- 1School of Computer and Information Technology, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing, China
- 2Dr. Eric Jackman Institute of Child Study, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 3Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
- 4Department of Psychology, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China
- 5Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China
Numerous developmental studies have suggested that other-race effect (ORE) in face recognition emerges as early as in infancy and develops steadily throughout childhood. However, there is very limited research on the neural mechanisms underlying this developmental ORE. The present study used Granger causality analysis (GCA) to examine the development of children's cortical networks in processing own- and other-race faces. Children were between 3 and 13 years. An old-new paradigm was used to assess their own- and other-race face recognition with ETG-4000 (Hitachi Medical Co., Japan) acquiring functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data. After preprocessing, for each participant and under each face condition, we obtained the causal map by calculating the weights of causal relations between the time courses of [oxy-Hb] of each pair of channels using GCA. To investigate further the differential causal connectivity for own-race faces and other-race faces at the group level, a repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on the GCA weights for each pair of channels with the face race task (own-race face vs. other-race face) as the within-subject variable and the age as a between-subject factor (continuous variable). We found an age-related increase in functional connectivity, paralleling a similar age-related improvement in behavioral face processing ability. More importantly, we found that the significant differences in neural functional connectivity between the recognition of own-race faces and that of other-race faces were modulated by age. Thus, like the behavioral ORE, the neural ORE emerges early and undergoes a protracted developmental course.
Introduction
From early childhood, children typically have asymmetrical exposures to own- and other-race faces. They see own-race faces significantly more frequently than other-race faces (Rennels and Davis, 2008; Sugden et al., 2014). One of the consequences of this early asymmetrical face exposure is that we recognize own-race faces faster and more accurately than other-race faces. This phenomenon is referred to as the other-race effect (ORE; for reviews, see Meissner and Brigham, 2001; Sporer, 2001; Anzures et al., 2013a). ORE has been replicated in adults from different racial backgrounds using various paradigms (Malpass and Kravitz, 1969; Levin, 2000; Ge et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2015). ORE has been also consistently observed in childhood (for a review, see Anzures et al., 2013a). Numerous developmental studies have revealed that ORE emerges as early as in infancy (Sangrigoli and de Schonen, 2004; Hayden et al., 2007; Kelly et al., 2009) and develops steadily throughout childhood (Chance et al., 1982; Pezdek et al., 2003; Walker and Hewstone, 2006; Goodman et al., 2007; Anzures et al., 2014).
In addition to the behavioral ORE, recent functional neuroimaging studies have found that adults show differential neural responses when recognizing own-race vs. other-race faces (Golby et al., 2001; Ito and Urland, 2005; Kim et al., 2006; Feng et al., 2011; Natu et al., 2011; Senholzi and Ito, 2013; Liu et al., 2015; for a review, see: Ito and Bartholow, 2009). This difference in neural response to different racial faces is referred to as the Neural Other-Race Effect (NORE, Ding et al., 2014). For example, using event related potential (ERP), several studies (Ito and Urland, 2005; Natu et al., 2011; Senholzi and Ito, 2013) found that own-race faces and other-race faces elicit significant differences in the N170, an important ERP component related to face processing (Bentin et al., 1996). Using the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) methodology, Golby et al. (2001) and Kim et al. (2006) found greater neural responses for own-race faces than for other-race faces in the right fusiform face area (FFA, Kanwisher et al., 1997). Further, using fMRI and the dynamic causal modeling, Liu et al. (2015) found that recognizing own- vs. other-race faces engenders differential causal functional connectivity between the key cortical regions specialized in face processing.
In contrast to the extensive studies of the behavioral ORE in children and adults, and those of the neural ORE in adults, little is known about the neural correlates of own- vs. other-race face processing in children. To date, no studies have examined the origins of neural ORE and how it develops with age, with two exceptions. One study by Balas et al. (2011) used the ERP technique. It demonstrated a neural ORE whereby 9-month-old infants respond differentially to own-race faces and other-race faces, consistently with the behavioral findings with infants (Anzures et al., 2013a). Another study by Ding et al. (2014) used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to investigate the neural correlates of own- and other-race face recognition in children between 7 and 13 years of age. They found that regardless of age, neural activations in the right MFG/IFG, and the left cuneus were greater when recognizing other-race faces than own-race faces. Further, they found that different age groups presented different correlational patterns between the neural ORE in these regions and those in other brain regions. For example, with increased age, the numbers of the significant correlations between the occipital ORE areas and the frontal areas increased. Ding et al. (2014) speculated that functional neural network underlying the neural ORE might undergo developmental changes whereby the posterior-frontal integration of the key cortical regions involved in face processing differs for own- vs. other race face. However, Ding et al. (2014) only examined the correlations of fNIRS activation levels between different brain regions rather than the relations of dynamic responses of these regions. Thus, they could not provide direct evidence supporting their speculation.
The present study aimed to bridge this significant gap in the literature. More specifically, we used Granger causality analysis (GCA, Granger, 1969) to examine the developmental patterns of neural networks underlying the ORE in children between 3 and 13 years of age. The most distinctive advantage of GCA over the traditional Pearson correlational analysis is that the GCA can provide the directional functional interactions (i.e., effective connectivity) between the different brain regions underlying own- vs. other-race face recognition (Valdés-Sosa et al., 2006; Friston, 2011; Seth et al., 2015). Thus, the GCA is specifically suited to address such important questions as how the information from visual cortex transfer to senior cognitive cortex located in frontal lobe, or whether and how information transfer between two lateral hemispheres. This method of effective connectivity analysis has been widely used in EEG and fMRI time series analysis (Kamiński et al., 2001; Valdés-Sosa et al., 2006; for a review, see Seth et al., 2015). It also has been recently used in fNIRS data processing (Holper et al., 2012; Yuan, 2013).
Most of previous behavioral studies of developmental ORE focused their investigation on the children with age 3–13 years (e.g., Chance et al., 1982; Pezdek et al., 2003; Sangrigoli and de Schonen, 2004; Goodman et al., 2007; Anzures et al., 2014; Suhrke et al., 2014). For example, Chance et al. (1982) examined ORE in children from 6 to 14 years old, and found only older children (from 9 years on) showed ORE. Goodman et al. (2007) tested 5- to 13-year-old children, and also found the older children (from 8 years on) showed ORE. However, with improved methodologies, Anzures et al. (2014) found 5-year-old children had already showed ORE. Suhrke et al. (2014) even found significant ORE in 3-year-old children. These researches demonstrated that the age range of 3 to 13 is the crucial period for the development of ORE. In addition, this range of age is also the age period during which children's general face processing ability is thought to undergo a protracted development from immaturity in early childhood to maturity in late adolescence (Mondloch et al., 2003; for a review, see Lee et al., 2013). To uncover the neurodevelopmental pattern of face processing ability in general and that of the ORE in particular, the present study thus focused on children across this large age span.
