Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 October 2016
Sec. Speech and Language
This article is part of the Research Topic The Evolution of Rhythm Cognition: Timing in Music and Speech View all 34 articles

“It Don’t Mean a Thing if It Ain’t Got that Swing”– an Alternative Concept for Understanding the Evolution of Dance and Music in Human Beings

  • Institute of Tropical Medicine and International Health, Charité Universitätsmedizin, Berlin, Germany

The functions of dance and music in human evolution are a mystery. Current research on the evolution of music has mainly focused on its melodic attribute which would have evolved alongside (proto-)language. Instead, we propose an alternative conceptual framework which focuses on the co-evolution of rhythm and dance (R&D) as intertwined aspects of a multimodal phenomenon characterized by the unity of action and perception. Reviewing the current literature from this viewpoint we propose the hypothesis that R&D have co-evolved long before other musical attributes and (proto-)language. Our view is supported by increasing experimental evidence particularly in infants and children: beat is perceived and anticipated already by newborns and rhythm perception depends on body movement. Infants and toddlers spontaneously move to a rhythm irrespective of their cultural background. The impulse to dance may have been prepared by the susceptibility of infants to be soothed by rocking. Conceivable evolutionary functions of R&D include sexual attraction and transmission of mating signals. Social functions include bonding, synchronization of many individuals, appeasement of hostile individuals, and pre- and extra-verbal communication enabling embodied individual and collective memorizing. In many cultures R&D are used for entering trance, a base for shamanism and early religions. Individual benefits of R&D include improvement of body coordination, as well as painkilling, anti-depressive, and anti-boredom effects. Rhythm most likely paved the way for human speech as supported by studies confirming the overlaps between cognitive and neural resources recruited for language and rhythm. In addition, dance encompasses visual and gestural communication. In future studies attention should be paid to which attribute of music is focused on and that the close mutual relation between R&D is taken into account. The possible evolutionary functions of dance deserve more attention.

The Evolution of Dance and Music, Current Concepts

The origin of dance and music, beautiful and powerful universals of humankind is a mystery. All over the world there are myths on how humankind received dance and music. In Hindu mythology, the god Shiva Nataraj created the world by dancing. In most traditional cultures dance plays a pivotal role (Métraux, 1958; Lapassade, 1976; Verger, 1982; Ginn, 1990; Christoph and Oberländer, 1996; Foix, 2007; Kouam and Mofor, 2011).

In the Western world attention has been usually paid to the origin of language and to its relation to the melodic attribute of music whereas dance and rhythm have been, for long, neglected. This might partly be an unintentional consequence of the duality of body and mind concept of Cartesian philosophy as well as the historical hostility of the Roman Catholic and Protestant Churches toward dance. This had led to the omission of percussion instruments in European classical music, thus, diverting the first attention from rhythm and dance to melody (Redmond, 1997; Wagner, 1997; Foix, 2007).

The evolution of music has become an important research topic rather recently (Falk, 2000; Cross, 2005; McDermott and Hauser, 2005; Mithen, 2007; Cross and Morley, 2008; Dissanayake, 2008; Patel, 2008; Patel et al., 2008; Tomasello, 2008; Panksepp, 2009; Merker et al., 2015).

The evolution of music has become an important research topic rather recently. The interest is partly awakened by neuroscience, basically to identify the core components of human cognition (Oberzaucher and Grammer, 2008; Thaut et al., 2008; Costa-Faidella et al., 2011; Nozaradan et al., 2011a,b, 2012; Alluri et al., 2012; Arnal and Giraud, 2012; De Guio et al., 2012; Teki et al., 2012; Leman and Maes, 2014) in comparison to animals (Fitch, 2006, 2012, 2013; Ravignani et al., 2013, 2014). The evolutionary role of dance is even more enigmatic than that of music considering that who dances dispenses considerably more energy than a singer or a musician. The evolutionary functions of dance have received more attention only recently (Dean et al., 2009; Hanna, 2010; Whitehead, 2010; Grammer et al., 2011; Neave et al., 2011; Davidson and Emberly, 2012; Fitch, 2012; Christensen et al., 2014; Morley, 2014; Woolhouse and Lai, 2014; Wang, 2015).

Definition of Dance

Neither the term dance nor the term music as such are precise. “Dance” in Oxford’s dictionary is defined as: “move rhythmically to music, typically following a set sequence of steps.” For our purpose we define as “dance” body movements coordinated to a basic rhythm. Rhythm is constituted by a pulse or sequence of beats which are organized hierarchically. There are four main sub-constituent elements of rhythm: (1) tactus represents identical short-duration periods subdivided into strong beats (“downbeats”) and weaker beats (“offbeats”); (2) tempo: the frequency of the tactus; (3) meter: cyclical groupings of beats into units marked by accents; (4) patterns: sequences of time intervals that may or may not extend across meter units (Fitch, 2013; Thaut et al., 2014). Dance differs from simple synchronization to a simple regular pulse, because dance offers the possibility to vary steps with respect to beats inside the tactus. Nevertheless, the dancer has to respect a basic groove (Janata et al., 2012; Fitch, 2013; Oota, 2016). We would therefore not consider soldiers marching or harvesters working in synchrony with a beat as dancers: they do not differentiate down- and offbeats of a rhythm and they have a defined purpose. We also see modern non-rhythmic expressive dance as theater rather than dance. We would also not describe as dance the repeated steps without keeping a regular basic pulse as described for some birds (Ota et al., 2015).

On the other hand, we extend our definition of dance to the beat-keeping movements of music performers. Embodied perception is the physiological fundament of this phenomenon. Unintentional body movements to a beat reflect the role of our body in rhythm perception. For dancing, the capacity of beat anticipation and of embodied rhythm perception are required (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005; Honing, 2012; Bouwer et al., 2014).

Conceptions of Music and Theories on the Origin of Music

“Music” and “dance” encompass overlapping spectral and temporal attributes. Spectral attributes are pitch, intervals, and harmony. Temporal attributes are covered by rhythm consisting in its sub-constituent elements tactus, meter, tempo, and pattern (Fitch, 2013; Janata and Parsons, 2013; Thaut et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2015; Table 1). Loudness and dynamics are, albeit important, not specific for music since these are also expressive means of other arts such as theater, poetry, rhetoric or cinema. For music we discern its three main specific attributes: rhythm, melody, and harmony. Rhythm is music’s central organizing structure. Rhythm is indispensable for both, dance, and music (Honing, 2012; Thaut et al., 2014). Whereas, rhythm can exist without melody or harmony, melody or harmony cannot exist without rhythm. The concepts of melody and harmony are partly defined by their temporal rhythmic fundament: melody is defined as a series of sounds with a different pitch over time. Harmony must be subdivided into “sequential harmony” which means the defined pitch intervals in a melodic time line and “polyphony” which means simultaneous sounds with different pitches following different melodic lines. Authors of publications on the evolution of music, usually do not mention which of these attributes are precisely meant. Usually, only the melodic and to a lesser extent the harmonic attribute of music have been focused on. On the other hand, rhythm has been relatively neglected. Also in the past, scholars who reflected on the origin of music referred to its melodic attribute. The philosopher Rousseau (1781) argued that ancestral humans would have used a proto-musilanguage and that people would have communicated by singing. The German poet Heine (1822) interpreted melodic music as a precursor of language. Darwin (1871, p. 572) in “The descent of man” noted: “as neither the enjoyment nor the capacity of producing musical notes are faculties of the least use to man in reference to his daily habits of life, they must be ranked amongst the most mysterious with which he is endowed.”

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Relations and overlap between dance and music attributes.

Most anthropological, psychological, and musicological references focus on the evolution of melodic music but not of rhythm nor of dance. A favorite theory on the evolution of music has been that it would have evolved just as a by-product of language evolution (Pinker, 1997; Jourdain, 2001; Brown and Jordania, 2013).

Current Hypotheses on the Evolution of Dance

Would also dance constitute a mere by-product of language evolution? It has been proposed that the capacity to move in time with an auditory pulse, i.e., entrainment would have evolved as a by-product of vocal mimicry (Schachner, 2013). We believe that R&D are not mere evolutionary by-products of language. Scientific studies showed that infants are able to extract and anticipate a rhythmical pulse and that they have a strong impulse to move spontaneously when exposed to an external rhythm (Hannon and Johnson, 2005; Hannon and Trehub, 2005; Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005; Winkler et al., 2009; Fujii et al., 2014). We hypothesize that R&D are part of an inborn series of physiological reflexes universal in all humans. The observation that children at a certain age inevitably dance when they are exposed to a rhythm has been supported by a cross-cultural questionnaire study conducted in three continents by our group (Ostovar, 2016). If such a reflex was constant and genetically determined, it could join the list of physiological reflexes used in developmental psychology and pediatric neurology. The confirmation of the existence of such an innate reflex would also support the concept that dance have had a considerable importance in the evolution of humankind.

