Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 22 December 2021
Sec. Brain-Computer Interfaces
This article is part of the Research Topic Real-World Applications of Neurophysiological Monitoring for Passive Brain-Computer Interfaces View all 7 articles

Multi-Parameter Physiological State Monitoring in Target Detection Under Real-World Settings

  • 1Institute of Bioinformatics and Systems Biology, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
  • 2Center for Intelligent Drug Systems and Smart Bio-Devices (IDS2B), College of Biological Science and Technology, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
  • 3Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
  • 4Drug Development and Value Creation Research Center, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung City, Taiwan

Mental state changes induced by stimuli under experimental settings or by daily events in real life affect task performance and are entwined with physical and mental health. In this study, we developed a physiological state indicator with five parameters that reflect the subject’s real-time physiological states based on online EEG signal processing. These five parameters are attention, fatigue, stress, and the brain activity shifts of the left and right hemispheres. We designed a target detection experiment modified by a cognitive attention network test for validating the effectiveness of the proposed indicator, as such conditions would better approximate a real chaotic environment. Results demonstrated that attention levels while performing the target detection task were significantly higher than during rest periods, but also exhibited a decay over time. In contrast, the fatigue level increased gradually and plateaued by the third rest period. Similar to attention levels, the stress level decreased as the experiment proceeded. These parameters are therefore shown to be highly correlated to different stages of the experiment, suggesting their usage as primary factors in passive brain-computer interfaces (BCI). In addition, the left and right brain activity indexes reveal the EEG neural modulations of the corresponding hemispheres, which set a feasible reference of activation for an active BCI control system, such as one executing motor imagery tasks. The proposed indicator is applicable to potential passive and active BCI applications for monitoring the subject’s physiological state change in real-time, along with providing a means of evaluating the associated signal quality to enhance the BCI performance.

Introduction

Brain-computer interfaces (BCI) are methods that offer direct communication pathways between the human brain and external devices which have attracted much attention in various fields. They provide great potential for controlling machines, especially through a hands-free approach. To translate electroencephalographic (EEG) signals into actual commands they depend on users to generate EEG patterns recognizable to the system. These signals can be improved by appropriate training (Neuper and Pfurtscheller, 2009), however, even if with such training, the inevitable mental state changes of the individual during BCI usage still impact performance. Effects of attention and fatigue are regarded as the main causes of variation in the performance of BCIs (Myrden and Chau, 2015). Research has found that task engagement and attentional processes may impact the performance of several BCI techniques, such as P300 (Riccio et al., 2013) and motor imagery (MI) (Zhang et al., 2016). Mental fatigue produced by prolonged sequences of a cognitive task affects the accuracy of BCIs, as it decreases the separability of EEG signals (Talukdar et al., 2019). Furthermore, in a recent study by Zhang et al. (2020), it has been found that resting EEG modulation induced by stress is one of the key factors to signal-to-noise ratio and BCI performance. To address such impact of mental state changes on active BCIs, passive BCI has been proposed to assess the user’s cognitive state during ongoing BCI tasks, allowing the improvement in the interaction of human-machine system (Zander and Kothe, 2011).

On the other hand, people encounter various situations and events in daily life that elicit different emotions and physiological states than those in laboratory settings. Beyond their influences on BCI systems, some physiological states, such as stress and fatigue, are responsible for certain physical and mental health problems. For example, stress accounts for physical problems such as high blood sugar, high blood pressure, high cholesterol, obesity (Clark et al., 2011), and mental problems, e.g., depressive or anxiety disorders (Virtanen et al., 2007). Fatigue leads to decreased attention, which is the main cause of transportation accidents (Roets and Christiaens, 2019), and is reported to affect several psychological and physiological aspects of health (Lock et al., 2018). Besides stress and fatigue, other physiological states affect human life in various ways. Since physiological states play such important roles in both BCI and human life, how to measure and visualize them both within and beyond experimental settings is crucial and valuable.

A popular approach of deriving physiological states is from physiological signals, including EEG, electrocardiography (ECG) (Lampert, 2015; Hsu et al., 2017; Brás et al., 2018; Huang et al., 2018), electromyography (EMG) (Wijsman et al., 2013; Luijcks et al., 2014; Van Cutsem et al., 2017; Kehri et al., 2019), respiration (Widjaja et al., 2013; Grassmann et al., 2016), and galvanic skin response (GSR) (Villarejo et al., 2012; Fernandes et al., 2014; Krupinski et al., 2015; Kurniawan et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2016). Different physiological states regulate these autonomic physiological signals which naturally respond to changes in the central and peripheral nervous systems. Compared with other physiological approaches, EEG more directly reflects responses to the environment and external stimuli present in the processing of these events by the central nervous system, thus yielding more reliable outcomes. One of the most common methods for the investigation of brain dynamics is to identify the EEG spectral characteristics under varied conditions. Typically, EEG power spectra are quantified and divided by frequency ranges, such as delta (1–3 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (13–31 Hz), and gamma (32–50 Hz) bands (Tatum, 2014), with these oscillations within different frequency bands representative of various brain activations and conditions. Several publications in recent years have appeared documenting that EEG is a robust approach to evaluate physiological states, including alertness, drowsiness (Lin et al., 2010; Bajaj et al., 2020), fatigue (Monteiro et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2019), and stress (Xia et al., 2018; Asif et al., 2019). To assess the changes of physiological states exploiting EEG signals, five parameters are employed to develop the proposed pentagonal physiological state indicator: attention, stress, fatigue, and left and right brain activity levels.

Attention has been one of the most discussed cognitive functions in the field of neuroscience. It is an ability of an individual to selectively concentrate on relevant information while filtering irrelevant information. The attentional networks composed of alerting, orienting, and executive attention proposed by Posner and Petersen have been widely studied in terms of functional and anatomical perspectives (Posner and Petersen, 1990; Raz, 2004; Xuan et al., 2016). The attentional network test (ANT) is a behavioral task designed to evaluate the three stages of attention (Fan et al., 2002). The experiment that was developed to validate the proposed indicator also utilized ANT as a reference. Studies based on EEG indicate that attention can be estimated by detecting changes in alpha power activity, especially in the frontal lobe which is known to be responsible in regulating concentration (Quilodran et al., 2008; Doesburg et al., 2016). Research showed that the power spectra of theta and alpha increase when the subject is going into drowsiness (Jung et al., 1997; Lin et al., 2005); consistent experimental evidence has also demonstrated that mental engagement is inversely correlated to theta and alpha activities in the frontal region (Pope et al., 1995; Szafir and Mutlu, 2012). Our previous study has also demonstrated a real-time alertness detection system using this approach (Lin et al., 2010). Furthermore, research has applied similar approaches to determine attentional states during BCI experiments, providing a means to improve BCI performance (Lee et al., 2015).

