Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 February 2019
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation

Rebound or Entrainment? The Influence of Alternating Current Stimulation on Individual Alpha

  • 1Department of Neurology, Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Neural Information Processing Group, University of Technology Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Alternating current stimulation (ACS) is an established means to manipulate intrinsic cortical oscillations. While working towards clinical impact, ACS mechanisms of action remain unclear. For ACS’s well-documented influence on occipital alpha, hypotheses include neuronal entrainment as well as rebound phenomena. As a retinal origin is also discussed, we employed a novel form of ACS with the advantage that it specifically targets occipital alpha-oscillations via retinofugal pathways retinofugal ACS (rACS). We aimed to confirm alpha-enhancement outlasting the duration of stimulation with 10 Hz rACS. To distinguish entrainment from rebound effects, we investigated the correlation between alpha peak frequency change and alpha-enhancement strength. We quantified the alpha band power before and after 10 Hz rACS in 15 healthy subjects. Alpha power enhancement and alpha peak frequency change were assessed over the occipital electrodes and compared to sham stimulation. RACS significantly enhanced occipital alpha power in comparison to sham stimulation (p < 0.05). Alpha peak frequency changed by a mean 0.02 Hz (± 0.04). A greater change in alpha peak frequency did not correlate with greater effects on alpha power. Our findings show an alpha-enhancement consistent with studies conducted for transcranial ACS (tACS) and contribute evidence for a retinal involvement in tACS effects on occipital alpha. Furthermore, the lack of correlation between alpha peak frequency change and alpha-enhancement strength provides an argument against entrainment effects and in favor of a rebound phenomenon.

Introduction

Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS), including transcranial electric stimulation (tES; Paulus, 2011), has shown impressive effects ranging from short changes in neural activity to long lasting recovery maximization following neural injury (Hallett, 2005; Talelli and Rothwell, 2006; Hummel et al., 2008; Sandrini and Cohen, 2013). A novel form of NIBS is transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), an oscillatory stimulation technique associated with psychophysical changes (Antal et al., 2008; Kanai et al., 2010; Wach et al., 2013), enhancement of working memory, learning, and long-term memory formation (Marshall et al., 2006; Kuo and Nitsche, 2012; Santarnecchi et al., 2013), as well as clinical improvements including tumor growth slowing (Kirson et al., 2007) and tremor suppression in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Brittain et al., 2013). Despite these encouraging results, little is known about the mechanism of action (Zaghi et al., 2010). Due to this lack of knowledge tACS may consequently be in danger of facing, similar to transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS; Horvath et al., 2015), the challenges of large effect variability and poor result reproducibility (Zaghi et al., 2010; Feurra et al., 2013; Wach et al., 2013). There is also evidence that the effects of tACS are at least in part due to retinal stimulation (Schutter, 2016). To efficiently and efficaciously apply tACS, especially in the visual system, it critically important for its users to further investigate the retinal as well as cortical mechanisms of action behind the observed effects.

In general, the effects of AC stimulation have been mainly attributed to synchronization of neural oscillations (Antal et al., 2008; Paulus, 2011; Herrmann et al., 2013), with most effects reported on alpha (α) oscillations (8–12 Hz; Antal et al., 2008; Helfrich et al., 2014; Vossen et al., 2015), the dominant frequency of the visual cortex (Klimesch, 1999). As the mechanism of action is unknown and effects of photic stimulation (Photic Driving; Walker et al., 1944) as well as AC stimulation on α oscillations via the retinofugal (visual) pathway are well-known (Brindley, 1955; Grützner et al., 1958; Sakamoto et al., 1993; Rager and Singer, 1998; Schmidt et al., 2013a), a better understanding of the underlying neurophysiology of alternating current stimulation (ACS) effects on oscillations should help address these issues.

Hypotheses concerning the mechanism of action for ACS synchronization effects include entrainment of neural oscillations as well as rebound effects. While entrainment describes the synchronization of one oscillator to another (Ermentrout and Rinzel, 1984), rebound is defined as an increase in excitability typically following inhibition (Perkel and Mulloney, 1974).

Several studies have shown frequency-specific effects on perceptual or cognitive task performance, ascribed to a potential frequency entrainment during or shortly after a single train of rhythmic stimulation (Klimesch et al., 2003; Marshall et al., 2006; Kanai et al., 2008; Zaehle et al., 2010). Entrainment requires an increase of spectral power as well as phase- and frequency-lock of a neural oscillation to an external stimulus (Vossen et al., 2015).

Phase-lock has been reported during stimulation (Helfrich et al., 2014; Ruhnau et al., 2016), although electrical stimulation artifacts in EEG and MEG aggravate the acquisition of reliable neurophysiological data (Soekadar et al., 2013; Helfrich et al., 2014). Newly presented data by Noury et al. suggests that these artifacts may be mistaken for entrainment effects (Noury et al., 2016).

Consequently, most reports present spectral power enhancement only after the cessation of ACS (Zaehle et al., 2010; Schmidt et al., 2013a; Helfrich et al., 2014; Vossen et al., 2015). This offers the hypothesis of a rebound effect as an alternative explanation. Classical post-inhibitory rebound is an increase in excitability following inhibition, generating responses ranging from threshold lowering up to a train of impulses (Perkel and Mulloney, 1974). This, considered for α-pacemaker neurons, would also result in an increase of spectral power after stimulation. Such an increase would occur at an intrinsic frequency and without a phase-lock (Perkel and Mulloney, 1974). Neural rebound effects after stimulation have been found in animal models (Pape and McCormick, 1995; Tong et al., 1996; Huang et al., 2008) as well as in the human brain (Fuggetta et al., 2005; Brignani et al., 2008; Manganotti et al., 2012).

The primary goal of this study was to examine the mechanisms of action behind ACS effects on occipital α oscillations. We therefore investigated a possible contribution of retinal stimulation to the observed α enhancement by employing a periorbital montage. Furthermore, to address the differentiation between neural entrainment and rebound effects after AC stimulation, any frequency peak shift in endogenous α rhythms towards the stimulation frequency was investigated. The presence of such a frequency peak shift would indicate an entrainment, while its absence would provide evidence in favor of a rebound phenomenon.

