Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 28 October 2021
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Rehabilitation for Motor Symptoms in Neurodegenerative Disease View all 8 articles

Functional Connectivity and Networks Underlying Complex Tool-Use Movement in Assembly Workers: An fMRI Study

\r\nSeira Taniguchi*Seira Taniguchi1*Yuichiro HigashiYuichiro Higashi2Hirotaka KataokaHirotaka Kataoka2Hiroshi NakajimaHiroshi Nakajima2Tetsuya ShimokawaTetsuya Shimokawa1
  • 1Center for Information and Neural Networks, Advanced ICT Research Institute, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Suita, Japan
  • 2Omron Corporation, Kizugawa, Japan

The aim of this study was to identify the functional connectivity and networks utilized during tool-use in real assembly workers. These brain networks have not been elucidated because the use of tools in real-life settings is more complex than that in experimental environments. We evaluated task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging in 13 assembly workers (trained workers, TW) and 27 age-matched volunteers (untrained workers, UTW) during a tool-use pantomiming task, and resting-state functional connectivity was also analyzed. Two-way repeated-measures analysis of covariance was conducted with the group as a between-subject factor (TW > UTW) and condition (task > resting) as a repeated measure, controlling for assembly time and accuracy as covariates. We identified two patterns of functional connectivity in the whole brain within three networks that distinguished TW from UTW. TW had higher connectivity than UTW between the left middle temporal gyrus and right cerebellum Crus II (false discovery rate corrected p-value, p-FDR = 0.002) as well as between the left supplementary motor area and the pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus (p-FDR = 0.010). These network integrities may allow for TW to perform rapid tool-use. In contrast, UTW showed a stronger integrity compared to TW between the left paracentral lobule and right angular gyrus (p-FDR = 0.004), which may reflect a greater reliance on sensorimotor input to acquire complex tool-use ability than that of TW. Additionally, the fronto-parietal network was identified as a common network between groups. These findings support our hypothesis that assembly workers have stronger connectivity in tool-specific motor regions and the cerebellum, whereas UTW have greater involvement of sensorimotor networks during a tool-use task.

Introduction

The unique ability to handle tools with high speed and accuracy for performing daily tasks and work can distinguish our species from non-human primates. Greater skills with manipulating tools and conceptual knowledge facilitate tool-use performance in humans (Liepmann, 2001; Koski et al., 2002). For instance, humans have more pronounced skills than chimpanzees in these cognitive domains of tool-use: fine motor control in hand–eye coordination, inferential causal reasoning, function representation, inhibition and foresight in executive control, teaching, time estimation in contingent reciprocity, and language (Vaesen, 2012).

Neuropsychological studies have revealed tool-specific regions in the brain, including the superior parietal lobule (SPL), ventral premotor cortex (PMv), and middle temporal gyrus (MTG), especially in the left hemisphere. Indeed, left hemisphere lesions involving the inferior parietal lobule cause “apraxia,” which is a clinical syndrome underscored by difficulties in performing learned and skilled gestures (Gross and Grossman, 2008). Additionally, “ideational apraxia” is a subtype of apraxia that refers to the inability to grasp and manipulate tools accurately on the basis of their perceptual properties (Buxbaum et al., 2003). The cerebellum is important for motor learning and control. Imamizu et al. (2000) identified that cerebellar activation occurs not only in the early phase of tool-use learning but also after the learning is complete. Yoo et al. (2013) have proposed that the fronto-parietal network is a core network underpinning skilled tool-use during the resting state. They reported that sensorimotor networks exhibited decreased connectivity after handling chopsticks (Yoo et al., 2013). However, they only investigated functional connectivity and networks in the resting state and did not assess task-related states during tool-use. Although brain activation studies have provided insight into the neural bases of human tool-use, the neural networks involved in the execution of professional tool-use in assembly workers have not been elucidated.

