Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 June 2016
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience

Absence of Evidence or Evidence of Absence? Commentary: Captured by the pain: Pain steady-state evoked potentials are not modulated by selective spatial attention

  • 1Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
  • 2Center for Pain and the Brain, Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative and Pain Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

A commentary on
Captured by the pain: Pain steady-state evoked potentials are not modulated by selective spatial attention

by Blöchl, M., Franz, M., Miltner, W. H. R., and Weiss, T. (2015). Brain Res. 1603, 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.01.035

A broad range of cognitive factors modulates behavioral, and cortical responses to painful stimuli, and pain perception. Among them, attention plays an important role (Wiech et al., 2008). Selective attention to a sensory modality or selective spatial attention to a given body part can modulate the processing of painful stimuli (Spence et al., 2002; Legrain et al., 2012). Painful stimuli also yield the ability to capture involuntary attentional processes depending on their salience and relevance for current goals (Legrain et al., 2012). Yet, previous electrophysiological studies that have assessed the effect of selective attention on pain have mostly used very short and transient stimuli (Legrain et al., 2012). Recently, Blöchl et al. (2015) used steady-state evoked brain potentials (SS-EPs) to investigate the effect of selective spatial attention on the cerebral processing of sustained painful stimuli. SS-EPs reflects a sustained cortical response induced by the periodic modulation of a long-lasting stream of sensory input (Regan, 1989). Using this approach, top-down effects of attention on the cortical processing of intramodal sensory inputs have been demonstrated in visual, auditory, and somatosensory modalities (Morgan et al., 1996; Giabbiconi et al., 2004, 2007; Bidet-Caulet et al., 2007). Typically, selectively attending to one of several concurrently presented streams of sensory inputs increases the magnitude of the SS-EP elicited by the attended stream. Blöchl and collaborators hypothesized that selectively attending to one of two painful inputs applied on the hands would lead to a selective enhancement of the magnitude of the SS-EP elicited by the attended input. Unlike their prediction, they failed to demonstrate such a modulation. They argued that attention cannot be effectively shifted between two simultaneously applied sustained painful stimuli, and that this would constitute a unique property of pain as compared to other senses. Although, we understand their interpretation, in our opinion, their results do not fully justify this interpretation.

A first concern is that other studies have failed to demonstrate top-down attentional modulation of SS-EPs elicited by two sensory inputs belonging to the same sensory modality. In a pioneering EEG study, Linden et al. (1987) found no evidence of an attentional modulation of auditory SS-EPs whereas more recent research found an effect and suggested that the attentional modulation may depend on the experimental context (Müller et al., 2009). Attentional modulation of innocuous somatosensory SS-EPs may also depend on the modulation frequency, the task difficulty, or the experimental design (Adler et al., 2009; Katus et al., 2014). Taken together, this suggests that top-down attentional modulation of SS-EPs is highly context-dependent. Therefore, the lack of effect reported by Blöchl and collaborators could result from the specific experimental context of their study, rather than to the fact that the eliciting stimuli were painful.

Accordingly, the modulation frequencies used by Blöchl and collaborators (31 and 37 Hz) are quite different from those usually used to elicit somatosensory SS-EPs. Tobimatsu et al. (1999) found that the optimal frequency range to elicit non-nociceptive somatosensory SS-EPs lies between 20 and 30 Hz with a maximum around 21 Hz. Moreover, attentional modulation of non-nociceptive somatosensory SS-EPs has been mostly reported using modulation frequencies between 20 and 26 Hz (Giabbiconi et al., 2004, 2007), and intermodal attentional modulation of nociceptive SS-EP has been shown at 6 Hz (Colon et al., 2014). Furthermore, using similar frequencies (30 and 34 Hz) and a simple detection task, Adler et al. (2009) failed to demonstrate attentional modulation of non-nociceptive somatosensory SS-EPs. However, when slightly decreasing the modulation frequencies (28 and 30 Hz) and using a more demanding discriminative task, they observed a significant effect. Consequently, both the modulation frequency and the task may be critical to observe top-down attentional modulation of SS-EPs.

Another methodological difference concerns the timing of the cue that defined the attended stream in Blöchl and collaborators. Their cue occurred 3 s after the onset of the stimulation trains, whereas in most previous studies in the somatosensory modality, the attended stream was cued 200–800 ms before the onset of the stimulation train (Giabbiconi et al., 2004, 2007; Adler et al., 2009). We also demonstrated top-down attentional modulation of concomitant nociceptive and visual SS-EPs only when the onsets of the concomitantly presented stimulation trains were shortly delayed to facilitate the selection of the attended stream (Colon et al., 2014). Therefore, the delayed cue in Blöchl and collaborators could have impaired the attentional selection of the attended stream. Moreover, the attentional effect was assessed only during the 2 s of stimulation that followed the cue. This time interval might have been too short for an attentional modulation to be highlighted.

A second important concern relates to the functional significance of the painful SS-EPs (PSS-EPs). As highlighted by the authors, it is not clear whether PSS-EPs actually differed from SS-EPs elicited by innocuous somatosensory input. The scalp topographies of their PSS-EPs were significantly lateralized toward the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulated hand, and resembled closely the scalp topographies of innocuous somatosensory SS-EPs (Giabbiconi et al., 2004, 2007). Most importantly, these scalp topographies were clearly different from the non-lateralized fronto-central scalp topography of SS-EPs elicited by periodic nociceptive stimulation (Mouraux et al., 2011; Colon et al., 2012, 2014). Moreover, the concentric electrode used to elicit PSS-EPs has been suggested to activate selectively nociceptive afferents only when very low stimulation intensities are used (de Tommaso et al., 2011; Perchet et al., 2012; Legrain and Mouraux, 2013). In Blöchl and collaborators, it is likely that the average intensity of 1.03 mA activated a significant proportion of non-nociceptive somatosensory fibers. Consequently, the PSS-EPs could predominantly reflect activity generated by the activation of non-nociceptive somatosensory afferents.

