Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 September 2017
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience

Straightening the Eyes Doesn't Rebalance the Brain

\r\nJiawei Zhou*Jiawei Zhou1*Yonghua WangYonghua Wang1Lixia FengLixia Feng2Jiafeng WangJiafeng Wang2Robert F. HessRobert F. Hess3
  • 1School of Ophthalmology and Optometry and Eye Hospital, and State Key Laboratory of Ophthalmology, Optometry and Vision Science, Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, China
  • 2Department of Ophthalmology, First Affiliated Hospital, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
  • 3McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada

Surgery to align the two eyes is commonly used in treating strabismus. However, the role of strabismic surgery on patients' binocular visual processing is not yet fully understood. In this study, we asked two questions: (1) Does realigning the eyes by strabismic surgery produce an immediate benefit to patients' sensory eye balance? (2) If not, is there a subsequent period of “alignment adaptation” akin to refractive adaptation where sensory benefits to binocular function accrue? Seventeen patients with strabismus (mean age: 17.06 ± 5.16 years old) participated in our experiment. All participants had normal or corrected to normal visual acuity (LogMAR < 0.10) in the two eyes. We quantitatively measured their sensory eye balance before and after surgery using a binocular phase combination paradigm. For the seven patients whose sensory eye balance was measured before surgery, we found no significant change [t(6) = −0.92; p = 0.39] in the sensory eye balance measured 0.5–1 months after the surgery, indicating that the surgical re-alignment didn't by itself produce any immediate benefit for sensory eye balance. To answer the second question, we measured 16 patients' sensory eye balance at around 5–12 months after their eyes had been surgically re-aligned and compared this with our measurements 0.5–1 months after surgery. We found no significant change [t(15) = −0.89; p = 0.39] in sensory eye balance 5–12 months after the surgery. These results suggest that strabismic surgery while being necessary is not itself sufficient for re-establishing balanced sensory eye dominance.

Introduction

Ocular dominance, an eye preference in binocular viewing, commonly exists in humans (Chaurasia and Mathur, 1976). In clinical practice, ocular balance is normally determined by qualitative tests, e.g., the hole-in-the-card test (Dane and Dane, 2004), the Worth-4-dot test (Mustonen et al., 1993), which are convenient for clinical assessments, but not sufficient in providing a quantitative measure of the magnitude of asymmetry in binocular visual processing. In the recent decade, several studies have shown that the magnitude of asymmetry in different binocular visual processes, ranging from the binocular phase combination (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Huang et al., 2009), binocular orientation combination (Yehezkel et al., 2016), dichoptic motion coherence perception (Mansouri et al., 2008), dichoptic orientation coherence perception (Zhou et al., 2013a) to binocular rivalry (Ooi and He, 2001; Xu et al., 2011), can be quantitatively determined by the contrast difference of the visual inputs to achieve a binocular balanced status (i.e., the extent of binocular imbalance between the sensory inputs of each eye, or called “sensory eye balance”).

An illustration of the binocular phase combination task is provided in Figure 1, in which the two eyes are presented with two horizontal sine-wave gratings; the phase of the gratings in the two eyes are of same value but of opposite sign; the sensory eye balance is quantified by the interocular contrast ratio that is needed when the two eyes contribute equally to the binocular percept (i.e., when the binocular perceived phase is 0 degrees). Using this task, studies have shown that the sensory eye balance is abnormal in patients with amblyopia (Huang et al., 2009), anisometropia (Zhou et al., 2016), and strabismus (Kwon et al., 2014), which can be explained by a contrast-gain control theory in which the good eye suppresses the weak eye, thus unbalancing the monocular contributions to binocular processing (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Huang et al., 2010). Recently, we have also shown that the sensory eye balance remains abnormal in surgically corrected intermittent exotropes (Feng et al., 2015), indicating an abnormality of binocular processing in these patients. A related question that remains unanswered is, what is the effect of squint surgery on sensory eye balance? In other words, would straightening the eyes by itself lead to an improvement in sensory eye balance? Several previous studies have shown that in about 30–75% of cases binocularity and stereopsis are improved following successful surgical alignment, including adults with strabismus (Kushner and Morton, 1992; Morris et al., 1993; O'Neal et al., 1995; Yildirim et al., 1999; Lal and Holmes, 2002; Mets et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2007; Fatima et al., 2009; Dickmann et al., 2013). Sensory eye balance is an important binocular visual function and one would expect similar improvement to that referred to above for binocularity and stereopsis. However, there is a possibility that correcting the motor deficits may not be sufficient by itself to improve the binocular balance as reflected in the binocular phase combination task which has a cortical basis (Smith et al., 1997; Ding and Sperling, 2006; Huang et al., 2010). Besides, there is evidence that different binocular processes that share a similar interocular contrast-gain control stage may have separate pathways (Huang et al., 2010, 2011; Hou et al., 2013). It is possible that patients have deficits at different sites within the binocular pathway and this might explain inconsistencies in the effect of surgery on binocular function.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the binocular phase combination paradigm. Two horizontal sine-wave gratings with equal and opposite phase-shifts of 22.5° (relative to the center of the screen) were dichoptically presented to the two eyes through the polarized glasses. The perceived phase of the cyclopean grating depended on the internal weights given to the two inputs. Sensory eye balance was quantified by the interocular contrast difference that was needed to achieve a 0-degree of perceived phase, i.e., the balance point, where the two eyes were balanced in binocular combination.

