right to Near/Far

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
  • 2Unit of Recovery and Functional Rehabilitation, Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Institute of Castel Goffredo, Mantova, Italy

In this study, we explored the time and space relationship according to two different spatial codings, namely, the left/right extension and the reachability of stimulus along a near/far dimension. Four experiments were carried out in which healthy participants performed the time and spatial bisection tasks in near/far space, before and after short or long tool-use training. Stimuli were prebisected horizontal lines of different temporal durations in which the midpoint was manipulated according to the Muller-Lyer illusion. The perceptual illusory effects emerged in spatial but not temporal judgments. We revealed that temporal and spatial representations dynamically change according to the action potentialities of an individual: temporal duration was perceived as shorter and the perceived line’s midpoint was shifted to the left in far than in near space. Crucially, this dissociation disappeared following a long but not short tool-use training. Finally, we observed age-related differences in spatial attention which may be crucial in building the memory temporal standard to categorize durations.

Introduction

In everyday activities, we commonly experience an intimate coupling between time and space. In this respect, it was largely demonstrated that the stimulus spatial position, on the left or on the right side, influences time perception (see Bonato et al., 2012 for a review). More recently, it was demonstrated that also the stimulus position in the depth dimension, near and far space, impacts time perception (Anelli et al., 2015). As far as the influence of left/right spatial coding on time estimation, it was demonstrated that the duration of stimuli presented on the left is perceived shorter as compared to stimuli presented on the right (Vallesi et al., 2008; Vicario et al., 2008; Frassinetti et al., 2009). These findings have contributed to formulating the hypothesis that humans represent time along a horizontal line (mental timeline) where temporal intervals are typically represented in ascending order from left to right (Santiago et al., 2007, 2010; Ishihara et al., 2008; Müller and Schwarz, 2008; Weger and Pratt, 2008; Magnani et al., 2011; Vallesi et al., 2011; Di Bono et al., 2012; Oliveri et al., 2013; Isham et al., 2018).

Near/far spatial coding influences time perception too: stimuli presented in far space are perceived shorter in duration as compared to stimuli presented in near space (Anelli et al., 2015). The distinction between near and far space depends on the extent to which an action can be performed (Rizzolatti et al., 1997; Berti and Frassinetti, 2000; Cardinali et al., 2009). Thus, near space is defined as the reachable space inside the arm’s reaching distance, and far space is defined as the unreachable space outside the reaching distance (Maravita and Iriki, 2004; Bartolo et al., 2014; De Vignemont and Iannetti, 2015).

Further evidence supporting the role of performing action per se or action capability of an individual in mediating the distinction between near and far space is provided by the effects of tool-use which extends action capabilities (Witt, 2011; Patané et al., 2016b; Biggio et al., 2017; Candini et al., 2019; Forsberg et al., 2019; see, for reviews, Johnson-Frey, 2004; Maravita and Iriki, 2004; Reynaud et al., 2016). In fact, tool-use allows to reach objects located in a far/unreachable space and, consequently, extends the reaching of boundary space (Berti and Frassinetti, 2000; Longo and Lourenco, 2006; Hunley et al., 2017; but see also D’Angelo et al., 2018; see, for review, Brozzoli et al., 2014; Martel et al., 2016).

The distinction between near and far space and its plasticity has widely been demonstrated in several behavioral studies adopting visuospatial tasks, such as the line bisection and landmark task, which require to identify the subjective midpoint of a horizontal line (Bisiach et al., 1998; Lindbergh and Kieffaber, 2013; Patané et al., 2016a). In these tasks, when stimuli are presented in near space, healthy participants typically exhibit a leftward bias that gradually shifts rightward as the stimulus is moved further away (Bowers and Heilman, 1980; Jewell and McCourt, 2000; Longo and Lourenco, 2006, 2007). However, some studies adopting a line bisection task reported the opposite, which is a rightward bias in near space compared to far space (Cowie and Hamil, 1998; Braun and Kirk, 1999; McCourt and Garlinghouse, 2000; Varnava et al., 2002). What is of interest here is that when using a tool, that extends the near/reachable space up to the far/unreachable space (i.e., a long tool), a bias similar to that observed in near space emerged also in far space (Longo and Lourenco, 2006). This is not true when a short tool is used that does not extend the action capability and does not allow to reach the far space (Farnè et al., 2005; Costantini et al., 2011; Bourgeois et al., 2014; Anelli et al., 2015; Patané et al., 2016b; Candini et al., 2019).

Furthermore, a relevant issue is that age influences the bias found in the line bisection task. However, the nature of this effect is somewhat controversial: some studies have demonstrated an attenuated bias with age (Schmitz and Peigneux, 2011; Schmitz et al., 2013; Benwell et al., 2014; Learmonth et al., 2015; Friedrich et al., 2018), whereas other studies have put in evidence an accentuated bias with age (Harvey et al., 1995, 2000) or no effects of age (Learmonth et al., 2017). In a similar vein, a still debated question concerns the role of age in time estimation: temporal processing appears to be altered in healthy aging which is often associated with a faster passage of subjective time (Craik and Hay, 1999; Perbal et al., 2002; Rueda and Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2009; Turgeon et al., 2016; Paraskevoudi et al., 2018). For instance, Craik and Hay (1999) found that older participants perceived stimuli duration as shorter, compared to the veridical temporal duration than younger participants. However, other studies found the opposite tendency in aging, which is to perceive longer stimuli duration compared to the veridical interval (Block et al., 1998; Turgeon et al., 2011). Thus, it remains unknown how aging actually influences and determines the response bias observed either in spatial or temporal judgments.

In this study, we concurrently investigated the relationship between time and two different spatial codings, namely, the horizontal extension along a transversal axis (from left to right) and the depth dimension along a sagittal axis (from near/reachable to far/unreachable). Moreover, according to several studies demonstrating that age influences different abilities (Sardone et al., 2020), such as spatial and time perception, we expect a different performance between young and elderly participants in temporal estimation as a function of the spatial coding of stimulus. To these aims, we study the effects on time perception of a visual illusion, altering the left/right spatial coding (Muller-Lyer illusion), and of the tool-use, modifying the boundary between near and far space.