This prompted us to perform a literature search in the fields of medicine and developmental psychology, philosophy, archeology, anthropology, ethnology, and musicology focusing on the evolution of dance in both, children and in humankind. We were surprised about the scarcity of references. Neither in textbooks of pediatrics nor of developmental psychology a physiological dance reflex or dance reaction to a beat occurring during infantile development is mentioned (Ringwalt, 2008; Berk, 2014). A PubMed search on “dance reflex” yielded a total of 22 hits, of which none dealt with our topic.

Music and Dance as Pre-verbal Communication Tools

What could be the evolutionary functions of rhythm and dance? One has to bear in mind that people who dance, stomp, and clap hands are noisy and less aware of predators (Hart and Sussmann, 2009). Such a behavior must not, at least, have had survival disadvantages if it was to be conserved in evolution.

Donald (2001) formulated the theory that long before the evolution of language humans were communicating by extra-verbal means which he called mimesis. Mimesis can be imagined as how we communicate in a foreign country without any knowledge of the local language. This theory was later taken up by Mithen (2007) who called extra-verbal communication the “hmmmm”-communication (hmmmm stands for holistic multi-modal manipulative musical). In this context Mithen (2007) argued that music would have been a means of pre-verbal communication, calling his book “The Singing Neanderthals.” This title reflects the wide-spread opinion that melody is the main attribute which characterizes music.

Since we believe that rhythm is the fundament of music we would propose for the next book the title “The Swinging Neanderthals.” Masataka (2009) sustains the view that both language and music evolved from a pre-linguistic communication system which was neither language nor music. Similarly, Brown (2007) proposes that music has evolved from a pre-linguistic precursor present also in animals which he called “contagious heterophony.” An alternative account is proposed by Livingstone and Thompson (2009) where music originated from a more general adaptation known as the “Theory of Mind” which would allow an individual to recognize the mental and emotional state of conspecifics. Underpinned by the mirror neuron system of empathy and imitation, music would achieve engagement by drawing from pre-existing functions across multiple modalities (Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Wu et al., 2016). This, in our opinion, applies even more to dance because of its strong interaction between perception and motor response. Whitehead (2010) puts forward the hypothesis that song and dance would have even preceded mimesis in hominid evolution. Since most authors looked for the origin of music in melody and singing, some of them interpreted as their precursors the interaction between pre-musical utterances of the infant by modulation of crying and melodic modulation of language by the caretaker, also called “motherese.” The origins of “motherese” would already be established during prenatal development of the fetus (Parncutt, 2009). “Motherese” has been proposed as proto-melody paving the way for the evolution of music. Rhythm in this respect has not been considered (Dissanayake, 2004; Falk, 2005; Hagen and Hammerstein, 2009; Panksepp, 2009; Wermke and Mende, 2009; Trevarthen, 2011; Brown and Jordania, 2013). Furthermore, although all mothers of all cultures and all ages know that babies are soothed by being rocked, this is, if ever, only exceptionally mentioned and has also never been proposed as a human universal (Brown, 1991; Antweiler, 2015).

Summarizing, the functions of R&D as pre- and powerful extraverbal communication tools have found little attention.

Defining A Differentiated Framework of Music and Dance

For defining the evolutionary functions of music a more fine-grained concept is required (Hagen and Bryant, 2003; Fitch, 2006; Levitin, 2013). Dance is usually conceived as being distinct from music. The close relationship between rhythm and dance has been acknowledged only recently (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005; Dean et al., 2009; Phillips-Silver et al., 2010; Janata et al., 2012; Phillips-Silver and Keller, 2012; Stevens, 2012; Morley, 2014). The pivotal importance of rhythm, synchronized body movements and dancing as prerequisites for the emergence of all attributes of music has found relatively little attention. Whereas, rhythm and dance can exist without melody, there is no music without rhythm (Honing, 2012). Music has also been considered for a long time from the viewpoint of a unimodal phenomenon (auditory processing), whereas, it is multimodal (through action–perception coupling; Lesaffre and Leman, 2013). There is no reason to assume that the different attributes of music evolved at the same time and pace. Only two attributes of music constitute human universals: all cultures have rhythm and almost all have melody (songs; Table 1). Polyphonic harmony has developed only some 1000s of years ago and has been explored only since historic times starting in Ancient Greece (Jordania, 2005). Among the attributes of melody and sequential harmony only the octave, the perfect fifth and building a melodic scale by the division of the octave into unequal intervals are human universals [Nettl, 2000; Savage et al., 2015 (see also Table 1)]. Many cultures such as in the Arabic world, in Turkey, in Persia or in India, have, instead of polyphony, pursued to evolve melody by adding particular intervals and bending melody sounds in a sequential harmonic way (modal music).

On the other hand, in most cultures, worldwide, the concept and terms of R&D are not separated from each other (e.g., the terms “Samba,” “Salsa,” “Guaguancó,” “Tango,” “Waltz” apply to both, to the rhythm and to the dance).

In line with this, we propose an alternative conceptual framework which focuses on the mutual co-evolution of R&D (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005; Panksepp, 2009; Lesaffre and Leman, 2013; Levitin, 2013; Maes and Leman, 2013). In other words, R&D are two sides of the same coin. Not only do we move to what we hear but what we hear depends on how we move (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005). The unintentional body movements when we perceive a “groove” (defined as the aspect of music which compels us to move), among all body parts primarily involve the lower limbs confirming the close relation to dance (Janata et al., 2012). The mutual connection between R&D is also reflected by the musical terms “downbeat” and “offbeat,” where the downbeat indicates the dance step which carries the weight of the body when it comes back down onto the legs.

Movement, rhythm and emotional well-being have particular neural pathways involving cerebellar structures which coordinate sensory neuronal inputs with motoric responses (Molinari et al., 2003; Levitin, 2006; Thaut et al., 2008; Lehmann, 2010; Nozaradan et al., 2011a,b, 2012; Grahn, 2012; Teki et al., 2012; see the comprehensive reviews of Levitin, 2013; Repp and Su, 2013; Chauvigné et al., 2014). Even deaf children have the impulse and the capacity to dance by perceiving the beat through their cutaneous pallesthesic and visual receptors (Phillips-Silver et al., 2015).

There are relatively few references on the specific evolutionary functions of rhythm in humans, mainly in comparison with non-human animals (Honing, 2012; Fitch, 2013; Ravignani et al., 2013, 2014). Rhythm cognition depends on body movement and vice versa (Lesaffre and Leman, 2013; Christensen et al., 2014). Fitch (2013) emphasizes that necessary prerequisites of research on rhythm are unambiguous definitions of the terms “rhythm” and its sub-constituent elements “beat,” “pulse,” “meter,” “tempo,” and “tactus” (Table 1). Dancing requires the perception of such hierarchically structured rhythms in order to coordinate and differentiate those steps which carry the whole bodyweight (usually the downbeats) from steps carrying less or no weight (offbeats). Sequences of arbitrary pulses are therefore spontaneously classified into a rhythmical tactus by the dancer, a capacity observed already in infants (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005).

For people of traditional societies the importance of dance and the tight connection between R&D are beyond any doubt (Ginn, 1990; Morley, 2014). In societies with strong dance traditions, singing out of tone is tolerated more easily than drumming only slightly out of beat. This is what the composer Duke Ellington meant with his song: “it don’t mean a thing if it ain’t got that swing.” Instead, passive listening to music without moving, as seen in listeners to Western classical music, requires an educational effort, as it can easily be seen when children are obliged to sit still in a concert. In fact, spontaneous unintentional body movements when listening to a rhythmical beat are difficult to suppress (Molinari et al., 2003; Levitin, 2013).

Moreover, dance is a comprehensive art encompassing attributes which go beyond music such as its external visual signals. The dancer is seen by others acting as a moving picture (Hanna, 2010; Lee et al., 2015). This also applies to a widespread and very old form of dance, i.e., round dance, where the group dances and different dancers enter into the round to perform their solo before rejoining the round as it is also commonly observed in children. Furthermore, dance may encompass gestural and dramatic codes and, thus, has paved the way for the development of theater. The comprehensiveness of dance made already Curt Sachs argue that dance would be the mother of all arts (Sachs, 1933/1980).