To test this, we measured the sensory eye balance using the binocular phase combination in a group of non-amblyopic, strabismic patients before, 0.5–1 months after and 5–12 months after strabismic surgery. We found no consistent short-term or long-term change in the binocular balance after the eyes had been straightened, suggesting that strabismic surgery while being necessary is not sufficient to reinstate this normal binocular function.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventeen strabismic patients (mean age: 17.06 ± 5.16 years old) participated in our experiment. They were recruited from the department of Ophthalmology of the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University (Anhui, China). All participants had normal or corrected to normal visual acuity (LogMAR < 0.10) in the two eyes. Some of the patients had anisometropia or myopia; their refractive errors had been corrected with eyeglasses at least 16 weeks before they participated in this study. None of the patients had amblyopia, a previous history of ocular surgery or strabismus surgery, sensory or paralytic exotropia, had nystagmus or other conditions limiting ocular movements, neurologic disorders or any other medical problems, or had undergone any kinds of vision training or patching treatment. Patients were excluded from the study if they were not successfully surgically corrected (a successful surgical alignment was defined as an exotropia of no more than 10 prism diopters at both far and near distance using the prism cover test) or any vertical misalignment. The left eyes of patients S3, S8, S9, and S17 and the right eyes of the other patients were strabismic. Clinical details of patients before and after surgery are provided in Table 1. Individuals' sensory eye balance was assessed before and after the surgery at different time points. Observers wore their prescribed optical correction, if needed, in the data collection.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical details of the participants.

All subjects were naive as to the purpose of the experiment. A written informed consent was obtained from each of them or from the parents or legal guardian of participants aged less than 18 years old, after explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. This study complied with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Wenzhou Medical University, Anhui Medical University and McGill University.

Apparatus

All measurements were conducted on a PC computer running Matlab (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA) with PsychToolBox 3.0.9 extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). The stimuli were presented on a gamma-corrected LG D2342PY 3D LED screen (LG Life Science, Korea) with a 1,920 × 1,080 resolution and a 60 Hz refresh rate. Subjects viewed the display dichoptically with polarized glasses in a dimly lit room at a viewing distance of 136 cm. The background luminance was 46.2 cd/m2 on the screen and 18.8 cd/m2 through the polarized glasses. A chin-forehead rest was used to minimize head movements during the experiment. Prisms (no more than 50 prism diopters) were added for some observers in the pre-surgery measurement to enable patients to align the two eyes. This was conducted, base-in for exotropia and base-out for esotropia, by fixing the prism on a trial frame using tape.

Design

We quantitatively accessed patients' sensory eye balance before the surgery, 0.5–1 months after the surgery and 5–12 months after the surgery. Limited by the ability to align the two eyes, we were only able to measure the pre-surgery sensory eye balance in seven of the 17 patients. All patients were able to fuse the two eyes and thus were able to measure their sensory eye balance after the surgery.