Young and aging healthy participants were involved in two time experiments and two spatial experiments. In the time bisection task, participants estimated whether stimuli were “short” or “long” in duration, with respect to a previously acquired pair of references (1,600 and 2,400 ms). Stimuli consisted of horizontal prebisected lines and were displayed according to the Brentano version of the Muller-Lyer illusion. This type of illusion uses three different arrows arranged in such a way that one half of the line is apparently expanded, whereas the other half appears to be compressed (Coren and Girgus, 1978; Weidner and Fink, 2007). We predicted that if the spatial bias induced by the Brentano version of the Muller-Lyer illusion influences time perception when the transector is perceived as nearer to the left end of the line, time should be perceived as shorter, and when the transector is perceived as nearer to the right end of the line, time should be perceived as longer. Furthermore, the effects of depth on time and its plasticity were assessed by presenting stimuli both in near and far spaces, before and after long tool-use training. According to previous findings, time is expected to be perceived as shorter in the far compared to near space before but not after long tool-use training (Anelli et al., 2015; Petrizzo et al., 2021).

To verify the presence of illusory effects induced by the Muller-Lyer configuration, participants were involved in the spatial bisection task, in which the same stimuli of the time bisection task were used, except for stimulus duration which was not manipulated and was set at 2,000 ms. Participants were required to judge which end of the prebisected line the transector was closer to. Furthermore, two additional control experiments were also conducted in which the same time bisection task and spatial bisection task previously described were performed before and after a short tool-use training (Farnè et al., 2005; Costantini et al., 2011; Bourgeois et al., 2014; Anelli et al., 2015; Patané et al., 2016b; Candini et al., 2019).

We expect that testing the effect of Muller-Lyer illusion on time estimation will provide a critical contribution to understanding how visual perception influences time processing. Moreover, changes in time estimation might be driven by the possibility to act in space, regardless of the perceived illusory spatial bias. If that is the case, we should find that the body proximity and the active long tool-use training but not short tool-use training are effective to modulate time estimation.

Experiment 1: Time Bisection Task

Participants

A total of 36 right-handed neurologically healthy volunteers with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited. The sample size was calculated by using G*Power 3.1.7, hypothesizing an estimated effect size of f = 0.4 and a power at 80% (Anelli et al., 2015). Notably, 18 volunteers were elderly participants (6 men, mean ± SD age = 57.9 ± 6.9 years; mean ± SD education = 13 ± 4.6 years), and the remaining 18 volunteers were younger participants (5 men, mean ± SD age = 23.4 ± 3 years; mean ± SD education = 17 ± 1.7 years).

All participants were naive to the purpose of the research and provided written informed consent to participate in the study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Department of Psychology, University of Bologna), and all procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 2008 Declaration of Helsinki.

Stimuli

Stimuli were black prebisected horizontal lines displayed on a white screen positioned at 60 cm (near space) or 120 cm (far space) from the eyes of participants. To ensure that the retinal angle remained constant [0.48° × 5.25° visual angle (VA)] across viewing distances, the size of stimuli was adjusted near space: 0.5 cm height and 5.5 cm length; far space: 1 cm height and 11 cm length). This phenomenon can be ascribed to the size constancy, a perceptual mechanism that enables us to perceive an object of different sizes presented at different viewing distances as having the same size. Indeed, our brain combines retinal images and distance cues to perceive objects as having the same physical properties, i.e., a constant size, despite changes in retinal input (Sperandio and Chouinard, 2015). Each horizontal line was bisected by a black arrow (transector) and two more black arrows were presented at the line’s ends, which could be oriented to the left or to the right side. Indeed, two different illusory conditions were presented: Left expanded (left-sided outgoing fin/right-sided ingoing fin) and Right expanded (left-sided ingoing fin/right-sided outgoing fin). Throughout this study, the two types of illusory stimuli are distinguished with reference to the orientation of outgoing fins (left or right expanded side). The fin formed with the line in 45° (ingoing; > <) or 315° (outgoing; < >) angle. Across trials, the stimuli varied according to (1) the 2 stimuli orientations (left/right), (2) the 3 positions at which the horizontal line was bisected (shift to the left: –0.2°; true center: 0.0° shift to the right: + 0.2°), and (3) the 5 stimuli durations (i.e., 1,600, 1,800, 2,000, 2,200, or 2,400 ms; Figure 1) for a total of 30 stimuli combinations. Each stimulus was repeated six times, thus obtaining a total of 360 trials. To reduce the effect of fatigue on the performance of participants, three different blocks of 60 trials were presented for each condition, i.e., 180 in the near space and 180 in the far space.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Stimuli and procedure adopted in the experimental tasks. In the upper panel, (A) experimental stimuli were depicted: stimuli varied according to the left/right side orientation, and the transector positions (1: −0.2°; 2: 0.0°; 3: + 0.2°). The vertical dot lines represent the true center of each prebisected line. In the lower panel, (B) a schematic representation of the procedure adopted was represented. Both tasks were performed before and after a tool-use training session in which the participant (blue) reached a series of colored chips placed by the experimenter outside the reaching space by using a rake.

Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross displayed on a white background for 500 ms, after which the stimulus was displayed for one of the five temporal durations (i.e., 1,600, 1,800, 2,000, 2,200, and 2,400 ms) and then replaced by a white background which remained visible until a verbal response was made. After the response, the subsequent trial was presented following a random inter-trial interval (range 600–1,000 ms; as shown in Figure 2A). Stimuli were centrally displayed on a 15” LCD monitor (resolution 1,366 × 768 pixels) and were presented by using the E-Prime 2.0 software package (Psychology Software Tool© 1996–2012, United States).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The experimental paradigm and performance of participants at time bisection task. In the time bisection task, (A) participants were asked to judge whether the duration of each stimulus was “short” or “long” with respect to pair of short (1,600 ms) and long (2,400 ms) reference durations. (B) The percentage of long response as a function of duration interval. (C) The point of subjective equality (PSE) judgments expressed as a function of Group. (D) The PSE is expressed as a function of tool-use session (pre and post) and conditions (near and far). Error bars indicate SEMs. Asterisks indicate significant differences.