Fatigue is a state that follows excessive physical or cognitive activity, thus it is typically partitioned as physical fatigue or mental fatigue (Jason et al., 2010). Physical fatigue is associated with body or muscle exhaustion, which leads to unsatisfactory physical performance. In contrast with physical fatigue, mental fatigue generally denotes a feeling of sleepiness or drowsiness, and it manifests as decreased attentional capacities and prolonged reaction times (Boksem et al., 2005), resulting in reduced efficiency. In a simulated driving experiment designed to study fatigue by Ma et al. (2019) subjects were allowed limited sleep and performed driving on tedious paths. It was found that alpha and beta activities in parietal and occipital lobes were strongly associated with the driver’s fatigue level (Ma et al., 2019). Another experiment exploiting simulated driving by Huang et al. (2016) demonstrated a closed-loop fatigue mitigation system that detected fatigue from the alteration of theta and alpha power in occipital components of recorded EEG. Similarly, experimental results of an ICA-based BCI study by Chuang et al. (2013) showed that increasing power in theta and alpha ranges were highly correlated with poor task performance and fatigue.

With the rapid pace of modern lifestyles, people race against time and experience different kinds of stressors. It is a well-known fact that stress causes negative effects on our health, emotions, and cognitive functions (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; Pearlin et al., 2005; Lazarus, 2006). However, mild-to-moderate stress has been found to be salutary, with this relationship between stress and performance described as the “inverted-U theory” (Sapolsky, 2015). The frontal lobes of the brain have been proposed as moderators of emotional regulation; extensive research has shown that frontal alpha asymmetry, representing the homologous differences between right and left frontal sites (typically F4–F3), of the is associated with affective processing (Harmon-Jones and Gable, 2018; Reznik and Allen, 2018). The asymmetry score is derived by subtracting natural log-transformed left hemisphere alpha power from the right, with positive asymmetry scores indicating stronger relative activity (Pfurtscheller et al., 1996). Specifically, it has been demonstrated that positive asymmetry scores are linked to positive emotions, whereas negative asymmetry scores are associated with stress responses and negative emotions (Meyer et al., 2014; Quaedflieg et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2018a).

Motor imagery has been a widely discussed technique in BCI applications, and acts as an endogenous BCI, as it relies on spontaneous brain activities, as opposed to brain dynamics elicited by an external stimulus. The signals employed in MI-based BCI are based on principles of event-related desynchronizations and synchronizations, which correspond to changes of EEG power in the mu rhythm during either mental imagination of movement or actual motor execution (Pfurtscheller and Neuper, 1994; Pfurtscheller and Neurophysiology, 1997). Previous studies revealed electrode positions C3 and C4 are the most important ones for differentiating different motor imagery tasks by acquiring mu rhythm activities (Ramoser et al., 2000; Pfurtscheller et al., 2006).

To investigate the effectiveness of the proposed physiological state indicator, a prolonged experiment based on target detection was conducted. As acquiring EEG signals outside of experimental settings is impractical, a portable multi-channel EEG device was incorporated into the system development, as the long typical setup time and procedure needed for preparation of a standard off-the-shelf EEG system could affect the results in a study focusing on a time dependent effect like fatigue. In this study, a physiological state indicator with five parameters that reflect the physiological state changes based on EEG dynamics was developed, specifying the real-time fluctuations of the proposed parameters during the usage of BCI. These five parameters are attention, fatigue, stress, and the brain activities of the left and right hemispheres. The incorporated physiological states have been well-studied, with each having significant evidence for adopting specific EEG channels and frequency bands to derive them. The proposed experiment was designed to explore the neurophysiological changes using the indicator during a prolonged, attention-related task. With the proposed indicator, BCI usage can be evaluated according to a user’s physiological state changes, yielding improved BCI performances.

Materials and Methods

The parameters in the indicator were derived by EEG signal processing. We used portable wireless EEG equipment to acquire the EEG signals during the experiment to validate the effectiveness of the indicator. The signal processing flowchart is illustrated in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Signal processing for parameters. The EEG signals were acquired through a 32-channel wireless EEG device and preprocessed for artifact removal. Then, the signals of the particular sites of interest were decomposed and normalized to yield five parameters.

Participants

Forty college students (30 males, 10 females) aged from 18 to 30 were recruited for participation in the study. No subjects reported histories of psychiatric or neurological disorders. All the participants were instructed to perform the task using their right hand. The experiments were administered at National Chaio Tung University (Hsinchu, Taiwan). The study was approved by the institutional review boards (IRB) of National Chaio Tung University.

Experimental Paradigm

In this study, we designed a target detection experiment that simulates a battlefield scenario to elicit state changes assessed by the proposed indicator (He et al., 2021). The experiment was composed of a 30-s EEG baseline recording and five blocks of 12-min sessions along with a 5-min rest session. First, the participant was seated in front of a 23.6” monitor with a relaxed posture. Then, the wireless EEG device was placed on the participant’s head and adjusted to fit. The participant was asked to fixate at the monitor showing a cross-mark for 30 s to establish a baseline recording. Afterward, the participant underwent a 68-min task to induce attention, fatigue, and stress level changes. In each trial, the participant was shown a cue representing in an asterisk on the left or right, indicating the probable location that a target would appear. The cue was followed by the presentation of a target, which was a figure of a soldier holding a rifle, accompanied with five other figures as distractors. The participants were required to press the button on the keyboard according to the direction of the target as soon as possible (Figure 2). Congruent conditions occurred if the direction of the target and the cue were the same and incongruent if they were opposite. To mimic more realistic circumstances, a condition with an absence of the target represented a no-target condition, for which the participant was required to press the “up” button. As the reaction time (RT) of the no-target condition is assumed to be longer, the criterion of maximal possible reaction time before a trial to be considered as fail is set at 2 s. Within a 12-min session, each of the three conditions were distributed between the 180 trials, resulting in 60 trials of each condition presented randomly during the session. Each session is followed by a 5-min rest. The total duration of the experiment was approximately 1 h, which was designed to bring about a mental state of fatigue. During the rest period, subjects were instructed to relax in order to ameliorate some of the neurological effects resulting from prolonged tension or discomfort produced during the active tasks; resting data from the period after the first 2 min of rest were used so as to minimize signal artifacts carrying over from the previous active stage. The combination of session and rest repeated four times, comprising the whole experiment. The attention parameter was assessed in both active and resting periods, while the fatigue was assessed and validated in the rest periods because other physiological state changes could interference the measurement of the fatigue parameter. To further investigate the effect of fatigue caused by continuous cognitive performance, the subjects were inquired to rate their self-evaluated fatigue level they perceived during task performances on a 10-point rating scale according to the method described by Karrer et al. (2004).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experimental paradigm based on target detection. The experiment was composed of five sessions and five resting periods, with each session consisting of 180 trials. Each subject was instructed to respond to the target, and the conditions of the stimulus referred to the relationship between the cue and the target (He et al., 2021). To induce the effect of fatigue, the experiment lasted about 1 h.