We decided on the α frequency band as a target, as ACS has shown robust effects in this frequency range (Kanai et al., 2010; Helfrich et al., 2014; Vossen et al., 2015). Cortical α is associated with numerous perceptional processes (Surwillo, 1961; VanRullen and Koch, 2003; Mathewson et al., 2009; Ai and Ro, 2014; Lange et al., 2014; Cecere et al., 2015; Samaha and Postle, 2015) as well as cognitive performance (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 2003; Hanslmayr et al., 2005; Zoefel et al., 2011) and characterized by a peak in spectral analysis, the individual α frequency (IAF; Klimesch et al., 2007), which presents an optimal opportunity to investigate synchronization to external stimulation. The Berger effect, describing the repression of the highest physiological occipital α in a subject via opening of the eyes (Berger, 1929), presents further opportunities. Firstly, we can employ our stimulation in an eyes open condition and expect little interference from intrinsic α. Furthermore, the measurement of high occipital α during an eyes closed baseline condition can serve as a reference for effect size, since it allows us to compare exogenous stimulation effects to the highest intrinsically generated α power.

Retinal contribution to tACS effects is part of an ongoing discussion (Schwiedrzik, 2009; Paulus, 2010; Schutter and Hortensius, 2010). A recent review by Schutter (2016) suggested that phosphenes might be involved in tACS α-enhancement in the visual system.

To further investigate this and effectively target α oscillations in the visual cortex, we employed a periorbital application type of ACS termed retinofugal ACS (rACS). This technique has been applied aiming for vision restoration in a therapeutic regimen called repetitive transorbital ACS (rtACS; Gall et al., 2010). It utilizes signal transmission along well-defined retinofugal tracts terminating predominantly in cortical visual areas (Gray and Singer, 1989) and offers a comparatively focal (Peterchev et al., 2012) method to investigate ACS effects on intrinsic frequencies in the well-circumscribed visual system (Gall et al., 2011; Schmidt et al., 2013a).

Materials and Methods

Participants

We stimulated 15 healthy volunteers in the rACS group (eight female, seven male, mean age 23.9 ± 2.5) as well as in the sham group (four female, 11 male, mean age 25.8 ± 5.3). The subjects were interviewed prior to experimentation regarding their state of health and gave written informed consent. We applied established exclusion criteria for NIBS (Brunoni et al., 2012), and added evidence for photophobia and photosensitive epilepsy. All protocols conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki, and were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin (“Ethikkommission der Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin”). This study adheres to the principles of good scientific practice of the Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin (“Grundsätze der Charité zur Sicherung guter wissenschaftlicher Praxis”).

Stimulation

RACS was applied by a multi-channel low-voltage stimulation device certified for clinical use, which delivered voltage-controlled weak periorbital sinusoidal current over four individual periorbital electrodes respectively (NextWave, Eyetronic, Germany). The four superficial active stimulating electrodes (sintered Ag/AgCl ring electrode, Easycap, Germany) were contained in foam-padded stimulation goggles and bilaterally made skin contact via small felt buffers (0.35 cm2) superior and inferior to the eye (“periorbital”). The passive electrode (rectangular electrode, 30 × 30 mm polished stainless steel) was fastened on the back of the neck at the midline relative to the occipital poles.

The electrical impedance of the four stimulating electrodes was measured for four different frequencies (5 Hz, 10 Hz, 15 Hz and 20 Hz) and held below 15 kΩ. This measurement at different frequencies was done as part of the innate safety procedure of the CE-certified stimulation device. Phosphene thresholds, defined as the current intensity when participants first subjectively perceived phosphenes, were determined employing an ascending method of limits (Herrick, 1967) provided by the NextWave software.

In the rACS group, stimulation was applied with bandwidth restricted to 10 Hz at an amplitude of 120% phosphene threshold (resulting in a mean 354.15 μA ± 50.6 peak-to-peak amplitude) and delivered in six 30 s blocks followed by 30 s pauses each. Solely 10 Hz as stimulation frequency was chosen to stay close to the critical α-eigenfrequencies of our subjects.

In the sham group, six blocks of a 5 s ramp-up/ramp-down DC stimulus followed by 25 s signal silence was applied at 120% phosphene threshold (resulting in a mean 292.10 μA ± 68.9 peak-to-peak amplitude), again followed by 30 s pauses each. During the signal silence, the stimulator remained switched on while no current was delivered. The sham signal was designed to match initial skin sensations known from other forms of NIBS (Siebner et al., 2004) as well as rACS phosphene perception.

EEG

EEG measurements were performed during the sessions using a 32-electrode EEG cap (Waveguard EEG caps, ANT BV, Enschede, Netherlands), according to the 10–20-system, with impedances kept below 10 kΩ. Data acquisition was carried out in a shielded room without natural light in the electrophysiological unit of the neurological department.

Two minutes of baseline EEG with four conditions were recorded prior to stimulation: 30 s eyes open, 30 s eyes closed and 30 s blinking followed by 30 s of muscle artifact production. To avoid artifacts, the subjects were told to focus a fixed point on a white surface in 1 m distance for the duration of the experiment. Moreover, the experimenter controlled the eyes open/closed conditions and reminded the subjects to follow the instructions and keep alert if necessary.

The EEG data was imported into MATLAB R2014a (The MathWorks Inc, Natick, MA, USA) and analyzed using the FieldTrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011). We segmented the EEG signals in 25 s-intervals for the eyes-open (EO) baseline, the eyes-closed (EC) baseline and the EO post-stimulation (Post-Stim) condition, including data from 2.5 s after stimulation cessation to 2.5 s before the beginning of a new stimulation block. Remaining artifacts, especially blinking and muscle twitches, were excluded via visual artifact rejection for each trial and channel. The blinking and muscle artifact conditions run prior to baseline EEG served as a decision support in borderline cases. EEG signals were referenced against the common average and filtered (Band-Pass Filter from 2 to 70 Hz with filter-slope 24 dB/oct). Following resampling of the data from originally 2,000–500 Hz to streamline and accelerate data analysis, a fast fourier transformation (FFT) with a discrete prolate spheroidal sequences (DPSS) multitaper determined the bandwidth-specific power (mV2). The sliding time window was set to 0.5 s, while the frequency analysis window was set to 0.04 Hz. The frequency with the highest resulting α power was defined as the IAF/α peak. For illustration, we calculated the EEG-power spectra grand average during EC and post-stimulation and baseline-corrected them by dividing it by the EO baseline grand average. Topographical plots of these values were then generated with FieldTrip and smoothed using a moving average filter with a span of two.

