- 1Event Lab, Department of Personality, Evaluation and Psychological Treatment, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- 2IR3C Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- 3Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute, L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
- 4Department of Basic Psychology, L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- 5Neurophysiology Section, Neurology Service, Hospital Universitari de Bellvitge, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute, Barcelona, Spain
- 6Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Barcelona, Spain
Previous studies on body ownership illusions have shown that under certain multimodal conditions, healthy people can experience artificial body-parts as if they were part of their own body, with direct physiological consequences for the real limb that gets ‘substituted.’ In this study we wanted to assess (a) whether healthy people can experience ‘missing’ a body-part through illusory ownership of an amputated virtual body, and (b) whether this would cause corticospinal excitability changes in muscles associated with the ‘missing’ body-part. Forty right-handed participants saw a virtual body from a first person perspective but for half of them the virtual body was missing a part of its right arm. Single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied before and after the experiment to left and right motor cortices. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were recorded from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and the extensor digitorum communis (EDC) of each hand. We found that the stronger the illusion of amputation and arm ownership, the more the reduction of MEP amplitudes of the EDC muscle for the contralateral sensorimotor cortex. In contrast, no association was found for the EDC amplitudes in the ipsilateral cortex and for the FDI amplitudes in both contralateral and ipsilateral cortices. Our study provides evidence that a short-term illusory perception of missing a body-part can trigger inhibitory effects on corticospinal pathways and importantly in the absence of any limb deafferentation or disuse.
Introduction
Body ownership illusions refer to a class of perceptual illusions where an external object is experienced as part of, or even as one’s entire own body under certain multisensory/sensorimotor conditions (Tsakiris, 2010; Ehrsson, 2012; Kilteni et al., 2015). The most classic example is the rubber hand illusion (RHI) where participants see a rubber hand in front of them that is being touched at the same time and at homologous body areas as their real unseen hand. After some seconds of such spatiotemporally congruent visuotactile stimulation, the participants experience the illusion that the rubber hand is their real hand (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998; Ehrsson et al., 2004). Using similar methodology, participants can experience the illusion of owning an artificial body when they see it from a first person visual perspective (1PP) and while this is being touched in correspondence with their real body (Petkova and Ehrsson, 2008; Maselli and Slater, 2013). Besides the subjective experience, it has been shown that these illusions produce physiological changes on the real body; owning a fake hand decreases the temperature (Moseley et al., 2008; Hohwy and Paton, 2010) and changes the temperature sensitivity of the real hand (Llobera et al., 2013), increases the histamine reactivity in the real arm (Barnsley et al., 2011), slows the tactile processing (Moseley et al., 2008) and attenuates tactile detection performance on the real hand (Zopf et al., 2011). Additionally, a threat to the fake body while experiencing the illusion elicits brain activity in insula and anterior cingulate cortex that is comparable to that triggered when the real counterpart is threatened (Ehrsson et al., 2007), elicits motor cortex activation (González-Franco et al., 2013), triggers the defensive mechanisms for immediate action (Kilteni et al., 2012), and produces higher skin conductance responses (Petkova and Ehrsson, 2008) and heart rate deceleration (Slater et al., 2010) compared to when the illusion is not experienced.
Experimental studies on body ownership illusions suggest that while a basic resemblance between the non-bodily object and a generic human body is needed (Kilteni et al., 2015), morphological differences with the specific participant’s real counterpart, as for example in terms of skin color (Farmer et al., 2012; Kilteni et al., 2013; Maister et al., 2013; Peck et al., 2013) or age (Banakou et al., 2013), do not inhibit the illusion (Maister et al., 2014). Of particular interest are the cases of body ownership illusions under differences in body integrity. Ehrsson et al. (2008) considered whether upper-limb amputees can experience ownership toward an artificial hand and applied synchronous visuotactile stimulation between the index finger of the rubber hand and points on the amputees’ stump that could elicit phantom sensations on the phantom index finger. Although, the reported illusion was weaker compared to when the amputees’ intact hand was tested, neuroimaging data revealed activation in multisensory brain areas (Schmalzl et al., 2014) that had been previously shown to operate the RHI for non-amputated people (Ehrsson et al., 2004; Brozzoli et al., 2012). Similar illusory sensations were reported under visuotactile stimulation combined with myoelectric motor control of an advanced humanoid robotic prosthesis (Rosén et al., 2009), whereas a full body ownership illusion triggered by visuotactile stimulation was reported toward an intact mannequin (Schmalzl et al., 2011).