Connecting Studies in Various Scientific Disciplines with Hypotheses on the Evolution of Dance and Music in Humankind

Evidence of Dance and Music in Archeological Records

When we look for archeological proof for music and dance we must bear in mind that archeology depends on the finding of artifacts indicating the presence of a certain human behavior at a certain time. The earliest artifacts confirming musical activities are around 45,000 years old. Examples are preserved instruments such as flutes in the neolithic caves of “Hohle Fels” and “Geissenklösterle” (Higham et al., 2012). Before, we assume that music and dance did not develop earlier than this, we must acknowledge that in a hunter-trapper-gatherer society not only any artifact constitutes an additional weight to carry but not to leave one’s traces also constitutes part of the survival strategy. Moreover, many instruments are natural objects, such as conch shells, or are made of perishable materials, such as wood and animal skins, which are not preserved for long (Ginn, 1990). Furthermore, humans always possessed a versatile instrument without the necessity of producing a musical instrument, i.e., their own body (Barbagiovanni, 2006). Some examples of human behavior appear so omnipresent and obvious to us that it has, in fact, never been investigated if these are universal in all human societies. One of these is accompanying a beat by clapping hands, a behavior which is observable in humans of all ages (Brown, 1991; Antweiler, 2015; Savage et al., 2015). Another actual example of body percussion and dance is “Flamenco” which in its original form was performed only by singing, clapping hands and stepping on the ground without the use of any musical instruments (Caballero, 1992). This practice is also illustrated by the “Akonhoun” dance tradition of Benin, where dancers perform percussion on their body while dancing as well as “Schuhplattler” in German folk music. To support our view, one may consider in analogy the development of painting, where the human body was the first canvas as confirmed by ca. 200,000-year-old red ochre findings in several places where Neanderthals were living (Wrescher, 1980; Roebroeks et al., 2011). Cave paintings and sculptures confirming dancing are relatively recent. The oldest cave paint possibly representing a dancer is the around 35,000-year-old “magician” (French: “sorcier”) of the “trois frères cave” in Southern France, a zoomorphic figure with animal and human characteristics. Such mask dancing is still practiced in traditional societies aiming at being possessed by an animal spirit (Ginn, 1990; Christoph and Oberländer, 1996). Unequivocal dancing scenes are represented in paintings in the “Valcamonica” and “Addaura” caves in Italy which are no older than 10,000 years (Anati, 1995).

Studies on Infants and Children

Studies on infants, toddlers, and children contribute to elucidate the evolution of dance and music. The study group of Henkjan Honing showed in a very elegant experiment that newborns already perceive and anticipate musical pulses, a phenomenon which was called “beat induction” (Winkler et al., 2009). Beat processing has been shown to be pre-attentive for metrically simple rhythms with clear accents (Bouwer et al., 2014). Three- to four-month-old infants demonstrate spontaneous limb movements coordinated to a musical pulse (Fujii et al., 2014). Furthermore, infants are able to stratify musical pulses into meters (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005). This capacity is linked with body movement. Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies and learn more readily to tune into musical rhythms than adults (Hannon and Johnson, 2005; Hannon and Trehub, 2005). Human infants spontaneously engage in significantly more rhythmic movement to music and other rhythmically regular sounds than to language (Zentner and Eerola, 2010). The precocity of beat induction already being observed in newborns, the ability to stratify musical beats into tactus and meters, the efficacy of rocking, the unintentional movements to rhythm, the pre-attentive characteristic of beat processing, and the intensity of the emotional impact of R&D on humans, support our view that in human evolution communication through R&D preceded verbal and melodic communication (Bergeson and Trehub, 2005; Winkler et al., 2009; Hagen et al., 2010; Whitehead, 2010; Nozaradan et al., 2011a,b, 2012; Honing, 2012; Lesaffre and Leman, 2013; van Noorden, 2013; Bouwer et al., 2014; Leman and Maes, 2014; Morley, 2014; Ravignani et al., 2014; Miura et al., 2016). Not only environmental but also genetic factors have been shown to play a role in our ability to perceive rhythm (Seesjärvi et al., 2015).

What Could the Evolutionary Functions of Rhythm and Dance Be?

With this differently defined framework one may reformulate the question “what was the function of music in human evolution?” in a more particularized way: “what was the function of rhythm and dance?”

Did this module pave the way for further evolution of other attributes of music and of language? Future studies may elucidate to what extent R&D are exclusively human or to which capacities in this respect non-human animals are capable. Also some birds and a captive sea lion are able to anticipate beat and move to it to some extent, but apparently animals do not subdivide rhythm into more and less accentuated beats (Patel, 2008; Patel et al., 2008, 2009; Kirschner and Tomasello, 2009; Cook et al., 2013; Ravignani et al., 2013, 2014). Chimpanzees display spontaneous rhythmical behaviors (drumming and carnival display; Arcadi et al., 1998, 2004; Fitch, 2006; de Waal, 2013; Ravignani et al., 2013) and an experiment has shown that a trained chimpanzee was able to anticipate a rhythmical beat (Hattori et al., 2014). Dancing, however, contrary to marching in lockstep or synchronized working to a beat, requires a sophisticated hierarchical perception of rhythm (Fitch, 2013). Therefore it is, in our opinion, very unlikely that dancing simply constitutes a mere by-product of entrainment. What are R&D in humans good for? Why is the impulse to dance so powerful? What could be the evolutionary functions of R&D?

Reproductive Fitness and Sexual Attractiveness

Contrary to other scholars of his time, Darwin (1871, p. 880) had also the rhythmic aspect of music in mind as well as its reproductive fitness advantage, as he wrote “we may assume that musical tones and rhythm were used by our half-human ancestors, during the season of courtship.” “Dancing is the vertical expression of a horizontal desire” a quote attributed to Robert Frost to which George Bernard Shaw added: “legalized by music,” confirms this observation in poetry. Sexual attractiveness has been since long hypothesized to be the main evolutionary function of dance but has become a scientific research focus only recently (Dissanayake, 2008; Oberzaucher and Grammer, 2008; Dean et al., 2009; Hanna, 2010; Grammer et al., 2011). Rhythmicity has been proposed as an indicator of mate quality (van den Broek and Todd, 2009). Furthermore, dancers are able to communicate subtle non-verbal signals (Oberzaucher and Grammer, 2008; Hanna, 2010; Grammer et al., 2011). The Latin-derived French word “emotion” does not by mere chance contain the word “motion” (Harper, 2001–2016). R&D move us profoundly. Motion alone can effectively communicate emotion, charisma and sex appeal (Oberzaucher and Grammer, 2008).

To say it with the words of Hanna (2010, p. 2): “Dance and sex both use the same instrument — namely, the human body — and both involve the language of the body’s orientation toward pleasure. Thus, dance and sex may be conceived as inseparable even when sexual expression is unintended. The physicality of dance imbued with “magical” power to enchant performer and observer, threatens some people (Wagner, 1997; Karayanni, 2005; Shay and Sellers-Young, 2005). The dancing body is symbolic expression that may embody many notions. Among these are romance, desire, and sexual climax.”

Movement quality not only seems to indicate mate quality, but also the interest of a potential partner, which could denote the probability of successful mating (Grammer et al., 2011; Neave et al., 2011).

Social Fitness

Synchronization of Many Individuals

Synchronization is a behavior not limited to humans (Ravignani et al., 2014). It may have a direct effect on predators or reflect the general advantages of cooperation via positive social interactions, a finding also observed in macaques (Nagasaka et al., 2013). Rhythm enables the synchronization of 1000s of dancing human beings such as in a rock concert (Canetti, 1960). The dynamics of rhythmic synchronization differ fundamentally from that of a swarm: a swarm is coordinated by an energy wave passing very quickly but consecutively through many individuals sensing the movement of the adjacent individual. This confounds a predator on which individual prey to catch. Humans, presenting the simultaneous movement of a stomping crowd screaming and armed with fire, may delude a predator by producing the impression of being a homogeneous enormous animal which would be too powerful to attack. This effect may be taken advantage of also in hunting battues (Hagen and Bryant, 2003; Bispham, 2006; Trevarthen, 2011; Phillips-Silver and Keller, 2012; Repp and Su, 2013).

It has recently been argued that self-generated sounds of locomotion and ventilation interfere with the perception of the surroundings. The synchronization of the movement of a number of individuals would thus increase the duration of the intervals where the surroundings can be heard better (Larsson, 2014). This means that synchronization would constitute a by-product of hunting abilities. To prove this hypothesis one would expect that traditional hunters follow animals to hunt in a kind of lockstep, an observation that has not been provided so far.

Social Bonding

In humans, the synchronic movement leads to “muscular bonding” which enables to overcome emotional boundaries between individuals and, thus, strengthens the community (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009).

A pivotal fitness strategy of hominids is cooperation (Nowak, 2006; Nowak et al., 2010). Drumming and dancing are profoundly social activities (van Noorden, 2013). Some of the countless examples of this way of social engagement are “Samba de Roda,” “Flamenco,” and Senegalese “Sabar,” where the audience supports the musicians and dancers rhythmically, members of the audience enter into the round for dancing and the drummers and/or other musicians interact directly with the dancer. In many musical cultures the dancer is a percussionist at the same time, as it may be observed not only in traditional societies but also on ancient Egyptian and Greek frescoes or in actual tap-dancing (Caballero, 1992; Redmond, 1997). In many societies dancing is an integral part of important group ceremonies such as initiation rites or weddings. In hunter-gatherer societies, groups may be limited to 40–50 people. The future spouse has to leave her or his group after the wedding in order to join the partner’s group. By dancing, future spouses demonstrate their ability, strength and elegance not only to the future partner but also to other members of the group which will admit the spouse as a new member. In other words, promised spouses need also to catch the eyes of the mothers and fathers in law or other group members who have a say. This latter aspect has, to our knowledge, not yet been explored. Bonding by R&D strengthens the community. Musicians delight dancers. They offer the fundament for the joy of the dancers. This profoundly emotional type of embodied extra-verbal communication increases the group’s cohesion and the identification with the group (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2009, 2010; Boer et al., 2011, 2012, 2013; Davidson and Emberly, 2012; Boer and Abubakar, 2014; Kirschner and Ilari, 2014). Music and especially rhythm constitute a deeply rooted signaling system for extra-verbal communication evoking emotional reactions of other potentially cooperating individuals (Bryant, 2013). The propensity to move in time to rhythmic percussive sounds is manifest from an early age on, as seen in children’s impulsive body movement in response to music (Zentner and Eerola, 2010). Joint drumming facilitates the synchronization in preschool children (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2009). Interpersonal synchrony increases helpfulness already in 14 month-old toddlers and the promotion of prosocial behavior by interpersonal rhythmic synchrony has been confirmed in cross-cultural studies in 4-year-old children as compared to matched controls (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010; Cirelli et al., 2014; Kirschner and Ilari, 2014; Trainor and Cirelli, 2015). R&D are also potent collective mood synchronizers (Hagen and Bryant, 2003; Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; van Noorden, 2013). The emotional impact of the synchronization of many individuals in military drill has impressively been described by McNeill (1995).