Moreover, neuroimaging studies have indicated an overlap in regions underscoring actual tool-use and pantomiming (Choi et al., 2001; Fridman et al., 2006; Buxbaum et al., 2014; Hoeren et al., 2014; Lausberg et al., 2015), mental imagery (Grèzes et al., 2003; Creem-Regehr and Lee, 2005), and action observation (Beauchamp et al., 2002; Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Macdonald and Culham, 2015). Many neuroimaging paradigms have employed tasks involving pantomiming tool-use because it is thought to approximate the process of planning to use actual tools (Lewis, 2006). A meta-analysis revealed that the SPL, dorsolateral lobule, PMv, inferior parietal lobule, and MTG are selectively activated when pantomiming tool-use (Lewis, 2006). Nevertheless, most neuroimaging studies involving pantomiming employ relatively simple tasks, such as tool-use demonstrations with either the right or left hand (Choi et al., 2001; Fridman et al., 2006; Buxbaum et al., 2014; Hoeren et al., 2014; Lausberg et al., 2015). In contrast, the use of tools in real-life settings is more complex than that in experimental environments. For example, using a hammer and nails, scissors and papers, or screwdrivers and screws requires the use of both hands. Furthermore, performance may vary depending on the physical environment (Jovanovic et al., 2007), including the height of the platform and chair and the size or weight of tools, which may impact tool-use efficiency. Nevertheless, the brain networks underscoring complex tool-use performance in real-life environments have not been elucidated. The aim of the current study was therefore to investigate the characteristics of real assembly workers in a tool-use pantomiming task. We examined the functional connectivity and neural networks underlying tool-use movement in professional assembly workers, who perform complex tool-use in their daily work. We hypothesized that real assembly workers have stronger connectivity in the tool-specific regions and cerebellum than untrained workers (UTW) and that UTW have greater involvement of sensorimotor networks than trained workers (TW).

Materials and Methods

Ethical Approval

This study was approved by the Ethics Committees of National Institute of Information and Communications Technology (No. 1809070050) and was conducted in conformance with the Declaration of Helsinki. We obtained written informed consent from each participant after explaining the purpose, expected benefits, and potential harm of this research.

Participants

We recruited 13 workers (mean age, 35.8 ± 10.5 years; range, 21–52 years old; 11 women and 2 men) from an assembly plant for factory automation parts owned by Omron Co. (henceforth referred to as TW), and 27 age-matched volunteers who had no experience with assembly tasks (mean age, 37.9 ± 10.0 years; range, 21–54 years old; 13 women and 14 men) via advertisements and consultation referrals (henceforth referred to as UTW).

The inclusion criteria were: (1) being able to precisely follow the process of the actual assembly task, including sequence errors and (2) having a normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and (3) having no contraindications for MRI.

Behavioral Assessment

The protocol summary and the assembly task process are presented in Figures 1A,B, respectively. Prior to fMRI scanning, participants underwent two behavioral sessions consisting of: (1) actual assembly task, and (2) manual hand dexterity test.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Protocol summary. The arrow indicates the order of the tasks, from outside the MR scanner to inside the MR scanner. There are two phases in the protocol: (1) two tests were conducted outside the MR scanner. First, the participants performed the manual hand dexterity test using the Purdue Pegboard Test, and then they underwent the actual assembly task. (2) Imaging using the MR scanner. Second, the participants underwent resting-state fMRI for 5 min. Thereafter, an fMRI scan was executed as the participants were instructed to pantomime the assembly task while viewing their own performance. (B) Assembly task process. The arrow indicates the workflow of the assembly task. The participants were instructed to pick up a screw from the basket on the left with their left hand and attach it to the tip of the device, then insert the point of the screw into the hole of the square base. Finally, the thumb of the right hand was used to push the button on the handle of the device for tightening the screw. This process was repeated from the top hole of the base to the sixth hole.

Actual Assembly Task

Participants were required to complete a screwing task as precisely as possible. For the display of the pantomiming task in the MR (magnetic resonance) scanner, the performance of each participant’s assembly task was recorded using a wearable camera (Tobii Technology, Danderyd, Sweden, Tobii Pro Glasses 2) with a video resolution of 1920 × 1080 at 25 fps. Performance time was measured with a stopwatch. Each participant practiced tightening screws in order to confirm each step of the task prior to scanning.

The index of “accuracy” of the assembly performance was defined by the number of mistakes while tightening screws, ranging from 0 to 6 errors. Zero errors were indicative of good accuracy, while higher errors were indicative of poor accuracy. The two investigators (ST and YH) independently viewed videos of participants’ performances to check whether each screw was fully seated in the base. If the shank remained above the surface of the base, the investigator counted this as a mistake. If there was a disagreement in numbers of missed tightening between the investigators, the final decision was made after a discussion.

Manual Hand Dexterity Test

This test involved the Purdue Pegboard Test (SAKAI Medical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan, A929-1), which reflects quantitative hand dexterity function and speed ability, and finger–eye coordination (Backman et al., 1992). The test was originally designed to assess the manual dexterity of assembly line employees (Radomski and Trombly, 2008).

MRI Scanning

Participants viewed a display while lying supine in the MRI bore. During scanning, participants were instructed to pantomime the assembly task (a simple block design) as precisely as possible without physically interacting with tools. The participants were asked to continuously view the video sequences showing their own performance on the screen at a distance of around 130 mm, using a mirror attached to a head coil. Resting-state fMRI runs of 5-min were performed immediately prior to fMRI runs of tasks, during which all participants were instructed to avoid repetitive thoughts. Thin foam pillows were positioned around each participant’s head to minimize head motions.