Addressing the same question but in the reverse direction, is it possible to induce the illusion of missing an actually present body-part in healthy individuals? In a series of experiments, Guterstam et al. (2010) showed that healthy people could experience strong sensations of having an invisible hand, when correlated visuotactile stimuli were applied between the participants’ occluded hand and the empty space in front of them. Similar sensations of having an invisible, vanished or completely transparent finger were reported when stroking the participants’ ring finger in synchrony with the empty space that corresponded to an invisible ring finger of a rubber hand (Lewis et al., 2012). Analogously, the illusory experience of having phantom or telescoped limbs was induced through a body ownership illusion toward an amputated mannequin (Schmalzl and Ehrsson, 2011). Nevertheless, all the above-mentioned studies aimed at inducing analogs of phantom sensations (i.e., the body-part is there but it is somehow invisible), and this is clearly reflected in the experimental methods, given that tactile stimulation on the real body-part should emphasize its presence rather its absence.
The present study aimed at investigating whether intact individuals can experience an illusory loss of their limb. Such an illusory perception – if at all possible – would theoretically entail a perceptually driven temporary reduction of the sensorimotor capacities and representations in the brain, given that the absence of the limb would mandate its disuse. Neurophysiological studies have shown that perturbations in the somatic afferent and/or motor information evoked by a decreased or no use of a body-part, produce neuroplastic changes in the motor cortex. For example, chronic deafferentation due to limb amputation leads to a reorganization of the motor pathways (Cohen et al., 1991) and cortical representations (Pascual-Leone et al., 1996) in the deafferented hemisphere. Transient deafferentation through anesthetic block of a body region induces an increase in the cortical excitability of the motor pathways for the muscles immediately proximal to the deafferented body area (Brasil-Neto et al., 1992), an increase in the excitability of the homotopic sites in the ipsilateral hemisphere (Werhahn et al., 2002) and a reduction in the cortical representation of the muscles that are encompassed by the anesthetized area and deprived from their normal sensory input (Rossini et al., 1996). Furthermore, limb disuse due to restriction of movement (i.e., immobilization) for several hours, days, or weeks has been shown to produce a decrease in the cortical thickness of the contralateral sensorimotor cortex (Langer et al., 2012), a decrease in the motor cortex area size (Liepert et al., 1995) and in the cortical excitability of the immobilized muscle (Facchini et al., 2002; Huber et al., 2006; Avanzino et al., 2011; Ngomo et al., 2012). On the basis of these experimental results, it could be speculated that an illusory amputation would drive a decrease in the excitability of the sensorimotor cortex that is contralateral to the amputation side.
Using immersive virtual reality (VR; Spanlang et al., 2014), all participants were shown a virtual body from a 1PP which for half of them, appeared as if amputated in its right upper limb. Single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied before and after the experiment on both right and left primary motor cortices, and motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from two muscles associated with the amputation area were recorded from each hand. We expected that participants would experience the virtual body as their own body, and that those in the amputation condition would experience the illusion of missing part of their right arm. Moreover, we hypothesized that the illusory experience of missing part of the right arm would yield changes in the corticospinal excitability of the associated left hemisphere. Specifically, we speculated that the post-VR corticospinal excitability of the left hemisphere (right hand) would decrease compared to the pre-VR one. No changes were expected in the right hemisphere and in neither of the hemispheres of the control participants.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Forty-nine right-handed participants with no previous history of epileptic episodes or neurological disorders participated in the study. The study was approved by the Comitè Ètic d’ Investigació Clínica and it was conducted in the Bellvitge University Hospital of Barcelona in Spain. The procedures for the TMS protocol accomplished the corresponding safety guidelines (Rossi et al., 2009). All participants gave written informed consent and were compensated with 30€ for their participation after the end of the experiment.
Nine participants were excluded because of technical problems during either the TMS or the VR session. Data analysis was therefore based on the data of 40 participants. Before the experiment basic demographic details were recorded for all participants and they completed the Edinburgh handedness questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971). Given previous evidence that personality characteristics may influence the experience of body illusions (Kilteni et al., 2013), all participants were further asked to fill in the Spanish version of the NEO-FFI personality inventory (Costa and McCrae, 1992; Table 1).
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
Transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied over the primary motor cortex under a single pulse protocol. A 70 mm figure-of-8 coil attached to a Magstim Rapid 2 Stimulator (Magstim Company, Carmathenshire, Wales) was used to elicit MEPs from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and the extensor digitorum communis (EDC) for each hand. For each pulse, the electromyographic (EMG) activity was recorded through surface Ag/AgCl disk electrodes in a belly tendon montage from both contralateral FDI and EDC muscles, for a total of 700 ms including a 100 ms pre-stimulus window (Medelec Synergy, Oxford Instruments, Pleasantville, NY, USA). The EMG signal was sampled at 5 KHz and band-pass filtered at 1–1000 Hz. All data were exported for off-line analysis using specialized software (Matlab, MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA).