A binocular phase combination paradigm (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Huang et al., 2009), which quantified the contributions of each eye to the fused binocular percept, was used for quantifying sensory eye balance. The design was the same as the one we have used in previous studies (Zhou et al., 2013a,b; Feng et al., 2015), in which observers were asked to dichoptically view two horizontal sine-wave gratings having equal and opposite phase-shifts of 22.5° (relative to the center of the screen) through polarized glasses; the perceived phase of the grating in the cyclopean percept was measured as a function of the interocular contrast ratio. By this method, we were able to find a specific interocular contrast ratio where the perceived phase of the cyclopean grating was 0° indicating equal weight to each eye's image. This specific interocular contrast ratio is the “balance point” for binocular phase combination since the two eyes under these stimulus conditions contribute equally to binocular vision (Figure 1). For each interocular contrast ratio, two configurations were used in the measurement so that any potential starting positional bias will be cancelled out: in one configuration, the phase-shift was +22.5° in the non-dominant eye and –22.5° in the dominant eye and in the other, the reverse. The perceived phase of the cyclopean grating at each interocular contrast ratio (δ) was quantified by half of the difference between the measured perceived phases in these two configurations. Different conditions (2 configurations × 6 interocular contrast ratios × 8 repetitions) were randomized in different trials, thus adaptation or expectation of the perceived phase would not have affected our results. The perceived phase and its standard error were calculated based on eight measurement repetitions.

Before the start of data collection, proper demonstration of the task was provided by practice trials to ensure observers understood the task. During the test, observers were allowed to take short-term breaks whenever they felt tired.

Stimuli

The gratings in the two eyes, as shown in Figure 1, were defined as:

LumnonDE(y)=L0[1-C0cos (2πfy±θ2)]    (1)
LumDE(y)=L0[1-δC0cos (2πfyθ2)]    (2)

Where L0 is the background luminance; C0 is the base contrast in the non-dominant eye; f is the spatial frequency of the gratings, δ is the interocular contrast ratio, and θ is the interocular phase difference.

In our test, L0 = 46.2 cd/m2 (on the screen); C0 = 100%; f = 1 cycle/°; δ = [0, 0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8, 1.0] and θ = 45°.

The gratings had a size of 2° × 2°. Surrounding the gratings, a high-contrast frame (width, 0.11°; length, 6°) with four white diagonal lines (width, 0.11°; length, 2.83°) was always presented during the test to help observers maintain fusion.

Procedure

We used the same phase adjustment procedure as used by Huang et al. (2009) for measuring the perceived phase of the binocularly combined grating. In each trial, subjects first completed an alignment task. During which, a fixation display was presented in the center of the binocular presented high-contrast frame with four white diagonal lines to the each eye. The fixation display also contained two monocular presented dots in the first and third quadrants for the left eye and two in the second and fourth quadrants for the right eye. They adjusted the coordinates of images in their nondominant eye to make sure the images seen by the two eyes were perfectly fused. This was followed by the binocular phase combination task. Observers were asked to adjust the position of a binocular horizontal reference line to indicate the perceived phase of the fused sine-wave grating. They did so by aligning the line with the location of the center of the dark stripe of the grating. The reference line was presented on both sides of the gratings, with its initial vertical position randomly (–9 to 10 pixels) assigned relative to the center of the frame in each trial. The reference line was moved with a fixed step size of 1 pixel. Since the gratings had a period of 2 cycles corresponding to 180 pixels, the phase adjustment had a step size of 4° of phase/pixel (2 cycles × 360 phase-degree/cycle / 180 pixels). During one trial, the stimuli were presented continually until subjects finished the phase adjustment task. The next trial started immediately after observers reported their results using a key press.

Curve Fits

The functions of perceived phase (φ) vs. interocular contrast ratios (δ) i.e., the PvR functions, were fitted with a modified contrast-gain control model from Huang et al. (2009):

φ=tan1[1(δ/bp )1+γ1+(δ/bp )1+γ · tan(θ2)]    (3)

In which, bp and γ are two free parameters. “bp” represents the interocular contrast ratio when the two eyes make equally contributions to binocular combination (i.e., the balance point) and “γ” represents a non-linear factor.

Curve fitting was conducted in Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA) using the nonlinear least squares method to minimized Σ(φtheory–φobserved)2. The goodness-of-fit was statistically tested by computing the r-square value:

r2=1-(φtheory-φobserved)2[φobserved-mean(φobserved)]2    (4)