After this, the mean α power over the spectrum 8–12 Hz for each electrode and stimulation block was calculated. We then confirmed that there was no significant correlation between stimulation block number and α power increase in either the sham (r = 0.02, p = 0.83) or the rACS condition (r = −0.08, p = 0.45) via Spearman’s rho to ensure the absence of an alpha power ramp-up effect over consecutive trains of stimulation. Consecutively, we calculated the mean α power and peak frequency over all six stimulation blocks for each electrode, preparing the data for statistical analysis.

Statistics

To statistically investigate the effect of rACS on occipital alpha, we compared the mean spectral α (8–12 Hz) α power for rACS and Sham. Due to the lack of normal distribution determined by the Shapiro-Wilk test, we ln+1-transformed the data. Normal distribution and homoscedasticity of the data were then confirmed via the Shapiro-Wilk and Levene’s test respectively. We then calculated a repeated measure analyses of variance (ANOVA) with the dependent variable “α power”, consisting of the levels (EO Baseline, EC Baseline and EO Post-Stim) as well as the fixed factor “group.”

For our analysis of α power change, we baseline-corrected the ln-transformed EO Post-Stim α power data by dividing it by the EO Baseline and subtracting 1. After confirming normal distribution and homoscedasticity, we performed an ANOVA with “group” as fixed factor and baseline-corrected ln-transformed “alpha power” as dependent variable.

To gain insight on a possible frequency shift after rACS, the effect of rACS of the distance of α peak frequency to stimulation frequency was analyzed with an univariate ANOVA after confirming normal distribution of the data via Shapiro-Wilk test. A possible correlation between distance of individual α to stimulation frequency and α-power change was also assessed, again using the Spearman correlation coefficient.

P-values of ≤ 0.05 were considered significant. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 19.0.0.1 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). Spectral power bar plots as well as spectral α peak box- and scatter plots were created using GraphPad Prism version 7.02 for Windows (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA1).

Results

Stimulation Parameters

An average phosphene threshold at 290.24 μA (± 44.16) for the rACS and 243.33 μA (± 57.07) for the sham group, impedances at 12.57 kΩ (± 1.8) and 11.73 kΩ (± 2.7), as well as an average peak-to-peak amplitude of 354.15 μA (± 50.6) for rACS and 292.00 (± 68.5) μA for sham were noted. We additionally calculated the effective (root mean square) amplitude, resulting in a mean 250.41 μA (± 47.7) for rACS and 168.58 μA (± 39.5) for sham. The current density amounted to a mean 0.71 mA/cm2 (± 0.13) for rACS and a mean 0.48 mA/cm2 (± 0.11) for sham.

Alpha Power Enhancement After rACS

We found a spectral α power enhancement over the occipital scalp area after rACS, with subjects showing significantly greater α power increase after rACS as compared to sham. The enhancement was comparable in size to the one found in the EC condition.

Topographical plots of the frequency grand average (Figure 1) showed a strong focus of the overall α-power enhancement after rACS and during EC around the occipital scalp area. Additionally, a diffuse increase in 8–12 Hz spectral power can be observed after sham and rACS, with foci in the frontal and centroparietal scalp area after rACS and in the central scalp area after sham.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Topographical plots. Topographical plots of the α power change [divided by eyes open (EO) Baseline] in the conditions post-stimulation (Post-Stim) and eyes closed (EC) Baseline as well as the retinofugal alternating current stimulation (rACS)- and Sham group. Color spectrum ranges from 0.9 to 1.5. In the EC and rACS conditions, α increase is most prominent across the occipital electrodes O1, Oz and O2.

Mean ln+1-transformed EO Baseline α power (Figure 2A) amounted to 0.99 (± 0.13) for the rACS group and 0.88 (± 0.10) for the Sham group. In the EC Baseline condition, we found a mean α power of 1.26 (± 0.17) in the rACS and 1.11 (± 0.16) in the Sham group. Alpha power in the EO Post-Stim condition amounted to a mean 1.21 (± 0.14) in the rACS and a mean 0.83 (± 0.09) in the Sham group. The repeated measure ANOVA resulted in a significant main effect for mean α power (F(2,27) = 8.99, p = 0.001) as well as interaction between the stimulation type and mean alpha power (F(2,27) = 4.02, p = 0.03). Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons showed a significantly higher alpha power in the rACS Post-Stim condition compared to Sham (p = 0.036), but no significant differences in the baseline (p = 0.48) and EC (p = 0.52) conditions. The rACS group also showed significantly higher EO Post-Stim α power compared to EO Baseline (p = 0.01), whereas the Sham group showed no significant difference in this regard (p = 0.99). Furthermore, EC Baseline α power was significantly higher than the EO Baseline in both groups (rACS: 0.02, Sham: 0.05), replicating the Berger-effect. There was no significant difference between rACS group EO Post-Stim α and EC Baseline (p = 1.00), indicating a physiologically plausible α increase.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Spectral alpha power change. (A) Ln+1-transformed spectral α power over the occipital electrodes O1, Oz and O2 for the rACS group (green) and the Sham group (gray) in three conditions: eyes open (EO) Baseline, eyes closed (EC) Baseline and EO Post-Stim. Baseline condition bars are dot patterned, while the Post-Stim bars are plain. Error bars depict standard error of the mean. The EC Baseline shows a significant increase in α power compared to EO Baseline in both groups, replicating the Berger effect (indicated by dashed lines). The EO Post-Stim condition α in the rACS group is significantly higher than the Sham EO Post-Stim α, as well as significantly higher than its (rACS Group) EO Baseline (indicated by solid lines), showing a clear stimulation effect of rACS on occipital α. (B) Mean baseline-corrected α power change (EO Post-Stim/EO Baseline −1) over the occipital electrodes O1, Oz and O2 for the sham and rACS groups. Post-stimulation rACS is depicted as green, post-stimulation sham as gray. A value of 0 represents no change from baseline α power. Error bars depict standard error of the mean. The rACS group shows a significantly stronger increase in α power compared to the sham group. Significant differences (p < 0.05) are marked with an asterisk *.