During both TMS sessions, participants wore an elastic lycra fabric cap, on which a 10 cm × 10 cm grid centered on the vertex (Cz position of the international 10/20 EEG positioning system) was drawn in order to allow simple identification of stimulation coordinates for sites separated by 1 cm each. Midline points of the grid were placed 7 cm anterior and 3 cm posterior to the vertex, and from each midline point 10 points separated by one cm were distributed laterally for each hemisphere. During the registrations, the TMS coil was positioned tangentially to each site, with the handle pointing backward (in a lateral to medial and caudal to rostral position) ∼45° lateral from the midline.
Virtual Reality
During the session of VR, participants were fitted with a Sensics zSight 60 Head Mounted Display (HMD) with embedded headphones. Two mechanical coin type vibrators (model C1034-50L, 1200 ± 300 r.p.m., Shenzhen Linglong Electronics, Co.) connected to an Arduino microcontroller were attached on the participants’ right and left carpal respectively. Two unmediated auditory recordings from a triggered vibrator on human skin (vibratorSkin) or on a wooden table (vibratorTable) were used. All virtual models were modeled in 3D Studio Max 2010 and 3D Studio Max 2012. The virtual environment was implemented on the XVR platform (Tecchia, 2010) and the virtual body was displayed using a hardware accelerated avatar library (HALCA; Gillies and Spanlang, 2010).
Procedures
The experiment was a between-groups study with one single factor, the appearance of the body, with two levels: amputated or intact arm. Upon arrival, participants were randomly assigned to the amputation or control condition.
The experimental procedures included one VR session and two TMS sessions (one before and one after the VR), lasting 90 min. During all sessions, participants were seated in a chair, with their legs extended in a horizontal position on top of another support in order to feel comfortable. Both their arms rested on a table in front of them, while their hands were hanging out of the front edge of the table. A box was placed on top of the table in order to conceal the participants’ forearms and hands from view. Participants were told that for the rest of the experiment, they could freely move their head but they were requested to keep their body and especially their arms motionless.
TMS Sessions
The excitability of the corticospinal pathway was addressed for each hemisphere. In the pre-VR TMS session and before any registration, the grid location where the highest MEP of the FDI was elicited (Hot Spot) was detected and recorded for each hemisphere (Rossi et al., 2009). Following, the resting motor threshold (RMT), defined as the lowest stimulus intensity needed to evoke a visible MEP > 50 μv in 50% of 10 trials from the relaxed muscle, was explored in the Hot Spot (Rossini et al., 1994).
For each hemisphere, the MEP was assessed in the Hot Spot of the FDI. Ten MEPs were elicited and recorded for each muscle (FDI and EDC) in the FDI Hot Spot at 120% of the RMT. Since the registration of the MEPs for both muscles was done simultaneously after each pulse on each hemisphere, the EDC MEPs were also triggered through stimulating the FDI Hot Spot at 120% of the FDI RMT. The order of the assessed hemispheres was counterbalanced. Throughout both TMS sessions, subjects had to fixate on a marker placed on the wall, 2 m approximately in front of them.
VR Session
The session started for both groups identically. Through the HMD, participants saw a complete gender-matched virtual body from a 1PP, spatially coincident with their real body. In order to make the participants’ posture as comfortable as possible while being motionless, a virtual mirror was placed just opposite so that they could see the virtual body both when looking down but also when looking straight ahead (Figure 1A). Participants were asked to move their head and describe what they saw around themselves, including the virtual body (1 min). Once the scene was described, the experimenter asked them to focus on the virtual hands for the remainder of the session by looking either directly or through the mirror. Then, a virtual ball appeared and tapped the right virtual carpal, bouncing between the virtual hand and the virtual mirror in random velocities for 2 min (Figure 1B). Every time the ball made contact with the virtual carpal and for the whole duration of this contact, the vibrator on the participants’ right hand was triggered and the vibrationSkin sound file was reproduced. Therefore, all seen and heard tactile events on the virtual hand were temporally registered with physical touch on the real hand.