We used the fNIRS methodology to obtain cortical neural response data in children between 3 and 13 years of age. Similar to fMRI, the fNIRS indirectly examines the brain activity through the measurement of the hemodynamic response (for reviews, see: Ferrari and Quaresima, 2012; Scholkmann et al., 2014). Although its spatial resolution is relatively inferior to fMRI, fNIRS presents some distinctive advantages for functionally neuroimaging the child population (Lloyd-Fox et al., 2010; Ding et al., 2014). First, fNIRS is much less susceptible to movement artifacts than fMRI and ERP, and is thus highly suitable for young children who are known to have difficult to control their spontaneous movements (Lloyd-Fox et al., 2010; Scholkmann et al., 2014). Second, fNIRS offers a less noisy and more comfortable experimental environment than fMRI to collect data from children in a more naturalistic manner (Scholkmann et al., 2014). Third, the operating costs of a NIRS machine are very limited, making it possible to test a large sample of children. Fourth, fNIRS signals are strongly correlated with those obtained with fMRI (Cui et al., 2011; Eggebrecht et al., 2012; Sato et al., 2013), making it possible to compare the results of fNIRS study with previous fMRI findings directly. Further and more relevant to the present research, compared to fMRI, fNIRS has higher temporal resolution (10 Hz) than the conventional fMRI (~1 Hz). Thus, fNIRS can more accurately characterize the temporal properties of the hemodynamic changes, making it specifically suitable for brain connectivity analysis (Scholkmann et al., 2014; Tak and Ye, 2014).
According to the recent functional neuroimaging findings regarding the development of face processing in children in general (Gathers et al., 2004; Aylward et al., 2005; Passarotti et al., 2007; Scherf et al., 2007) and functional connectivity specifically (Kadosh et al., 2011; Haist et al., 2013), regardless of face race, we expected more inter-area connections between the frontal and occipital brain areas with increased age, since previous research has found that the extended face network is hyper-activated around 10 years of age (Haist et al., 2013). Furthermore, according to the recent functional neuroimaging findings regarding the neural ORE in adults and children (Feng et al., 2011; Ding et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2015), we hypothesized that there would be differences in functional causal neural networks between own-race faces and other-race faces in children, and further this difference would change as a function of age. More specifically, there would be more inter-regional, but not intra-regional, connections (i.e., between the frontal and occipital brain areas) for own-race faces than other-race faces. In addition, according to the previous studies, because children have asymmetrical exposure to own-race vs. other-race faces (more exposure to the former than the latter), they would be more inclined to extract the identity level information from the visual input to process own-race faces than other-race faces. Thus, we hypothesized that with increased age there would be more connections from the occipital areas to the frontal areas for own-race faces than other-race faces.
Materials and Methods
Participants
A total of 124 children (Male: 61; Mean age = 7.77 years, SD = 2.80 years, rang from 3.25 to 13.49 years) participated in the study. They were from preschools or primary schools in a medium sized city in Zhejiang Province, China, where 99.9% of the population was Han Chinese. All children were healthy, right-hand Han Chinese children with normal or correct-to-normal vision. According to the parental reports, they had never had direct contact with individuals from racial groups other than Han Chinese. A written informed consent was signed by parents or legal guardians of all children before their participation. Children also gave their oral consent. The present study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Zhejiang Normal University.
Stimuli
The materials used in the present study included 40 black-and-white face photos of 20 female Chinese and 20 female Caucasian young adults. All photos were digitized in 24-bit depths with a resolution of 450 × 600 pixels and had not been seen by participants before the present study. Using Adobe Photoshop, we overlaid a black oval frame on the photo such that each face's ears, hair, and contour were concealed so as to prevent participants from using non-key face information to recognize the face. Each face was centered with a frontal view and presented in a neutral expression. In addition, the low-level physical attributes such as brightness and contrast were balanced using the SHINE program (Willenbockel et al., 2010).
Experimental Procedure
We used an old/new paradigm to assess children's face recognition ability (Goodman et al., 2007; Ding et al., 2014). All of children were asked to perform two tasks, namely Chinese faces task (CH) and Caucasian faces task (CA), separately. The CH task only included Chinese face images and consisted of a learning phase followed by a testing phase. During the learning phase, each participant was required to remember 10 target Chinese faces successively (i.e., 10 trials in total), each of which was presented on the computer screen for 3 s followed by an interval of 2 s fixation (“*”). Then, during testing phase, the 10 “old” faces that had been learned were mixed with additional 10 “new” faces that had never been seen before. Each face image was presented for 3 s followed by an interval of 10 s fixation (“*”), during which the child was asked to make decision as to whether the currently seen face had been viewed in the learning phase via button presses. The brain functional data were recorded only in the testing phase because our research aimed to reveal neural activations associated with recognizing own- vs. other-race faces to reveal the neural ORE. The CA task used the same paradigm as that of CH task except that Chinese faces were replaced by Caucasian faces (see Figure 1). The order of two tasks was counterbalanced between participants.
Figure 1. The experimental procedure. Two trials are displayed as samples to explain the procedure of Chinese faces task (A,B) and Caucasian faces task (C,D) in both the learning phase and the testing phase, respectively. The hand icon during the testing phase denoted that participants were required to make decision whether the face had been seen in the learning phase by pressing the keyboard.
Before the formal experiment, children were instructed to complete practice trials to insure that they understood the task requirements. The practice trials were identical to the experimental trials except that only four Chinese faces and four Caucasian faces were used. The faces were not used in the formal experiment.
Behavior Data Acquisition
A Windows desktop computer presented the stimuli using E-prime 1.2 (PST Inc., U.S.A) and recorded children's behavior data. Elementary school children responded by pressing either “1” (an “old” target face) or “2” (a “new” foil face) themselves on the keyboard in each trial during the testing phase (key assignments were also counterbalanced between participants). However, for preschool children, considering their generally limited executive function ability, the experimenter pressed the keys on their behalf based on the children's oral report. Thus, no response latency data were directly collected from the preschool children. As a result, the present study only used recognition accuracy to measure both preschool and elementary school children's behavioral performance in own- and other-race face recognition.
fNIRS Data Acquisition
A multichannel continuous-wave NIRS system (ETG-4000, Hitachi Medical Co., Japan) was used to acquire and record the optical density changes, from which we obtained the local changes of oxygenated hemoglobin ([oxy-Hb]) and deoxygenated hemoglobin ([doxy-Hb]) concentrations using the modified Beer-Lambert Law (Cope and Delpy, 1988; Scholkmann et al., 2014). ETG-4000 measures the optical density changes with two near-infrared lights (wavelengths ~695 and ~830 nm at each source fiber). Two sets of probes of ETG-4000 were mounted on a plastic helmet with a 30 cm source–detector distance, resulting in 46 measurement channels (See Figure 2). The placement of optodes was performed according to the international 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958). One set consisted of a pair of 3 × 3 arrays with five light emitters and four detectors, forming 24 channels, with its lowest probes positioned along the Fp1–Fp2 line to primarily cover the frontal cortex. The second other set consisted of a 3 × 5 array with eight light emitters and seven detectors, forming the 22 channels, with its lowest probes positioned along the O1–O2 line to primarily cover the occipital cortex. These measured brain areas are thought to be involved in processing faces stimuli (Haxby et al., 2000; Fairhall and Ishai, 2007; Ito and Bartholow, 2009). The sampling rate of ETG-4000 was set to 10 Hz. The communication between the task computer and ETG-4000 was implemented by E-prime.