Electroencephalographic Signal Acquisition and Preprocessing

To be compatible with the real-time state indicator, a wireless 32-channel EEG system called VEGA developed by Artise Biomedical, Taiwan was used for EEG acquisition. It consists of a Bluetooth module, miniature amplifiers, and semi-dry sponge-based electrodes with 32 designated channels: Fp1, Fp2, AF3, AF4, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FT7, FC3, FCz, FC4, FT8, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, TP7, CP3, CPz, CP4, TP8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, Oz, and O2. The sponge electrodes used in the experiment function by absorbing saltwater prior to use; their design makes them flexible, adequate for hairy scalps, and usable for long-term EEG acquisition (Ko et al., 2019). The impedance of the skin-electrode interface was monitored constantly during the experiment. The references were located at the earlobes via two ear clips, and the sampling rate was set at 500 Hz. To obtain the features for physiological states, the acquired EEG signal was preprocessed for artifact removal and feature extraction. The signal underwent a 1–50 Hz bandpass filter for the preliminary removal of artifacts. The artifact removal from the EEG signal was carried out using the artifact subspace reconstruction (ASR) in EEGLAB toolbox, which is a component-based method capable of removing large-amplitude noise or artifacts in real-time, and is therefore practical for embedding in the software of portable devices (Chang et al., 2018). The ASR process comprises three steps: (1) Extracting reference, (2) determining threshold, (3) rejecting and reconstructing. To implement online ASR, the reference data were extracted using the baseline before the experiment onset. The reference data was applied to define the rejection threshold. Therefore, the ASR could reject artifact components and reconstruct cleaned data in near real-time using the established threshold (Chang et al., 2019). The signal was then processed via fast Fourier transform to obtain power spectral density (PSD) in decibel (dB). The PSD from the baseline was subtracted from the task periods. Finally, the signals went through band decomposition, yielding four commonly used frequency bands, delta (1–3 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), and beta (13–31 Hz).

Development of Physiological State Indicator

The features used to develop the five parameters were pre-defined, yet their scales were not specified a priori. To obtain easily understandable output parameters for each subject, the EEG power retrieved from 40 subjects was normalized to a usable scale. The general transformation formula was defined based on normalized t-score, but with the scale ranging from 0 to 10. The average power for each parameter was derived by averaging specific power across all subjects, as the power has been processed through baseline removal. Considering the existing and future outlier data, the normalization factor is modified to σ/2 instead of σ. Therefore, it enables 98.75% of the derived physiological state levels, i.e., y(t), to be distributed within 0–10 (μ ± 2.5σ). Previous study has posited the feasibility of using this approach for normalization of EEG power values (Thatcher, 1998). The formula is defined as:

y(t)=5±1nC1nPC(t)-PC¯fC

where P is the EEG band power of the region of interest (ROI), c is the channels included in ROI, n is the number of channels, and f is the normalization factor.

For a better, intuitive visualization, the magnitudes of the normalized parameters were displayed in a pentagonal indicator (Figure 3). Attention is known to be inversely proportional to the theta- and alpha-band amplitudes in the frontal region (Lin et al., 2010; Szafir and Mutlu, 2012), which were the F3, Fz, and F4 sites according to the channel locations on the EEG equipment. Therefore, the level of the attention parameter was defined as:

Attention(t)=5-13(PF3(t)-PF3¯fF3+PFz(t)-PFz¯fFz+PF4(t)-PF4¯fF4)

where P is the EEG power includes theta and alpha frequency bands.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. System information flow. The EEG acquisition is conducted with the software in the computer through Bluetooth. The acquired signal could be applied to a BCI system; at the same time, it is transmitted to the monitoring system running on portable devices. In the system, the signal is computed to easily assess the parameters which are displayed in a pentagon plot.

As previous studies have reported, fatigue level is positively proportional to wide-band EEG power in the occipital region, including theta-, alpha-, and beta-band amplitudes, corresponding to the O1, Oz, and O2 electrodes (Chuang et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2016; Ma et al., 2019). The level of the fatigue parameter is defined as:

Fatigue(t)=5+13(PO1(t)-PO1¯fO1+POz(t)-POz¯fOz+PO2(t)-PO2¯fO2)

where P is the EEG power includes theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands.

Alpha asymmetry in the frontal region, measured F4/F3 signal ratios, was utilized as the stress parameter, defined as:

Stress(t)=5-PF4(t)-PF3(t)fC

where P is the natural log-transformed alpha power and fC is std(PF4-PF3)2. Positive asymmetry scores represent stronger relative left than right activity as higher alpha band power reflects lower brain activity.

Similar to the parameters that positively correlate to the amplitudes of EEG power, the parameters of the left and right brain activity also followed a normalized formula. Hence, the parameter for left brain activity is defined as:

Left(t)=5+PC4(t)-PCz(t)fC

and the parameter of right brain activity is defined as:

Right(t)=5+PC3(t)-PCz(t)fC

where P is the EEG power within mu rhythm (8–13 Hz), fC is std(PC4-PCz)2 for left brain activation and std(PC3-PCz)2 for right brain activation, respectively.

Figure 3 depicts an informational flow diagram underlying the state indicator. While utilizing a BCI accompanied by the physiological state indicator, the acquired EEG signal was transmitted to the computer through Bluetooth. The user data would be recorded for 30 s to establish a baseline for removal, also as the reference data for ASR, as previously described. The recorded signal was then sent to a portable device by a computer program based on C# via local area networks with a refresh rate of 1 s. Any EEG state changes of the user during BCI usage would be converted into corresponding values of the proposed parameters based upon the mean and standard deviation of EEG power acquired by the 40-subject dataset. The indicator on the portable device displays the five parameters acquired from the connected computer on an easily accessible pentagonal plot in near real time.

Results

Behavioral Statistics

In the experiment, the stimuli are categorized as congruent, with the direction of the cue matching the target, incongruent, with the direction of the cue different from the target, or no-target, where the target is absent. To investigate the effects caused by different conditions, the RT and accuracy of the users’ responses were recorded. The condition of the congruent stimuli induced the shortest median RT of 634.7 ms (IQR 567.5–731.0). The incongruent stimuli yielded a slightly longer median RT of 665.7 ms (IQR 613.7–765.9). Other than the left and right stimulus correspondence choices, the subjects were required to press the “up” button after they recognized the absence of the target, which led to the longest median RT of 868.0 ms (IQR 789.5–983.0). The median accuracies of the congruent, incongruent, and no-target conditions were 98.75% (IQR 97.91–99.17), 99.17% (IQR 98.75–100.00), and 98.54% (IQR 97.50–99.58), respectively. The variables of RT and accuracy were compared using Kruskal-Wallis test, as they were not normally distributed. The p-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Dunn and Sidák’s approach. A false discovery rate of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant represented by an asterisk (*) (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Reaction time and accuracy of congruent, incongruent, and no-target stimulus. (A) The mean RT of the three conditions. The RT of no-target stimulus was significantly longer than other conditions. (B) The accuracy of the three conditions. The incongruent stimulus induced the highest accuracy of 99.17% (IQR 98.75–100.00). *indicates that the difference between the two groups was statistically significant. The whiskers extend to the most extreme data points not considered outliers, and the outliers are plotted individually using the “+” marker symbol.