Procedure

Participants were comfortably seated directly in front of the monitor screen and were asked to rest their hands on their thighs. The experimenter ensured that the body position of the subject remained constant during the whole experiment.

The time bisection task consisted of the verbal classification of a series of prebisected lines that were displayed for different durations at the center of the computer screen. We adopted two-alternative forced-choice paradigms in which participants have to judge whether the duration of each line was “short” or “long” with respect to previously acquired pair of reference durations (1,600 and 2,400 ms). In the practice session, a total of eight trials were randomly presented, and participants had to classify four intervals of 1,600 ms as “short” and four intervals of 2,400 ms as “long.” Feedback was given on accuracy only for these eight practice trials. All participants reached at least 80% of accuracy with no more than two practice sessions (mean ± SD accuracy = 89.7% ± SD 0.09). Then, they were involved in the experimental task in which all the possible prebisected lines were randomly displayed. Participants were instructed to make their best guess if they were unsure and to respond as accurately and as quickly as possible. No feedback was given in the experimental blocks.

The experimenter seated behind the participants (at least 1.5 m) and recorded their verbal responses by pressing one of the two keys on a wireless mouse (left for “short”/right for “long”). Each block of the time bisection task took approximately 6 min to complete.

The time bisection task was performed in two experimental conditions: in near space (60 cm from the computer screen) and in far space (120 cm from the computer screen), presented in separate blocks, counterbalanced across participants. The task was repeated two times, in the same order, before and after a tool-use session.

Tool-Use Session

During the tool-use training, the participant stood at the short side of a rectangular table. The experimenter randomly placed on the table a series of colored poker chips (red, green, yellow, white, and black) outside the reaching space, at a distance of ≈100 cm from the trunk of a participant. The chips were presented in the midsagittal axis (0°) of participants, or at 10° and 20° to the left and to the right of the central position (–10°, –20°, + 10°, and +20°; as shown in Figure 1). Each participant performed about a total of 100 reaching movements in order to bring the objects close to their body with no time constraint. During the tool-use session, participants had to use a 70-cm-long rake with their right hand. The tool-use training lasted for 15 min.

Data Analysis

In our paradigm, the bisection point is the estimated temporal value (expressed in milliseconds) for which participants would respond “long” or “short” with equal probability. To estimate the bisection point, we first computed the percentage of “long” responses for each interval duration (1,600, 1,800, 2,000, 2,200, and 2,400 ms), and then cumulative Gaussian functions were fitted to the percentage of “long” responses across different stimulus durations using logistic regression. The point at which the psychometric function cuts 50% of “long” responses indicates the duration at which a participant is equally likely to classify the stimuli as short or long [point of subjective equality (PSE)]. For each participant, the PSE was separately calculated for each type of illusion (left/right orientation) and transector position (–0.2°; 0.0°; +0.2°) in each of the four conditions (i.e., pre-tool-use/near space, pre-tool-use/far space, post-tool-use/near space, and post-tool-use/far space). Bisection points below 2,000 ms reflect duration overestimation (i.e., durations are perceived longer than they actually are), whereas bisection points above 2,000 ms reflect duration underestimation (i.e., durations are perceived shorter than they actually are). An increase of “longer” responses after the tool-use session, as compared to the pre-tool session, induces a decreased PSE, reflecting that durations are perceived as being longer with respect to before tool-use session. Conversely, an increase of “short” responses after the tool-use session induces an increased PSE, reflecting that durations are perceived shorter. Thus, the PSE allows us to observe whether tool-use sessions induced a temporal judgment bias toward either an underestimation or an overestimation of durations in near and far spaces.

Statistical Analysis

The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was conducted to verify whether or not our dataset was normally distributed. Since we did not obtain significant results (p > 0.05), we assumed the dataset was not significantly different from a normal distribution. First, to explore the effect of temporal duration on time estimation judgment of participants, an ANOVA was conducted on the percentage of “long” responses recorded with intervals (1,600, 1,800, 2,000, 2,200, and 2,400 ms) as a within-subject variable.

Second, to explore the effect of a tool-use training on the temporal task, an ANOVA was conducted on mean PSE value with Group (young and aging participants) as a between-subject variable, and Session (pre- and post-tool), Condition (near and far spaces), Orientation (left and right), and Transector (–0.2°; 0.0°; +0.2°) as within-subject variables. When necessary, post hoc analyses were conducted using Newman–Keuls’s correction. The magnitude of effect size was expressed by partial eta squared (η2p).

Results of Experiment 1: Time Bisection Task

The Effect of Time Interval

The variable interval was significant [F(4,140) = 470.2; p < 0.0001; η2p = 0.93]: post hoc analyses revealed that all temporal durations differ each other, revealing an increase of percentage of long response according to the increase of temporal durations (1,600 = 15%; 1,800 = 33%; 2,000 = 55%; 2,200 = 75%; 2,400 = 86%; p < 0.0001 for all comparisons; as shown in Figure 2B).