Figure 2. The corresponding MNI locations and AAL labels of all channels. The MNI locations of all channels were showed in the frontal view (A) and occipital view (B) respectively. No. 1 ~ No. 24 channels (A) were located almost entirely in the frontal lobe, and No. 25 ~ No. 46 channels (B) mainly covered the occipital cortex. From a sketch (C), the 46 channels overlapped the 21 corresponding anatomical cortex regions according to the AAL template, namely SFGdor, the dorsolateral superior frontal gyrus; MFG, the middle frontal gyrus; IFGtriang, the triangular inferior frontal gyrus; IFGoperc, the opercula inferior frontal gyrus; PreCG, the precentral gyrus; SPG, the superior parietal gyrus; ANG, the angular gyrus; CUN, the cuneus; SOG, the superior occipital gyrus; MOG, the middle occipital gyrus; IOG, the inferior occipital gyrus; CAL, the calcarine cortex. L indicates the left hemisphere, and R indicates the right hemisphere.
After children finished the experiment, their relative spatial coordinates of the probes were measured by a 3D digitizer (PATRIOT, Polhemus Co., U.S.A), with its reference points defined as the following five anatomical landmarks: nasion, inion, Cz, the anterior tragus point of the left ear, and the anterior tragus point of the right ear. The coordinates of the probes were then used to calculate the coordinates of the 46 NIRS channels. These spatial data were then normalized using NIRS-SPM (Near Infrared Spectroscopy-Statistical Parametric Mapping) software package (Ye et al., 2009; Tak and Ye, 2014). We conducted spatial registrations of the NIRS channels according to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard coordinate system using algorithms by Singh et al. (2005) (See Table 1). Then, the neuroanatomical labels for each channel locations were determined according to the widely adopted AAL (automated anatomical labeling) template (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002; See Figure 2). Additional, we obtained probabilistic estimations of the Brodmann area (BA) that each channel was on (Brodmann, 2007; See Table 1).
Preprocessing for fNIRS Data
Although we obtained the [oxy-Hb] and [doxy-Hb] concentration change data concurrently, for the present study, we only analyzed the [oxy-Hb] data because [deoxy-Hb] signals have been found to be highly noisy and unreliable (Strangman et al., 2002; Homae et al., 2007). The fNIRS data preprocessing was implemented in NIRS-SPM software package based on MATLAB. The time series of the [oxy-Hb] concentration changes for each channel was first low-pass filtered with the shape of the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) in statistical parametric mapping (SPM; Friston et al., 2003; Ye et al., 2009). Then, the detrending based on the wavelet-minimum description length algorithm (Wavelet-MDL) was applied to eliminate global drift produced by breathing, cardiac motion, vasomotion, body motion, and so on (Jang et al., 2009; Ye et al., 2009). In other words, the preprocessing of fNIRS data removed low- and high-frequency noises to improve the signal-to-noise ratio so as to extract the task related fNIRS signals. In addition, the baseline correction by linear least-squares fitting was performed for each channel based on the two baseline data: the mean of 2-s periods pre-/post-task sections.
Granger Causality Analysis (GCA)
To investigate whether and how own- and other-race faces recognition processes differed in terms of functional neural connectivity, we performed the Granger causality analysis (GCA) with the preprocessed [oxy-Hb] time series data. GCA is able to explore the directed effective connectivity among multiple brain cortices based on vector auto-regressive models (VAR; Seth et al., 2015), and thus offers the potential for defining the functional circuits underlying neural interactions (Goebel et al., 2003; Yuan, 2013). Concerning the two face race tasks in the present study, we tested Granger causality for every participant under either the own- or other-race faces task conditions, respectively. More specifically, a Granger causality testing was accomplished in the following steps:
First, a linear auto-regression model of each channel's preprocessed [oxy-Hb] temporal sequence data based on their past observations was built to estimate their error of prediction. For example, assuming that XA and XB were time series of Channel A and Channel B, respectively. XA and XB could be fitted with two autoregressive models of order p (Goebel et al., 2003):
Where p determined by Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and the residuals of the two models (i.e., εA and εB) were the white noise process (Yuan, 2013).
Next, the auto-regression of XA was augmented by including previous observations of XB; similarly, the auto-regression of XB was augmented by including previous observations of XA:
Then, we would test whether the inclusion of past observations of one channel particularly benefited the reduction of another's prediction error to define the causal relationship between these two channels (Goebel et al., 2003; Sato et al., 2010; Yuan, 2013). We defined the magnitude of Granger causality from B to A as:
In other words, according to Granger causality a variable B “causes” a variable A if the past of B helps predict the future of A better than can be done by knowing only the past of A. Similarly, we defined the magnitude of Granger causality from A to B as:
In the present study, for each participant and under each task condition, the causal map was obtained by calculating the weights of Granger causal relations between the time series of [oxy-Hb] of each pair of channels. For each pair of channels, a two-way repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on their GCA weights with the face race task (own-race face vs. other-race face) as the within-subject factor and the age as the between-subject factor (continuous variable).
To ensure reliability of the ANOVA results and reduce the likelihood of false discoveries, capitalizing the large sample size of the present study, we used the Bootstrapping-based method. Bootstrapping is a method to verify the reliability of statistical findings with the use of random down-sampling of the sample of the data on hand. It has been widely used in various fields (Davison and Hinkley, 1997; Lee and Rodgers, 1998; Burleigh et al., 2006). Specifically, we first randomly down-sampled 50 percent of the participants to form a subset, then we performed the ANOVA of face race by age on the participants' GCA weights in this subset. Then, instead of performing an F-test, we only computed the partial eta-squared effect size statistics () in ANOVA. We repeated this step 10,000 times. As a result, we obtained 10,000 partial eta-squared effect size statistics. Finally, we obtained the averaged effect size by calculating the mean value of these 10,000 partial eta-squared effect size statistics.