FIGURE 1. The participant’s view from the HMD. (A) For both groups, a gender-matched virtual body was seen from 1PP with the same posture with the real body, as if they were spatially coincident. Participants were asked to look around and describe what they see (1 min). (B) For both groups, a virtual ball touched the right virtual hand various times while the real hand was physically touched at the same timings (2 min). (C) For the amputation group, part of the right virtual arm disappeared, as if amputated (3 min). (D) Following, the virtual ball touched the area previously occupied by the right virtual hand and part of the forearm (i.e., the table surface) without triggering any physical touch to the participant’s right hand (10 min).
The remaining procedures differed between the two conditions in the appearance of the virtual body. For the amputation condition, part of the virtual arm disappeared and the virtual body appeared as if missing part of its right arm (3 min; Figure 1C). For the following 10 min, the virtual ball appeared again but it now bounced between the virtual mirror and the table, landing always at the former position of the virtual hand (Figure 1D). There was no physical vibration on the real right hand and the sound that accompanied the seen collision of the ball was the vibrationTable one. This last phase was inserted to emphasize the absence of the virtual hand: besides the visual input that the body-part was no longer there, additional visual and auditory evidence from the bouncing ball emphasized that expected sensations were not experienced. In contrast, the control participants kept seeing the intact virtual body for 3 min (Figures 1A,B) and for the following 10 min, when the virtual ball touched the virtual hand, there was no physical vibration and the sound was changed to vibrationTable. All together, both groups received the exact same sensory events with the only difference being the presence or the absence of part of the right virtual arm.
The VR session lasted 16 min. When the session finished, the HMD was removed, participants were reminded to keep their hands motionless and the post-VR TMS session began immediately.
Post-experiment Session
When the second TMS session finished, participants were asked to complete a questionnaire with respect to their experience. Then, the purpose of the experiment was explained and they were paid. For ethical purposes, all participants of the amputation condition were asked again to don the HMD and were shown the virtual body with an intact right arm. Finally, all subjects were contacted few weeks after the experiment and they were asked about having any thoughts or sensations concerning the experiment. All of them found the study interesting and none reported any negative feelings or thoughts.
Response Variables
Questionnaire
The post-experiment questionnaire consisted of 19 statements (Table 2). We expected that participants of the amputation condition would give higher ratings to the statements on amputation compared to the control subjects, while high scores without significant differences were expected for arm and full body ownership. Low ratings were expected from the control items in both groups and exploratory items with respect to temperature, pain, numbness, and movement related sensations were also included.
MEP Amplitudes
Peak-to peak amplitudes of the MEPs in μV were extracted from the EMG registrations (Rossini et al., 1994) using Matlab routines, and after careful visual inspection. As stated above, 10 MEPs were recorded for each hemisphere and muscle, before and after the VR session, resulting to 80 registrations per participant and 320 sets of 10 registrations in total. There were 133 (4.1%) bad registrations (i.e., due to poor signal quality or no observable MEPs). The analysis was performed on 314 registrations (six missing values).
Outlier detection was computed by calculating the mean MEP amplitude for each muscle, hemisphere, time and condition per participant. MEP amplitude values exceeding two standard deviations from the mean were considered as outliers and discarded (Nogueira-campos et al., 2014). Based on this criterion, less than 6% of the trials were discarded from the analyses (181 discarded trials out of 3067 valid trials). After removing the outliers, MEP amplitudes were normalized by applying a logarithmic transformation (Tremblay et al., 2001).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis was performed with Stata 2013. Between groups differences in questionnaires were compared with Mann–Whitney test. MEP amplitudes were analyzed through two mixed full factorial 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVAs, one per each muscle, with condition as between groups factor, and time and hemisphere as within groups factors. For each post hoc t-test, confidence intervals together with the corresponding p-value are reported. Residual errors were assessed for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Correlations were evaluated using the Spearman correlation coefficient (ρ).
Results
Questionnaire
Table 3 shows the medians and interquartile ranges of the questionnaire responses for the two conditions. Participants of the amputation condition rated the amputation statements (missright, noright, onehand, tellright, lessvivid) significantly higher than the controls (Figure 2). No differences were found in arm and full body ownership (ownarm, ownbody) between the conditions, with both median scores being high (four out of five). No other significant differences were detected, besides the statement about numb feelings on the left hand, where participants in the amputation condition reported significantly less numb sensations for their left hand compared to controls.
TABLE 3. Medians and interquartile ranges of the questionnaire responses for the Control and Amputation conditions.
FIGURE 2. Boxplot of the amputation related statements per condition. The height of the boxes represents the interquartile ranges, the black lines represent the medians and the blue dots represent the outliers. Participants in the amputation condition rated the statements related to the illusion of amputation significantly higher than the control group.