MRI Acquisition and Pre-processing

MRI scans were collected using a 64-channel head coil and MAGNETOM Vida 3T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at the Center for Information and Neural Networks (Osaka, Japan). Functional data were obtained using a T2-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE) = 1000/30 ms, flip angle = 60°, 39 slices, matrix size = 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm × 4.0 mm, field-of-view (FoV) = 192 mm, slice thickness = 4.0 mm, and an interleaved mode of slice acquisition. For the T1-weighted data, EPI employed the following parameters: TR/TE = 3.15/1.37 ms, 300 volumes, 39 slices, flip angle = 8°, voxel size = 1.6 mm × 1.6 mm × 1.6 mm, FoV = 260 mm, slice thickness = 1.6 mm.

Functional and structural MRI data were pre-processed using CONN toolbox version 17.f1 implemented in MATLAB (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, United States). On pre-processing, the data were realigned, unwarped, slice-timing corrected, co-registered to each individual T1 anatomical image, spatially normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute space, outlier detected (ART-based scrubbing), and smoothed with 8-mm full-width-at-half maximum Gaussian kernel. Subsequently, a denoising pipeline was performed by implementing the aCompCor strategy (Behzadi et al., 2007), including linear regression (regression of noise variables derived from motion, cerebrospinal fluid, and white matter) and band-pass filtering (0.008–0.09 Hz) (Nieto-Castanon, 2020).

Participants were excluded if excessive head movement determined by the mean framewise displacement (FD) was more than 0.5 mm (Power et al., 2014), which revealed no between-group difference in mean FD between TW and UTW in the pantomiming task (TW = 0.170 ± 0.038 vs. UTW = 0.192 ± 0.055, p = 0.31) and resting-state (TW = 0.138 ± 0.042 vs. UTW = 0.158 ± 0.048, p = 0.28).

ROI-to-ROI Analysis

The commonly used approach to analyze task-related fMRI, such as psychophysiological interaction (PPI) and dynamic causal modeling (DCM); however, they usually implicitly assume that specific functional connectivity linked to a task exists. To the best of our knowledge, no report regarding important functional connectivity or regions or networks linked to complex assembly tool-use exists. Therefore, we applied region-of-interest (ROI)-to-ROI analysis to identify functional connectivity in TW, as a precursor to PPI or DCM analysis.

Recent studies used ROI-to-ROI analysis to identify specific functional connectivity in martial arts performers (Fujiwara et al., 2019) and in people with visual snow (Aldusary et al., 2020).

Region-of-interest-to-ROI connectivity matrices were computed for each subject and for each source ROI in the first-level analysis. A 264 × 264 matrix for each participant was defined as the Fisher transformed bivariate correlation coefficients for each pair of the 264 regions. ROIs were labeled based on the Power et al. (2011) atlas (Power et al., 2011) and were divided into eight networks: somatomotor hand network (SMN) with 30 ROIs, visual network with 31 ROIs, auditory network with 13 ROIs, default mode network (DMN) with 58 ROIs, fronto-parietal network (FPN) with 25 ROIs, cingulo-opercular network with 14 ROIs, dorsal attention network with 11 ROIs, and ventral attention network (VAN) with 9 ROIs.

The weighted general linear model used for the second-level analysis. Two-way repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted with the group as a between-subject factor (TW > UTW) and condition (task > resting) as a repeated measure, controlling for assembly time and accuracy as covariates. Post hoc two-sample t-tests were performed for ROIs with significant group × condition interactions. Significant connections were identified by calculating the false discovery rate (FDR) corrected with two-sided p-values < 0.01.

Statistical Analysis

Between-group differences in hand dexterity test scores and time of actual assembly task were assessed using the Mann–Whitney U test. The scores of accuracy in the assembly task and gender differences between groups were assessed using the Chi-squared test. The correlation between identified functional connectivity and other variables of assembly task performance was assessed using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient (Rs).

Results

Participant Characteristics and Behavioral Results

Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. TW had a larger proportion of females than that of UTW (p < 0.05) and exhibited significantly higher speed in the assembly task (p < 0.001). Similar group differences were observed in assembly task accuracy (p = 0.146) and manual hand dexterity test scores (p = 0.568). No participant exceeded the mean FD value of 0.5 mm.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics.