Then, to estimate the statistical significance of the averaged effect size, we conducted a permutation test. First, we down-sample 50% of the participants as a new sample and randomly scrambled the participants' GCA weights from the own- race and other-race conditions into two new pseudo conditions (a pseudo “own-race” condition and a pseudo “other-race” condition). We then performed the same ANOVA as mentioned above to obtain the partial eta-squared effect size statistics of the scrambled data set. Again, we repeated this step 10,000 times to obtained 10,000 partial eta-squared effect size statistics that forms a distribution of the effect sizes based on the scrambled data. Using this random distribution of effect sizes, we then compared the mean effect size based on the real data to determine whether the mean effect size was significantly different from the chance. We considered the effect sizes to be significant only when they were in the top 5% of the random distribution.
Results
Because all participants of the present study were Chinese, we will henceforth refer to Chinese faces as own-race faces while Caucasian faces as other-race faces, respectively.
Behavior Results
According to previous studies, we referred to better recognition performance for own-race faces than own-race faces as ORE. The mean accuracy of own-race faces recognition and that of other-race faces was 66.01 ± 14.86% and 64 ± 13.97%, respectively. The repeated measure 2 (stimulus race: own vs. other race) × 1 (age in years: continuous) ANOVA was performed on accuracy with stimulus race as a within-subject variable and children's age as a continuous variable. Only the main effect of age was significant, F(1, 122) = 88.72, p < 0.001, = 0.42: with increased age, children's accuracy improved significantly for both own- and other-race faces. Neither the interaction nor the main effect of face race was significant [The interaction effect: F(1, 122) = 0.04, p = 0.84, < 0.001; the main effect of face race: F(1, 122) = 0.60, p = 0.44, < 0.001; See Table 2].
Table 2. Means (SD) recognition accuracy of Own- and Other-race faces and results of one repeated measure of ANOVA with face race as a within-subject variable and age as a continuous variable.
GCA Results
We obtained two Granger causal networks, one for own-race faces, and the other for other-race faces. To obtain own-race face Granger causal networks, the effective connectivities were calculated with different intensities and directions among the 46 channels while children were recognizing the own-race faces. To obtain the other-race face Granger causal networks, the effective connectivities were calculated with different intensities and directions among the 46 channels while children were recognizing the other-race faces. In keeping with the definition for the behavioral ORE for own-race faces relative to other-race faces, we defined the Neural ORE as the differences in effective connectivity intensity between own-race faces causal networks and other-race face causal networks.
To examine whether the own-race face causal networks differed from the other-race face causal networks in children, as mentioned above, we conducted 2 (race: own-race faces vs. other-race faces) × 1 (age in years: continuous) repeated measures ANOVAs with intensity of each effective connectivity as the dependent variable. Specifically, we used the Bootstrapping method mentioned above to perform significance test. Here we report the significant test results based on the Bootstrapping.
We found significant main effects of age for 77 effective connections (See Figure 3). Among them, 63 connections in Figure 3A increased significantly with age (P's < 0.05), whereas 14 connections in Figure 3B decreased with increased age (P's < 0.05). These causal connections revealed the overall developmental pattern in face processing from 3 to 13 years of age. More specifically, regardless of face race, more causal connections were strengthened with the increased age. Among them, 31 were inter-area connections (4 connections from the frontal area to the occipital area, 27 connections from the occipital area to the frontal area) and 32 were intra-area connections (25 connections in the occipital area, and 7 connections in the frontal area). In contrast, only a few connections weakened with increased age, among which 2 were inter-area connections (from the frontal area to the occipital area) and 12 were intra-area connections (all in the frontal area).
Figure 3. The Granger causal network with a significant main effect of age. The red connections in (A) presented the positive correlation between age and Granger weights regardless of face race, while the blue connections in (B) presented the negative correlation between age and Granger weights regardless of face race.
We also found significant main effects of face race for 10 causal connections (See Figure 4), suggesting that regardless of age, the effective connectivities differed significantly for own-race vs. other-race faces causal networks. More specifically, as shown in Figure 4A, 4 effective connections in the own-race face causal network were stronger than those in the other-race face network (P's < 0.05). Among these connections, 2 were inter-area connections (1 connection from the frontal area to the occipital area, and 1 connection from the occipital area to the frontal area) and 2 were intra-area connections (1 connection in the frontal area, and 1 connection in the occipital area). In contrast, there were 6 connections from the other-race face causal network that were stronger than those in the own-race face network (P's < 0.05; See Figure 4B). These connections were intra-area connections (all connections in the frontal area).
Figure 4. The Granger causal network with a significant main effect of Face race. The red connections in (A) showed that the own-race faces elicited greater Granger causality than that of other-race faces regardless of age, while the blue connections in (B) showed that the other-race faces elicited greater Granger causality than that of own-race faces regardless of age.
Further and specifically relevant to our age-related hypotheses regarding the neural ORE, the repeated measures ANOVAs yielded significant face race by age interactions for nine connections (p's < 0.05; See Figure 5). To future explore these significant interactions, we calculated the Pearson correlations between age in years and the Neural ORE. The results also are presented in Figure 6 and Table 3.
Figure 5. The Granger causal network with a significant interaction effect of Face Race (Chinese faces task vs. Caucasian faces task) and Age (from 3 to 13 years-old). (A) Presented a positive correlation between the Neural ORE and age, while (B) presented a negative correlation between the Neural ORE and age. The red and green connections in (A) represented two subtypes of positive development. Similarly, the blue and purple connections in (B) represented two subtypes of negative development.
Figure 6. The scatter plot of Pearson correlations between Age in years and the NORE. The red plots in (A–C) were corresponding scatter plot of Pearson correlation for the first positive subtype, and the green plots in (D–G) were corresponding scatter plot of Pearson correlation for the second positive subtype. Similarly, the blue plot in (H) were corresponding scatter plot of Pearson correlation for the first negative subtype, and the purple plots in (I) were corresponding scatter plot of Pearson correlation for the second negative subtype.
Table 3. The coefficients of Pearson correlation between age in years and the NORE for the connections with the significant interactions.
As shown in Figure 5, nine connections with the significant interactions had two different trends. Seven connections (Figures 5A, 6A–G) presented a positive correlation between the Neural ORE and age: The neural ORE were significantly greater with increased age. There were two subtypes of developmental patterns associated with this positive correlation. One positive pattern was that the connection intensity of 3 inter-area connections (all from the occipital area to the frontal area) in the own-race face causal network increased with age but those in the other-race face causal network did not (Figures 6A–C, 5A red lines). The other pattern was that the intensity of 3 intra-area connections (all in the frontal area) and 1 inter-area connection (from the occipital area to the frontal area) in the own-race causal network increased with age whereas those in the other-race face network decreased with age (Figures 6D–G, 5A green lines).