Validation of Parameters

The five parameters within the proposed physiological state indicator were developed using particular combinations of brain regions and EEG frequency bands. Each parameter, respectively, represents a distinct mental state, therefore, we utilized different periods in the experiment for validation. To assess the attention parameter, the whole experiment was included to obtain the dynamics of attention throughout the four experimental sessions and rests. The results demonstrated that attention levels during the active sessions were significantly higher than their subsequent rest periods, but also exhibited a decay over time (Figure 5). The attention level during the rest periods also declined over time and eventually stabilized by the final period. The rest periods in the experiment were used to study the fatigue levels affected by the long sessions during which the subjects were highly focused. The experiment was intentionally lengthy, reaching nearly 1 h, with the intention of inducing fatigue. Consequently, the fatigue levels increased gradually and plateaued by the third rest period (Figure 6). In addition to the assessment by EEG signals, self-evaluated fatigue levels were recorded for comparison (Figure 6C). The stress parameter was intended to respond to the stress that the users experience across different sessions. It illustrated that the stress levels were at their highest in the beginning of the experiment, and then significantly decreased over the four session periods (Figure 7). Since the participants were instructed to perform the experiment with their right hand, hypothetically the derived right brain activity levels should be higher than the left brain activity level during all session periods and approximate to the left brain activity level in all rest periods. However, the results revealed that the left and right brain activities were close to each other, and did not appear to exhibit significant trends (Figure 8). The levels of the parameters of each subject were averaged per period for statistical analysis, yielding one average value of the physiological state level per subject in one period (session or rest). The comparable distributions passed the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for normality. The average levels of the attention parameter between each pair of session and rest were compared using the paired t-test. The average levels of the fatigue and stress parameters were compared using the repeated measure ANOVA with post-hoc test using Dunn and Sidák’s approach. The alpha level of 0.05 was used for statistical hypothesis testing.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Attention level fluctuations during the whole experiment. This parameter was developed using theta and alpha-band power at F3, Fz, and F4. (A) The average attention level across the four session and rest periods. (B) The attention level during the sessions decreased over time and was significantly higher than during the following rest periods in pairs 2 and 3. *indicates that the difference between the two groups was statistically significant. The whiskers extend to the most extreme data points not considered outliers, and the outliers are plotted individually using the “+” marker symbol.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Fatigue level fluctuations during the rest periods. This parameter was developed using theta, alpha and beta-band power at O1, Oz, and O2. (A) The average fatigue level across the four rest periods. (B) The fatigue level increased from rest 1 to rest 3 and mitigated in rest 4. (C) The self-evaluated fatigue level over four rest periods. *indicates that the difference between the two groups was statistically significant. The whiskers extend to the most extreme data points not considered outliers, and the outliers are plotted individually using the “+” marker symbol.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Stress level fluctuations during the active sessions. This parameter was developed using log-transformed alpha-band power at F3 and F4. (A) The average stress level across the four session periods. (B) The stress level significantly decreased over time. *indicates that the difference between the two groups was statistically significant. The whiskers extend to the most extreme data points not considered outliers, and the outliers are plotted individually using the “+” marker symbol.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Left and right brain activity level fluctuations during the whole experiment. The parameters were developed using alpha-band power of C4–Cz and C3–Cz. It shows that the left and right brain activities are approximate to each other, and neither ascended nor descended significantly.

Discussion

In this study, we developed a physiological state indicator integrated with wireless EEG equipment, enabling the assessment of physiological states for potential passive and active BCI applications. Prior active BCI studies have documented the effects of physiological state changes on BCI performance (Riccio et al., 2013; Myrden and Chau, 2015; Talukdar et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2020). During long-term BCI performance users’ physiological states change spontaneously, which lead to unsatisfactory outcomes. However, passive BCIs may facilitate the use of traditional, active BCIs by providing an estimation of the underlying user state variations (Millán et al., 2010). Parameters that were reported to affect BCI performance, including attention, fatigue, and stress were adopted in the proposed system, and the left and right brain activity parameters were designed specifically for MI-based BCI, yielding a collection of five parameters for BCI assessment. The experiment based on ANT not only induced attentional alterations, the prolonged duration also caused fatigue in the subjects and the moderate difficulty allowed subjects to adapt, producing transitions in stress levels.

To induce attention differences between active sessions and rest periods, but not produce excessive fatigue over the 1-h duration, we designed an experiment that was based on target detection and ANT, somewhat modified to be closer to real-world scenarios. With this experiment, the relationship among these three conditions and the physiological state changes of the users can be evaluated. Generally, incongruent conditions introduce “conflict” that requires longer information processing times, resulting in longer RTs. In our experiment, the target in the no-target condition is absent, hypothetically causing even longer RTs than the incongruent conditions. The no-target condition demonstrated a lower accuracy of 98.54% and a significantly longer median RT of 868.0 ms, compared to the congruent and incongruent conditions. The conflict in the incongruent condition did bring about slightly longer median RT, however, the had the highest accuracy among the conditions. This may have been caused by the relatively simple experimental design compared to similar cognitive experiments, such as the classical ANT protocol. As the result, the median accuracies for all conditions exceeded 98%. Nevertheless, the experimental procedure provided the necessary factors to elicit physiological state changes, especially for the adopted parameters. Although the participants were instructed to respond as soon as possible, the maximal possible reaction time before a trial was considered as failed was set at 2 s because the experiment included the no-target condition. This was assumed to require a longer reaction time, resulting a ceiling effect for performance. However, several participants performed relatively poorly, and there were significant differences between congruent/incongruent and incongruent/no-target conditions, indicating that the three conditions could elicit the variations in performance within participants, despite the ceiling effect.

A large body of prior literature has indicated that neurophysiological signals contain information related to physiological state changes, which were acquired via EEG in our experiment (Lin et al., 2010; Xia et al., 2018; Asif et al., 2019; Monteiro et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2019; Bajaj et al., 2020). Some of the research in recent years has applied machine learning or deep learning methods to identify different stages of physiological states (Jebelli et al., 2019; Ma et al., 2019; Khessiba et al., 2021). However, the high computational power that deep learning demands does not necessarily provide better classification performance due to inter-subject variabilities. For example, in studies that investigated human fatigue during driving simulation experiments, one experiment deployed an EEG classifier based on convolutional neural networks (CNN), which demonstrated an average accuracy of 92.68% in the intra-subject test (Zeng et al., 2018). By contrast, it demonstrated a reduced performance of 84.38% in an inter-subject binary classification, falling beneath the performance reported by Ko et al. (2015), which employed a linear regression model on a 1–30 Hz EEG power spectrum array. A review study by Monteiro et al. (2019) also demonstrated that linear models like fisher linear discriminant analysis (FLDA) could outperform deep belief network (DBN) while using PSD to assess mental fatigue in cross-subject classification. In consideration of the computational capacity necessary to process five parameters in real-time on portable devices, we implemented models that determine physiological state levels using linear transforms of EEG power amplitudes. Additionally, adaptive BCIs have been proposed to deal with the ubiquitous non-stationarities in EEG signals (Shenoy et al., 2006). Linear transformation models like our indicator benefit the such adaptive models by providing consecutive levels that correspond to user physiological state changes rather than binary classification (Krol et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2018b).