Specific Functional Connectivity for Assembly Tool-Use in Trained Workers and Untrained Workers

Results of the whole-brain analysis are presented in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Between-network connectivity in TW and UTW. The lines represent the functional connectivity with other ROIs (FDR-corrected, p < 0.01). Group differences: red lines represent higher connectivity in TW than in UTW (TW > UTW); blue lines represent higher connectivity in UTW than in TW (UTW > TW). Networks: colored dots indicate the four networks with significance at p < 0.01. Green, somatomotor hand network (SMN); yellow, ventral attention network (VAN); pink, default mode network (DMN); light blue, fronto-parietal network (FPN). ROIs: l-aMTG, left anterior middle temporal gyrus; r-Crus II, right cerebellum crus II; l-SMA, left supplementary motor area; r-IFGtri, pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus; l-PCL, left paracentral lobule; r-AG, right angular gyrus.

For better identification of connectivity during the assembly task, the thresholds for ROI-to-ROI connectivity in the whole brain analysis were increased to p-FDR < 0.01.

At the ROI level for group comparisons of functional connectivity, as compared with UTW, TW had increased connectivity between the left MTG and right cerebellum Crus II [T(36) = 5.24, p-FDR = 0.002], which were components of the DMN, as well as between the left supplementary motor area (SMA) and the pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFGtri) of Broca’s area [T(36) = 4.71, p-FDR = 0.010], which are components of the VAN and FPN, respectively. In contrast, UTW showed a stronger integrity compared to TW between the left paracentral lobule (PCL) and right angular gyrus (AG) [T(36) = −5.03, p-FDR = 0.004], which are components of the SMN and FPN, respectively.

Relationship Between Identified Functional Connectivity and Assembly Task Performance

The relationship between identified functional connectivity and the scores of assembly task performance, and the relationship between assembly time and identified functional connectivity is illustrated in a scatterplot.

Assembly time was inversely correlated with functional connectivity between the MTG and Crus II in all the participants, indicating a higher speed in the assembly task related to stronger connectivity between the MTG and Crus II. The UTW’ slower assembly time correlated with a stronger TW-specific connectivity, while a faster assembly time was only related to a stronger UTW-specific connectivity. Further, there was no significant relationship between the scores of accuracy and functional connectivity.

Discussion

Behavioral Results

Our study is the first to demonstrate the functional connectivity and associated networks among 264 nodes underscoring skillful tool-use performance in assembly workers in a real tool-use environment by investigating the differences between TW and UTW. TW exhibited greater speed in the assembly task, although no group differences were observed in assembly task accuracy or manual hand dexterity. These findings suggest that complex tool-use ability and hand dexterity are distinct in relation to their neural substrates.

As for the gender differences in this study, a recent systematic review by Hirsch et al. (2019) summarized that there were no gender differences in both sequential and concurrent multitasking conditions. This may support the gender imbalance of this study group that might not have greatly influenced our results in the assembly task.

Functional Connectivity and Networks

Two patterns of functional connectivity between anterior MTG and Crus II, and between SMA and IFGtri were TW-specific, while that between PCL and AG was UTW-specific. Moreover, our results from the correlation analysis would further support these characteristic findings.

At the network level, the DMN and the VAN were TW-specific, while the SMN was UTW-specific. Additionally, the FPN was identified as a common network between groups.

Trained Workers Specific Functional Connectivity Between Anterior Middle Temporal Gyrus and Crus II

The posterior MTG has consistently been reported to be involved in tool-use (Lewis, 2006), whereas, in this study, the MTG was located anteriorly (x: −53, y: 3, z: −27), which indicated that the anterior activity was more strongly associated with frequently used tools than with rarely used tools (Vingerhoets, 2008) and was a key region for the retrieval of stored semantic information (Xu et al., 2015). Goodale et al. (1994) suggested the pantomimed task was elicited by stored perceptual information even if the object was visually present, while actual grasping relies on the visuo-motor online control system. Collectively, these findings indicate that the anterior MTG is involved in retrieving conceptual knowledge about familiar tools in TW.

Interestingly, the cerebellar Crus II was identified as a previously unreported region associated with tool-use. Crus II has been often referred to as the “cognitive cerebellum” (Buckner et al., 2011). For example, retrieval of episodic memories (Huo et al., 2018) and verbal working memory (Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). Similarly, the DMN is involved in working memory (Piccoli et al., 2015; Sormaz et al., 2018). However, retroviral tracing studies have revealed that the Crus II is also related to motor functions (Kelly and Strick, 2003; Greger et al., 2004). Another study showed that the Crus II contributes to the accurate temporal prediction of absolute timing in voluntary movement tasks (Yamaguchi and Sakurai, 2016) and short-term prediction through internal modeling (Lesage et al., 2017). Based on the cerebellar internal model theories, a feedforward control in the motor control system acts for relatively fast movements, while a feedback control is crucial for unskilled movement (Ghez et al., 1990). Thus, the rapid performance of TW during the pantomiming task may reflect a feedforward motor control by the cerebellum internal model.