In addition, there were the other two connections whereby the Neural ORE decreased with age (Figures 5B, 6H,I). Among them, there were also two subtypes in developmental patterns associated with this negative correlation. One pattern was that the intensity of one inter-area connection (from the occipital area to the frontal area) in the other-race face network increased with age but that in the own-race face network did not (Figures 6H, 5B blue line). The other pattern was that the intensity of one intra-are connection (in the occipital area) in the other-race network increased with age, whereas that in the own-race face network decreased with age (Figures 6I, 5B purple line). For the cortical areas that formed significant causal connections described above, see Table 4.
Discussion
The present study, using fNIRS methodology, examined the Neural ORE by comparing the causal network differences between own-race face and other-race face recognition in a large sample of preschool and elementary school children. We first obtained the Granger causal connectivity between each pair of channels when children with different ages recognized own-race faces and other-race faces, and then performed repeated measures ANOVAs on each Granger causal connectivity with age and face race (own-race faces vs. other-race faces) as independent variables. To ensure result reliability and minimize false discoveries, we used the Bootstrapping-based method. To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the developmental trajectory of the neural ORE on the basis of effective connectivity analyses.
About the Granger Analysis Results
First, consistent with our hypothesis, we found robust and widespread significant main effects of age. This effect revealed that, regardless of face race, the overall causal connectivity underlying individual face recognition changed with age. We found that the majority of causal connections increased with age, whereas only a few of connections decreased with age. These findings suggest that with increased age, children recruit an increasingly more complex network for face recognition. This age-related change in functional connectivity parallels that in behavior. It is well-established that as age increases, children develop greater expertise in face recognition. More specifically, they become increasingly able to gather larger amounts of information about the faces they see and do so at faster speeds (Mondloch et al., 2003, 2010; Lee et al., 2013). Our findings suggest that the increase in the ability to recognize faces efficiently may involve an increase in functional connectivity complexity between the different brain regions of the extended face processing network. This increase in network functional connectivity may be needed to support the different stages of face processing, such as perception of face shape, integration of local face features, and retrieval of personal information, and face sematic information (e.g., race, gender, familiarity).
We found that the age-related enhancement of functional connectivity included two patterns of connections: One was the increase in the intra-area connection inside the occipital visual cortex or inside the frontal cortex, and the other was in the inter-area connection between the occipital visual cortex and the frontal cortex. Regarding with the intra-area connection, the occipital visual cortex is believed to be part of the core face network and important for face feature detection (Pitcher et al., 2011; Arcurio et al., 2012; Joseph et al., 2012). Previous studies also found that the OFA/FFA connectivity in the occipital area increased with age (He et al., 2015; Song et al., 2015). Thus, although fNIRS is unable to reach the depth of OFA/FFA, the increased connectivity inside the occipital area indicated that children become more sensitive to the face features with increased age. The frontal cortex areas, especially the MFG and IFG, are believed to be involved in more complex face processing such as linking faces to sematic information such as familiarities, categories (e.g., race; Haxby et al., 1996, 2000; Ishai et al., 2005; Fairhall and Ishai, 2007). Thus, the increased connectivity inside the frontal cortex suggested that children became increasing active in integrating face sematic information. Regarding with the inter-area connection, since the face processing involves a core network in the occipital-temporal cortex (Rossion et al., 2003; Pitcher et al., 2011) and the extended network that includes the frontal cortex (Renzi et al., 2013), it is not surprising that, with increased age, children had increased connectivity between the occipital cortex and the frontal cortex. In addition, we also found that with increased age, there were more effective connectivity from the occipital area to the frontal area than from the frontal area to the occipital area. This finding is consistent with the finding of Joseph et al. (2012) who found increased connectivities between the primary visual area and the right inferior orbitofrontal area as the age increased. It was suggested that this increase reflected an increased face processing expertise to process faces at the individual level.
Second, we also found that only a limited number of connections showed significant main effects of face race on the functional connectivity between different cortical areas, in sharp contrast to the large number of connections showing the main effect of age. This is consistent with the behavioral and neuroimaging findings that although both behavioral and neural OREs are often observed in children and adults (Chance et al., 1982; Walker and Hewstone, 2006; Scott et al., 2007; Anzures et al., 2014; Ding et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2015), the size of the main effects of face race is typically small. The present finding is consistent with this general finding, suggesting that there may exist a limited number of functional connections that differentiate between own- and other-race faces regardless of age. One possibility is that the differentiation between own- and other-race faces may change with increased age.
Third, we found significant face race by age interactions for nine connections, indicating that the differences in effective connectivity elicited by own-race faces relative to other-race faces were indeed modulated by age. For the connectivities with significant interaction effects, some of them showed a positive correlation between children's age and the difference in causal connectivity of own-race faces relative to other-race faces. In other words, the older the children's age, the greater the causal connectivity elicited by own-race faces than other-race faces. In fact, this positive correlation between age and ORE-related connectivity has two developmental patterns.
The first pattern was that there were initially no significant differences in connectivity between own-race face and other-race faces in younger children. However, with increased age, the causal connectivity elicited by own-race faces was increasingly enhanced, whereas those elicited by other-race faces did not change with age. Thus, in the end, with increased age, own-race faces elicited stronger causal connectivity than that elicited by other-race faces. This developmental pattern was seen among connections from the occipital visual cortex to the frontal cortex (i.e., the right IFG and the left SFG, Figure 5A, red lines). It has been suggested that the right IFG is part of the extended neural system for face recognition, and this region plays a crucial role in the storage and retrieval of such semantic information as familiarity, race, and identity (Haxby et al., 1996, 2000; Fairhall and Ishai, 2007; Ito and Bartholow, 2009). The left SFG is also reported to be involved in memory processing of faces (Petrides, 2005; Li et al., 2013). With increased age, there is an increased demand for children to learn to discriminate individuals due to their expanding social milieu that typically only involve own-race individuals. As a result, children's core and extended face processing network might need to be coupled more closely to address this increased processing demand, which might become automated with increased experience in processing own-race faces (Levin, 2000; Gauthier and Nelson, 2001; Lee et al., 2013; Senholzi and Ito, 2013; Liu et al., 2015).
In contrast, people have few experience of other-race face recognition, and therefore fail to automatically recognize other-race faces at individual level (Levin, 2000; Young et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2015). Although in the present study, both own-race faces and other-race faces are asked to be individually recognized, the mechanism of the individual processing of other-race faces is different from that of automatic individual processing of own-race faces (Levin, 2000; Senholzi and Ito, 2013; Liu et al., 2015). As mentioned before, the own-race faces need to extract more information from visual input or take finer processing on this visual information, and accordingly was represented by the stronger interaction or connectivity from the occipital visual cortex to the frontal cortex.