Baseline-corrected α power (change; Figure 2B) amounted to a mean −0.02 (± 0.07) for EO Post-Stim Sham and +0.28 (± 0.09) for EO Post-Stim rACS. The ANOVA showed a significantly higher alpha power change after rACS compared to Sham (F(1,28) = 7.139, p = 0.01).

Individual α Frequency Shift

We found no significant shift of the individual α peak frequency after rACS compared to baseline. The spectral peak of the α-band amounted to a mean 10.22 Hz (± 1.29) prior to rACS and a mean 10.26 Hz (± 1.17) after rACS (Figure 3). The mean absolute distance to stimulation frequency (10 Hz) amounted to 1.05 Hz (± 0.73) before and 0.97 Hz (± 0.67) after application of rACS. There was no significant effect of rACS on the distance of α peak frequency to stimulation frequency (F(1,28) = 0.009, p = 0.93) as assessed by an univariate ANOVA. This indicates the lack of an overall frequency lock of intrinsic α to stimulation frequency.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean spectral peak analysis. Individual α peak frequency before (blue) and after (green) rACS. Black points and lines depict individual subjects. No consistent shift of the individual α peak to stimulation frequency can be observed.

Moreover, the proximity of IAF to the stimulation frequency of 10 Hz did not correlate to a greater increase in α-power (r = 0.04, p = 0.88) employing Spearman’s rho. This provides evidence against accidental frequency-locked stimulation leading to enhanced occipital α.

Discussion

The effects of 10 Hz rACS on neural oscillations showed the following characteristics: (1) 10 Hz stimulation resulted in an enhancement of α-power; (2) the post-stimulation α-peak did not significantly differ from baseline IAF; and (3) there was no significant correlation between α-power and proximity to stimulation frequency. These findings are not consistent with the hypothesized entrainment effects. Since entrainment of neural oscillations is the most proposed mechanism of action for tACS effects (Helfrich et al., 2014), an in-depth discussion of these conflicting findings is required.

Enhancement

The specific enhancement of α-power over the occipital electrodes after 10 Hz rACS is consistent with studies conducted for tACS (Antal et al., 2008; Zaehle et al., 2010; Helfrich et al., 2014; Vossen et al., 2015). While a diffuse increase in 8–12 Hz spectral power in the frontal and centroparietal scalp can be observed after rACS, the distribution of the enhancement was comparable to that of the highest physiological α in the EC condition. Additionally, it did not significantly differ from EC regarding effect size, showing a physiologically plausible increase in synchrony. A similar α-enhancement outlasting stimulation duration has already been reported after 10 Hz audiovisual (Rosenfeld et al., 1997) and photic stimulation (Sakamoto et al., 1993; Spaak et al., 2014). This supports the hypothesis of Schutter (2016) that retinal stimulation may induce neural effects similar to tACS. Retinal phosphenes induced by tACS and their possible role in ACS effects of neural oscillations are subject of a long ongoing discussion.

Early work by Kanai et al. (2008) showed phosphene elicitation during occipital tACS in a frequency- and illumination-dependent manner. While this was interpreted as cortical stimulation, others proposed a retinal origin. The higher sensitivity of the retina and an experimentally confirmed occipital-to-frontal threshold decrease supported this hypothesis (Schwiedrzik, 2009; Schutter and Hortensius, 2010). Interestingly, moving the electrode from the occipital area towards the retina did not change the latency of phosphene perception (Kar and Krekelberg, 2012). The effects of tACS over the visual cortex may therefore be a result of stimulation along the retinofugal pathway similar to rACS.

Yet, due to the respective montages there should be a magnitude of difference between methods (Peterchev et al., 2012) with rACS inducing the most, traditional occiput-vertex montages intermediate and bilateral or 4 × 1 ring electrode montages inducing the least retinal activation (Paulus, 2010; Neuling et al., 2012; Datta et al., 2013; Laakso and Hirata, 2013).

In sum, we provide further evidence for α-power-enhancement after ACS being induced via retinofugal pathway activation.

Entrainment

Neural oscillators, as the target of tACS, share features of relaxation oscillators and harmonic oscillators (Glass, 2001; Winfree, 2001) and therefore present a stable eigenfrequency as well as synchronization capabilities (Somers and Kopell, 1993; Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004).

This enables entrainment to an external force resulting in effects that could outlast stimulation duration. Such entrainment effects would present themselves as phase- and frequency-locked to stimulation and have been shown in vitro (Fröhlich and McCormick, 2010) and in the animal model (Ali et al., 2013).

We observed no such frequency-lock after rACS.

This lack of a shift towards stimulation frequency is also consistent with several reports of tACS α-enhancement post-stimulation (Zaehle et al., 2010; Helfrich et al., 2014; Vossen et al., 2015). Vossen et al. (2015) have also presented this as strong evidence against entrainment, whereas Helfrich et al. (2014) and Zaehle et al., 2010 focused on ACS effects during stimulation via artifact rejection. The lack of a correlation between proximity of IAF to stimulation frequency has also been reported for tACS (Helfrich et al., 2014), even going as far as a negative correlation (Vossen et al., 2015). Both groups have reported these findings as counterintuitive. While this presents an argument against entrainment, one would need reliable recordings during stimulation to falsify this hypothesis.

Still, entrainment effects could produce the results at hand in the following ways.