The Effect of Muller-Lyer Illusion and Tool-Use on Temporal Estimation

The ANOVA conducted on the PSE revealed a significant effect for the variable group [F(1,34) = 3.19; p = 0.028; η2p = 0.13]: an underestimation of temporal interval was found in elderly (mean ± SEM 2,019 ± 19.0) compared to younger participants (1,948 ± 17.7; see Figure 2C). Crucially, the interaction Session × Condition [F(1,34) = 5.69; p = 0.02; η2p = 0.14] was significant. In the pre-tool session, temporal durations are perceived shorter (i.e., participants underestimated temporal interval) when stimuli were presented in far condition (2,075 ± 59.6) compared to near condition (1,954 ± 16.5; p = 0.045). In contrast, no such effect was observed in the post-tool session between near and far conditions (1,975 ± 27.9 vs. 1933 ± 21.7; p = 0.66). Interestingly, when pre- and post-tool sessions were directly compared, a significant reduction of time underestimation emerged comparing far conditions (p = 0.03), whereas no significant differences emerged comparing near conditions (p = 0.66; as shown in Figure 2D). No significant interaction for the factor Group and other variables was found (all p-values > 0.05).

Interim Discussion of Experiment 1

First, time intervals were classified consistently with their effective durations, since the percentage of long response increased as the time interval increased. Second, age influences time estimation: elderly participants perceived temporal durations as shorter compared to young participants. Third, considering the depth dimension along the sagittal axis, our results highlight how temporal estimation is modulated by the space where the temporal stimulus is presented: temporal durations presented in far space are perceived shorter as compared to those presented in near space. This dissociation disappears following a long tool-use session, in which participants actively perform a series of consecutive movements to reach objects located in a far and unreachable space by means of a long rake. Indeed, after tool-use training, temporal stimuli presented in far space are processed as if they occurred in near space, demonstrating the role of action in the “remapping of time.” Crucially, after an equivalent tool-use training but with a short rake, this effect was not observed (refer to Supplementary Section Experiment 3). Indeed, temporal durations presented in far space are perceived shorter as compared to those presented in near space in the pre-training session, and this dissociation was still present after the short tool-use training (see Supplementary Figure 1). Finally, we found that the Muller-Lyer illusion, which alters the left/right coding, did not affect temporal estimation. This last result can be explained in two different ways. First, the near/far space, coded by action possibilities, is more relevant for time estimation than the left/right spatial bias induced by visual illusion. Second, participants could have not experienced the Muller-Lyer illusion at all. To rule out this last possibility, we assessed the occurrence of Muller-Lyer’s illusion by using a spatial bisection task.

Experiment 2: Spatial Bisection Task

Stimuli

We used the same stimuli of Experiment 1 (see Figure 1) but stimuli duration was fixed at 2,000 ms. According to the two illusory conditions adopted (left and right expanded) and the three transector positions (–0.2°; 0.0° +0.2°), six different prebisected lines were obtained. Each stimulus was presented six times in a random order, yielding a total of 72 trials (36 in the near space and 36 in the far space). Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross lasting 500 ms, then the stimulus was displayed for 2,000 ms and replaced by a white background, which remained visible until a response was made (as shown in Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The experimental paradigm and performance of participants at the spatial bisection task. In the spatial bisection task, (A) participants had to verbally classify each stimulus as “left” if the transector was perceived as being closer to the left end of the line, and as “right” if they perceived it to be closer to the right end. (B) The percentage of right response expressed as a function of Muller-Lyer illusion. (C) The midpoint values are expressed as a percentage (%) of the right response as a function of Group. (D) The percentage of right response expressed as a function of conditions (near and far) and the tool-use session (pre and post). Error bars indicate SEM. Asterisks indicate significant differences.

Procedure

The spatial bisection task was performed in near and far spaces and presented in separate blocks, counterbalanced across participants. The task was repeated two times, in the same order, before and after a tool-use session.

We adopted a left/right alternative forced-choice paradigm in which participants have to judge which end of the line the transector was closer to Weidner and Fink (2007). On each trial, participants were asked to verbally classify each prebisected line as “left” if the transector was perceived as being closer to the left end of the line, and as “right” if they perceived it to be closer to the right end.

A practice session was administered before the experimental task. In this practice session, participants had to classify four prebisected lines at –0.2° as “left,” and four prebisected lines at + 0.2° as “right,” all randomly presented. Participants received feedback only in these eight practice trials, which was repeated until they had reached at least 80% of accuracy (mean ± SD accuracy = 90.2% ± SD 0.05). Then, they were involved in the experimental task in which all the possible six prebisected lines were randomly displayed. Participants were instructed to make their best guess if they were unsure and to respond as accurately and as quickly as possible. No feedback was given in the experimental blocks.

The experimenter seated behind the participants (at least 1.5 m) and recorded the verbal responses of participants by pressing one of the two keys on a wireless mouse (left/right). Each block of the spatial bisection task took approximately 5 min to complete.

Data Analysis

For each participant, to obtain an objective measure of the perceived line midpoint, we calculated the proportion of “right” responses (i.e., when subjects judged the transector as being at the right of the true center) in each of the four conditions (i.e., pre-tool-use/near space, pre-tool-use/far space, post-tool-use/near space, and post-tool-use/far space). As expected in line with the visual illusion, participants typically fail to correctly judge the midpoint of the line because half of the line appears shorter or longer than the real one with the ingoing fins (> <) and outgoing fins (< >), respectively (Ro and Rafal, 1996; Vallar et al., 2000; Daini et al., 2001).

An increase of “right” responses after the tool-use session, as compared to the pre-tool session, induces a relative shift toward the left of the perceived midline. On the contrary, an increase of “left” responses after the tool-use session induces a relative shift to the right.

Results of Experiment 2: Spatial Bisection Task

The Effect of Muller-Lyer Illusion and Tool-Use on Spatial Estimation

To explore the effect of a tool-use training on the spatial task, an ANOVA was conducted on the percentage of “right” responses recorded in each condition with Group (young and aging participants) as a between-subject variable, and Session (pre- and post-tool), Condition (near and far spaces), Orientation (left and right), and Transector (–0.2°; 0.0°; +0.2°) as within-subject variables.