Therefore, although Blöchl and collaborators present an interesting approach to investigate cortical responses evoked by sustained painful stimulation, we believe that critical factors regarding the used methodology and the functional significance of the SS-EPs elicited by intra-epidermal electrical stimulation should be considered prior to concluding that, unlike other sensations, tonic pain is relatively insensitive to top-down modulation by spatial attention. Yet, uncertainty still abounds regarding the exact parameters required to observe attentional modulation of PSS-EPs.

Author Contributions

EC had the initial ideas and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors then revised the manuscript critically and contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The writing of this paper was supported by an ERC Starting Grant awarded to AM (336130) and also received the support from the Belgian American Educational Foundation (awarded to EC). These foundations did not exert any editorial direction or censorship on any part of this manuscript.

References

Adler, J., Müller, M. M., and Giabbiconi, C. M. (2009). Shift of attention to the body location of distracters is mediated by perceptual load in sustained somatosensory attention. Biol. Psychol. 81, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidet-Caulet, A., Fischer, C., Besle, J., Aguera, P. E., Giard, M. H., and Bertand, O. (2007). Effects of selective attention on the electrophysiological representation of concurrent sounds in the human auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 9252–9261. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blöchl, M., Franz, M., Miltner, W. H., and Weiss, T. (2015). Captured by the pain: pain steady-state evoked potentials are not modulated by selective spatial attention. Brain Res. 1603, 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.01.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colon, E., Legrain, V., and Mouraux, A. (2014). EEG frequency tagging to dissociate the cortical responses to nociceptive and non-nociceptive stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 2262–2274. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colon, E., Nozaradan, S., Legrain, V., and Mouraux, A. (2012). Steady-state evoked potentials to tag specific components of nociceptive cortical processing. Neuroimage 60, 571–581. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Santostasi, R., Devitofrancesco, V., Franco, G., Vecchio, E., Delussi, M., et al. (2011). A comparative study of cortical responses evoked by transcutaneous electrical vs CO(2) laser stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2482–2487. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giabbiconi, C. M., Dancer, C., Zopf, R., Gruber, T., and Müller, M. M. (2004). Selective spatial attention to left or right hand flutter sensation modulates the steady-state somatosensory evoked potential. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giabbiconi, C. M., Trujillo-Barreto, N. J., Gruber, T., and Müller, M. M. (2007). Sustained spatial attention to vibration is mediated in primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroimage 35, 255–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katus, T., Andersen, S. K., and Muller, M. M. (2014). Common mechanisms of spatial attention in memory and perception: a tactile dual-task study. Cereb. Cortex 24, 707–718. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Mancini, F., Sambo, C. F., Torta, D. M., Ronga, I., and Valentini, E. (2012). Cognitive aspects of nociception and pain. Bridging neurophysiology with cognitive psychology. Clin. Neurophysiol. 42, 325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., and Mouraux, A. (2013). Activating selectively and reliably nociceptive afferents with concentric electrode stimulation: yes we can! provided that low stimulus intensities are used! Clin. Neurophysiol. 124:424. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, R. D., Picton, T. W., Hamel, G., and Campbell, K. B. (1987). Human auditory steady-state evoked-potentials during selective attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 66, 145–159. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90184-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, S. T., Hansen, J. C., and Hillyard, S. A. (1996). Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 4770–4774. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.10.4770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouraux, A., Iannetti, G. D., Colon, E., Nozaradan, S., Legrain, V., and Plaghki, L. (2011). Nociceptive steady-state evoked potentials elicited by rapid periodic thermal stimulation of cutaneous nociceptors. J. Neurosci. 31, 6079–6087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3977-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, N., Schlee, W., Hartmann, T., Lorenz, I., and Weisz, N. (2009). Top-down modulation of the auditory steady-state response in a task-switch paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:3. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.001.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perchet, C., Frot, M., Charmarty, A., Flores, C., Mazza, S., Magnin, M., et al. (2012). Do we activate specifically somatosensory thin fibres with the concentric planar electrode? A scalp and intracranial EEG study. Pain 153, 1244–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regan, D. (1989). Human Brain Electrophysiology. Evoked Potentials and Evoked Magnetics Fields in Science and Medicine. New York, NY: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Spence, C., Bentley, D. E., Philips, N., McGlone, F. P., and Jones, A. K. P. (2002). Selective attention to pain: a psychophysical investigation. Exp. Brain Res. 145, 395–402. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1133-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobimatsu, S., Zhang, Y. M., and Kato, M. (1999). Steady-state vibration somatosensory evoked potentials: physiological characteristics and tuning function. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1953–1958. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00146-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiech, K., Ploner, M., and Tracey, I. (2008). Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed). 12, 306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, selective attention, spatial attention, top-down attention, EEG, neurophysiology, human neurosciences, painful stimulation

Citation: Colon E and Mouraux A (2016) Absence of Evidence or Evidence of Absence? Commentary: Captured by the pain: Pain steady-state evoked potentials are not modulated by selective spatial attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:252. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00252

Received: 23 October 2015; Accepted: 16 May 2016;
Published: 14 June 2016.

Edited by:

Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, USA

Reviewed by:

Tobias Katus, Birkbeck, University of London, UK

Copyright © 2016 Colon and Mouraux. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elisabeth Colon, ZWxpc2FiZXRoLmNvbG9uQHVjbG91dmFpbi5iZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.