To study and validate the indicator that was developed based on cognitive state changes, a prolonged experiment incorporating EEG signal recording was administered. In general, long-term task performance results in increases in fatigue and reductions in goal-directed attention (Boksem et al., 2005). In the validation of the attention parameter, the attention levels in the active sessions surpassed those in the subsequent rest periods, and fluctuations indicated that the users’ mental engagements dropped in the resting periods, then ascended right after the task onset. In addition, attention level exhibited a decay over time in both active sessions and rest periods, suggesting that attention reduced throughout the experiment. In other words, the subject’s theta and alpha band amplitudes in the frontal region increased over time under both conditions. However, it is generally accepted that attention is categorized into a variety of types based upon functional aspects. Sustained attention represents the attentional functionalities that determine the cognitive capacity and efficacy of selective and divided attention (Sarter et al., 2001), which is represented by the attention parameter, and reflects the vigilance, of an individual during long-term BCI usages.

The cognitive demands during the experiment cause mental fatigue, which is assumed to increase over time. Trejo et al. (2015) demonstrated that the EEG signs of developing mental fatigue are present 15–30 min after task onset. In our paradigm, physiological state changes other than fatigue could confound the measurement of the fatigue parameter. Therefore, the fatigue was assessed and validated in the rest periods, as opposed to the attention parameter which was assessed in both active and resting periods. The results showed that the estimated fatigue levels during the rest periods, which followed 12-min cognitive performance sessions, progressively ascended and peaked in the third period, indicating the subjects were experiencing maximal fatigue by that time. Correspondingly, the self-evaluated fatigue level in period 3 reached an average of 7.25 points out of 10, which was also the highest among the four rest periods. The fatigue level demonstrated a similar trend to the cluster of “strugglers” proposed by Karrer et al. (2004), which presented a high level of fatigue during a simulated driving test and subsequent reduction of fatigue toward the end of the driving test. We inferred that the perception of the impending end of the experiment resulted in a reduction of fatigue level, possibly indicating a mental relaxation in the subject.

Stress is a parameter that was not specifically elicited in the experimental design, instead, the spontaneous responses of the subjects during four sessions were recorded. Compared with the EEG characteristics that correlate to attention and fatigue, frontal alpha asymmetry is a relatively well-established electrophysiological feature for stress assessment. In a study proposed by Arpaia et al. (2020), frontal alpha asymmetry was applied to machine learning classifiers and achieved more than a 90% accuracy in classifying 2-s EEG epochs using a wearable EEG instrument. Similarly, the F4/F3 asymmetry was adopted in this study to assess the stress that the subject experienced in the successive performance of the cognitive task. As the experiment proceeded, subjects’ stress levels exhibited a descending trend, indicating that the subjects felt less anxious toward the last session, according to gradually increased F4-F3 asymmetry scores. Generally speaking, situations that are unpredictable, ambiguous, or unfamiliar are more likely to elicit stress (Michie and medicine, 2002). In our experiment, which did not include induced stressors, the unfamiliarity of the first session, where the subjects experienced the highest level of stress, was posited as the main factor of stress; this elevated stress level was mitigated as the subjects habituated the experiment.

One limitation of the proposed indicator is that the advanced algorithms of MI-BCIs were not included in the parameters of left and right activity. The MI-BCI algorithms have been extensively studied to distinguish the EEG patterns representative of the mental imagination of different movements. Simply measuring the mu rhythm power of the left and right motor cortices might not be sufficient to differentiate motor executions based on the results of the left and right brain activity parameters. Estimating the subject’s event-related desynchronization/synchronization is fundamental to MI, however, additional algorithms utilizing the existing parameters are required for accurate MI-BCI control.

In this study, the features for the five parameters were adopted based on online signal processing using the findings of previous studies. The experiment intended to elicit the variations in the parameters of the subjects, specify the EEG power ranges of the parameters, and thus develop a real-time multi-parameter physiological state indicator with proper scaling. In the experimental design, the general attention and fatigue were assessed, while the stress, left activity, and right activity were studied through the spontaneous responses of the participants. However, to investigate each of the parameters throughout, sophisticated experimental design for each of the parameters would benefit the investigation of the physiological state changes. Furthermore, the parameters have intricate interactions and are not independent mechanisms. For instance, the impact of fatigue on attention (Boksem et al., 2005; Faber et al., 2012), the interactions between attention and stress (Chajut and Algom, 2003; Zhang et al., 2020), and even the effects of fatigue and stress on MI (Talukdar et al., 2019; Schlatter et al., 2020). Such usage of a general indicator doesn’t warrant a detailed intra-parametric analysis, though that could be an interesting direction for generating a future indicator.

Mental workload is a parameter we would like to investigate and incorporate into the system in the future. Despite different causes, the physiological correlates of mental workload and stress manifest in rather similar ways (Parent et al., 2019). Likewise, research by Käthner et al. (2014) reported that a high mental workload, as well as fatigue, affects event-related potential (ERP) -based BCI performance. The results of our previous work reported in Zhang et al. (2020) also indicated that stress-dependent effects are similar to the outcome of fatigue, leading to a potential conclusion that stress and fatigue both impact attention capacity and interact to affect BCI performance.

All in all, future experiments should be designed and managed carefully as the causes and consequences of the physiological states including mental workload, attention, stress, and fatigue can be easily confounded. It is worth mentioning that currently the values of the parameters in the indicator are derived from the 40-subject EEG data. The indicator software on portable devices can instantly reflect the physiological states of the user based on the existing dataset. In addition, with accurate labeling of corresponding physiological states, parameters in future indicator can benefit from the acquired EEG signals during potential experiments, as the collected data provide more neurophysiological information for parameter calibration. As such, the dataset would be expanded and bring about more generalized results.