Keeping Peace

There is evidence of intra- and intergroup aggression in primates such as chimpanzees (de Waal, 2000), and hominids (Kelly, 2000; Zollikofer et al., 2002; Kelly, 2005). Hominids possessed spears for more than 400,000 years (Thieme, 1997). The advent of tools of potential use as weapons among hominids required even more effective reconciliation means (Wilkins et al., 2012).

To say it with the words of Zollikofer et al. (2002, p. 6447): “The intentional use of implements in the context of intragroup conflict must have had a major impact during hominid evolution because the availability of highly effective hunting and or food-processing tools in interpersonal conflict created a new and considerable potential for intragroup damage, a potential that required specific behavioral adjustments with which to cope. Intragroup aggression in primate societies must be understood as one specific behavioral option in a complex network of social interactions, which is typically balanced by active reconciliatory behavior […].”

This ability is confirmed by the relative scarceness of traces of violence in prehistoric bone findings as compared to skeletons from historic times (Haas and Piscitelli, 2013). Dancing as an effective reconciliatory means has been well-described among potentially hostile Andaman groups by Kelly (2005). Dancing enabled to appease our most dangerous enemies: other men of other tribes or even of the own group (Kelly, 2005; Evans Pim, 2013). Similar to symbolic fights present in many non-human animal species, dance may serve for getting to know who is stronger before undertaking a fight, thus reducing the risk of injury and preventing casualties (Evans Pim, 2013). As an actual example, ghetto dance battles may contribute to avoid deadly duels (McDermott and Hauser, 2005).

Dance Rituals, Trance, Shamanism, and Religion

(Nietzsche, 1883–1885) argued that he would not believe in any god unless this god was able to dance. Dance in many societies is not only delightment, but it means also to enter into contact with spirits and gods (Métraux, 1958; Lapassade, 1976; Verger, 1982; Ginn, 1990; Christoph and Oberländer, 1996; Jilek, 2009; Herbert, 2011). Although, trance may in some cultures be also reached without dancing, rhythmical techniques including breathing, hyperventilation and dance, as in the Indonesian island of Bali, are the means which are used in the majority of societies for entering trance. Some historical and actual examples for trance dances include the medieval European St. Vitus’ dance, the Italian Tarantella, the Brazilian Candomblé, the Cuban Santería, the Japanese Nô, the Senegalese N’doep or the Sufi Dervish dances. Trance dance serves as catharsis reached through ecstasy. An ancestor, a spirit or a god drives the dancer; the dancer is possessed. Mask dances are common throughout societies worldwide including Malian Dogon, Japanese Kabuki, Dan acrobats in Ivory Coast, Egungun in Benin and Nigeria. Pre-Christian religious mask dances are the origin of present time Carnival traditions. Dancers moving like puppets on the strings such as in Indian Kathakali and Japanese Kabuki are the precursors of theater and pantomime. In this respect, it is interesting that a 15,000-year-old marionette puppet with moveable limbs has been found in a grave of an adult man believed to be a shaman in Brno, Czech Republic (Williams, 2011). In Ethiopian and Greek Orthodox Churches people dance for God. It is still matter of debate whether religion is an adaptive complex itself or a by-product of adaptive behaviors in other non- religious contexts. Since there is no evidence of “natural” non-religious control populations, it cannot be excluded that religious beliefs, at least in hunter-gatherer societies might have provided evolutionary advantages (Boyer, 2001; Dow, 2008; Antweiler, 2015).

Embodied Pre-verbal Memorizing and Transfer of Traditions

In a pre-verbal context the importance of dance for individual and collective memorizing cannot be overemphasized. Dance in many traditional societies is an instrument to memorize hunting techniques and to preserve traditions by telling stories about the past of the community. In South India Kathakali is danced to tell tales of the Mahabharata epic (Ginn, 1990). Since the mirror motor neurons of who observes dancers are activated dance is an excellent method to train children and adolescents and to communicate experiences and skills which are later internalized by imitation (Rizzolatti et al., 1996). Also in this function, dance is the predecessor of theater (Sachs, 1933/1980).

Paving the Way for Verbal Communication

Language might have evolved alongside melody, possibly passing through a “musilanguage” stage as already argued by Rousseau (Rousseau, 1781; Brown and Jordania, 2013). However, the evolution of language requires an underlying rhythmic and gestural understanding, i.e., embodied communication (Oberzaucher and Grammer, 2008; Phillips-Silver et al., 2010; Honing, 2012; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2014). Rhythm perception enables to discern words and is necessary to codify and decode language. The observation of a dancer aids to recapitulate and decode gestures (Patel and Daniele, 2003; Patel, 2008; Hausen et al., 2013; Fujii and Wan, 2014; Magne et al., 2016). Thus, it is likely that R&D paved the way for the evolution of language.

Individual Fitness

Individual Psychological Fitness

The individual benefits from R&D in several ways. R&D have anti-depressive effects and divert thoughts from sorrows and boredom. Fetuses are able to hear their mother’s physical functions already from the middle of pregnancy on (Trehub, 2003; Parncutt, 2009; Grahn, 2012). Mother’s breathing and heartbeat may produce an incessant conditioning effect which one could describe as a “soothing fetal brainwash.” Up to here, there is no difference between humans and other mammals. Human babies are, however, especially immature at birth as compared to other animals. Therefore, human infants may require more specific soothing efforts such as rocking. To rock the baby one needs free arms. Soothing a baby by rocking is probably a human universal which, however, has not been investigated in this respect. A recent study comparing cultural effects on rocking a baby for soothing showed more similarities than differences between different cultures (Vinall et al., 2011). There is some research on the effects of rocking in the medical literature. In PubMed we found 157 hits from 1948 to 2014. Especially premature babies benefit from rocking (Malcuit et al., 1988; Clark et al., 1989; Sammon and Darnall, 1994). Rocking has a positive effect on the entrainment of respiration as well as on neuromuscular development of infants (Malcuit et al., 1988; Clark et al., 1989). Intuitively, one may assume that experienced caretakers know that rocking is an effective means to soothe a baby, but if we look very carefully at infants’ behavior we appreciate that the infants themselves induce their caretakers to rock them since other means are less effective. Infants and toddlers exhibit also active physiological stereotypic movements (Sallustro and Atwell, 1978; Thelen, 1979; Barry et al., 2011; Lutz, 2014). Interestingly, physiological rhythmical stereotypies not only have a self-soothing effect as reflected by heart rate reduction but frequently involve the legs, a behavior that could be the starting point of dancing (Soussignan and Koch, 1985). In fact, also later spontaneous unintentional movements to a musical beat most frequently involve the lower limbs reflecting an unconscious proneness to dance (Woods and Miltenberger, 1996; Janata et al., 2012). Whether or not rocking a baby is a behavior strictly confined to humans is an interesting research question which deserves to be explored by evolutionary biologists. We did not find any report of animals or non-human primates rocking their offspring. Moreover, whereas rhesus monkeys have not been found to be good detectors of beat (Honing et al., 2012), chimpanzees display rhythmical behaviors (Ravignani et al., 2013).

It is conceivable that the sensitivity of babies for being rocked and physiological stereotypes paved the way for the evolution of R&D in humans (Soussignan and Koch, 1985). Rocking may also promote the ability of infants to stratify rhythm (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005). Dance enables to self-induce the soothing effect of being rocked. Dance appeases the tormented soul and leads to the secretion of hormones like dopamine and endorphins (Sutoo and Akiyama, 2004; Harris, 2007; Salimpoor et al., 2011; Dunbar et al., 2012). The particularly strong emotional impact of R&D is underscored by recent applications in medicine. Their capacity to influence mood, to reach autistic patients otherwise refractory to any emotional involvement and to make Parkinson patients start moving are taken advantage of in medicine (Hayakawa et al., 2000; Sacks, 2007; See, 2012; Moore, 2013; Nombela et al., 2013; Boehm et al., 2014; Ashoori et al., 2015). Playing musical instruments and dancing reduce the risk of dementia in the elderly (Verghese et al., 2003).