Results

The Immediate Effect of Strabismic Surgery

Figure 2 shows the perceived phase vs. interocular contrast ratio (PvR) curves for seven patients, whose sensory eye balance could be measured before the surgery (other patients were not measureable because they couldn't align the two eyes during the test). For observer S3, her nondominant eye (determined by the hole-in-the-card test) was more dominant in the pre-surgery PvR test, to simplify the statistical analysis, S3's balance points were calculated as the reciprocal of the fitted balance point in the comparison. Accordingly, all the seven patients had imbalanced eyes before the surgery, as evidenced by the significant distance between the balance points (the zero-crossing point of the PVR curves) and 1.0 (i.e., the ideal observers' balance point): t(6) = –4.03, p = 0.007. At 0.5–1 months after the surgery, patients' eyes were more imbalanced (balance point decreased by 0.2 or more: S4 and S7), less imbalanced (balance point increased by 0.2 or more: S3, S5, and S6) or remained imbalanced to the same extent (balance point changed by less than 0.2: S1 and S2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The immediate effect of strabismic surgery on patients' sensory eye balance. Binocular perceived phase was measured at different interocular contrast ratios (dominant eye/non-dominant eye) before and 0.5–1 months after surgery. Seven observers who were able to fuse the two eyes before surgery participated in the before surgery measurements. Error bars are standard errors.

In Figure 3 we plot the balance points derived from the results shown in Figure 2 for the seven observers whose balance was measured before and immediately after strabismic surgery. The means and standard error are illustrated. A 2-tailed paired samples t-test showed that there was no significant change in the balance point as a consequence of surgery: t(6) = –0.92, p = 0.39.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Summary of the balance point changes before and immediately after strabismic surgery. The mean and standard errors are indicated and suggest the surgery does not in itself contribute to a more balanced ocular dominance.

The Long-Term Effect of Strabismic Surgery—“Surgical Adaptation”

The results in Figure 3 suggest that strabismic surgery doesn't produce any immediate benefit in terms of patients' sensory eye balance. One possibility is that a period of post-surgical “adaptation,” somewhat akin to refractive adaptation after an interocular refractive imbalance is first corrected (Zhou et al., 2016). To test this possibility, we evaluated the long-term effect of strabismic surgery on patients' sensory eye balance by measuring patients' PvR functions at 5–12 months after surgery. These results are plotted in Figure 4. Except for observers S12 and S16, whose balance point increased by more than 0.2 (two eyes became more balanced) at 5–12 months after the surgery, all other observers' balance points generally remained unaltered (balance point changed by less than 0.2) by surgery.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The long-term effect of strabismic surgery. Binocular perceived phase was measured at different interocular contrast ratios (dominant eye/non-dominant eye) at 5–12 months after surgery and compared to that at 0.5–1 months after surgery. Sixteen observers participated. Error bars are standard errors.

Figure 5 summarizes the range of balance points we measured in our strabismic patients immediately after surgery and how they changed 5–12 months after surgical straighting of the eyes. A 2-tailed paired samples t-test also showed that there was no significantly change of balance point after 5–12 months: t(15) = −0.89, p = 0.39. There was also no significant correlation between the interval between the two post-operation measure days and the change of balance point (p = 0.24, 2-tailed Pearson Correlation).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Summary of the balance point changes immediately after (0.5–1 months) strabismic surgery compared with at 5–12 months after the surgery. The mean and standard errors are indicated and suggest the surgery does not in itself contribute to a more balanced ocular dominance.

Discussion

We have shown previously (Zhou et al., 2016) that anisometropes have imbalanced ocular dominance as a consequence of an uncorrected refractive difference between the eyes and that this binocular balance anomaly, although unaffected by the initial optical correction, is nevertheless improved by a period of long-term refractive correction. We speculate that this may be the foundation for what is referred to as “refractive adaptation” in anisometropic amblyopia where visual acuity can improve solely as a consequence of long-term spectacle wear. In the present study we wanted to know whether there was a similar beneficial adaptation (i.e., surgical adaptation) for balanced binocular function after correction of an interocular eye misalignment. We quantified eye balance by measuring the contribution that each eye made to the binocularly fused percept using a standard psychophysical approach that is based on a well-accepted neural model of binocular combination (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Meese et al., 2006). By varying the interocular contrast we derived balance point measures that reflect the eye dominance. The results indicated that while strabismics could exhibit a range of eye dominances, the mean eye dominance for a group of observers did not change immediately (0.5–1 months) after surgery, nor did it change 5–12 months after surgery. In other words, we found no benefit of the surgery per se on binocular eye balance in the short or the long term, hence no evidence for “surgical adaptation” when it comes of eye balance.