The second positive pattern was that the other-race faces initially elicited significantly stronger causal connectivity than own-race faces in younger children. However, with increased age, the causal connectivity elicited by own-race faces was enhanced, whereas those elicited by other-race faces either did not change with age or even decreased. Thus, in the end, own-race faces elicited connectivity either equal to or even stronger causal connectivity than, that elicited by other-race faces. This developmental pattern was mainly seen among connection from the left frontal cortex to the right frontal cortex (Figure 5A, green lines). Recent studies have suggested that the left hemisphere may play an important role in the processing of local features (e.g., facial feature), whereas the right hemisphere may play a dominant role in holistic processing (Hillger and Koenig, 1991; Maurer et al., 2007; Cattaneo et al., 2014). The automatic individual recognition of own-race faces is highly dependent on holistic processing (Rhodes et al., 2006; Michel et al., 2006a,b, 2010; Hancock and Rhodes, 2008). With increased age, the gradually increased expertise at processing own-race faces might have led to more automatic holistic processing that needs to integrate both local feature information (e.g., the shapes of eyes) and spatial information (e.g., the relative spatial relations between facial features). Thus, it is likely that this positive pattern reflected age-related changes in connectivity from the left frontal cortex to the right cortex for automatic integration of local information into holistic processing.
We also found two negative age-related patterns: As age increased, causal connectivity elicited by other-race face became greater than own-race faces. One of them was the causal connectivity from the left cuneus to the left IFG (Figure 5B, blue lines). For this connection, there are no significant differences of causal connectivity between own-race faces and other-race faces among the younger children. However, this connection presented stronger connectivity for other-race faces than own-race faces among the older children. The enhanced connectivity of this connection when comparing other-race faces to own-race faces may be due to an additional demand to decode other-race face feature information (Levin, 2000; Senholzi and Ito, 2013; Liu et al., 2015). Another causal connectivity with negative correlation was from the left SOG to the left MOG (Figure 5B, purple lines). This connection shows stronger for own-race faces than other-race faces at the younger age, but with increased age, the difference between own- and other-race disappeared. This finding was unexpected and awaits further verification and investigation.
About the Behavior Results
It should be noted that the present study failed to obtain behavioral ORE from our child participants. Although the robust behavioral ORE has been well-established in adults, there is great inconsistence about the behavior ORE of children (Goodman et al., 2007; for a review, see Anzures et al., 2013b). As children are sensitive to the changes of experimental conditions, the inconsistence of the behavior ORE among the existing studies might be due to the different experimental environments or methods used in these studies. For example, to prevent children from using the non-key facial features (e.g., face contour or hairstyle) to recognize faces, we used an oval to cover such cues, which might have made the stimuli less naturalistic. Indeed, previous studies that used full faces typically produced behavioral ORE (e.g., Anzures et al., 2010), whereas those that used stimuli similar to ours failed to obtain behavioral ORE (e.g., Hu et al., 2014). Nevertheless, despite the lack of behavioral ORE, we found significant differences in neural network connectivity between own-race faces and other-race faces. Our neural imaging finding thus suggests that the neural responses might be more sensitive than behavior responses to own- vs. other-race face processing differences, in line with the suggestion of Balas et al. (2011).
Conclusion
The present study examined the causal neural networks for own- and other-race face recognition in preschool and elementary school children. We found significant age-related increases in functional connectivity in children's face processing neural network in general and inter-regional connectivity specifically, paralleling a similar age-related improvement in behavioral face processing ability. In addition, we also found that significant differences in neural functional connectivity between the recognition of own-race faces and that of other-race faces were modulated by age. These finding suggested that like the behavioral ORE, the neural ORE also emerges in early childhood and undergoes a protracted developmental course. The present study also demonstrated the advantages of using fNIRS methodology to study the neural functional connectivity associated with perceptual-cognitive development.
Author Contributions
Experimental design and data recording: JL, GF, KL, and XD; Experimental data analyze: GZ and JL; Manuscript writing: GZ and JL; Manuscript revision: JL, KL, and XD.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This paper is supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant No. 61375110, 31470993, 61673051, 31371041 the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2015JBM031) and NIH (R01HD046526).
References
Anzures, G., Kelly, D. J., Pascalis, O., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., De Viviés, X., et al. (2014). Own-and other-race face identity recognition in children: the effects of pose and feature composition. Dev. Psychol. 50, 469. doi: 10.1037/a0033166
Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., and Lee, K. (2010). Categorization, categorical perception, and asymmetry in infants' representation of face race. Dev. Sci. 13, 553–564. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00900.x
Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., and Lee, K. (2013b). Development of own-race biases. Vis. Cogn. 21, 1165–1182. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.821428
Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., and Lee, K. (2013a). Developmental origins of the other-race effect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 173–178. doi: 10.1177/0963721412474459
Arcurio, L. R., Gold, J. M., and James, T. W. (2012). The response of face-selective cortex with single face parts and part combinations. Neuropsychologia 50, 2454–2459. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.016
Aylward, E. H., Park, J. E., Field, K. M., Parsons, A. C., Richards, T. L., Cramer, S. C., et al. (2005). Brain activation during face perception: evidence of a developmental change. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 308–319. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124884
Balas, B., Westerlund, A., Hung, K., and Nelson, C. A. III. (2011). Shape, color and the other-race effect in the infant brain. Dev. Sci. 14, 892–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01039.x
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551
Brodmann, K. (2007). Brodmann's: Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.
Burleigh, J. G., Driskell, A. C., and Sanderson, M. J. (2006). Supertree bootstrapping methods for assessing phylogenetic variation among genes in genome-scale data sets. Syst. Biol. 55, 426–440. doi: 10.1080/10635150500541722
Cattaneo, Z., Renzi, C., Bona, S., Merabet, L. B., Carbon, C. C., and Vecchi, T. (2014). Hemispheric asymmetry in discriminating faces differing for featural or configural (second-order relations) aspects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 363–369. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0484-2
Chance, J. E., Turner, A. L., and Goldstein, A. G. (1982). Development of differential recognition for own-and other-race faces. J. Psychol. 112, 29–37. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1982.9923531
Cope, M., and Delpy, D. T. (1988). System for long-term measurement of cerebral blood and tissue oxygenation on newborn infants by near infra-red transillumination. Medi. Biol. Eng. Comput. 26, 289–294. doi: 10.1007/BF02447083
Cui, X., Bray, S., Bryant, D. M., Glover, G. H., and Reiss, A. L. (2011). A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 54, 2808–2821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.069
Davison, A. C., and Hinkley, D. V. (1997). Bootstrap Methods and Their Application, Vol. 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ding, X. P., Fu, G., and Lee, K. (2014). Neural correlates of own-and other-race face recognition in children: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroimage 85, 335–344. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.051
Eggebrecht, A. T., White, B. R., Ferradal, S. L., Chen, C., Zhan, Y., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2012). A quantitative spatial comparison of high-density diffuse optical tomography and fMRI cortical mapping. Neuroimage 61, 1120–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.124
Fairhall, S. L., and Ishai, A. (2007). Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical network for face perception. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2400–2406. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl148
Feng, L., Liu, J., Wang, Z., Li, J., Li, L., Ge, L., et al. (2011). The other face of the other-race effect: an fMRI investigation of the other-race face categorization advantage. Neuropsychologia 49, 3739–3749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.031
Ferrari, M., and Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage 63, 921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049
Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008
Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J. O. H. N., and Heather, J. (2003). “Statistical parametric mapping,” in Neuroscience Databases: A Practical Guide, ed R. Kötter (Berlin; Heidelberg: Kluwer Academic Publishers (KAP)), 237–250.