With the increasing quality of commercial EEG devices and advancements in artifact removal techniques, using BCIs in parallel with physiological state assessment tools in daily living has become more realistic. The validation results suggest that the neurophysiological manifestations of state changes are consistent across a variety of cognitive processes, setting the stage for practical applications of mental state indicators in passive BCIs. The proposed multi-parameter indicator has since been applied to a variety of fields in real-world environments to strengthen the practicability of the system. In 2018, a study adopted the physiological state indicator to aid professional and amateur Go players in competing with smart machines (Lee et al., 2020; Figure 9A). The wearable BCI system benefits machine-human co-learning as it can be integrated with training methods or neurofeedback to monitor and regulate Go players’ physiological states, helping them to maintain their attention and overcome the competitive pressure experienced in the game. Later, the system was extended to the educational domain interfacing with an intelligent agent for robotic edutainment in 2019 (Lee et al., 2019; Figure 9B). The experimental results showed that through playing games, the system improved the interest and performance of students in mathematical and language learning. Currently, the proposed indicator is also being applied to a game-based assessment for cognitive function in Kaohsiung Medical Hospital (Figure 9C) and a training paradigm for military soldiers using a first person shooting scenario in National Chung Shan Institute of Science and Technology (Figure 9D). To sum up, by estimating physiological state changes precisely using artifact-free EEG signals, the indicator can advance applications in interdisciplinary fields.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Real-world applications of the proposed physiological states indicator. (A) A Go player using the system while playing Go with smart machines for machine-human co-learning. (B) Intelligent agent for robotic edutainment in mathematical and language learning. (C) Game-based assessment of cognitive functions. (D) Training of soldiers using first person shooting scenario.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study presenting a system that indicates easily accessible metrics regarding multiple physiological states via wireless EEG in real-time. While BCI has been demonstrating great potential for controlling external devices with our minds, the physiological state changes induced by excessive stimulus under experimental settings or real conditions not only impact task performance and BCI accuracy, but are also interconnected with physical and mental health. Therefore, the concept of a passive BCI indicator is proposed to address such conditions by identifying the users’ spontaneous brain activities in real time. In this study, the parameters integrated into the state indicator include attention, fatigue, stress, and left and right brain activities, which were derived according to the cognitive state changes gathered from EEG signal processing. Furthermore, a prolonged experiment based on target detection was developed to validate the proposed indicator. The experimental results demonstrated a feasible outcome that corresponded to different stages during the experiment. The proposed physiological state indicator is applicable to online BCI applications, simultaneously allowing the state of the user to be assessed, along with providing a means of continuously evaluating the associated control signal quality.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards of National Chaio Tung University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

L-WK initialized the research idea, designed the experimental paradigm, and advised the methods of EEG data analysis. L-WK, YC, and CH performed the data analysis of EEG signals and drafted the manuscript. CH and B-YT performed the EEG data collection and provided advice on the manuscript revisions. L-WK, YC, and B-YT developed the software of the physiological state indicator. All authors agreed on the final manuscript submission.

Funding

This work was supported by the Higher Education Sprout Project of the National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University and Ministry of Education (MOE), Taiwan, under Contracts 110W153, and in part by the Center for Intelligent Drug Systems and Smart Bio-Devices (IDS2B) from the Featured Areas Research Center Program within the framework of the Higher Education Sprout Project by MOE in Taiwan, and in part by the Artificial Intelligence to Precision Health: Integrating Dynamic Physiological Signals and EMR to build a Medical Digital Twins Platform sponsored by the Taiwan Ministry of Science and Technology under Grants MOST 110-2321-B-010-005-, MOST 110-2321-B-A49-003-, MOST 110-2622-E-A49-009-, and MOST 108-2221-E-009-045-MY3.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Cory Stevenson for editing assistance, and the Information and Communications Research Division, National Chung-Shan Institute of Science and Technology for supporting the research.

References

Arpaia, P., Moccaldi, N., Prevete, R., Sannino, I., and Tedesco, A. (2020). A wearable EEG instrument for real-time frontal asymmetry monitoring in worker stress analysis. IEEE Trans. Instrument. Measure. 69, 8335–8343.

Google Scholar

Asif, A., Majid, M., and Anwar, S. M. (2019). Human stress classification using EEG signals in response to music tracks. Comput. Biol. Med. 107, 182–196. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2019.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajaj, V., Taran, S., Khare, S. K., and Sengur, A. (2020). Feature extraction method for classification of alertness and drowsiness states EEG signals. Appl. Acoustics 163:107224. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5334491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brás, S., Ferreira, J. H., Soares, S. C., and Pinho, A. J. (2018). Biometric and emotion identification: an ECG compression based method. J. Conf. Endorsement 9:467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chajut, E., and Algom, D. J. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: implications for theories of social cognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85:231. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. Y., Hsu, S. H., Pion-Tonachini, L., and Jung, T. P. (2018). “Evaluation of artifact subspace reconstruction for automatic EEG artifact removal,” in Proceeding of the 2018 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 1242–1245.

Google Scholar

Chang, C.-Y., Hsu, S.-H., Pion-Tonachini, L., and Jung, T.-P. (2019). Evaluation of artifact subspace reconstruction for automatic artifact components removal in multi-channel EEG recordings. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 67, 1114–1121. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2019.2930186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuang, C.-H., Ko, L.-W., Lin, Y.-P., Jung, T.-P., and Lin, C.-T. (2013). Independent component ensemble of EEG for brain–computer interface. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 22, 230–238. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2013.2293139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, M. M., Warren, B. A., Hagen, P. T., Johnson, B. D., Jenkins, S. M., Werneburg, B. L., et al. (2011). Stress level, health behaviors, and quality of life in employees joining a wellness center. Am. J. Health Promotion 26, 21–25. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.090821-QUAN-272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doesburg, S. M., Bedo, N., and Ward, L. M. (2016). Top-down alpha oscillatory network interactions during visuospatial attention orienting. IEEE Trans. Biomed. 132, 512–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, L. G., Maurits, N. M., and Lorist, M. M. (2012). Mental fatigue affects visual selective attention. PLoS One 7:e48073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, A., Helawar, R., Lokesh, R., Tari, T., and Shahapurkar, A. V. (2014). “Determination of stress using blood pressure and galvanic skin response,” in Proceeding of the 2014 International Conference on Communication and Network Technologies, (IEEE), 165–168.

Google Scholar

Grassmann, M., Vlemincx, E., von Leupoldt, A., Mittelstädt, J. M., and Van den Bergh, O. (2016). Respiratory changes in response to cognitive load: a systematic review. Neural Plasticity 2016, 8146809. doi: 10.1155/2016/8146809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., and Gable, P. A. (2018). On the role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in approach and withdrawal motivation: an updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 55:e12879. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, C., Chikara, R. K., Yeh, C.-L., and Ko, L.-W. (2021). Neural dynamics of target detection via wireless EEG in embodied cognition. Sensors 21:5213. doi: 10.3390/s21155213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, Y. L., Wang, J. S., Chiang, W. C., and Hung, C. H. (2017). Automatic ECG-based emotion recognition in music listening. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 11, 85–99.

Google Scholar

Huang, K.-C., Huang, T.-Y., Chuang, C.-H., King, J.-T., Wang, Y.-K., Lin, C.-T., et al. (2016). An EEG-based fatigue detection and mitigation system. Int. J. Neural Syst. 26:1650018. doi: 10.1142/S0129065716500180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, S., Li, J., Zhang, P., and Zhang, W. J. I. (2018). Detection of mental fatigue state with wearable ECG devices. Int. J. Med. Inform. 119, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2018.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jason, L. A., Evans, M., Brown, M., and Porter, N. (2010). What is fatigue? pathological and nonpathological fatigue. PM R. 2, 327–331. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jebelli, H., Khalili, M. M., and Lee, S. (2019). “Mobile EEG-based workers’ stress recognition by applying deep neural network,” in Proceedings of the 35th CIB W78 2018 Conference: IT in Design, Construction, and Management, (Springer), 173–180.