Additionally, the faster assembly time was related to stronger connectivity between the anterior MTG and Crus II during the pantomiming task. This result suggests that rapid performance indicates a stronger neural network integrity between the anterior MTG and Crus II.

In summary, these results would suggest that familiarization of the tool-use related performance and a feedforward motor control are the key components for rapid tool-use in TW, underscored by the stronger network integrity between anterior MTG and Crus II.

Trained Workers Specific Functional Connectivity Between Supplementary Motor Area and Pars Triangularis of Inferior Frontal Gyrus

Although most SMA neuroimaging studies focus on motor planning and execution and IFGtri (Broca’s area) focus on linguistic function, the recruitment of SMA (Choi et al., 2001; Vingerhoets et al., 2011) and IFGtri (Higuchi et al., 2009) during tool-use related performance has been reported.

Importantly, both SMA and IFG are parts of mirror neuron system and such mirror neuron system has been demonstrated to be related to motor imagery, action recognition, and on-line imitation (Buccino and Riggio, 2006). This is widely known as functionally equivalent hypothesis (Jeannerod, 1994, 2001) since they have many similarities at a neural basis. In addition, considering the nature of the pantomiming task of this study that is accompanied by actual movements, recently proposed “dynamic motor imagery” (Schuster et al., 2011; Fusco et al., 2016, 2019) seems to be more related to the pantomiming task than conventional static motor imagery.

Furthermore, an fMRI study showed that IFG was activated in only the condition of finger motion imitation while that of hand motion did not, because there is a greater reliance on visual rather than sensory feedback in a finger imitation (Tanaka and Inui, 2002).

Moreover, at the network level, a study showed that VAN is involved in reorientation of attention as well as top-down and attentional capture processes (Solis-Vivanco et al., 2021). Thus, the recruitment of VAN may reflect cognitive loads in attention processing during the pantomiming task that involved sequential and repetitive tool-use movement.

Collectively, these findings suggested the superior ability of mental rehearsing by dynamic motor imagery is related to executing motor programs of the tool-use performance in a top-down manner, therefore, this connectivity was the TW’ specific during the assembly task. As for vocational rehabilitation, dynamic motor imagery training especially for fingers may be effective in the later stages of motor learning.

Untrained Workers Specific Functional Connectivity Between Paracentral Lobule and Angular Gyrus

The PCL is considered to be a combined motor and sensory region, primarily associated with the leg motor region (Baker et al., 2018). However, it remains unclear how the PCL is involved in manual movements in tool-use since the PCL has not been studied extensively in the context of tool-use compared to other regions. Further research is warranted to elucidate how this region is involved in assembly work. In addition, the AG is vital for perceptual sequence learning (Rosenthal et al., 2009) and finger recognition (Perry, 2014). Further, the AG is considered a cross-modal hub that merges multisensory information (Seghier, 2013). Indeed, Grèzes and Decety (2002) proposed that sensorimotor experience may be important for processing information to acquire access to semantic knowledge for certain classes of objects.

At the network level, the SMN was identified as being UTW-specific. Unsurprisingly, the somatosensory system contributes more strongly compared to the motor system in the early stages of motor learning (Bernardi et al., 2015).

Supporting these findings, the observed connectivity and network suggest that UTW may rely more heavily on sensory information to understand tool functions in order to acquire complex tool-use ability, similar to the development of tool-use skills in young children. As such, sensory discrimination training for hands may be effective for vocational rehabilitation in the early stage of learning.

Commonality Between Trained Workers and Untrained Workers: Fronto-Parietal Network

The FPN was a significant network for both TW and UTW groups. Neuroimaging studies have indicated that the FPN is involved in the processing of tools. FPN activation has been reported for both prospective and retrospective cues (Wallis et al., 2015). A positron emission tomography study revealed that the IFGtri and inferior parietal lobule were activated during the perception of tools, regardless of the tasks (Grèzes and Decety, 2002). This seems to contradict a specific role for these regions in tool-use; however, a recent review by Marek and Dosenbach (2018) suggested that “the frontoparietal network is a functional hub for influencing brain-wide communication to meet task demands.” This may explain involvement of the FPN during the assembly work in both TW and UTW.