Gathers, A. D., Bhatt, R., Corbly, C. R., Farley, A. B., and Joseph, J. E. (2004). Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and object recognition. Neuroreport 15, 1549–1553. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000133299.84901.86
Gauthier, I., and Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development of face expertise. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00200-2
Ge, L., Zhang, H., Wang, Z., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Kelly, D., et al. (2009). Two faces of the other-race effect: recognition and categorisation of Caucasian and Chinese faces. Perception 38, 1199. doi: 10.1068/p6136
Goebel, R., Roebroeck, A., Kim, D. S., and Formisano, E. (2003). Investigating directed cortical interactions in time-resolved fMRI data using vector autoregressive modeling and Granger causality mapping. Magn. Reson. Imaging 21, 1251–1261. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2003.08.026
Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Chiao, J. Y., and Eberhardt, J. L. (2001). Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 845–850. doi: 10.1038/90565
Goodman, G. S., Sayfan, L., Lee, J. S., Sandhei, M., Walle-Olsen, A., Magnussen, S., et al. (2007). The development of memory for own-and other-race faces. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 98, 233–242. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.004
Granger, C. W. J. (1969). Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica 37, 424–438. doi: 10.2307/1912791
Haist, F., Adamo, M., Wazny, J. H., Lee, K., and Stiles, J. (2013). The functional architecture for face-processing expertise: FMRI evidence of the developmental trajectory of the core and the extended face systems. Neuropsychologia 51, 2893–2908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.005
Hancock, K. J., and Rhodes, G. (2008). Contact, configural coding and the other-race effect in face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 99, 45–56. doi: 10.1348/000712607X199981
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0
Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Horwitz, B., Maisog, J. M., Rapoport, S. I., and Grady, C. L. (1996). Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922–927. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.922
Hayden, A., Bhatt, R. S., Joseph, J. E., and Tanaka, J. W. (2007). The other-race effect in infancy: evidence using a morphing technique. Infancy 12, 95–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00235.x
He, W., Garrido, M. I., Sowman, P. F., Brock, J., and Johnson, B. W. (2015). Development of effective connectivity in the core network for face perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2161–2173. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22762
Hillger, L. A., and Koenig, O. (1991). Separable mechanisms in face processing: evidence from hemispheric specialization. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.1.42
Holper, L., Scholkmann, F., and Wolf, M. (2012). Between-brain connectivity during imitation measured by fNIRS. Neuroimage 63, 212–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.028
Homae, F., Watanabe, H., Nakano, T., and Taga, G. (2007). Prosodic processing in the developing brain. Neurosci. Res. 59, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.05.005
Hu, C., Wang, Q., Fu, G., Quinn, P. C., and Lee, K. (2014). Both children and adults scan faces of own and other races differently. Vision Res. 102, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.010
Ishai, A., Schmidt, C. F., and Boesiger, P. (2005). Face perception is mediated by a distributed cortical network. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.05.027
Ito, T. A., and Bartholow, B. D. (2009). The neural correlates of race. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 524–531. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.002
Ito, T. A., and Urland, G. R. (2005). The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: an ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 21–36. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.1.21
Jang, K. E., Tak, S., Jung, J., Jang, J., Jeong, Y., and Ye, J. C. (2009). Wavelet minimum description length detrending for near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Biomed. Opt. 14, 034004. doi: 10.1117/1.3127204
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 10, 371–375.
Joseph, J. E., Swearingen, J. E., Clark, J. D., Benca, C. E., Collins, H. R., Corbly, C. R., et al. (2012). The changing landscape of functional brain networks for face processing in typical development. Neuroimage 63, 1223–1236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.021
Kadosh, K. C., Kadosh, R. C., Dick, F., and Johnson, M. H. (2011). Developmental changes in effective connectivity in the emerging core face network. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1389–1394. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq215
Kamiński, M., Ding, M., Truccolo, W. A., and Bressler, S. L. (2001). Evaluating causal relations in neural systems: Granger causality, directed transfer function and statistical assessment of significance. Biol. Cybern. 85, 145–157. doi: 10.1007/s004220000235
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311.
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., et al. (2009). Development of the other-race effect during infancy: evidence toward universality? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 104, 105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.006
Kim, J. S., Yoon, H. W., Kim, B. S., Jeun, S. S., Jung, S. L., and Choe, B. Y. (2006). Racial distinction of the unknown facial identity recognition mechanism by event-related fMRI. Neurosci. Lett. 397, 279–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.061
Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., and Slater, A. (2013). “12 development of face-processing ability in childhood,” in The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 1, Body and Mind, ed P. D. Zelazo (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 338.
Lee, W. C., and Rodgers, J. L. (1998). Bootstrapping correlation coefficients using univariate and bivariate sampling. Psychol. Methods 3:91. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.1.91
Levin, D. T. (2000). Race as a visual feature: using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129:559. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.4.559
Li, W., Qin, W., Liu, H., Fan, L., Wang, J., Jiang, T., et al. (2013). Subregions of the human superior frontal gyrus and their connections. Neuroimage 78, 46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.011
Liu, J., Wang, Z., Feng, L., Li, J., Tian, J., and Lee, K. (2015). Neural trade-offs between recognizing and categorizing own-and other-race faces. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2191–2203. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu025
Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., and Elwell, C. E. (2010). Illuminating the developing brain: the past, present and future of functional near infrared spectroscopy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.008
Malpass, R. S., and Kravitz, J. (1969). Recognition for faces of own and other race. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 13, 330. doi: 10.1037/h0028434
Maurer, D., O'craven, K. M., Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Springer, M. V., Lewis, T. L., et al. (2007). Neural correlates of processing facial identity based on features versus their spacing. Neuropsychologia 45, 1438–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.016
Meissner, C. A., and Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Public Policy Law 7:3. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3
Michel, C., Caldara, R., and Rossion, B. (2006a). Same-race faces are perceived more holistically than other-race faces. Vis. Cogn. 14, 55–73. doi: 10.1080/13506280500158761
Michel, C., Corneille, O., and Rossion, B. (2010). Holistic face encoding is modulated by perceived face race: evidence from perceptual adaptation. Vis. Cogn. 18, 434–455. doi: 10.1080/13506280902819697
Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, C. S., and Caldara, R. (2006b). Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one's own race. Psychol. Sci. 17, 608–615. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01752.x
Mondloch, C. J., Geldart, S., Maurer, D., and Le Grand, R. (2003). Developmental changes in face processing skills. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 86, 67–84. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00102-4
Mondloch, C. J., Le Grand, R., and Maurer, D. (2010). “Development of expertise in face recognition,” in Perceptual Expertise; Bridging Brain and Behavior, eds I. Gauthier, M. J. Tarr, and D. Bub (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 67–106.