The amputation score, calculated as the average of the five amputation statements, was significantly and positively correlated with arm ownership (ownarm – ρ = 0.528, n = 40, p < 0.001) and full body ownership (ownbody – ρ = 0.464, n = 40, p = 0.002; Figure 3), and with feelings of not being able to move the right hand (nomoveright – ρ = 0.689, n = 40, p < 0.0001). No other significant correlations were detected between the amputation score and the rest of the items.
FIGURE 3. Scatterplot of the amputation illusion with arm and full body ownership illusion. Numbers inside circles indicate the number of overlapping points and the red lines represent the fitted regression lines. The amputation illusion was significantly correlated with arm and full body ownership illusion (see text). Moreover, it was significantly predicted by arm ownership (b = 0.49, t = 3.86, p < 0.001) and full body ownership (b = 0.52, t = 3.52, p = 0.001) respectively. Both arm and full body ownership explained a significant proportion of variance in the amputation illusion [arm ownership – R2 = 0.26, F(1,38) = 14.86, p < 0.001; full body ownership- R2 = 0.23, F(1,38) = 12.42, p < 0.01].
MEP Amplitudes
Figures 4A,B show the means and standard errors for the log-transformed MEP amplitudes per time, hemisphere and condition, for the FDI and EDC muscles respectively.
FIGURE 4. Mean and standard error plots for the log-transformed MEP amplitudes per hemisphere, time and condition for the FDI muscle (A) and for the EDC muscle (B).
For the FDI muscle, there was no main effect of condition [F(1,38) = 0.42, p = 0.523], nor of time [F(1,38) = 1.19, p = 0.281] but a main effect of hemisphere [F(1,38) = 7.81, p = 0.008, = 0.384] was detected. None of the two or three way interactions were significant. Residuals errors were normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk test, p = 0.502). Post hoc t-tests revealed that the FDI amplitudes for the left hemisphere (right hand) were significantly greater than those for the right hemisphere [t(78) = 4.84, p < 0.001, CI[-0.52, -0.21]].
For the EDC muscle, there was no main effect of condition [F(1,37) = 0.45, p = 0.507], nor of time [F(1,37) = 0.56, p = 0.460] but a main effect of hemisphere [F(1,37) = 4.76, p = 0.035] was detected. None of the two or three way interactions were significant. Residuals errors were not normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk test, p < 0.0001). Visual inspection of the residuals’ plot clearly identified four outlier values corresponding to one participant (Supplementary Figure S1). After removal of the outliers, the ANOVA revealed again no main effect of condition [F(1,36) = 0.58, p = 0.452], nor of time [F(1,36) = 0.11, p = 0.747] but a main effect of hemisphere [F(1,36) = 4.68, p = 0.037, = 0.370] was detected. None of the two-way interactions were significant. However, the three-way interaction condition × time × hemisphere had significance level p = 0.053 [F(1,35) = 3.98]. Residual errors were normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk test, p = 0.575). Post hoc t-tests for the hemisphere effect revealed that the EDC amplitudes for the left hemisphere (right hand) were significantly lower than those for the right hemisphere [t(74) = -4.61, p < 0.001, CI[0.13, 0.33]]. For explorative reasons and despite the three-way interaction being non-significant (p = 0.053), we performed the post hoc comparisons of interest. For the control condition, there were no significant differences in MEP amplitudes before and after the VR session, neither for the left hemisphere [t(18) = 0.14, p = 0.886, CI[-0.184, 0.212]], nor for the right one [t(17) = -0.09, p = 0.927, CI[-0.218, 0.199]]. In contrast, for the amputation condition and for the left hemisphere, MEPs after the VR session were significantly smaller compared to those before [t(18) = -2.34, p = 0.025, CI[-0.426, -0.030]]. No significant differences were observed for the right hemisphere in the amputation condition [t(18) = 1.46, p = 0.153, CI[-0.055, 0.341]].