R&D enable to divert the otherwise unstoppable flow of thinking (Steiner, 2006). Dance and music playing enable to psychological “flow” experiences that wipe away unpleasant thoughts, sorrows and boredom (Thomson and Jaque, 2012; Chirico et al., 2015). Boredom may be not only a phenomenon of modern societies but also a problem of traditional societies. Men seem to be more prone to both, boredom and violence, which also are associated with suicide (Wrangham and Peterson, 1996; Heinsohn, 2003). R&D help to overcome boredom and, thus, contribute to keep peace and save lives (Sundberg et al., 1991; Choquet et al., 1993; Wexler and Goodwin, 2006).

R&D are particularly powerful means to express the essential “joie de vivre” (joy of life), i.e., the pure “raison d’être” (reason to exist) a philosophical aspect which has been particularly emphasized by Latin-American and African authors (Giglio and Giglio, 1980; Foix, 2007; Kouam and Mofor, 2011), as Jean Massoulier texted: “Je danse donc je suis” (I dance therefore I am).

Individual Physical Fitness

Dance, rhythm, music and being rocked have been shown to have painkilling effects (Lehmann, 2010; Pillai Riddell et al., 2011; Dunbar et al., 2012; Johnston et al., 2014). The capacity of music to reduce the dosage of painkilling medication in intensive care patients is documented in medicine (Lehmann, 2010). A recent study has shown that rhythmical music reduces the perceived exertion induced by strenuous physical performance an observation which was well-known to the cotton harvesters in the USA and is reflected by specific working songs. This effect occurs not only on a psychological but also on a proprioceptive level (Fritz et al., 2013). The pain threshold is elevated more by active drumming, dancing or singing than by passive music listening (Dunbar et al., 2012). Rhythmic movements or breathing into hyperventilation are effective means for entering trance, an effect that Hindu yogis take advantage of when they perforate their skin, tongue, or lips before starting their processions.

Furthermore, active and passive rhythmical movements improve body coordination (Trainor and Cirelli, 2015). Although, a major evolutionary advantage is to be expected from cooperation, in some given moments preparation for fighting may be useful for a given group to succeed in winning against enemies and thereby improving the access to resources (Kelly, 2000). Individual and collective coordination skills are trained in martial dances for example in Brazilian Capoeira and Maculelê, in Sicilian Taratatà, Indian Kalaripayattu (Phillips-Silver et al., 2010).

From an evolutionary perspective, all these more or less overlapping aspects are likely to have played a role although these are not equally important at the same time and age. We would tentatively rank reproductive fitness, cooperation and bonding as the driving evolutionary forces whereas the individual aspects may have further contributed to the evolutionary functions of dance in specific age, gender, and prehistoric contexts (Nowak, 2006; Nowak et al., 2010). Survival is particularly important for children in traditional societies with high infant mortality (Carter and Mendis, 2002; Hart and Sussmann, 2009). Reproductive fitness applies to sexually mature individuals who may even risk their lives in order to find potential partners. Peace-keeping and martial dancing could have been particularly important for young men during periods of high violence. On the other hand, martial dances are not human universals and high violence periods have been more widespread in historic times than in prehistory (Kelly, 2000; Haas and Piscitelli, 2013).

In summary, dance offers evolutionary advantages to humans by contributing to sexual reproduction signaling, cooperation, social bonding, infant care, violence avoidance as well as embodied individual and social communication and memorization. Anticipating one consequence of our R&D concept we would expect that not only beat induction is innate but that during their development infants and toddlers spontaneously start to dance earlier than to express other musical utterances such as singing and that this behavior does not depend on the cultural background of their parents. For further investigating the specific functions of R&D in humans, it would be highly interesting to compare the timing of their emergence during the lifespan of humans with the emergence of synchronic behavior in non-human animals.

Conclusion

The main intention of this article is to provide a refined concept for further interdisciplinary research on the evolution of dance and music in humankind. It is proposed that in future studies on the evolution of music, attention should be paid on which attribute of music precisely is focused whether rhythm, melody, or harmony. The same applies to rhythmical attributes, i.e., pulse of beats, stronger or weaker beats (downbeats, offbeats), tactus, tempo, meter, and patterns. The evolutionary functions of dance have been relatively neglected. The close mutual relationship between rhythm and dance and embodied rhythm perception should be fully acknowledged in future research.

Author Contributions

JR did the literature search, developed the hypothesis and wrote the manuscript; RO contributed to the literature search, to developing the hypothesis and writing the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are deeply indebted to Prof. Marc Leman of the Institute for Psychoacoustics and Electronic Music (IPEM), Department of Musicology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium as well as to the reviewers who have read several drafts of this manuscript and importantly contributed to improve this article.

References

Alluri, V., Toiviainen, P., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Glerean, E., Sams, M., and Brattico, E. (2012). Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. Neuroimage 59, 3677–3689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anati, E. (1995). Il Museo Immaginario della Preistoria (The Imaginary Museum of Prehistory). Milano: Ed Jaca book.

Google Scholar

Antweiler, C. (2015). Cosmopolitanism and pancultural universals. Our common denominator and an anthropologically based cosmopolitanism. J. Internat. Glob. Stud. Fall 2015, 50–66.

Google Scholar

Arcadi, A., Clark, R. D., and Boesch, C. (1998). Buttress drumming by wild chimpanzees: temporal patterning, phrase integration into loud calls, and preliminary evidence for individual distinctiveness. Primates 39, 503–516. doi: 10.1007/BF02557572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arcadi, A., Clark, R. D., and Mugurusi, F. (2004). A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations. Primates 45, 135–139. doi: 10.1007/s10329-003-0070-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnal, L. H., and Giraud, A. L. (2012). Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 390–398. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashoori, A., Eagleman, D. M., and Jankovic, J. (2015). Effects of auditory rhythm and music on gait disturbances in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Neurol. 6:234. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbagiovanni, G. (2006). L’identità del Corpo. Viaggio nell’Antropologia della Danza. (The Identity of the Body. Travel in the Anthropology of Dance). Torino: Ananke srl.

Barry, S., Baird, G., Lascelles, K., Bunton, P., and Hedderly, T. (2011). Differentiating between tics and stereotypies. Neuro-developmental movement disorders -an update on childhood motor stereotypies. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53, 979–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04058.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergeson, T. R., and Trehub, S. (2005). Infants perception of rhythmic patterns. Music Percept. 23, 345–360.

Google Scholar

Berk, L. E. (2014). Development through the Lifespan, 6th Edn. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.

Google Scholar

Bispham, J. (2006). Rhythm in music: what is it? who has it? and why? Music Percept. 24, 125–134. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehm, K., Cramer, H., Staroszynski, T., and Ostermann, T. (2014). Arts therapies for anxiety, depression, and quality of life in breast cancer patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2014: 103297. doi: 10.1155/2014/103297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, D., and Abubakar, A. (2014). Music listening in families and peer groups: benefits for young people’s social cohesion and emotional well-being across four cultures. Front. Psychol. 5:392. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, D., Fischer, R., González Atilano, M. L., de Garay Hernández, J., Moreno, L., Mendoza, S., et al. (2013). Music, identity and musical ethnocentrism of young people in six Asian, Latin-American and Western cultures. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 2360–2376. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, D., Fischer, R., Strack, M., Bond, M. H., Lo, E., and Lam, J. (2011). How shared preferences in music create bonds between people: values as the missing link. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1159–1171. doi: 10.1177/0146167211407521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, D., Fischer, R., Tekman, H. G., Abubakar, A. A., Njenga, J., and Zenger, M. (2012). Young people’s topography of musical functions: personal, social and cultural experiences with music across genders and six cultures. Int. J. Psychol. 47, 355–369. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.656128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouwer, F. L., Van Zuijen, T. L., and Honing, H. (2014). Beat processing is pre-attentive for metrically simple rhythms with clear accents: an ERP study. PLoS ONE 9:e97467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyer, P. (2001). Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Brown, D. E. (1991). Human Universals. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Brown, S. (2007). Contagious heterophony: a new theory about the origins of music. Music. Sci. 11: 326. doi: 10.1177/102986490701100101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., and Jordania, J. (2013). Universals in the world’s music. Psychol. Music 41, 229–248. doi: 10.1177/0305735611425896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, G. A. (2013). Animal signals and emotion in music: coordinating affect across groups. Front. Psychol. 4:990. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caballero, A. A. (1992). Las Màscaras de lo Jondo (the Masks of he Jondo). Madrid: Ediciones del Prado.