The analysis revealed a significant effect for the variable Group [F(1,34) = 6.55; p = 0.02; η2p = 0.16]: younger participants perceived the transector as closer to the left end of the line (mean ± SEM 48% ± 0.018) compared to the elderly adults (51% ± 0.013; see Figure 3C). Furthermore, confirming the effect of Muller-Lyer illusion, the interaction Orientation × Transector [F(2,70) = 10.9; p < 0.0001; η2p = 0.23] was also significant. Post hoc comparisons revealed that for right-side- and left-side-oriented stimuli, the difference in judging the transector position was at 0.0°. Participants judged the transector as closer to the right end of the line when it was at +0.2° (98% ± 0.009) and 0.0° (98% ± 0.012) compared to –0.2° (84% ± 0.036; p < 0.0001). Conversely, for left-side-oriented stimuli, an increase in right responses was found when the transector was at +0.2° (14% ± 0.028) compared to 0.0° (2% ± 0.008; p < 0.0001) and at –0.2° (2% ± 0.008; p < 0.0001). Both these significant effects confirmed that responses of participants were affected by the direction of the illusion: right-side-oriented lines induce the opposite effect that is a shift of the midpoint toward the right, whereas left-side-oriented lines induce a shift of the midpoint (transector) toward the left (as shown in Figure 3B).

Crucially, the interaction Session × Condition [F(1,34) = 3.98; p = 0.05; η2p = 0.10] was significant. In the pre-tool session, an increase in right response was found in near condition (50% ± 0.006) compared to far condition (48% ± 0.009; p = 0.048). In contrast, no such effect was observed in the post-tool session (near space = 50% ± 0.007 vs. far space = 50% ± 0.008; p = 0.89). More relevant for the purpose of this study, when pre- and post-training sessions were directly compared, no differences emerged when near conditions were considered (p = 0.91), whereas following the tool-use session, a significant increase in right response was found in far conditions (p = 0.046; as shown in Figure 3D). No significant interaction for the factor Group and other variables was found (all p-values > 0.05).

Interim Discussion of Spatial Bisection Task

Results of the spatial bisection task put in evidence the effect of Muller-Lyer illusion on the perception of lines midpoint. In fact, right-oriented stimulus results in a shift of the midpoint toward the right. In contrast, left-oriented stimulus results in a shift of the midpoint toward the left. This result perfectly fits with the well-known effects of the Muller-Lyer illusion (Restle and Decker, 1977; Weidner and Fink, 2007). Moreover, age influenced the strength of the illusion: elderly participants showed a stronger rightward shift in the perceived line midpoint as compared to young participants. More interestingly for the current study, considering the depth dimension along the sagittal axis, our results demonstrate that the spatial judgment is also influenced by the stimulus position in near or far space: a leftward shift in the perceived line midpoint emerged in far as compared to near space. Furthermore, after a long tool-training, this dissociation disappears: when participants judged the midpoint of the line, no difference was found between stimuli presented in near and far spaces. Importantly, this effect was not found after training with a short tool (as shown in Supplementary Section Experiment 4). Indeed, the leftward shift in the perceived line midpoint reported in far as compared to near space was still present before and after the short tool-use training (see Supplementary Figure 2). Thus, the lack of an effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on time perception in Experiment 1 (time bisection task) is not explained by the fact that participants did not perceive the illusion. Rather, these results suggest that time is spatially organized, specifically when the space is encoded as a “working space” on which to act.

General Discussion

The main purpose of this study was to explore the interaction between left/right and near/far spatial coding in temporal estimation and their functional relationship. To address this, in the time bisection task, stimuli were manipulated both along the transversal left-right axis, according to the Muller-Lyer illusion, and along the sagittal axis, by presenting stimuli in reachable/near and unreachable/far space.

The first relevant result is the influence of near/far spatial coding on temporal estimation. Before the tool-use training, there was a clear dissociation between near and far spaces: temporal durations are perceived shorter when stimuli are presented in far as compared to near space. To explain this finding, we hypothesize that the temporal duration of stimuli presented close to the body (i.e., near space) was perceived as slower and prolonged because of the influence of action preparation on time perception. Curiously, experts in motor performance, such as tennis and football players, usually report the feeling of the ball “slowing-down” just before hitting it and comment that they “see” the ball more clearly before striking it (Murphy and White, 1978; Hagura et al., 2012). This implies that the subjective passage of time may be influenced by action, especially when participants performed movements close to their bodies (De Kock et al., 2021). Supporting this evidence, in a temporal judgment task, Hagura et al. (2012) found that stimuli were significantly overestimated (i.e., higher PSE values) when participants prepared for action compared with a control condition in which they had to detect visual stimuli. These results confirmed the claim that during motor preparation, participants perceived the visual stimulus to have a longer duration. Then, a possible interpretation is that to accurately perform movements, the optimal strategy for the brain is to enhance the detection of any environmental changes before motor execution and that this leads to a perception of slowing-down time. In addition, the space near the body is also a multisensory space in which sensory inputs that are spatially and temporally close are likely to be bound (Meredith et al., 1987; Stein and Stanford, 2008). Considering that, recent evidence demonstrated that the temporal binding window, i.e., the temporal window which is more likely that different sensory stimuli are integrated and perceived as synchronous, is larger in the near than in the far space (Noel et al., 2016). Indeed, in a simultaneity judgment task, when multisensory stimuli were presented close to the body, individuals judged sensory inputs as co-occurring over a wide range of temporal intervals. A larger temporal binding window in the near space can be responsible for slowing down the temporal perception observed near the body. This, in turn, can provide a more efficient working space for the body to act in time on nearby objects.

Interestingly, we demonstrated that tool-use training eliminates the dissociation between near and far spaces in time estimation, reporting the so-called remapping of time. This finding is in line with a previous study on young healthy participants (Anelli et al., 2015) and corroborates the idea that time is a physical dimension that interacts with the reachability and action potentialities of an individual in space.