Entrainment During Stimulation

The effects of stimuli on neuronal oscillators are mainly dependent on phase and stimulus amplitude (Glass, 2001). RACS was not applied at IAF and consequently not applied phase-locked as well. Therefore, the stimulus was unlikely to arrive at the opening phase of the target oscillator. This could have been compensated through a higher stimulus amplitude (Ai and Ro, 2014). Although possible for TMS (Thut and Miniussi, 2009), this is unlikely for most types of tES (including rACS), which reach comparatively low current densities at the target site (Poreisz et al., 2007). As we encountered stimulation artifacts as well as a residual stimulation cessation artifact covering the first 100–300 ms after stimulation, an entrainment effect in this time period cannot be reliably investigated and therefore remains a possibility. There are reports presenting such entrainment during stimulation utilizing artifact rejection techniques (Soekadar et al., 2013; Helfrich et al., 2014; Neuling et al., 2017), which are still controversially discussed (Noury et al., 2016).

Frequency Pulling

A stable entrainment during stimulation with an external oscillator would also not explain the immediate loss of synchronization to the external oscillator following stimulus cessation found in this study. A possible explanation is a weak coupling during stimulation, resulting in not a frequency lock, but rather a frequency pulling (Cross et al., 2004) with frequent phase walk-throughs (Ermentrout and Rinzel, 1984). Hereby, an oscillator, instead of fully synchronizing, only appeases the frequency of an external stimulus and desynchronizes quickly (Hoppensteadt and Keener, 1982). This can be the result of the external stimulus being too weak or too far removed from the oscillators eigenfrequency (Cross et al., 2004). A short period of synchronization followed by desynchronization would be the result, which has been reported for the neurophysiologically similar photic stimulation (Jin et al., 2000). A polysynaptic recruitment of further neurons after the initial driving could then explain the observed enhancement (Ozen et al., 2010).

Subthreshold Modulation

The lack of a frequency appeasement may also be due to the applied stimulation inducing only subthreshold modulation as shown in the rat model (Ozen et al., 2010). This would result in a lower threshold for network-induced membrane voltage fluctuations to generate spikes in a fraction of the neuronal population. The effects of transcranial stimulation, specifically tES, are mostly attributed to such subthreshold modulation (Zaghi et al., 2010). In contrast to the cortical stimulation of tES, rACS stimulated the more excitable retina (Lindenblatt and Silny, 2002) with amplitudes above phosphene threshold. We can therefore assume the generation of action potentials at least in the visual cell (Grützner et al., 1958; Schutter and Hortensius, 2010). Nevertheless, a threshold-lowering effect of rACS would provide an explanation for similar effects on oscillations found after retinal random noise stimulation (RNS; Jooss et al., 2015) through the mechanism of stochastic resonance (Wiesenfeld and Moss, 1995; Gluckman et al., 1996).

Rebound

As the entrainment-hypothesis delivers no conclusive explanation for the alpha enhancement following ACS, our findings can be interpreted as evidence for a rebound effect.

Rebound firing is a long known mechanism of action for signal transmission in neurons (Creutzfeldt et al., 1962; Grenier et al., 1998). Diffuse electric fields have been shown to promote a rebound-like burst firing compared to direct somatic current injection (Radman et al., 2009), especially after cessation of stimulation (Reato et al., 2010). Several studies presented rebound firing in thalamocortical networks and neurons in response to oscillatory electric stimuli (Destexhe et al., 1996; Birdno et al., 2014; Sakata, 2016). Thalamocortical networks are well-known to be vitally involved in the generation of the α rhythm (Hughes and Crunelli, 2005).

TMS effects on neural oscillations have also been attributed to rebound firing, with studies finding an α- and/or β-enhancement following single TMS-Pulses (Paus et al., 2001; Fuggetta et al., 2005; Brignani et al., 2008). Paus et al. (2001) explained this as a combination of rebound-like firing and recruitment of “idle” neurons. Fuggetta et al. (2005) as well as Brignani et al., 2008 discuss α-rebound as a possible effect of corticothalamic feedback mechanisms. Following thalamic input, primary visual cortex layer 6 neurons are able to deliver antiphasic feedback to the lateral geniculate nucleus (Yousif and Denham, 2007), which in turn may result in burst firing inducing α- or θ-rhythms (Hughes and Crunelli, 2005). This is particularly interesting, as cortical tACS should activate layer 6 neurons as well and might trigger similar rebound mechanisms.

Applying these considerations, the findings reported in this article can be interpreted as follows: a possible initial entrainment of layer 4 and 6 neurons could result in a corticothalamic feedback triggering thalamic rebound firing, which would drive the cortically observed α-rhythm and lead to further polysynaptic recruitment of cortical neurons. The rebound effect could explain an enhancement and stabilization at an intrinsic frequency after neither phase- nor frequency-locked stimulation (Perkel and Mulloney, 1974). This would also sufficiently explain the lack of correlation between proximity of stimulation frequency to IAF and α-power increase also reported by Helfrich et al. (2014). However, due to the lack of recordings during stimulation, we cannot report a period of inhibition preceding a rebound (Perkel and Mulloney, 1974).

Generally, the challenges of the rebound hypothesis lie in frequency-specific psychophysical effects of ACS reported during stimulation (Pogosyan et al., 2009; Joundi et al., 2012) and the findings of photic driving (Walker et al., 1944), especially as we stimulate the retina and the optic nerve.

Outlook

A combination of entrainment and rebound hypotheses as presented above may prospectively provide an integrative model of ACS effects on the α-rhythm. Investigation of closed-loop phase-locked stimulation (Brittain et al., 2013) as well as better knowledge of psychophysical changes during stimulation could contribute to a deeper understanding of such mechanisms of action.

Users of tACS in the visual system should take note of the accumulating evidence of a retinal contribution, at least to the effects of cortical α power. Furthermore, cortical tACS should activate layer 6 neurons similar to rACS and might therefore trigger the same rebound mechanisms described in this article instead of achieving entrainment effects.

Finally, a thorough study of the effects achieved by frequency-unspecific RNS, especially in combination with tACS, could provide further insight into subthreshold modulation induced by tES. This would be an intriguing step towards the understanding of a common framework utilizing both noise and oscillation in the human brain (Schmidt et al., 2013b).