Subjective Experience and MEP Amplitudes
Next, we investigated correlations between the subjective illusion of ownership and amputation with the difference between the pre and post-VR MEP amplitudes (Figures 5 and 6). The amplitude difference for the FDI muscle of the left hemisphere was not correlated with the illusion of amputation (ρ = 0.250, n = 40, p = 0.120) or arm ownership (ρ = 0.222, n = 40, p = 0.168). Similarly, no correlation was found between the FDI amplitudes’ difference of the right hemisphere with amputation (ρ = 0.137, n = 39, p = 0.407) or arm ownership (ρ = 0.131, n = 39, p = 0.427). Additionally, the EDC amplitudes’ difference of the right hemisphere was not significantly correlated with the illusion of amputation (ρ = -0.044, n = 38, p = 0.795) and arm ownership (ρ = 0.159, n = 38, p = 0.339). In contrast, the difference of the EDC muscle of the left hemisphere was significantly and positively correlated with the illusion of amputation (ρ = 0.409, n = 39, p = 0.010) and arm ownership (ρ = 0.417, n = 39, p = 0.008). When removing the 4 outlier points revealed by the ANOVA and repeating the correlation analysis, the results remained the same; only the MEPs’ difference of the EDC muscle of the left hemisphere was significantly and positively correlated with the illusion of amputation (ρ = 0.392, n = 38, p = 0.015) and arm ownership (ρ = 0.408, n = 38, p = 0.011). In other words, the stronger the experienced ownership toward the virtual arm and the illusion of amputation, the smaller was the post-VR EDC amplitude compared to the pre-VR EDC amplitude for the left hemisphere (right hand).
FIGURE 5. Scatterplot of the difference between pre and post-VR MEP amplitudes with the illusion of amputation per each muscle and hemisphere. The difference in amplitudes was significantly correlated with the amputation illusion only for the EDC muscle of the left hemisphere (see text). Similarly, only in this case, the amputation illusion predicted significantly the MEPs’ difference (b = 174.33, t = 2.53, p = 0.016) and explained a significant proportion of its variance [R2 = 0.12, F(1,37) = 6.39, p = 0.016].
FIGURE 6. Scatterplot of the difference between pre and post-VR MEP amplitudes with the illusion of arm ownership per each muscle and hemisphere. The difference in amplitudes was significantly correlated with the arm ownership illusion only for the EDC muscle of the left hemisphere (see text). Similarly, only in this case, the ownership illusion predicted significantly the MEPs’ difference (b = 139.27, t = 2.10, p = 0.043) and explained a significant proportion of its variance [R2 = 0.08, F(1,37) = 4.40, p = 0.043].
Discussion
The present study investigated whether it is possible for intact individuals to experience the illusion of missing a body-part and whether such perception could yield changes in the excitability of the sensorimotor cortex.
Our results on subjective reports indicate that while participants in both amputation and control conditions experienced the virtual body as their own, similar to previous findings on demographic differences and body ownership (Banakou et al., 2013; Kilteni et al., 2013; Maister et al., 2013; Peck et al., 2013), owning a virtual body of an amputee generated additional sensations of missing the corresponding parts from the real body. Previous studies on body ownership have explored the possibilities of ‘extending’ the participants’ body through the illusion of having a third (Ehrsson, 2009; Guterstam et al., 2011) or a very long limb (Kilteni et al., 2012), or even ‘fading’ it through the illusion of having a phantom body-part (Guterstam et al., 2010; Schmalzl and Ehrsson, 2011; Lewis et al., 2012; Martini et al., 2015). In contrast, the present study asked whether one could experience an illusory ‘reduction’ in the body representation; an amputated virtual body was seen from a 1PP, as if it were spatially coincident with the real body, except for a part of the right arm. Visual and auditory stimuli that emphasized the absence of the body-part were provided: a virtual object was seen to touch the virtual table that is, the former area of the virtual hand, no tactile feedback was provided on the real limb and the collision sound was contextually dissociated from touch on body surface. Our results suggest that through such a disintegration of crossmodal body-related stimuli (i.e., visual, tactile, auditory, and proprioceptive), intact individuals can be made to experience the illusion of missing part of their right arm (Newport and Gilpin, 2011).
Our analysis on the MEP amplitudes and subjective illusion revealed a correlation between the illusion of ownership and amputation with the difference between the pre and post-VR amplitudes; the stronger the illusory experience, the larger the decrease in the EDC corticospinal excitability in the contralateral sensorimotor cortex. How could the amputation illusion be associated with a decrease in corticospinal excitability?
First, previous neurophysiological studies have shown that the corticospinal excitability can significantly change during action observation. For example, when seeing an action, the human motor system is activated in order to ‘resonate’ with the observed action and the excitability of the muscles involved in the action is increased (Fadiga et al., 2005). Therefore, one possible interpretation of our results could be that observing the motor impairments of one’s body (i.e., due to amputation) triggers the motor system to resonate with these limitations and consequently the corticospinal excitability decreases. Nevertheless, this interpretation seems highly unlikely since just seeing an amputated body from a 1PP per se was not sufficient to produce excitability changes but it was one’s illusory perception of ownership and amputation that produced the decrease.