Supporting this hypothesis, a similar dissociation between near and far spaces, and a remap of far and near spaces after the long tool-use training, were also found in the spatial bisection task. Taken together, our findings converged to the idea that temporal and spatial representations dynamically change according to the action potentialities of an individual. Coherently with this interpretation, following the short tool-use training, no changes were observed either when temporal or spatial judgments were provided in time and spatial bisection tasks, respectively. This evidence is in line with several studies, demonstrating that a short tool-use does not expand the action capability of an individual (Farnè et al., 2005; Costantini et al., 2011; Bourgeois et al., 2014; Anelli et al., 2015; Patané et al., 2016b; Candini et al., 2019). The second finding is that the Muller-Lyer illusion, which changes the perception of stimulus extension along the left/right dimension, does not influence time estimation. This is surprising if we consider the numerous evidence showing an underestimation of left stimuli duration compared to the right ones (Vallesi et al., 2008; Vicario et al., 2008; Frassinetti et al., 2009). However, in previous studies, such link between space and time emerged when the spatial position of stimuli was prioritized (Magnani et al., 2012) or when responses were lateralized by using a motor response code (Torralbo et al., 2006; Santiago et al., 2007; Ishihara et al., 2008; Vallesi et al., 2008; Anelli et al., 2016, 2018). Moreover, left/short and right/long association was demonstrated when a left or right shift of visuospatial attention was induced through the prismatic adaptation procedure (Frassinetti et al., 2009; Magnani et al., 2011; Oliveri et al., 2013; see for a review Anelli and Frassinetti, 2019). Thus, the Muller-Lyer illusion does not affect time estimation, probably because it is based on visual perception rather than on spatial attention. The perceptual nature of this illusion is supported by neuroimaging studies, showing that activity in the occipital extrastriate cortex, especially the lateral occipital cortex, correlates with the strength of the Muller-Lyer illusion (Weidner and Fink, 2007; Plewan et al., 2012). Neuropsychological evidence on patients affected by stroke, which is one of the leading causes of disability (Scrutinio et al., 2020), also indicates that the lack of the illusion correlates with the degree of damage in occipital regions in patients with the visual deficit, i.e., hemianopia (Vallar et al., 2000; Daini et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2001). Therefore, one of the novelties of our results is that time perception is not modulated by a pure perceptual visual illusion that does not affect visual spatial attention. A further critical result for this study concerns the role of age on time and space processing. In the temporal bisection task, young participants perceived temporal duration as longer compared to elderly participants, and in the spatial bisection task, they showed a leftward bias. Coherent with the hypothesis, that time is spatially represented, this bias due to a leftward shift of spatial attention (Friedrich et al., 2018) may influence time perception, inducing a shorter reference-time-interval stored in memory in young than in elderly participants. Consequently, when young people compare interval duration with the reference time in memory, they overestimate the duration of stimuli. In contrast, in elderly people, who do not show the leftward bias, the reference-time-interval stored in memory is longer than in young, and then they underestimate the stimuli duration. These data could suggest that individual differences in spatial attention may play a role in the construction of the memory temporal standard by which durations are categorized.

Our results are in line with previous works on temporal processing which demonstrated that the underestimation bias found in temporal judgment in elderly people is due to a deceleration of the internal clock speed (Craik and Hay, 1999; Perbal et al., 2002). For instance, Craik and Hay (1999) argued that the pacemaker of elderly participants emits lower rate pulses that contribute to slowing down of the internal clock. This interpretation well fits with the common feeling that the sense of the passage of time is fast in aging.

Conclusion

Our results provide further support to the existence of a close relationship between time and space which is mainly driven by the closeness of the stimulus to the body. This finding reveals the highly flexible and plastic nature of time representation which could be for humans an optimal strategy to enhance detection of sensory information in the environment to accurately tune both spatial and temporal information during everyday activities.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Bologna. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MC and FF designed this study. MC collected and analyzed the data and wrote this manuscript. MD’A and FF provided revisions. All authors approved the final version of this manuscript for submission.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Health (Ricerca Finalizzata PE-2016-02362477) and by grants from Fondazione Carisbo (2016/0088) and Ricerca Fondamentale Orientata (RFO; University of Bologna) to FF.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.796799/full#supplementary-material