Canetti, E. (1960). Masse und Macht. (Crowds and Power). Germany: Fischer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Carter, R., and Mendis, K. N. (2002). Evolutionary and historical aspects of the burden of malaria. Clin. Microbiol. Rev. 15, 564–594. doi: 10.1128/CMR.15.4.564-594.2002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chauvigné, L. A., Gitau, K. M., and Brown, S. (2014). The neural basis of audiomotor entrainment: an ALE meta-analysis. Front. Neurosci. 8:776. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Gaggioli, A., and Riva, G. (2015). When music “flows”. State and trait in musical performance, composition and listening: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 6:906. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choquet, M., Kovess, V., and Poutignat, N. (1993). Suicidal thoughts among adolescents: an intercultural approach. Adolescence 28, 649–659.

Google Scholar

Christensen, J. F., Gaigg, S. B., Gomila, A., Oke, P., and Calvo-Merino, B. (2014). Enhancing emotional experiences to dance through music: the role of valence and arousal in the cross-modal bias. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:757. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoph, H., and Oberländer, H. (1996). Voodoo. Cologne: Taschen Verlag.

Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., and Trainor, L. (2014). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Biol. Sci. 369, 20130400. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. L., Cordero, L., Goss, K. C., and Manos, D. (1989). Effects of rocking on neuromuscular development in the premature. Biol. Neonate 56, 306–314. doi: 10.1159/000243139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, P., Rouse, A., Wilson, M., and Reichmuth, C. (2013). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non-vocal mimic. J. Comp. Psychol. 127, 412–427. doi: 10.1037/a0032345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa-Faidella, J., Baldeweg, T., Grimm, S., and Escera, C. (2011). Interactions between “what” and “when” in the auditory system: temporal predictability enhances repetition suppression. J. Neurosci. 31, 18590–18597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2599-11.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, I. (2005). “Music and meaning, ambiguity and evolution,” in Musical Communication, eds D. Miell, R. MacDonald, and D. Hargreaves (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 27–43.

Google Scholar

Cross, I., and Morley, I. (2008). “The evolution of music: theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence,” in Communicative Musicality, eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 61–82.

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man. Chap. XIX, 572, 880. Available at: http://darwin-online.org.uk/converted/pdf/1871_Descent_F939.1.pdf

Google Scholar

Davidson, J., and Emberly, A. (2012). “Embodied musical communication across cultures: singing and dancing for quality of life and wellbeing benefit,” in Music, Health and Wellbeing, eds R. MacDonald, G. Kreutz, and L. Mitchell (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 136–149.

Google Scholar

De Guio, F., Jacobson, S. W., Molteno, C. D., Jacobson, J. L., and Meintjes, E. M. (2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging study comparing rhythmic finger tapping in children and adults. Pediatr. Neurol. 46, 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.11.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Waal, F. B. (2000). Primates – a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science 289, 586–590.

Google Scholar

de Waal, F. B. M. (2013). “Foreword,” in War, Peace and Human Nature. The Convergence of Evolutionary and Cultural Views, ed. D. P. Fry (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Dean, R. T., Byron, T., and Bailes, F. A. (2009). The pulse of symmetry: on the possible co-evolution of rhythm in music and dance. Music. Sci. 13, 341–367. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissanayake, E. (2004). Motherese is but one part of a ritualized, multimodal, temporally organized, affiliative interaction. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 512–513. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0432011X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissanayake, E. (2008). If music is the food of love, what about survival and reproductive success? Music. Sci. 12, 169–195. doi: 10.1177/1029864908012001081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donald, M. (2001). A Mind so Rare. The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York, NY: Ww Norton & Co.

Google Scholar

Dow, J. (2008). ‘Is religion an evolutionary adaptation?’. J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul. 11:2. Available at: http://jasss.soc.surrey.ac.uk/11/2/2.html

Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I, Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I., and Barra, V. (2012). Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evol. Psychol. 10, 688–702. doi: 10.1177/147470491201000403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans Pim, J. (2013). “Man the singer,” in War, Peace and Human Nature. The Convergence of Evolutionary and Cultural Views, ed. D. P. Fry (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 524–540.

Falk, D. (2000). “Hominid brain evolution and the origins of music,” in The Origins of Music, eds N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (London: MIT Press), 197–216.

Google Scholar

Falk, D. (2005). Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 491–541.

Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: a comparative perspective. Cognition 100, 173–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2012). “The biology and evolution of rhythm: unraveling a paradox,” in Language and Music as Cognitive Systems, eds P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. A. Hawkins, and I. Cross (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 73–92.

Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2013). Rhythmic cognition in humans and animals: distinguishing meter and pulse perception. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:68. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foix, A. (2007). Je Danse Donc je Suis (I Dance Therefore I Am). Paris: Éditions Gallimard.

Fritz, T. H., Hardikarb, S., Demoucrona, M., Niessend, M., Demeya, M., Giota, O., et al. (2013). Musical agency reduces perceived exertion during strenuous physical performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 17784–17789. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217252110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujii, S., and Wan, C. Y. (2014). The role of rhythm in speech and language rehabilitation: the SEP hypothesis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:777. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujii, S., Watanabe, H., Oohashi, H., Hirashima, M., Nozaki, D., and Taga, G. (2014). Precursors of dancing and singing to music in three-to four-months-old infants. PLoS ONE 9:e97680. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giglio, J. S., and Giglio, Z. G. (1980). [Contributions to the psychodynamic approach to understanding the phenomenon of music]. Acta Psiquiatr. Psicol. Am. Lat. 26, 293–299.

Google Scholar

Gillespie-Lynch, K., Greenfield, P. M., Lyn, H., and Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2014). Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Front. Psychol. 5:1228. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginn, V. (1990). The Spirited Earth. New York, NY: Rizzoli International Publications.

Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A. (2012). Neural mechanisms of rhythm perception. Current findings and future perspectives. Topics Cognit. Sci. 4, 508–606. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01213.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grammer, K., Oberzaucher, E., Holzleitner, I., and Atmaca, S. (2011). “Dance: the human body as a dynamic motion system,” in The Implications of Embodiment, eds W. Tschacher and C. Bergomi (Exeter: Imprint Academic), 173–192.

Google Scholar

Haas, J., and Piscitelli, M. (2013). “The prehistory of warfare misled by ethnography,” in War, Peace and Human Nature. The Convergence of Evolutionary and Cultural Views, ed. D. P. Fry (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 168–190.

Google Scholar

Hagen, E. H., and Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Hum. Nat. 14, 21–51. doi: 10.1007/s12110-003-1015-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagen, E. H., and Hammerstein, P. (2009). Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves. Music. Sci. 13(2 Suppl.), 291–320. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagen, E. H., Hammerstein, P., and Hess, N. (2010). “Strategic aspects of communication,” in Evolution of Communication and Language in Embodied Agents, eds S. Nolfi and M. Mirolli (Berlin: Springer Verlag), 55–65.

Google Scholar

Hanna, J. (2010). Dance and sexuality: many moves. J. Sex Res. 47, 212–241. doi: 10.1080/00224491003599744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannon, E. E., and Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: implications for musical structure learning. Cogn. Psychol. 50, 354–377. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannon, E. E., and Trehub, S. E. (2005). Tuning in to musical rhythms: Infants learn more readily than adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 12639–12643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504254102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, D. (2001–2016). The Online Etymology Dictionary. Available at: http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term$=$emotion [accessed March 8, 2016].

Google Scholar

Harris, D. A. (2007). Dance/movement therapy approaches to fostering resilience and recovery among African adolescent torture survivors. Torture 17, 134–155.

Google Scholar

Hart, D., and Sussmann, R. W. (2009). Man the Hunted. Primates, predators and human evolution. Boulder, CO: Westview press.

Google Scholar

Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2014). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci. Rep. 3, 1566. doi: 10.1038/srep01566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausen, M., Torppa, R., Salmela, V. R., Vainio, M., and Särkämö, T. (2013). Music and speech prosody: a common rhythm. Front. Psychol. 4:566. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayakawa, Y., Miki, H., Takada, K., and Tanaka, K. (2000). Effects of music on mood during bench stepping exercise. Percept. Mot. Skills 90, 307–314. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.90.1.307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heine, H. (1822). Reisebilder. Zweiter Brief aus Berlin, 16.03.1822. Heinrich Heine: Werke und Briefe in zehn Bänden, Vol. 3. Berlin: Zeno.org, 510–536.

Google Scholar

Heinsohn, G. (2003). Söhne und Weltmacht: Terror im Aufstieg und Fall der Nationen (Sons and Global Power. Terror in the Rise and Fall of Nations). Munich: Piper Verlag.