Natu, V., Raboy, D., and O'Toole, A. J. (2011). Neural correlates of own-and other-race face perception: spatial and temporal response differences. Neuroimage 54, 2547–2555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.006
Passarotti, A. M., Smith, J., DeLano, M., and Huang, J. (2007). Developmental differences in the neural bases of the face inversion effect show progressive tuning of face-selective regions to the upright orientation. Neuroimage 34, 1708–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.045
Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631
Pezdek, K., Blandon-Gitlin, I., and Moore, C. (2003). Children's face recognition memory: more evidence for the cross-race effect. J. Appl. Psychol. 88:760. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.760
Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., and Duchaine, B. (2011). The role of the occipital face area in the cortical face perception network. Exp. Brain Res. 209, 481–493. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2579-1
Rennels, J. L., and Davis, R. E. (2008). Facial experience during the first year. Infant Behav. Dev. 31, 665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.04.009
Renzi, C., Schiavi, S., Carbon, C. C., Vecchi, T., Silvanto, J., and Cattaneo, Z. (2013). Processing of featural and configural aspects of faces is lateralized in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a TMS study. Neuroimage 74, 45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.015
Rhodes, G., Hayward, W. G., and Winkler, C. (2006). Expert face coding: configural and component coding of own-race and other-race faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 499–505. doi: 10.3758/BF03193876
Rossion, B., Caldara, R., Seghier, M., Schuller, A. M., Lazeyras, F., and Mayer, E. (2003). A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain 126, 2381–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg241
Sangrigoli, S., and de Schonen, S. (2004). Recognition of own-race and other-race faces by three-month-old infants. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1219–1227. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00319.x
Sato, H., Yahata, N., Funane, T., Takizawa, R., Katura, T., Atsumori, H., et al. (2013). A NIRS–fMRI investigation of prefrontal cortex activity during a working memory task. Neuroimage 83, 158–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.043
Sato, J. R., Fujita, A., Cardoso, E. F., Thomaz, C. E., Brammer, M. J., and Amaro, E. (2010). Analyzing the connectivity between regions of interest: an approach based on cluster Granger causality for fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 52, 1444–1455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.022
Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., Humphreys, K., and Luna, B. (2007). Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. Dev. Sci. 10, F15–F30. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00595.x
Scholkmann, F., Kleiser, S., Metz, A. J., Zimmermann, R., Pavia, J. M., Wolf, U., et al. (2014). A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. Neuroimage 85, 6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004
Scott, L. S., Pascalis, O., and Nelson, C. A. (2007). A domain-general theory of the development of perceptual discrimination. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 197–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00503.x
Senholzi, K. B., and Ito, T. A. (2013). Structural face encoding: How task affects the N170's sensitivity to race. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 937–942. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss091
Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., and Barnett, L. (2015). Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging. J. Neurosci. 35, 3293–3297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4399-14.2015
Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019
Song, Y., Zhu, Q., Li, J., Wang, X., and Liu, J. (2015). Typical and atypical development of functional connectivity in the face network. J. Neurosci. 35, 14624–14635. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0969-15.2015
Sporer, S. L. (2001). The cross-race effect: beyond recognition of faces in the laboratory. Psychol. Public Policy Law 7:170. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.170
Strangman, G., Culver, J. P., Thompson, J. H., and Boas, D. A. (2002). A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation. Neuroimage 17, 719–731. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1227
Sugden, N. A., Mohamed-Ali, M. I., and Moulson, M. C. (2014). I spy with my little eye: typical, daily exposure to faces documented from a first-person infant perspective. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 249–261. doi: 10.1002/dev.21183
Suhrke, J., Freitag, C., Lamm, B., Teiser, J., Fassbender, I., Poloczek, S., et al. (2014). The other-race effect in 3-year-old German and Cameroonian children. Front. Psychol. 5:198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00198
Tak, S., and Ye, J. C. (2014). Statistical analysis of fNIRS data: a comprehensive review. Neuroimage 85, 72–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.016
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
Valdés-Sosa, P. A., Bornot-Sánchez, J. M., Vega-Hernández, M., Melie-García, L., Lage-Castellanos, A., and Canales-Rodríguez, E. (2006). “18 granger causality on spatial manifolds: applications to neuroimaging,” in Handbook of Time Series Analysis: Recent Theoretical Developments and Applications, eds B. Schelter, M. Winterhalder, and J. Timmer (Freiburg: Wiley-VCH), 461–491.
Walker, P. M., and Hewstone, M. (2006). A developmental investigation of other-race contact and the own-race face effect. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 451–463. doi: 10.1348/026151005X51239
Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiset, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., and Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 671–684. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.671
Ye, J. C., Tak, S., Jang, K. E., Jung, J., and Jang, J. (2009). NIRS-SPM: statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 44, 428–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036
Young, S. G., Hugenberg, K., Bernstein, M. J., and Sacco, D. F. (2012). Perception and motivation in face recognition a critical review of theories of the cross-race effect. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 116–142. doi: 10.1177/1088868311418987
Keywords: other-race effect, developmental ORE, face recognition, fNIRS, effective connectivity, Granger causal network
Citation: Zhou G, Liu J, Ding XP, Fu G and Lee K (2016) Development of Effective Connectivity during Own- and Other-Race Face Processing: A Granger Causality Analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:474. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00474
Received: 05 February 2016; Accepted: 07 September 2016;
Published: 22 September 2016.
Edited by:
Nobuo Masataka, Kyoto University, JapanReviewed by:
Satoru Otani, Ryotokuji University, JapanBenjamin J. Balas, North Dakota State University, USA
Hirokazu Doi, Nagasaki University, Japan
Copyright © 2016 Zhou, Liu, Ding, Fu and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jiangang Liu, bGl1amdAYmp0dS5lZHUuY24=
Genyue Fu, ZnVnZW55dWVAaHpudS5lZHUuY24=
Kang Lee, a2FuZy5sZWVAdXRvcm9udG8uY2E=