Second, an alternative explanation could be based on motor imagery. Imagining an action leads to an enhancement of corticomotor excitability, specifically for the hemisphere that is contralateral to the imagined movement, and for the muscles that are involved in the imagined action (Stinear, 2010). Interestingly, negative motor imagery that is, imagining suppression of hand twitching movements induced by TMS has been shown to reduce the amplitudes of MEPs and suppress the corticospinal excitability (Sohn et al., 2003). This explanation seems unlikely too, since our participants were not instructed to imagine performing any movement but they were explicitly told to remain relaxed and motionless and observe the scene.
In contrast, the pattern of our results tends to support the results from previous neurophysiological studies that investigated the influence of limb deafferentation on the functional reorganization of the motor cortex. Depriving a body area from its sensorimotor information, as for example through immobilization, produces a reduction in the corticospinal excitability of the motor area that is associated with the restricted muscles (Facchini et al., 2002; Huber et al., 2006; Avanzino et al., 2011; Ngomo et al., 2012). Similarly in the present study, we found that experiencing an illusory amputation can yield a decrease in the corticospinal excitability of the EDC muscle and crucially in a correlated fashion; the stronger the illusion of ownership and amputation, the larger the suppression.
Surprisingly, this relationship was not found for the right FDI muscle, a muscle that was clearly included in the amputated area. It is clear that one would expect effects on both right FDI and EDC since both muscles are included in the amputation area. There are possible explanations for the absence of such effect on FDI. First, although participants were explicitly instructed to remain motionless during the experiment, the possibility that they made brief, subtle finger movements that would consequently ‘resurrect’ the missing body-part and eliminate any effect cannot be excluded. An alternative explanation concerns the attention participants paid to specific parts of the virtual body. During most of the VR experimental duration, the virtual ball that was bouncing between the virtual mirror and the former area of the missing body-part was visible, triggering -as a visual moving stimulus- the attention to the forearm rather than the hand (Figure 1D). Several studies have found that attention can affect both motor cortex excitability as well as plasticity (Stefan et al., 2004; Conte et al., 2007; Kamke et al., 2012, 2014; Ruge et al., 2014). Attending a specific muscle, influences its corticospinal excitability compared to unattended muscles (Gandevia and Rothwell, 1987). It could be speculated therefore, that the failure to observe changes in the right FDI muscle was due to an increased attention paid to the end of the right forearm rather than the area of the missing virtual hand.
Previous research has provided convergent evidence in favor of a relationship between corticospinal excitability and processing of self-related information. For example, it was observed that during exposure to images containing elements of one’s own face, the excitability of the motor cortex -assessed via TMS- was increased in the right compared to the left hemisphere (Keenan et al., 2001), even when these images were masked (Théoret et al., 2004). In addition, the discrimination between images of self-faces and other-faces was found to be deteriorated when applying repetitive TMS on the right inferior parietal lobule, suggesting again a role of the right hemisphere in processing self-related information (Uddin et al., 2006). Moreover, this modulation in corticospinal excitability of the right hemisphere was found not to be limited to vision of one’s own face, but to extend to vision of one’s own hand and one’s own objects (Salerno et al., 2012). Complementary to these studies, the present research investigated changes in corticospinal excitability as a function of body perception and more particularly of perceived body integrity. Rather than comparing the view of one’s own body (body-part) with viewing somebody else’s body (body-part), we compared two conditions where the viewed body was perceived as one’s own one but was either missing part of its right arm or not. Consistent with this manipulation and in line with the subjective reports of the participants, we found a decrease in the excitability of the left motor cortex (corresponding to the right side of the body) in relation to the illusion of missing part of the right arm and the illusion of owning the viewed right arm.
The present study focused exclusively on MEP amplitudes and it should be noted that analyzing the MEP amplitudes per se (i.e., ANOVA) did not reveal any significant differences but just a trend. Future studies should aim at including a larger sample of participants, collecting a larger number of TMS stimuli in order to acquire a stable estimate of corticospinal excitability (Cuypers et al., 2014) and they could fully characterize the nature of these changes, for example by measuring stimulus-response curves or TMS based motor mapping. Moreover, in the present study, only young participants were evaluated. Although, our results cannot be generalized to older population given previous evidence on age effects on MEP amplitudes (Pitcher et al., 2003), we speculate that similar findings would be observed based on the fact that our results are due to the experimental condition and not to the baseline measurement. Finally, although the pattern of our results is similar to that revealed in immobilization studies, the duration of our experimental procedure (13 min) is clearly shorter than that used in those studies (several hours, days, or weeks). Future studies could directly contrast the effects of the body illusion with that of immobilization in order to contrast the size of the effect (magnitude of MEPs’ change), the nature of the effect (including changes in RMTs) as well as its temporal prevalence once the procedure stops.