Author Contributions

LH, SS, MS and SB conceived and designed the study. LH, SS, LK and AJ carried out data acquisition and analysis. All authors participated in the interpretation of the data. The manuscript was drafted by LH and SS and critically revised by AJ, AK, MR, MS, LK and SB.

Funding

This work was supported by the German Research Foundation, Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) grant BR 1691/8-1 and OB 102/22-1.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Selina Greuel for help with data collection and visualization.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.graphpad.com

References

Ai, L., and Ro, T. (2014). The phase of prestimulus alpha oscillations affects tactile perception. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 1300–1307. doi: 10.1152/jn.00125.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, M. M., Sellers, K. K., and Fröhlich, F. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates large-scale cortical network activity by network resonance. J. Neurosci. 33, 11262–11275. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5867-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Boros, K., Poreisz, C., Chaieb, L., Terney, D., and Paulus, W. (2008). Comparatively weak after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on cortical excitability in humans. Brain Stimul. 1, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, H. (1929). Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 87, 527–570.

Google Scholar

Birdno, M. J., Tang, W., Dostrovsky, J. O., Hutchison, W. D., and Grill, W. M. (2014). Response of human thalamic neurons to high-frequency stimulation. PLoS One 9:e96026. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brignani, D., Manganotti, P., Rossini, P. M., and Miniussi, C. (2008). Modulation of cortical oscillatory activity during transcranial magnetic stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 603–612. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brindley, G. S. (1955). The site of electrical excitation of the human eye. J. Physiol. 127, 189–200. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1955.sp005248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brittain, J. S., Probert-Smith, P., Aziz, T. Z., and Brown, P. (2013). Tremor suppression by rhythmic transcranial current stimulation. Curr. Biol. 23, 436–440. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., Nitsche, M. A., Bolognini, N., Bikson, M., Wagner, T., Merabet, L., et al. (2012). Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): challenges and future directions. Brain Stimul. 5, 175–195. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cecere, R., Rees, G., and Romei, V. (2015). Individual differences in alpha frequency drive crossmodal illusory perception. Curr. Biol. 25, 231–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creutzfeldt, O. D., Fromm, G. H., and Kapp, H. (1962). Influence of transcortical d-c currents on cortical neuronal activity. Exp. Neurol. 5, 436–452. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(62)90056-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, M. C., Zumdieck, A., Lifshitz, R., and Rogers, J. L. (2004). Synchronization by nonlinear frequency pulling. Phys. Rev. Lett. 93:224101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.93.224101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Datta, A., Zhou, X., Su, Y., Parra, L. C., and Bikson, M. (2013). Validation of finite element model of transcranial electrical stimulation using scalp potentials: implications for clinical dose. J. Neural Eng. 10:036018. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/10/3/036018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Destexhe, A., Bal, T., McCormick, D. A., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1996). Ionic mechanisms underlying synchronized oscillations and propagating waves in a model of ferret thalamic slices. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 2049–2070. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.2049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ermentrout, G. B., and Rinzel, J. (1984). Beyond a pacemaker’s entrainment limit: phase walk-through. Am. J. Physiol. 246, R102–R106. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.1.r102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feurra, M., Pasqualetti, P., Bianco, G., Santarnecchi, E., Rossi, A., and Rossi, S. (2013). State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: what you think matters. J. Neurosci. 33, 17483–17489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1414-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fröhlich, F., and McCormick, D. A. (2010). Endogenous electric fields may guide neocortical network activity. Neuron 67, 129–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuggetta, G., Fiaschi, A., and Manganotti, P. (2005). Modulation of cortical oscillatory activities induced by varying single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation intensity over the left primary motor area: a combined EEG and TMS study. Neuroimage 27, 896–908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gall, C., Fedorov, A. B., Ernst, L., Borrmann, A., and Sabel, B. A. (2010). Repetitive transorbital alternating current stimulation in optic neuropathy. NeuroRehabilitation 27, 335–341. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2010-0617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gall, C., Sgorzaly, S., Schmidt, S., Brandt, S., Fedorov, A., and Sabel, B. A. (2011). Noninvasive transorbital alternating current stimulation improves subjective visual functioning and vision-related quality of life in optic neuropathy. Brain Stimul. 4, 175–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glass, L. (2001). Synchronization and rhythmic processes in physiology. Nature 410, 277–284. doi: 10.1038/35065745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluckman, B. J., Netoff, T. I., Neel, E. J., Ditto, W. L., Spano, M. L., and Schiff, S. J. (1996). Stochastic resonance in a neuronal network from mammalian brain. Phys. Rev. Lett. 77, 4098–4101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.77.4098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M., and Singer, W. (1989). Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual-cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 86, 1698–1702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.5.1698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grenier, F., Timofeev, I., and Steriade, M. (1998). Leading role of thalamic over cortical neurons during postinhibitory rebound excitation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 13929–13934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grützner, A., Grüsser, O.-J., and Baumgartner, G. (1958). Reaktionen einzelner neurone im optischen cortex der Katze nach elektrischer Reizung des nervus opticus. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. Z. Gesamte. Neurol. Psychiatr. 197, 377–404. doi: 10.1007/BF00345845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallett, M. (2005). Neuroplasticity and rehabilitation. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 42, xvii–xxii. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2005.07.0126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Doppelmayr, M., Schabus, M., and Klimesch, W. (2005). Increasing individual upper alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 30, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10484-005-2169-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Schneider, T. R., Rach, S., Trautmann-Lengsfeld, S. A., Engel, A. K., and Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Curr. Biol. 24, 333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrick, R. M. (1967). Psychophysical methodology: comparison of thresholds of the method of limits and of the method of constant stimuli. Percept. Mot. Skills 24, 915–922. doi: 10.2466/pms.1967.24.3.915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., Rach, S., Neuling, T., and Struber, D. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoppensteadt, F. C., and Keener, J. P. (1982). Phase locking of biological clocks. J. Math. Biol. 15, 339–349. doi: 10.1007/bf00275692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, J. C., Forte, J. D., and Carter, O. (2015). Evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) generates little-to-no reliable neurophysiologic effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation in healthy human subjects: a systematic review. Neuropsychologia 66, 213–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Levine, S., and Paradiso, M. A. (2008). Rebounding V1 activity and a new visual aftereffect. J. Vis. 8:25. doi: 10.1167/8.3.25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, S. W., and Crunelli, V. (2005). Thalamic mechanisms of EEG alpha rhythms and their pathological implications. Neuroscientist 11, 357–372. doi: 10.1177/1073858405277450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummel, F. C., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, A., Fregni, F., Byblow, W. D., Buetefisch, C. M., et al. (2008). Controversy: noninvasive and invasive cortical stimulation show efficacy in treating stroke patients. Brain Stimul. 1, 370–382. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y., Castellanos, A., Solis, E. R., and Potkin, S. G. (2000). EEG resonant responses in schizophrenia: a photic driving study with improved harmonic resolution. Schizophr. Res. 44, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00211-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jooss, A., Schmidt, S., Haberbosch, L., Köhn, A., Scholz, M., and Brandt, S. A. (2015). Investigating the effects of noisy stimulation on the retinofugal pathway. Brain Stimul. 8:402. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joundi, R. A., Jenkinson, N., Brittain, J. S., Aziz, T. Z., and Brown, P. (2012). Driving oscillatory activity in the human cortex enhances motor performance. Curr. Biol. 22, 403–407. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Chaieb, L., Antal, A., Walsh, V., and Paulus, W. (2008). Frequency-dependent electrical stimulation of the visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 18, 1839–1843. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Paulus, W., and Walsh, V. (2010). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) modulates cortical excitability as assessed by TMS-induced phosphene thresholds. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1551–1554. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kar, K., and Krekelberg, B. (2012). Transcranial electrical stimulation over visual cortex evokes phosphenes with a retinal origin. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 2173–2178. doi: 10.1152/jn.00505.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirson, E. D., Dbalý, V., Tovarys, F., Vymazal, J., Soustiel, J. F., Itzhaki, A., et al. (2007). Alternating electric fields arrest cell proliferation in animal tumor models and human brain tumors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 10152–10157. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702916104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Gerloff, C. (2003). Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1129–1133. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02517.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, M. F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 43, 192–199. doi: 10.1177/1550059412444975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laakso, I., and Hirata, A. (2013). Computational analysis shows why transcranial alternating current stimulation induces retinal phosphenes. J. Neural Eng. 10:046009. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/10/4/046009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, J., Keil, J., Schnitzler, A., van Dijk, H., and Weisz, N. (2014). The role of alpha oscillations for illusory perception. Behav. Brain Res. 271, 294–301. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenblatt, G., and Silny, J. (2002). Electrical phosphenes: on the influence of conductivity inhomogeneities and small-scale structures of the orbita on the current density threshold of excitation. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 40, 354–359. doi: 10.1007/bf02344219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manganotti, P., Formaggio, E., Storti, S. F., De Massari, D., Zamboni, A., Bertoldo, A., et al. (2012). Time-frequency analysis of short-lasting modulation of EEG induced by intracortical and transcallosal paired TMS over motor areas. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 2475–2484. doi: 10.1152/jn.00543.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., Helgadóttir, H., Mölle, M., and Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444, 610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., and Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness. J. Neurosci. 29, 2725–2732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3963-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuling, T., Ruhnau, P., Weisz, N., Herrmann, C. S., and Demarchi, G. (2017). Faith and oscillations recovered: on analyzing EEG/MEG signals during tACS. Neuroimage 15, 960–963. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuling, T., Wagner, S., Wolters, C. H., Zaehle, T., and Herrmann, C. S. (2012). Finite-element model predicts current density distribution for clinical applications of tDCS and tACS. Front. Psychiatry 3:83. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noury, N., Hipp, J. F., and Siegel, M. (2016). Physiological processes non-linearly affect electrophysiological recordings during transcranial electric stimulation. Neuroimage 140, 99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozen, S., Sirota, A., Belluscio, M. A., Anastassiou, C. A., Stark, E., Koch, C., et al. (2010). Transcranial electric stimulation entrains cortical neuronal populations in rats. J. Neurosci. 30, 11476–11485. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5252-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pape, H. C., and McCormick, D. A. (1995). Electrophysiological and pharmacological properties of interneurons in the cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuroscience 68, 1105–1125. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00205-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, W. (2010). On the difficulties of separating retinal from cortical origins of phosphenes when using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 987–991. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial electrical stimulation (tES - tDCS; tRNS, tACS) methods. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 602–617. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.557292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Sipila, P. K., and Strafella, A. P. (2001). Synchronization of neuronal activity in the human primary motor cortex by transcranial magnetic stimulation: an EEG study. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1983–1990. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkel, D. H., and Mulloney, B. (1974). Motor pattern production in reciprocally inhibitory neurons exhibiting postinhibitory rebound. Science 185, 181–183. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4146.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterchev, A. V., Wagner, T. A., Miranda, P. C., Nitsche, M. A., Paulus, W., Lisanby, S. H., et al. (2012). Fundamentals of transcranial electric and magnetic stimulation dose: definition, selection and reporting practices. Brain Stimul. 5, 435–453. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pogosyan, A., Gaynor, L. D., Eusebio, A., and Brown, P. (2009). Boosting cortical activity at Beta-band frequencies slows movement in humans. Curr. Biol. 19, 1637–1641. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poreisz, C., Boros, K., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2007). Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Res. Bull. 72, 208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radman, T., Ramos, R. L., Brumberg, J. C., and Bikson, M. (2009). Role of cortical cell type and morphology in subthreshold and suprathreshold uniform electric field stimulation in vitro. Brain Stimul. 2, 215–228.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rager, G., and Singer, W. (1998). The response of cat visual cortex to flicker stimuli of variable frequency. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 1856–1877. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00197.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reato, D., Rahman, A., Bikson, M., and Parra, L. C. (2010). Low-intensity electrical stimulation affects network dynamics by modulating population rate and spike timing. J. Neurosci. 30, 15067–15079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, J. P., Reinhart, A. M., and Srivastava, S. (1997). The effects of alpha (10-Hz) and beta (22-Hz) “entrainment” stimulation on the alpha and beta EEG bands: individual differences are critical to prediction of effects. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 22, 3–20. doi: 10.1023/A:1026233624772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruhnau, P., Neuling, T., Fuscá, M., Herrmann, C. S., Demarchi, G., and Weisz, N. (2016). Eyes wide shut: transcranial alternating current stimulation drives alpha rhythm in a state dependent manner. Sci. Rep. 6:27138. doi: 10.1038/srep27138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, H., Inouye, T., and Shinosaki, K. (1993). Preservation of alpha rhythm shortly after photic driving. Int. J. Neurosci. 73, 227–233. doi: 10.3109/00207459308986673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakata, S. (2016). State-dependent and cell type-specific temporal processing in auditory thalamocortical circuit. Sci. Rep. 6:18873. doi: 10.1038/srep18873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samaha, J., and Postle, B. R. (2015). The speed of alpha-band oscillations predicts the temporal resolution of visual perception. Curr. Biol. 25, 2985–2990. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandrini, M., and Cohen, L. G. (2013). Noninvasive brain stimulation in neurorehabilitation. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 116, 499–524. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-444-53497-2.00040-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Polizzotto, N. R., Godone, M., Giovannelli, F., Feurra, M., Matzen, L., et al. (2013). Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials. Curr. Biol. 23, 1449–1453. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, S., Mante, A., Rönnefarth, M., Fleischmann, R., Gall, C., and Brandt, S. A. (2013a). Progressive enhancement of alpha activity and visual function in patients with optic neuropathy: a two-week repeated session alternating current stimulation study. Brain Stimul. 6, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, S., Scholz, M., Obermayer, K., and Brandt, S. A. (2013b). Patterned brain stimulation, what a framework with rhythmic and noisy components might tell us about recovery maximization. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:325. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutter, D. J. (2016). Cutaneous retinal activation and neural entrainment in transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review. Neuroimage 140, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutter, D. J., and Hortensius, R. (2010). Retinal origin of phosphenes to transcranial alternating current stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1080–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwiedrzik, C. M. (2009). Retina or visual cortex? The site of phosphene induction by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 3:6. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.006.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siebner, H. R., Lang, N., Rizzo, V., Nitsche, M. A., Paulus, W., Lemon, R. N., et al. (2004). Preconditioning of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation with transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence for homeostatic plasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 3379–3385. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5316-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soekadar, S. R., Witkowski, M., Cossio, E. G., Birbaumer, N., Robinson, S. E., and Cohen, L. G. (2013). In vivo assessment of human brain oscillations during application of transcranial electric currents. Nat. Commun. 4:2032. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somers, D., and Kopell, N. (1993). Rapid synchronization through fast threshold modulation. Biol. Cybern. 68, 393–407. doi: 10.1007/bf00198772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaak, E., de Lange, F. P., and Jensen, O. (2014). Local entrainment of alpha oscillations by visual stimuli causes cyclic modulation of perception. J. Neurosci. 34, 3536–3544. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4385-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surwillo, W. W. (1961). Frequency of alpha rhythm, reaction time and age. Nature 191, 823–824. doi: 10.1038/191823a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talelli, P., and Rothwell, J. (2006). Does brain stimulation after stroke have a future? Curr. Opin. Neurol. 19, 543–550. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32801080d1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., and Miniussi, C. (2009). New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS-EEG studies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, Z. Y., Overton, P. G., and Clark, D. (1996). Stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the rat induces patterns of activity in midbrain dopaminergic neurons which resemble natural burst events. Synapse 22, 195–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199603)22:3<195::AID-SYN1>3.0.CO;2-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanRullen, R., and Koch, C. (2003). Is perception discrete or continuous? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 207–213. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00095-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossen, A., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2015). Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (α-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 8, 499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wach, C., Krause, V., Moliadze, V., Paulus, W., Schnitzler, A., and Pollok, B. (2013). The effect of 10 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on corticomuscular coherence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:511. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, A., Woolf, J. I., Halstead, W. C., and Case, T. J. (1944). Photic driving. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 52:117. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1944.02290320032004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiesenfeld, K., and Moss, F. (1995). Stochastic resonance and the benefits of noise: from ice ages to crayfish and SQUIDs. Nature 373, 33–36. doi: 10.1038/373033a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winfree, A. T. (2001). The Geometry of Biological Time. New York: Springer Science and Business Media.