Limitations and Future Research

Several limitations of the current study should be acknowledged. First, although the sample size of the present study involving healthy subjects was greater than that of previous studies on tool-use (Choi et al., 2001; Beauchamp et al., 2002; Grèzes et al., 2003; Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Creem-Regehr and Lee, 2005; Fridman et al., 2006; Vingerhoets et al., 2011; Yoo et al., 2013; Lausberg et al., 2015; Macdonald and Culham, 2015), more participants are necessary to draw more definitive conclusions. Second, data on factors influencing tool-use performance in participants, such as years of experience in assembly work, education, hobbies, and lifestyle, were not collected due to the company’s privacy policy. Third, our study adopted a cross-sectional design of two groups. A longitudinal study design may be more appropriate to investigate changes in tool-use learning over time. Furthermore, the supine position might have indeed influenced their pantomiming performance as they usually perform task in upright or sitting positions in real environments. However, we primarily focused on group differences between TW and UTW, having normalized by the same experimental setting. Moreover, although we focused on identifying the functional connectivity in TW by using ROI-to-ROI analysis, as a precursor to PPI or DCM analysis, further analysis should be performed to investigate the task-specific effect on the identified functional connectivity. Finally, from the scatterplot it appears that the TW had a relatively narrow range of distribution in assembly time compared to the UTW; therefore, it could not provide sufficient power for confirming the relationship with identified functional connectivity in TW. Larger samples within the TW group will enable further in-depth assessment of the relationship of functional connectivity with assembly time. Future studies should aim to address these limitations.

Conclusion

We investigated the characteristics of real assembly workers in a tool-use pantomiming task to identify the functional connectivity and networks underlying tool-use movement. We identified two patterns of functional connectivity in the whole brain that distinguished TW from UTW, and a common network between the groups. TW had two stronger functional connectivity, between MTG and Crus II and between SMA and IFGtri. These network integrities may allow for TW to perform rapid tool-use. Notably, the Crus II was identified as a previously unreported region associated with tool-use. In contrast, UTW exhibited a stronger integrity between PCL and AG, which may reflect a greater reliance on sensorimotor input to acquire complex tool-use ability than that of TW. At the network level, the fronto-parietal network was was identified as a common network between groups. Further longitudinal studies with larger sample sizes as well as investigation of task-specific effects on the identified functional connectivity are required to confirm our findings.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to ST, c2VpcmEudGFuaWd1Y2hpLnB0QGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Safety Committee of National Institute of Information and Communications Technology (NICT). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

HK, YH, and HN contributed to conception of the study. TS contributed design of the study. TS and ST organized the database. ST performed the statistical analysis and wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was funded by Omron Corporation (Kyoto, Japan).

Conflict of Interest

YH, HK, and HN were employed by the company Omron Corporation.

The authors declare that this study received funding from Omron Corporation (Kyoto, Japan). The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants and partners in Omron Corporation and Center for Information and Neural Network. We would also like to thank Takashi Ueguchi, Ph.D. for the invaluable advice on writing the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.707502/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Correlation between the assembly time and identified functional connectivity. Scatterplots show the relationship between assembly time (y-axes) and identified functional connectivity (x-axes) and indicate Fisher’s z-values of functional connectivity between MTG and Crus II (top figure), SMA and IFGtri (middle figure), and PCL and AG (bottom figure). The box in black line encloses TW-specific connectivity, while the box in gray line encloses UTW-specific connectivity. The blue dots indicate the values in UTW, whereas the orange squares represent the values in TW. Significant results are shown in bold font. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01. Rs, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient; Total, all participants; TW, trained workers; UTW, untrained workers; MTG, left middle temporal gyrus; Crus II, right cerebellum Crus II; SMA, left supplementary motor area; IFGtri, pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus; PCL, left paracentral lobule; AG, right angular gyrus.

Supplementary Table 1 | Spearman’s correlation coefficient between identified functional connectivity and the scores of assembly task performance.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.nitrc.org/projects/conn

References

Aldusary, N., Traber, G. L., Freund, P., Fierz, F. C., Weber, K. P., Baeshen, A., et al. (2020). Abnormal connectivity and brain structure in patients with visual snow. Hum. Neurosci. 14:476. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.582031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Backman, C., Gibson, S. C. D., and Parsons, J. (1992). Assessment of hand function: the relationship between pegboard dexterity and applied dexterity. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 59, 208–213. doi: 10.1177/000841749205900406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., Briggs, R. G., Sheets, J. R., Conner, A. K., Glenn, C. A., et al. (2018). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum-chapter 3: the motor, premotor, and sensory cortices. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S75–S121. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., and Martin, A. (2002). Parallel visual motion processing streams for manipulable objects and human movements. Neuron 34, 149–159. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00642-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behzadi, Y., Restom, K., Liau, J., and Liu, T. T. (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 37, 90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardi, N. F., Darainy, M., and Ostry, D. J. (2015). Somatosensory contribution to the initial stages of human motor learning. J. Neurosci. Res. 35, 14316–14326. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1344-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., and Riggio, L. (2006). The role of the mirror neuron system in motor learning. Kinesiology 38, 5–15.

Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Krienen, F. M., Castellanos, A., Diaz, J. C., and Yeo, B. T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buxbaum, L. J., Shapiro, A. D., and Coslett, H. B. (2014). Critical brain regions for tool-related and imitative actions: a componential analysis. Brain 137, 1971–1985. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buxbaum, L. J., Sirigu, A., Schwartz, M. F., and Klatzky, R. (2003). Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia 41, 1091–1113. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00314-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, S. H., Na, D. L., Kang, E., Lee, K. M., Lee, S. W., and Na, D. G. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging during pantomiming tool-use gestures. Exp. Brain Res. 139, 311–317. doi: 10.1007/s002210100777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creem-Regehr, S. H., and Lee, J. N. (2005). Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 457–469. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., and Zotow, E. (2015). Surface-based display of volume-averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLoS One 10:e0133402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridman, E. A., Immisch, I., Hanakawa, T., Bohlhalter, S., Waldvogel, D., Kansaku, K., et al. (2006). The role of the dorsal stream for gesture production. Neuroimage 29, 417–428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujiwara, H., Ueno, T., Yoshimura, S., Kobayashi, K., Miyagi, T., Oishi, N., et al. (2019). Martial arts “Kendo” and the motivation network during attention processing: an fMRI Study. Hum. Neurosci. 13:170. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusco, A., Gallotta, M. C., Iosa, M., Morone, G., Iasevoli, L., Trifoglio, D., et al. (2016). The dynamic motor imagery of locomotion is task-dependent in patients with stroke. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 34, 247–256. doi: 10.3233/RNN-150573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusco, A., Iasevoli, L., Iosa, M., Gallotta, M. C., Padua, L., Tucci, L., et al. (2019). Dynamic motor imagery mentally simulates uncommon real locomotion better than static motor imagery both in young adults and elderly. PLoS One 14:e0218378. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghez, C., Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., Christakos, C. N., and Cooper, S. E. (1990). Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 55, 837–847. doi: 10.1101/SQB.1990.055.01.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., and Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia 32, 1159–1178. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90100-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greger, B., Norris, S. A., and Thach, W. T. (2004). Spike firing in the lateral cerebellar cortex correlated with movement and motor parameters irrespective of the effector limb. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 576–582. doi: 10.1152/jn.00535.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., and Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 40, 212–222. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00089-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armony, J., Ellis, R., and Passingham, R. E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 2735–2740. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02695.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, R. G., and Grossman, M. (2008). Update on apraxia. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 8, 490–496. doi: 10.1007/s11910-008-0078-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higuchi, S., Chaminade, T., Imamizu, H., and Kawato, M. (2009). Shared neural correlates for language and tool use in Broca’s area. Neuroreport 20, 1376–1381. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283315570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, P., Koch, I., and Karbach, J. (2019). Putting a stereotype to the test: the case of gender differences in multitasking costs in task-switching and dual-task situations. PLoS One 14:e0220150. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeren, M., Kümmerer, D., Bormann, T., Beume, L., Ludwig, V. M., Vry, M. S., et al. (2014). Neural bases of imitation and pantomime in acute stroke patients: distinct streams for praxis. Brain 137, 2796–2810. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huo, L., Li, R., Wang, P., Zheng, Z., and Li, J. (2018). The default mode network supports episodic memory in cognitively unimpaired elderly individuals: different contributions to immediate recall and delayed recall. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:6. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imamizu, H., Miyauchi, S., Tamada, T., Sasaki, Y., Takino, R., Pütz, B., et al. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature 403, 192–195. doi: 10.1038/35003194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 187–202. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14, 103–109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson-Frey, S. H., Maloof, F. R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S., and Grafton, S. T. (2003). Actions or hand-object interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron 39, 1053–1058. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00524-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovanovic, V., Tomovic, M., Cosic, I., Miller, C., and Ostojic, G. (2007). “Ergonomic design of manual assembly workplaces,” in Annual ASEE IL/IN Section Conference.