Herbert, R. (2011). Reconsidering music and trance: cross-cultural differences and cross-disciplinary perspectives. Ethnomusicol. Forum 20, 201–227. doi: 10.1080/17411912.2011.592402

CrossRef Full Text

Higham, T., Basell, L., Jacobi, R., Wood, R., Ramsey, C. B., and Conard, N. J. (2012). Testing models for the beginnings of the Aurignacian and the advent of figurative art and music: the radiocarbon chronology of Geißenklösterle. J. Hum. Evol. 62, 664–676. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honing, H. (2012). Without it no music: beat induction as a fundamental musical trait. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 85–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06402.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honing, H., Merchant, H., Háden, G. P., Prado, L., and Bartolo, R. (2012). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) detect rhythmic groups in music, but not the beat. PLoS ONE 7:e51369. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janata, P., and Parsons, L. (2013). “Neural mechanisms of music, singing and dancing,” in Language, Music, and the Brain: A Mysterious Relationship, Vol. 10, ed. M. A. Arbib (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Janata, P., Tomic, S. T., and Haberman, J. M. (2012). Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J. Experim. Psychol. 141, 54–75. doi: 10.1037/a0024208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jilek, W. (2009). Music and trance: a theory of the relations between music and possession. Am. Ethnol. 14, 598–599. doi: 10.1525/ae.1987.14.3.02a00450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, C., Campbell-Yeo, M., Fernandes, A., Inglis, D., Streiner, D., and Zee, R. (2014). Skin-to-skin care for procedural pain in neonates. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 23, CD008435. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008435.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordania, J. (2005). “Interrogo ergo cogito – I am asking questions therefore I think: responsorial singing and the origins of human intelligence,” in Proceedings of the International Symposium on Traditional Polyphony, Tbilisi, 39–45.

Google Scholar

Jourdain, R. (2001). Music, the Brain, and Ecstasy: How Music Captures our Imagination. New York, NY: Harper & Collins Publisher.

Google Scholar

Karayanni, S. S. (2005). “Dismissal veiling desire: Kuchuk Hanem and imperial masculinity,” in Belly Dance. Orientalism, Transnationalism and Harem Fantasy, eds A. Shay and B. Sellers-Young (Costa Mesa, CA: Mazda), 114–143.

Google Scholar

Kelly, R. C. (2000). Warless Societies and the Origin of War. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Google Scholar

Kelly, R. C. (2005). The evolution of lethal intergroup violence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 15294–15298. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505955102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Ilari, B. (2014). Joint drumming in Brazilian and German preschool children: cultural differences in rhythmic entrainment, but not prosocial effects. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 45, 137–166. doi: 10.1177/0022022113493139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 102, 299–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouam, M., and Mofor, C. (2011). Philosophies et Cultures Africaines à l’heure de l’interculturalité: Anthologie (African Philosophy and Cultures in Times of Interculturality. An Anthology). Paris: L’Harmattan.

Lapassade, G. (1976). Essay sur la Transe. Paris: Éditions Univesitaires.

Larsson, M. (2014). Self-generated sounds of locomotion and ventilation and the evolution of human rhythmic abilities. Anim. Cogn. 17, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Lee, K. M., Barrett, K. C., Kim, Y., Lim, Y., and Lee, K. (2015). Dance and music in “Gangnam Style”: how dance observation affects meter perception. PLoS ONE 10:e0134725. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, C. (2010). Der Genetische Notenschlüssel (the Genetic Musical Key). Munich: Herbig Verlag München.

Leman, M., and Maes, P.-J. (2014). The role of embodiment in the perception of music. Empir. Musicol. Rev. 9, 236–246. doi: 10.18061/emr.v9i3-4.4498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesaffre, M., and Leman, M. (2013). The Power of Music. Researching Musical Experiences: A Viewpoint of IPEM. Leuven: Acco ed. Leuven.

Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J. (2006). This is Your Brain on Music: The Science of a Human Obsession. New York, NY: Dutton / Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J. (2013). Neuronal correlates of musical behaviors. Music Ther. Pers. 31, 15–24. doi: 10.1093/mtp/31.1.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingstone, S. R., and Thompson, W. F. (2009). The emergence of music from the theory of mind. Music. Sci. 13, 83–115. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, C. K. (2014). Stereotypic behavior in nonhuman primates as a model for the human condition. ILAR J. 55, 284–296. doi: 10.1093/ilar/ilu016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, P.-J., and Leman, M. (2013). The influence of body movements on children’s perception of music with an ambiguous expressive character. PLoS ONE 8:e54682. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magne, C., Jordan, D. K., and Gordon, R. L. (2016). Speech rhythm sensitivity and musical aptitude: ERPs and individual differences. Brain Lang. 153-154:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malcuit, G., Pomerleau, A., and Brosseau, N. (1988). Cardiac and behavioral responses to rocking stimulations in one- and three-month-old infants. Percept. Mot. Skills 66, 207–217.

Google Scholar

Masataka, N. (2009). The evolution of music: theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence. The origins of language and the evolution of music: a comparative perspective. Phys. Life Rev. 6, 11–22.

Google Scholar

McDermott, J., and Hauser, M. (2005). The origins of music: innateness, uniqueness, and evolution. Music Perc. 23, 29–60. doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History. Cambridge, MA: Harward University Press.

Google Scholar

Merker, B., Morley, I., and Zuidema, W. (2015). Five fundamental constraints on theories of the origins of music. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370:20140095. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Métraux, A. (1958). Le Voudou Haitien (The Voodoo of Haiti). Paris: Éditions Gallimard.

Mithen, S. (2007). The Singing Neanderthals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Miura, A., Fujii, S., Okano, M., Kudo, K., and Nakazawa, K. (2016). Finger-to-beat coordination skill of non-dancers, street dancers, and the world champion of a street-dance competition. Front. Psychol. 7:542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., De Martin, M., Cerasa, A., and Thaut, M. (2003). Neurobiology of rhythmic motor entrainment. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 313–321. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, K. S. (2013). A systematic review on the neural effects of music on emotion regulation: implications for music therapy practice. J. Music Ther. 50, 198–242. doi: 10.1093/jmt/50.3.198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morley, I. (2014). A multi-disciplinary approach to the origins of music: perspectives from anthropology, archaeology, cognition and behaviour. J. Anthropol. Sci. 92, 147–177. doi: 10.4436/JASS.92008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasaka, Y., Chao, Z. C., Hasegawa, N., Notoya, T., and Fujii, N. (2013). Spontaneous synchronization of arm motion between Japanese macaques. Sci. Rep. 3:1151. doi: 10.1038/srep01151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neave, N., McCarty, K., Freynik, J., Caplan, N., Hönekopp, J., and Fink, B. (2011). Male dance moves that catch a woman’s eye. Biol. Lett. 7, 221–224. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nettl, B. (2000). “An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sounds and musical cultures”. In The Origins of Music, eds N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Cambridge, MA: MIT press), 463–472.

Google Scholar

Nietzsche, F. (1883–1885). Also Sprach Zarathustra. Ein Buch für Alle und Keinen, Vom Lesen und Schreiben 2013, Reprint der Ausgabe von 1886, eds D. M. Hoffmann and L. Lütkehaus (Frankfurt: Stroemfeld-Verlag).

Nombela, C., Hughes, L. E., Owen, A. M., and Grahn, J. A. (2013). Into the groove: can rhythm influence Parkinson’s disease? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37(10Pt 2):2564–2570. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science 8, 314, 1560–1563.

Google Scholar

Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., and Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature 466, 1057–1062. doi: 10.1038/nature09205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaradan, S., Peretz, I., Missal, M., and Mouraux, A. (2011a). Tagging the neuronal entrainment to beat and meter. J. Neurosci. 31, 10234–10240. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0411-11.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaradan, S., Peretz, I., and Mouraux, A. (2011b). Steady-state evoked potentials as an index of multisensory temporal binding. Neuroimage 60, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaradan, S., Peretz, I., and Mouraux, A. (2012). Selective neuronal entrainment to the beat and meter embedded in a musical rhythm. J. Neurosci. 32, 17572–17581. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3203-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberzaucher, E., and Grammer, K. (2008). “Everything is movement: on the nature of embodied communication,” in Embodied Communication, eds I. Wachsmuth, M. Lenzen, and G. Knoblich (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 151–177.

Google Scholar

Oota, S. (2016). “The origin of dance: evolutionary significance on ritualized movements of animals,” in Dance Notations and Robot Motion, J.-P. Laumond, and N. Abe (cham: Springer International Publishing).

Google Scholar

Ostovar, R. (2016). Sensomotorische Entwicklung im Kindesalter. Medical thesis, Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf.

Ota, N., Gahr, M., and Soma, M. (2015). Tap dancing birds: the multimodal mutual courtship display of males and females in a socially monogamous songbird. Sci. Rep. 5:16614. doi: 10.1038/srep16614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2009). The emotional antecendents of the evolution of music and language. Music. Sci. 13, 229–259. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parncutt, R. (2009). Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion. Music. Sci. 13, 119–150. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., and Daniele J. R. (2003). An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music. Cognition 87, B35–B45. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00187-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Bregman, M. R., and Schulz, I. (2009). Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr. Biol. 19, 827–830. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Bregmann, M. R., Schulz, I., and Schulz, C. (2008). “Investigating the human-specificity of synchronization to music,” in Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Sapporo, eds K. Miyazaki, Y. Hiraga, M. Adachi, Y. Nakajima, and M. Tsuzaki (Adelaide, SA: Causal Productions).