Surgical re-alignment is clearly a first step in providing the conditions necessary for re-establishing full binocular function in strabismus. It has been shown that there can be benefits to binocularity including reduced suppression, better fusion and improvements in stereopsis in a significant percentage of cases (Kushner and Morton, 1992; Morris et al., 1993; O'Neal et al., 1995; Yildirim et al., 1999; Lal and Holmes, 2002; Mets et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2007; Fatima et al., 2009; Dickmann et al., 2013). However, surgical alignment per se does not seem to initiate any long-term changes in terms of rebalancing eye dominance in cases of strabismics without amblyopia. Thus, while some improvements can occur in a minority of cases (as detailed above) presumably even with a disorder ocular balance, we hypothesize that improvements will be more substantial and more frequent when any ocular dominance imbalance is first corrected. To test this it will be necessary for future studies to restore normal ocular balance after surgical correction by using active training approaches along the lines as that used in cases of amblyopia (Hess et al., 2010; To et al., 2011; Ooi et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014, 2015) and assess whether this leads to better binocular outcomes.

Author Contributions

JZ, LF, and RH designed research; LF and JW performed research; JZ and YW analyzed data; JZ and RH wrote the paper. All authors revised the paper and approved the version to be published.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China grant NSFC 81500754 and the Wenzhou Medical University grant QTJ16005 to JZ, the National Natural Science Foundation of China grant NSFC 81300796 to LF, and Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grants CCI-125686 and 228103, and an ERA-NET Neuron grant (JTC2015) to RH. The sponsor or funding organization had no role in the design or conduct of this research.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaurasia, B. D., and Mathur, B. B. (1976). Eyedness. Acta Anat. 96, 301–305. doi: 10.1159/000144681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dane, A., and Dane, S. (2004). Correlations among handedness, eyedness, monocular shifts from binocular focal point, and nonverbal intelligence in university mathematics students. Percept. Mot. Skills 99, 519–524. doi: 10.2466/pms.99.2.519-524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickmann, A., Aliberti, S., Rebecchi, M. T., Aprile, I., Salerni, A., Petroni, S., et al. (2013). Improved sensory status and quality-of-life measures in adult patients after strabismus surgery. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 17, 25–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2012.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, J., and Sperling, G. (2006). A gain-control theory of binocular combination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 1141–1146. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509629103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fatima, T., Amitava, A. K., Siddiqui, S., and Ashraf, M. (2009). Gains beyond cosmesis: recovery of fusion and stereopsis in adults with longstanding strabismus following successful surgical realignment. Indian J. Ophthalmol. 57, 141–143. doi: 10.4103/0301-4738.45505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, L., Zhou, J., Chen, L., and Hess, R. F. (2015). Sensory eye balance in surgically corrected intermittent exotropes with normal stereopsis. Sci. Rep. 5:13075. doi: 10.1038/srep13075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, R. F., Mansouri, B., and Thompson, B. (2010). A binocular approach to treating amblyopia: anti-suppression therapy. Optom. Vis. Sci. 87, 697–704. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181ea18e9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, F., Huang, C. B., Liang, J., Zhou, Y., and Lu, Z. L. (2013). Contrast gain-control in stereo depth and cyclopean contrast perception. J. Vis. 13, 1–19. doi: 10.1167/13.8.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. B., Zhou, J., Lu, Z. L., Feng, L., and Zhou, Y. (2009). Binocular combination in anisometropic amblyopia. J. Vis. 9, 11–16. doi: 10.1167/9.3.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. B., Zhou, J., Lu, Z. L., and Zhou, Y. (2011). Deficient binocular combination reveals mechanisms of anisometropic amblyopia:signal attenuation and interocular inhibition. J. Vis. 11, 1–17. doi: 10.1167/11.6.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. B., Zhou, J., Zhou, Y., and Lu, Z. L. (2010). Contrast and phase combination in binocular vision. PLoS ONE 5:e15075. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kushner, B. J., and Morton, G. V. (1992). Postoperative binocularity in adults with longstanding strabismus. Ophthalmology 99, 316–319. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(92)31970-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, M., Lu, Z. L., Miller, A., Kazlas, M., Hunter, D. G., and Bex, P. J. (2014). Assessing binocular interaction in amblyopia and its clinical feasibility. PLoS ONE 9:e100156. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lal, G., and Holmes, J. M. (2002). Postoperative stereoacuity following realignment for chronic acquired strabismus in adults. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 6, 233–237. doi: 10.1067/mpa.2002.123399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. L., Jost, R. M., Morale, S. E., Stager, D. R., Dao, L., Stager, D., et al. (2014). A binocular iPad treatment for amblyopic children. Eye 28, 1246–1253. doi: 10.1038/eye.2014.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. L., Reynaud, A., Hess, R. F., Wang, Y.-Z., Jost, R. M., Morale, S. E., et al. (2015). Dichoptic movie viewing treats childhood amblyopia. J. AAPOS 19, 401–405. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansouri, B., Thompson, B., and Hess, R. F. (2008). Measurement of suprathreshold binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res. 48, 2775–2784. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meese, T. S., Georgeson, M. A., and Baker, D. H. (2006). Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. J. Vis. 6, 1224–1243. doi: 10.1167/6.11.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mets, M. B., Beauchamp, C., and Haldi, B. A. (2004). Binocularity following surgical correction of strabismus in adults. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 8, 435–438. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2004.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R. J., Scott, W. E., and Dickey, C. F. (1993). Fusion after surgical alignment of longstanding strabismus in adults. Ophthalmology 100, 135–138. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(93)31703-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, A. D. N., Orpen, J., and Calcutt, C. (2007). Changes in the functional binocular status of older children and adults with previously untreated infantile esotropia following late surgical realignment. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 11, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2006.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mustonen, J., Rovamo, J., and Nasanen, R. (1993). The effects of grating area and spatial frequency on contrast sensitivity as a function of light level. Vision Res. 33, 2065–2072. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90005-H