Google Scholar

Yousif, N., and Denham, M. (2007). The role of cortical feedback in the generation of the temporal receptive field responses of lateral geniculate nucleus neurons: a computational modelling study. Biol. Cybern. 97, 269–277. doi: 10.1007/s00422-007-0171-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaehle, T., Rach, S., and Herrmann, C. S. (2010). Transcranial alternating current stimulation enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. PLoS One 5:e13766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaghi, S., Acar, M., Hultgren, B., Boggio, P. S., and Fregni, F. (2010). Noninvasive brain stimulation with low-intensity electrical currents: putative mechanisms of action for direct and alternating current stimulation. Neuroscientist 16, 285–307. doi: 10.1177/1073858409336227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoefel, B., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. Neuroimage 54, 1427–1431. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alternating current stimulation, neuromodulation, alpha rhythm, rebound, entrainment, phosphenes

Citation: Haberbosch L, Schmidt S, Jooss A, Köhn A, Kozarzewski L, Rönnefarth M, Scholz M and Brandt SA (2019) Rebound or Entrainment? The Influence of Alternating Current Stimulation on Individual Alpha. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:43. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00043

Received: 25 March 2018; Accepted: 25 January 2019;
Published: 12 February 2019.

Edited by:

Juliana Yordanova, Institute of Neurobiology (BAS), Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Victor Manuel Pulgar, Wake Forest School of Medicine, United States
Antonio Ivano Triggiani, University of Foggia, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Haberbosch, Schmidt, Jooss, Köhn, Kozarzewski, Rönnefarth, Scholz and Brandt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephan A. Brandt, c3RlcGhhbi5icmFuZHRAY2hhcml0ZS5kZQ==

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.