Google Scholar

Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, C. A., and Bryant, G. A. (2010). The ecology of entrainment: foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Percept. 28, 3–14. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips-Silver, J., and Keller, P. E. (2012). Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:26. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips-Silver, J., Toiviainen, P. C., Gosselin, N., Turgeon, C., Lepore, F., and Peretz, I. (2015). Cochlear implant users move in time to the beat of drum music. Hear. Res. 321, 25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2014.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influences infants rhythm perception. Science 308: 1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1110922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillai Riddell, R., Racine, N., Turcotte, K., Uman, L., Horton, R., Din Osmun, L., et al. (2011). Nonpharmacological management of procedural pain in infants and young children: an abridged Cochrane review. Pain Res. Manag. 16, 321–330. doi: 10.1155/2011/489286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.

Google Scholar

Ravignani, A., Bowling, D. L., and Fitch, W. T. (2014). Chorusing, synchrony, and the evolutionary functions of rhythm. Front. Psychol. 5:1118. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravignani, A., Gingras, B., Asano R., Sonnweber, R., Martellàn V., and Fitch, W. (2013). “The evolution of rhythmic cognition: new perspectives and technologies in comparative research,” in Cooperative Minds: Social Interaction and Group Dynamics, eds M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, and I. Wachsmuth Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (1199-1204). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society

Google Scholar

Redmond, L. (1997). When the Drummers were Women. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press.

Google Scholar

Repp, B. H., and Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization. A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 403–452. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ringwalt, S. (2008). Developmental Screening and Assessment Instruments with an Emphasis on Social and Emotional Development for Young Children Ages Birth through Five. Chapel Hill, NC: FPG Child Development Institute, National Early Childhood Technical Assistance Center, University of North Carolina, U.S.A.

Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebroeks, W., Sier, M. J., Kelleberg Nielsen, T., de Loecker, D., Pares, J. M., Arps, C. E. C., et al. (2011). Use of red ochre by early Neanderthals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1889–1894. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112261109

CrossRef Full Text

Rousseau, J.-J. (1781). Essai sur l’origine des langues. Vénissieux: Éditions La passe du vent.

Google Scholar

Sachs, C. (1933/1980). Eine Weltgeschichte des Tanzes (A World History of Dance), 2nd Edn, Italian translation. Milano: Il Saggiatore.

Sacks, O. (2007). Musicophilia. Tales of Music and the Brain. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf

Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., and Dagher, A. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sallustro, F., and Atwell, C. W. (1978). Body rocking, head banging, and head rolling in normal children. J. Pediatr. 93, 704–708. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(78)80922-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sammon, M. P., and Darnall, R. A. (1994). Entrainment of respiration to rocking in premature infants: coherence analysis. J. Appl. Physiol. 77, 1548–1554.

Google Scholar

Savage, P. E., Brown, S., Sakai, E., and Currie, T. E. (2015). Statistical universal reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 8987–8992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414495112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schachner, A. (2013). The origins of human and avian auditory-motor entrainment. Nova Acta Leopoldina 111, 243–253.

Google Scholar

See, C. M. (2012). The use of music and movement therapy to modify behaviour of children with autism. Pertanika J. Soc. Sci. Hum. 20, 1103–1116.

Seesjärvi, E., Särkämö, T., Vuoksimaa, E., Tervaniemi, M., Peretz, I., and Kaprio, J. (2015). The nature and nurture of melody: a twin study of musical pitch and rhythm perception. Behav. Genet. 46, 506–515. doi: 10.1007/s10519-015-9774-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shay, A., and Sellers-Young, B. (eds) (2005). Belly Dance. Orientalism, Transnationalism, and Harem Fantasy. Costa Mesa, CA; Mazda.

Google Scholar

Soussignan, R., and Koch, P. (1985). Rhythmical stereotypies (leg-swinging) associated with reductions in heart-rate in normal school children. Biol. Psychol. 21, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(85)90027-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, G. (2006). Dix Raisons (Possibles) Pour la Tristesse de la Pensée. Ten Possible Reasons for the Sadness of Thinking). Paris: Éditions Albin Michel.

Stevens, C. J. (2012). Music perception and cognition: a review of recent cross -cultural research. Top. Cogn. Sci. 4, 653–667. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01215.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Farmer, R. F., and Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in young adults: gender and cultural comparisons. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 22, 209–223. doi: 10.1177/0022022191222003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutoo, D., and Akiyama, K. (2004). Music improves dopaminergic neurotransmission: demonstration based on the effect of music on blood pressure regulation. Brain Res. 1016, 255–262. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teki, S., Grube, M., and Griffiths, T. D. (2012). A unified model of time perception accounts for duration-based and beat-based timing mechanisms. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Demartin, M., Sanes, J. N., and Miall, C. (2008). Brain networks for integrative rhythm formation. PLoS ONE 3:e2312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Trimarchi, P. D., and Parsons, L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of musical rhythm perception: common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo, and pattern. Brain Sci. 4, 428–452. doi: 10.3390/brainsci4020428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Anim. Behav. 27: 699–715. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thieme, H. (1997). Lower palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 385, 807–810. doi: 10.1038/385807a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, P., and Jaque, S. V. (2012). Dancing with the muses: dissociation and flow. J. Trauma Dissociation 13, 478–489. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2011.652345.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Bradford books. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Trainor, L. J., and Cirelli, L. (2015). Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 45–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trehub, S. E. (2003). “Musical predisposition in infancy: an update,” in The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music, eds I. Peretz and R. Zatorre (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–30.

Trevarthen, C. (2011). “Communicative musicality: the human impulse to create and share music,” in Musical Imaginations: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Creativity, Performance and Perception, eds D. Hargreaves, D. Miell, and R. MacDonald (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 259–284.

Google Scholar

van den Broek, E. M. F., and Todd, P. M. (2009). Evolution of rhythm as an indicator for mate quality. Music. Sci. 13, 364–386.

Google Scholar

van Noorden, L. (2013). “Fundamentals of music and movement: towards an understanding of the motivational power of music,” in The Power of Music: Researching Musical Experiences a Viewpoint from the Institute for Psychoacoustics and Electronic Music (IPEM), eds M. Lessaffre and M. Leman (Leuven: Accu ed), 97–112.

Google Scholar

Verger, P. (1982). Orisha. Les Dieux Yorouba en Afrique et au Nouveau Monde. Paris: Editions AM Métailié.

Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N. Engl. J. Med. 348, 2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinall, J., Pillai Riddell, R., and Greenberg, S. (2011). The influence of culture on maternal soothing behaviours and infant pain expression in the immunization context. Pain Res. Manag. 16, 234–238. doi: 10.1155/2011/707615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, A. (1997). Adversaries of Dance. From the Puritans to the Present. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Google Scholar

Wang, T. (2015). A hypothesis on the biological origins and social evolution of music and dance. Front. Neurosci. 9:30. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wermke, K., and Mende, W. (2009). Musical elements in human infants’ cries: in the beginning is the melody. Music. Sci. 13, 151–175. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wexler, L., and Goodwin, B. (2006). Youth and adult community member beliefs about inupiat youth suicide and its prevention. Int. J. Circumpolar Health 65, 448–458.

Google Scholar

Whitehead, C. (2010). The culture ready brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 168–179. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., and Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science 338, 942–946. doi: 10.1126/science.1227608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, M. (2011). “Chapter 3: Dawn of the spirits,” in Prehistoric Belief: Shamans, Trance and the Afterllife (Stroud: The History Press).

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, I., Haden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., and Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 2468–2471. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809035106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, D. W., and Miltenberger, R. G. (1996). Are persons with nervous habits nervous? A preliminary examination of habit function in a nonreferred population. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 29, 259–261. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1996.29-259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolhouse, M. H., and Lai, R. (2014). Traces across the body: influence of music-dance synchrony on the observation of dance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:965. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrangham, R., and Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence. Mariner Books. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Wrescher, E. E. (1980). Red ochre and human evolution: a case for discussion. Curr. Anthropol. 21, 631–644. doi: 10.1086/202541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C. C., Hamm, J. P., Lim, V. K., and Kirk, I. J. (2016). Mu rhythm suppression demonstrates action representation in pianists during passive listening of piano melodies. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 2133–2139. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4615-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zentner, M., and Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 5768–5773. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000121107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zollikofer, C. P., Ponce De Leon, M. S., Vandermeersch, B., and Leveque, F. (2002). Evidence for interpersonal violence in the St. Cesaire Neanderthal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 6444–6448. doi: 10.1073/pnas.082111899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: beat, rhythm, dance, embodied music cognition, embodied communication, rocking, human universals, human evolution

Citation: Richter J and Ostovar R (2016) “It Don’t Mean a Thing if It Ain’t Got that Swing”– an Alternative Concept for Understanding the Evolution of Dance and Music in Human Beings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:485. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00485

Received: 31 October 2015; Accepted: 13 September 2016;
Published: 07 October 2016.

Edited by:

Andrea Ravignani, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Alexandre Celma-Miralles, Pompeu Fabra University, Spain
Lisa Horn, University of Vienna, Austria
Vittoria Spinosa, University of Barcelona, Spain

Copyright © 2016 Richter and Ostovar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joachim Richter, am9hY2hpbS5yaWNodGVyQGNoYXJpdGUuZGU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.