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'neal, T. D., Rosenbaum, A. L., and Stathacopoulos, R. A. (1995). Distance stereo acuity improvement in intermittent exotropic patients following strabismus surgery. J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 32, 353–357; discussion 358.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ooi, T. L., and He, Z. J. (2001). Sensory eye dominance. Optometry 72, 168–178.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ooi, T. L., Su, Y. R., Natale, D. M., and He, Z. J. (2013). A push-pull treatment for strengthening the ‘lazy eye’ in amblyopia. Curr. Biol. 23, R309–R310. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelli, D. G. (1997). The Video toolbox software for visual psychophysics:transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. L. III., Chino, Y. M., Ni, J., Ridder, W. H. III., and Crawford, M. L. (1997). Binocular spatial phase tuning characteristics of neurons in the macaque striate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 351–365.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

To, L., Thompson, B., Blum, J., Maehara, G., Hess, R. F., and Cooperstock, J. (2011). A game platform for treatment of amblyopia. IEEE Trans. Neural. Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 19, 280–289. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2011.2115255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J. P., He, Z. J., and Ooi, T. L. (2011). A binocular perimetry study of the causes and implications of sensory eye dominance. Vision Res. 51, 2386–2397. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehezkel, O., Ding, J., Sterkin, A., Polat, U., and Levi, D. M. (2016). Binocular combination of stimulus orientation. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3:160534. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yildirim, C., Mutlu, F. M., Chen, Y., and Altinsoy, H. I. (1999). Assessment of central and peripheral fusion and near and distance stereoacuity in intermittent exotropic patients before and after strabismus surgery. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 128, 222–230. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9394(99)00079-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Feng, L., Lin, H., and Hess, R. F. (2016). On the maintenance of normal ocular dominance and a possible mechanism underlying refractive adaptation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 57, 5181–5185. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-19696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Huang, P.-C., and Hess, R. F. (2013a). Interocular suppression in amblyopia for global orientation processing. J. Vis. 13, 11–14. doi: 10.1167/13.5.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Jia, W., Huang, C.-B., and Hess, R. F. (2013b). The effect of unilateral mean luminance on binocular combination in normal and amblyopic vision. Sci. Rep. 3:2012. doi: 10.1038/srep02012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sensory eye balance, contrast-gain-control, binocular vision, strabismic surgery, interocular suppression

Citation: Zhou J, Wang Y, Feng L, Wang J and Hess RF (2017) Straightening the Eyes Doesn't Rebalance the Brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:453. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00453

Received: 22 May 2017; Accepted: 28 August 2017;
Published: 12 September 2017.

Edited by:

Michael A. Silver, University of California, Berkeley, United States

Reviewed by:

Stefan Pollmann, Otto von Guericke University of Magdeburg, Germany
Teng Leng Ooi, The Ohio State University Columbus, United States

Copyright © 2017 Zhou, Wang, Feng, Wang and Hess. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jiawei Zhou, zhoujw@mail.eye.ac.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.