References

Anelli, F., Candini, M., Cappelletti, M., Oliveri, M., and Frassinetti, F. (2015). The Remapping of Time by Active Tool-Use. PLoS One 10:e0146175. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anelli, F., Ciaramelli, E., Arzy, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2016). Prisms to travel in time: investigation of time-space association through prismatic adaptation effect on mental time travel. Cognition 156, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anelli, F., and Frassinetti, F. (2019). Prisms for timing better: a review on application of prism adaptation on temporal domain. Cortex 119, 583–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anelli, F., Peters-Founshtein, G., Shreibman, Y., Moreh, E., Forlani, C., Frassinetti, F., et al. (2018). Nature and nurture effects on the spatiality of the mental time line. Sci. Rep. U K. 8:11710. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29584-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolo, A., Coello, Y., Edwards, M. G., Delepoulle, S., Endo, S., and Wing, A. M. (2014). Contribution of the motor system to the perception of reachable space: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 3807–3817. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benwell, C. S., Thut, G., Grant, A., and Harvey, M. (2014). A rightward shift in the visuospatial attention vector with healthy aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:113. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berti, A., and Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 415–420. doi: 10.1162/089892900562237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggio, M., Bisio, A., Avanzino, L., Ruggeri, P., and Bove, M. (2017). This racket is not mine: the influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia 103, 54–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisiach, E., Ricci, R., Lualdi, M., and Colombo, M. R. (1998). Perceptual and response bias in unilateral neglect: two modified versions of the milner landmark task. Brain Cogn. 37, 369–386. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, R. A., Zakay, D., and Hancock, P. A. (1998). Human aging and duration judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Aging 13, 584–596. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.4.584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., and Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: evidence for a mental time line. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 36, 2257–2273. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourgeois, J., Farnè, A., and Coello, Y. (2014). Costs and benefits of tool-use on the perception of reachable space. Acta. Psychol. 148, 91–95. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowers, D., and Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18, 491–498. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(80)90151-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, J. G., and Kirk, A. (1999). Line bisection performance of normal adults: two subgroups with opposite biases. Neurology 53, 527–532. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.3.527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brozzoli, C., Ehrsson, H., and Farnè, A. (2014). Multisensory representation of the space near the hand: from perception to action and inter individual interactions. Neuroscientist 20, 122–135. doi: 10.1177/1073858413511153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Candini, M., Giuberti, V., Santelli, E., di Pellegrino, G., and Frassinetti, F. (2019). When social and action spaces diverge: a study in children with typical development and autism. Autism 23, 1687–1698. doi: 10.1177/1362361318822504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardinali, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., and Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr. Biol. 19, R478–R479. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coren, S., and Girgus, J. S. (1978). Seeing is Deceiving: The Psychology of Visual Illusions. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Sinigaglia, C., and Gallese, V. (2011). Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand. Neuropsychologia 49, 2658–2663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowie, R., and Hamil, G. (1998). Variation among non-clinical subjects on a line bisection task. Percep. Motor Skill. 86:834. doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., and Hay, J. F. (1999). Aging and judgments of duration: effects of task complexity and method of estimation. Percept. Psychophis. 61, 549–560. doi: 10.3758/bf03211972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daini, R., Angelelli, P., Antonucci, G., Cappa, S. F., and Vallar, G. (2001). Illusions of length in spatial unilateral neglect. Cortex 37, 710–714. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70621-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, M., di Pellegrino, G., Seriani, S., Gallina, P., and Frassinetti, F. (2018). The sense of agency shapes body schema and peripersonal space. Sci. Rep. U K. 8:13847. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32238-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Kock, R., Gladhill, K. N., Ali, N., Joiner, W. M., and Wiener, M. (2021). How movements shape the perception of time. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 950–963. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vignemont, F., and Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia 70, 327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Bono, M. G., Casarotti, M., Priftis, K., Gava, L., Umiltà, C., and Zorzi, M. (2012). Priming the mental time line. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. 38, 838–842. doi: 10.1037/a0028346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farnè, A., Iriki, A., and Làdavas, E. (2005). Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia 43, 238–248. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsberg, A., O’Dowd, A., and Gherri, E. (2019). Tool use modulates early stages of visuo-tactile integration in far space: evidence from event-related potentials. Biol. Psychol. 145, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frassinetti, F., Magnani, B., and Oliveri, M. (2009). Prismatic lenses shift time perception. Psychol. Sci. 20, 949–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedrich, T. E., Hunter, P. V., and Elias, L. J. (2018). The trajectory of pseudoneglect in adults: a systematic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 28, 436–452. doi: 10.1007/s11065-018-9392-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagura, N., Kanai, R., Orgs, G., and Haggard, P. (2012). Ready steady slow: action preparation slows the subjective passage of time. Proc. Soc. B. 279, 4399–4406. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, M., Milner, A. D., and Roberts, R. C. (1995). An investigation of hemispatial neglect using the landmark task. Brain Cogn. 27, 59–78.

Google Scholar

Harvey, M., Pool, T. D., Roberson, M. J., and Olk, B. (2000). Effects of visible and invisible cueing procedures on perceptual judgments in young and elderly subjects. Neuropsychologia 38, 22–31. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00055-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunley, S. B., Marker, A. M., and Lourenco, S. F. (2017). Individual Differences in the Flexibility of Peripersonal Space. Exp. Psychol. 64, 49–55. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isham, E. A., Le, C. H., and Ekstrom, A. D. (2018). Rightward and leftward biases in temporal reproduction of objects represented in central and peripheral spaces. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 153, 71–78. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., and Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time: evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex 44, 454–461. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jewell, G., and McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 93–110. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00045-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson-Frey, H. S. (2004). The neural bases of complex tool use in humans. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 71–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Learmonth, G., Benwell, C., Thut, G., and Harvey, M. (2017). Age-related reduction of hemispheric lateralisation for spatial attention: an EEG study. NeuroImage 153, 139–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Learmonth, G., Gallagher, A., Gibson, J., Thut, G., and Harvey, M. (2015). Intra- and inter-task reliability of spatial attention measures in pseudoneglect. PLoS One 10:e0138379. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindbergh, C. A., and Kieffaber, P. D. (2013). The neural correlates of temporal judgments in the duration bisection task. Neuropsychologia 51, 191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., and Lourenco, S. F. (2006). On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44, 977–981. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., and Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Space perception and body morphology: extent of near space scales with arm length. Exp. Brain Res. 177, 285–290. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0855-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnani, B., Oliveri, M., Mancuso, G., Galante, E., and Frassinetti, F. (2011). Time and spatial attention: effects of prism adaptation on temporal deficits in brain damaged patients. Neuropsychologia 49, 1016–1023. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnani, B., Pavani, F., and Frassinetti, F. (2012). Changing auditory time with prismatic goggles. Cognition 125, 233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maravita, A., and Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 79–86.

Google Scholar

Martel, M., Cardinali, L., Roy, A. C., and Farnè, A. (2016). Tool use: an open window into body representation and its plasticity. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 33, 82–101. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1167678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCourt, M. E., and Garlinghouse, M. (2000). Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex 36, 715–731. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70548-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, M. A., Nemitz, J. W., and Stein, B. E. (1987). Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors. J. Neurosci. 7, 3215–3229. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-10-03215.1987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, D., and Schwarz, W. (2008). “1-2-3”: is there a temporal number line? Evidence from a serial comparison task. Exp. Psychol. 55, 143–150. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.3.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, M., and White, R. (1978). In the Zone, Transcendent Experience in Sports. England: Penguin Arkana.