Google Scholar

Kelly, R. M., and Strick, P. L. (2003). Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J. Neurosci. 23, 8432–8444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koski, L., Iacoboni, M., and Mazziotta, J. C. (2002). Deconstructing apraxia: understanding disorders of intentional movement after stroke. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 15, 71–77. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200202000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lausberg, H., Kazzer, P., Heekeren, H. R., and Wartenburger, I. (2015). Pantomiming tool use with an imaginary tool in hand as compared to demonstration with tool in hand specifically modulates the left middle and superior temporal gyri. Cortex 71, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesage, E., Hansen, P. C., and Miall, R. C. (2017). Right lateral cerebellum represents linguistic predictability. J. Neurosci. 37, 6231–6241. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3203-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, J. W. (2006). Cortical networks related to human use of tools. Neuroscientist 12, 211–231. doi: 10.1177/1073858406288327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liepmann, H. (2001). Agnosic disorders (1908) [classical article]. Cortex 37, 547–553. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70594-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macdonald, S. N., and Culham, J. C. (2015). Do human brain areas involved in visuomotor actions show a preference for real tools over visually similar non-tools? Neuropsychologia 77, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marek, S., and Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2018). The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 20, 133–140. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarek

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieto-Castanon, A. (2020). Handbook of Functional Connectivity Magnetic Resonance Imaging Methods in CONN. Boston, MA: Hilbert Press.

Google Scholar

Perry, D. C. (2014). “Angular Gyrus syndrome,” in Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences, 2nd Edn, eds M. J. Aminoff and R. B. Daroff (Oxford: Academic Press), 192–193. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-385157-4.00438-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccoli, T., Valente, G., Linden, D. E., Re, M., Esposito, F., Sack, A. T., et al. (2015). The default mode network and the working memory network are not anti-correlated during all phases of a working memory task. PLoS One 10:e0123354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Barnes, K. A., Church, J. A., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72, 665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 84, 320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radomski, M. V., and Trombly, C. A. (2008). Occupational Therapy for Physical Dysfunction. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Rosenthal, C. R., Roche-Kelly, E. E., Husain, M., and Kennard, C. (2009). Response-dependent contributions of human primary motor cortex and angular gyrus to manual and perceptual sequence learning. J. Neurosci. 29, 15115–15125. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2603-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., Scheidhauer, A., Andrews, B., Butler, J., et al. (2011). Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med. 9:75. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular Gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 19, 43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solis-Vivanco, R., Jensen, O., and Bonnefond, M. (2021). New insights on the ventral attention network: active suppression and involuntary recruitment during a bimodal task. Hum Brain Mapp. 42, 1699–1713. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sormaz, M., Murphy, C., Wang, H. T., Hymers, M., Karapanagiotidis, T., Poerio, G., et al. (2018). Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 9318–9323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721259115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, S., and Inui, T. (2002). Cortical involvement for action imitation of hand/arm postures versus finger configurations: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 13, 1599–1602. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200209160-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 203–218. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11001452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vingerhoets, G. (2008). Knowing about tools: neural correlates of tool familiarity and experience. Neuroimage 40, 1380–1391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vingerhoets, G., Vandekerckhove, E., Honoré, P., Vandemaele, P., and Achten, E. (2011). Neural correlates of pantomiming familiar and unfamiliar tools: action semantics versus mechanical problem solving? Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 905–918. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallis, G., Stokes, M., Cousijn, H., Woolrich, M., and Nobre, A. C. (2015). Frontoparietal and Cingulo-opercular networks play dissociable roles in control of working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 2019–2034. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., Wang, J., Fan, L., Li, H., Zhang, W., Hu, Q., et al. (2015). Tractography-based parcellation of the human middle temporal Gyrus. Sci. Rep. 5:18883. doi: 10.1038/srep18883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, K., and Sakurai, Y. (2016). Inactivation of cerebellar cortical crus II disrupts temporal processing of absolute timing but not relative timing in voluntary movements. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:16. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, K., Sohn, W. S., and Jeong, Y. (2013). Tool-use practice induces changes in intrinsic functional connectivity of parietal areas. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:49. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: complex tool-use, assembly work, fMRI, functional connectivity, network, rehabilitation

Citation: Taniguchi S, Higashi Y, Kataoka H, Nakajima H and Shimokawa T (2021) Functional Connectivity and Networks Underlying Complex Tool-Use Movement in Assembly Workers: An fMRI Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:707502. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.707502

Received: 10 May 2021; Accepted: 07 September 2021;
Published: 28 October 2021.

Edited by:

Akiyoshi Matsugi, Shijonawate Gakuen University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Kyota Bando, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Japan
Daniela De Bartolo, Santa Lucia Foundation, Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2021 Taniguchi, Higashi, Kataoka, Nakajima and Shimokawa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Seira Taniguchi, c2VpcmEudGFuaWd1Y2hpLnB0QGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.