Google Scholar

Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Stensmo, M., and Sejnowski, T. (1997). Estimating alertness from the EEG power spectrum. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 44, 60–69. doi: 10.1109/10.553713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karrer, K., Vöhringer-Kuhnt, T., Baumgarten, T., and Briest, S. (2004). “The role of individual differences in driver fatigue prediction,” in Proceeding of the Third International Conference on Traffic and Transport Psychology, (Nottingham: Citeseer), 5–9.

Google Scholar

Käthner, I., Wriessnegger, S. C., Müller-Putz, G. R., Kübler, A., and Halder, S. (2014). Effects of mental workload and fatigue on the P300, alpha and theta band power during operation of an ERP (P300) brain–computer interface. Biol. Psychol. 102, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kehri, V., Ingle, R., Patil, S., and Awale, R. (2019). “Analysis of facial emg signal for emotion recognition using wavelet packet transform and svm,” in Machine Intelligence and Signal Analysis, eds M. Tanveer and R. Pachori (Springer), 247–257.

Google Scholar

Khessiba, S., Blaiech, A. G., Ben Khalifa, K., Ben Abdallah, A., and Bedoui, M. H. (2021). Innovative deep learning models for EEG-based vigilance detection. Neural Comput. Appl. 33, 6921–6937.

Google Scholar

Ko, L.-W., Chang, Y., Wu, P.-L., Tzou, H.-A., Chen, S.-F., Tang, S.-C., et al. (2019). Development of a smart helmet for strategical BCI applications. Sensors (Basel) 19:1867. doi: 10.3390/s19081867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ko, L.-W., Lai, W.-K., Liang, W.-G., Chuang, C.-H., Lu, S.-W., Lu, Y.-C., et al. (2015). “Single channel wireless EEG device for real-time fatigue level detection,” in Proceeding of the 2015 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN), (IEEE), 1–5.

Google Scholar

Krol, L. R., Freytag, S.-C., Fleck, M., Gramann, K., and Zander, T. O. (2016). “A task-independent workload classifier for neuroadaptive technology: preliminary data,” in Proceeding of the 2016 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), (IEEE), 003171–003174.

Google Scholar

Krupinski, E. A., MacKinnon, L., and Reiner, B. I. (2015). “Feasibility of using a biowatch to monitor GSR as a measure of radiologists’ stress and fatigue,” in Proceeding of the Medical Imaging 2015: Image Perception, Observer Performance, and Technology Assessment: International Society for Optics and Photonics, 941613.

Google Scholar

Kurniawan, H., Maslov, A. V., and Pechenizkiy, M. (2016). “Stress detection from speech and galvanic skin response signals,” in Proceedings of the 26th IEEE International Symposium on Computer-Based Medical Systems, (IEEE), 209–214.

Google Scholar

Lampert, R. (2015). ECG signatures of psychological stress. J. Electrocardiol. 48, 1000–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.jelectrocard.2015.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (2006). Stress and Emotion: A New Synthesis. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Lee, C.-S., Wang, M.-H., Ko, L.-W., Lee, Y.-H., Ohashi, H., Kubota, N., et al. (2020). Human intelligence meets smart machine: a special event at the IEEE international conference on systems, man, and cybernetics 2018. IEEE Syst. Man Cybernetics Magazine 6, 23–31.

Google Scholar

Lee, C.-S., Wang, M.-H., Tsai, Y.-L., Ko, L.-W., Tsai, B.-Y., Hung, P.-H., et al. (2019). Intelligent agent for real-world applications on robotic edutainment and humanized co-learning. J. Ambient Intelli. Hum. Comput. 11, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Lee, Y.-C., Lin, W.-C., Cherng, F.-Y., and Ko, L.-W. (2015). A visual attention monitor based on steady-state visual evoked potential. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 24, 399–408. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2015.2501378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C.-T., Lin, F.-C., Chen, S.-A., Lu, S.-W., Chen, T.-C., and Ko, L.-W. (2010). EEG-based brain-computer interface for smart living environment auto-adjustment. J. Med. Biol. Eng. 30, 237–245. doi: 10.5405/jmbe.30.4.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C.-T., Wu, R.-C., Jung, T.-P., Liang, S.-F., and Huang, T.-Y. (2005). Estimating driving performance based on EEG spectrum analysis. J. Adv. Signal Proc. 2005, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Liu, M., Fan, D., Zhang, X., and Gong, X. (2016). “Human emotion recognition based on galvanic skin response signal feature selection and svm,” in Proceeding of the 2016 International Conference on Smart City and Systems Engineering (ICSCSE), (IEEE), 157–160.

Google Scholar

Lock, A., Bonetti, D., and Campbell, A. (2018). The psychological and physiological health effects of fatigue. Occup. Med. 68, 502–511. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqy109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luijcks, R., Hermens, H. J., Bodar, L., Vossen, C. J., Os, J., and Lousberg, R. (2014). Experimentally induced stress validated by EMG activity. PLoS One 9:e95215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Y., Chen, B., Li, R., Wang, C., Wang, J., She, Q., et al. (2019). Driving fatigue detection from EEG using a modified PCANet method. Comput. Intelli. Neurosci. 2019, 1–19. doi: 10.1155/2019/4721863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S., and Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, 205–216.

Google Scholar

Meyer, T., Quaedflieg, C. W., Giesbrecht, T., Meijer, E. H., Abiad, S., and Smeets, T. (2014). Frontal EEG asymmetry as predictor of physiological responses to aversive memories. Psychophysiology 51, 853–865. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, S., and medicine, E. (2002). Causes and management of stress at work. Occup. Environ. Med. 59, 67–72. doi: 10.1136/oem.59.1.67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millán, J. D. R., Rupp, R., Mueller-Putz, G., Murray-Smith, R., Giugliemma, C., Tangermann, M., et al. (2010). Combining brain–computer interfaces and assistive technologies: state-of-the-art and challenges. Front. Neurosci. 4:161. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monteiro, T. G., Skourup, C., and Zhang, H. (2019). Using EEG for mental fatigue assessment: a comprehensive look into the current state of the art. IEEE Trans. Hum. Mach. Syst. 49, 599–610. doi: 10.1109/THMS.2019.2938156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myrden, A., and Chau, T. (2015). Effects of user mental state on EEG-BCI performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:308. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuper, C., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2009). “Neurofeedback training for BCI control,” in Brain-Computer Interfaces. The Frontiers Collection, eds B. Graimann, G. Pfurtscheller, and B. Allison (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 65–78.

Google Scholar

Parent, M., Peysakhovich, V., Mandrick, K., Tremblay, S., and Causse, M. (2019). The diagnosticity of psychophysiological signatures: can we disentangle mental workload from acute stress with ECG and fNIRS? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 146, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearlin, L. I., Schieman, S., Fazio, E. M., Meersman, S. C., and Behavior, S. (2005). Stress, health, and the life course: some conceptual perspectives. J. Health Soc. Behav. 46, 205–219. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G. J. E., and Neurophysiology, C. (1997). EEG event-related desynchronization (ERD) and synchronization (ERS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 1:26.

Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Neuper, C. (1994). Event-related synchronization of mu rhythm in the EEG over the cortical hand area in man. Neurosci. Lett. 174, 93–96. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90127-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Brunner, C., Schlögl, A., and Da Silva, F. L. (2006). Mu rhythm (de) synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. Neuroimage 31, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Stancak, A. Jr., and Neuper, C. (1996). Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band—an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00066-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pope, A. T., Bogart, E. H., and Bartolome, D. S. (1995). Biocybernetic system evaluates indices of operator engagement in automated task. Biol. Psychol. 40, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05116-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42.

Google Scholar

Quaedflieg, C., Meyer, T., Smulders, F., and Smeets, T. (2015). The functional role of individual-alpha based frontal asymmetry in stress responding. Biol. Psychol. 104, 75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quilodran, R., Rothe, M., and Procyk, E. (2008). Behavioral shifts and action valuation in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuron 57, 314–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramoser, H., Muller-Gerking, J., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2000). Optimal spatial filtering of single trial EEG during imagined hand movement. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng. 8, 441–446. doi: 10.1109/86.895946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, A. (2004). Anatomy of attentional networks. Anat. Rec. B. New Anat. 281, 21–36. doi: 10.1002/ar.b.20035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznik, S. J., and Allen, J. (2018). Frontal asymmetry as a mediator and moderator of emotion: an updated review. Psychophysiology 55:e12965. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riccio, A., Simione, L., Schettini, F., Pizzimenti, A., Inghilleri, M., Olivetti Belardinelli, M., et al. (2013). Attention and P300-based BCI performance in people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:732. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roets, B., and Christiaens, J. (2019). Shift work, fatigue, and human error: an empirical analysis of railway traffic control. J. Trans. Safety Security 11, 207–224.

Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2015). Stress and the brain: individual variability and the inverted-U. Nat. Neurosci. 18:1344. doi: 10.1038/nn.4109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarter, M., Givens, B., and Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 146–160. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00044-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlatter, S., Guillot, A., Faes, C., Saruco, E., Collet, C., Di Rienzo, F., et al. (2020). Acute stress affects implicit but not explicit motor imagery: a pilot study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 152, 62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenoy, P., Krauledat, M., Blankertz, B., Rao, R. P., and Müller, K.-R. (2006). Towards adaptive classification for BCI. J. Neural. Eng. 3:R13. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/3/1/R02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szafir, D., and Mutlu, B. (2012). “Pay attention! designing adaptive agents that monitor and improve user engagement,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 11–20.

Google Scholar

Talukdar, U., Hazarika, S. M., and Gan, J. Q. (2019). Motor imagery and mental fatigue: inter-relationship and EEG based estimation. J. Comput. Neurosci. 46, 55–76. doi: 10.1007/s10827-018-0701-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatum, W. O. (2014). Ellen r. grass lecture: extraordinary eeg. Neurodiagn J. 54, 3–21.

Google Scholar

Thatcher, R. W. (1998). Normative EEG databases and EEG biofeedback. Databases Neurother. 2, 8–39.

Google Scholar

Trejo, L. J., Kubitz, K., Rosipal, R., Kochavi, R. L., and Montgomery, L. D. (2015). EEG-based estimation and classification of mental fatigue. Psychology 6:572.

Google Scholar

Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Bailey, S., Meeusen, R., and Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 47, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villarejo, M. V., Zapirain, B. G., and Zorrilla, A. M. (2012). A stress sensor based on galvanic skin response (GSR) controlled by ZigBee. Sensors 12, 6075–6101. doi: 10.3390/s120506075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Virtanen, M., Honkonen, T., Kivimäki, M., Ahola, K., Vahtera, J., Aromaa, A., et al. (2007). Work stress, mental health and antidepressant medication findings from the health 2000 study. J. Affect. Dis. 98, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.05.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widjaja, D., Orini, M., Vlemincx, E., and Van Huffel, S. (2013). Cardiorespiratory dynamic response to mental stress: a multivariate time-frequency analysis. Comput. Math Methods Med. 2013:451857. doi: 10.1155/2013/451857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wijsman, J., Grundlehner, B., Penders, J., and Hermens, H. (2013). Trapezius muscle EMG as predictor of mental stress. ACM Trans. Embed. Comput. Syst. 12, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Xia, L., Malik, A. S., and Subhani, A. R. (2018). A physiological signal-based method for early mental-stress detection. Biomed. Signal Proc. Control 46, 18–32.

Google Scholar

Xu, X., Gu, H., Yan, S., Pang, G., and Gui, G. (2019). Fatigue EEG feature extraction based on tasks with different physiological states for ubiquitous edge computing. IEEE Access 7, 73057–73064.

Google Scholar

Xuan, B., Mackie, M.-A., Spagna, A., Wu, T., Tian, Y., Hof, P. R., et al. (2016). The activation of interactive attentional networks. Neuroimage 129, 308–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zander, T. O., and Kothe, C. (2011). Towards passive brain–computer interfaces: applying brain–computer interface technology to human–machine systems in general. J. Neural Eng. 8:025005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/2/025005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, H., Yang, C., Dai, G., Qin, F., Zhang, J., and Kong, W. (2018). EEG classification of driver mental states by deep learning. Cogn. Neurodyn. 12, 597–606.

Google Scholar

Zhang, H. Y., Stevenson, C. E., Jung, T. P., and Ko, L. W. (2020). Stress-induced effects in resting EEG spectra predict the performance of SSVEP-based BCI. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 28, 1771–1780. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.3005771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, T., Liu, T., Li, F., Li, M., Liu, D., Zhang, R., et al. (2016). Structural and functional correlates of motor imagery BCI performance: insights from the patterns of fronto-parietal attention network. Neuroimage 134, 475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Bachmann, P., Schilling, T. M., Naumann, E., Schächinger, H., and Larra, M. F. (2018a). Emotional stress regulation: the role of relative frontal alpha asymmetry in shaping the stress response. Biol. Psychol. 138, 231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Krol, L. R., and Zander, T. O. (2018b). “Towards task-independent workload classification: shifting from binary to continuous classification,” in Proceeding of the 2018 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), (IEEE), 556–561.

Google Scholar

Keywords: BCI, EEG, physiological state, attention, fatigue, stress

Citation: Chang Y, He C, Tsai B-Y and Ko L-W (2021) Multi-Parameter Physiological State Monitoring in Target Detection Under Real-World Settings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:785562. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.785562

Received: 29 September 2021; Accepted: 03 December 2021;
Published: 22 December 2021.

Edited by:

Gianluca Di Flumeri, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marius Klug, Technical University of Berlin, Germany
Pietro Aricò, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Chang, He, Tsai and Ko. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Li-Wei Ko, lwko@nctu.edu.tw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.