Google Scholar

Noel, J. P., Lukowska, M., Wallace, M., and Serino, A. (2016). Multisensory simultaneity judgment and proximity to the body. J. Vis. 16:21. doi: 10.1167/16.3.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveri, M., Magnani, B., Filipelli, A., Avanzi, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2013). Prismatic adaptation effects on spatial representation of time in neglect patients. Cortex 49, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paraskevoudi, N., Balcı, F., and Vatakis, A. (2018). “Walking” through the sensory, cognitive, and temporal degradations of healthy aging. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. [Epub online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patané, I., Farnè, A., and Frassinetti, F. (2016a). Prismatic Adaptation Induces Plastic Changes onto Spatial and Temporal Domains in Near and Far Space. Neural Plast. 2016:3495075. doi: 10.1155/2016/3495075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patané, I., Iachini, T., Farnè, A., and Frassinetti, F. (2016b). Disentangling Action from Social Space: tool-Use Differently Shapes the Space around Us. PLoS One 11:e0154247. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perbal, S., Droit, S., Isingrini, M., and Pouthas, V. (2002). Relationships between age-related changes in time estimation and age-related changes in processing speed, attention, and memory. Aging Neuropsychol. C. 9, 201–216. doi: 10.1076/anec.9.3.201.9609

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrizzo, I., Castaldi, E., Anobile, G., Bassanelli, S., and Arrighi, R. (2021). Time and numerosity estimation in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Acta Psychol. 215:103296. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plewan, T., Weidner, R., Eickhoff, S. B., and Fink, G. R. (2012). Ventral and dorsal stream interactions during the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion: evidence derived from fMRI and dynamic causal modeling. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2015–2029. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restle, F., and Decker, J. (1977). Size of the Mueller-Lyer illusion as a function of its dimensions: theory and data. Percept. Psychophys. 21, 489–503. doi: 10.3758/BF03198729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., and Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: a critical review of neuroimaging data. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 64, 421–437. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science 277, 190–191. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5323.190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ro, T., and Rafal, R. D. (1996). Perception of geometric illusions in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 34, 973–978. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00018-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, A. D., and Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2009). Time estimation abilities in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 23, 178–188. doi: 10.1037/a0014289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santiago, J., Lupiánez, J., Pérez, E., and Funes, M. J. (2007). Time, also flies from left to right. Psychon. B. Rev. 14, 512–516. doi: 10.3758/bf03194099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santiago, J., Román, A., Ouellet, M., Rodríguez, N., and Pérez-Azor, P. (2010). In hindsight, life flows from left to right. Psychol. Res. 74, 59–70. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0220-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sardone, R., Lampignano, L., Guerra, V., Zupo, R., Donghia, R., Castellana, F., et al. (2020). Relationship between inflammatory food consumption and age-related hearing loss in a prospective observational cohort: results from the salus in apulia study. Nutrients 12:426. doi: 10.3390/nu12020426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, R., Dehon, H., and Peigneux, P. (2013). Lateralized processing of false memories and pseudoneglect in aging. Cortex 49, 1314–1324. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, R., and Peigneux, P. (2011). Age-related changes in visual pseudoneglect. Brain Cogn. 76, 382–389. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scrutinio, D., Ricciardi, C., Donisi, L., Losavio, E., Battista, P., Guida, P., et al. (2020). Machine learning to predict mortality after rehabilitation among patients with severe stroke. Sci. Rep. 10:20127. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77243-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperandio, I., and Chouinard, P. A. (2015). The mechanisms of size constancy. Multisens. Res. 28, 253–283. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, B., and Stanford, T. (2008). Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 255–266. doi: 10.1038/nrn2331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., and Lupiánez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cogn. Sci. 30, 745–757. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turgeon, M., Lustig, C., and Meck, W. H. (2016). Cognitive Aging and Time Perception: roles of Bayesian Optimization and Degeneracy. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:102. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turgeon, M., Wing, A. M., and Taylor, L. W. (2011). Timing and aging: slowing of fastest regular tapping rate with preserved timing error detection and correction. Psychol. Aging 26, 150–161. doi: 10.1037/a0020606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallar, G., Daini, R., and Antonucci, G. (2000). Processing of illusion of length in spatial hemineglect: a study of line bisection. Neuropsychologia 38, 1087–1097. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00139-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., and Shallice, T. (2008). An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition 107, 501–527. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., McIntosh, A. R., and Stuss, D. T. (2011). How time modulates spatial responses. Cortex 47, 148–156. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varnava, A., McCarthy, M., and Beaumont, J. G. (2002). Line bisection in normal adults: direction of attentional bias for near and far space. Neuropsychologia 40, 1372–1378. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00204-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicario, C. M., Pecoraro, P., Turriziani, P., Koch, G., Caltagirone, C., and Oliveri, M. (2008). Relativistic compression and expansion of experiential time in the left and right space. PLoS One 3:e1716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., Valenza, N., and Landis, T. (2001). Explicit and implicit perception of illusory contours in unilateral spatial neglect: behavioural and anatomical correlates of preattentive grouping mechanisms. Neuropsychologia 39, 597–610. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00148-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weger, U. W., and Pratt, J. (2008). Time flies like an arrow: space-time compatibility effects suggest the use of a mental timeline. Psychon. B. Rev. 15, 426–430. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.2.426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weidner, R., and Fink, G. R. (2007). The neural mechanisms underlying the Müller-Lyer illusion and its interaction with visuospatial judgments. Cereb. Cortex 17, 878–884. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witt, J. K. (2011). Action’s Effect on Perception. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 201–206. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: time perception, Muller-Lyer illusion, tool-use, near/far space, time bisection task

Citation: Candini M, D’Angelo M and Frassinetti F (2022) Time Interaction With Two Spatial Dimensions: From Left/Right to Near/Far. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:796799. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.796799

Received: 17 October 2021; Accepted: 20 December 2021;
Published: 18 January 2022.

Edited by:

Giuseppe Giglia, University of Palermo, Italy

Reviewed by:

Filippo Brighina, University of Palermo, Italy
Maria Pia Viggiano, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Candini, D’Angelo and Frassinetti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michela Candini, bWljaGVsYS5jYW5kaW5pMkB1bmliby5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.