Finally, previous research on the illusions of body ownership has investigated whether owning a fake hand implies a disownership of the real counterpart. On the basis of a cooling of the real hand as a result of the illusion, some studies have pointed in this direction (Moseley et al., 2008), but others not (Rohde et al., 2013). Assuming that disownership in the case of the RHI were clearly established, it could be argued that in our experiment we have only induced another kind of disownership. However, our experiment concentrated on the absence of the hand, where the real hand was coincident in space first with the intact virtual one, and later when the virtual one was entirely missing. From our results we cannot say whether absence is in any way related to the disownership proposed for the RHI. Second, the results of Llobera et al. (2013) suggest that when the virtual hand is collocated with the real one, and there is full body ownership, there is no disownership of the real hand, but rather a unification of the real and virtual body into one combined entity, which is ‘owned.’ Third, we have shown that when part of the body is missing, there is a decrease in the corticospinal excitability corresponding to that body-part. Whether these physiological changes reflect ‘disownership’ rather than just ‘absence’ is beyond the scope of this paper. However, if they did, then we should have observed similar changes in MEPs for the control group that experienced the same arm and full body ownership but we did not.
To conclude, our study demonstrates the possibility of inhibitory effects on corticospinal pathways, triggered by a short-term illusory perception of missing a body-part and importantly in the absence of any physical intervention (e.g., ischaemic nerve block or movement restriction); just a few minutes of a novel body experience seem to be sufficient to produce direction specific effects on the excitability of sensorimotor system. Although, future studies are needed to investigate whether the origin of the effects is primarily cortical or spinal, as well as their temporal prevalence, our results indicate new possibilities for inducing short-term functional reorganization and plasticity of the sensorimotor system by emphasizing the contribution of body experience. Experience is a known modulator of the brain in both functional and structural terms (May, 2011; Makin et al., 2013) and its role has been widely discussed in relation to increased use, disuse and transient deafferentation of a body-part in healthy participants. A novel perception of the body that suggests that a body-part is missing may also entail psychologically both the body- part’s disuse (inability to move it) and deafferentation (inability to receive afferent input from it), since it is no longer there.
Author Contributions
KK conceived and designed the experiment with the help of AR-F and MS. KK and JG-S carried out the experiment. The TMS recordings were designed by JG-S, MV, AR-F and carried out by JG-S. The analysis was carried out by KK and MS. The paper was written by KK, MS, AR-F with the help of all the authors, and reviewed by all the authors.
Funding
KK was supported by the ERC project TRAVERSE (#227985) and MS by the ERC project TRAVERSE (#227985). The research leading to these results was partially funded from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 604102 (Human Brain Project).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgment
We wish to thank Dr. Jordi Montero Homs and Dr. Julià Amengual Roig for their helpful comments.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00145
FIGURE S1 | Residual errors’ plot. Visual inspection of the plot clearly indicates the existence of four outlier values.
References
Avanzino, L., Bassolino, M., Pozzo, T., and Bove, M. (2011). Use-dependent hemispheric balance. J. Neurosci. 31, 3423–3428. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4893-10.2011
Banakou, D., Groten, R., and Slater, M. (2013). Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 12846–12851. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306779110
Barnsley, N., McAuley, J. H., Mohan, R., Dey, A., Thomas, P., and Moseley, G. L. (2011). The rubber hand illusion increases histamine reactivity in the real arm. Curr. Biol. 21, R945–R946. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.039
Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756. doi: 10.1038/35784
Brasil-Neto, J. P., Cohen, L. G., Pascual-Leone, A., Jabir, F. K., Wall, R. T., and Hallett, M. (1992). Rapid reversible modulation of human motor outputs after transient deafferentation of the forearm: a study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 42, 1302–1306. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.7.1302
Brozzoli, C., Gentile, G., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). That’s near my hand! Parietal and premotor coding of hand-centered space contributes to localization and self-attribution of the hand. J. Neurosci. 32, 14573–14582. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2660-12.2012
Cohen, L. G., Bandinelli, S., Findley, T. W., and Hallett, M. (1991). Motor reorganization after upper limb amputation in man. A study with focal magnetic stimulation. Brain 114(Pt 1), 615–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.1.615
Conte, A., Gilio, F., Iezzi, E., Frasca, V., Inghilleri, M., and Berardelli, A. (2007). Attention influences the excitability of cortical motor areas in healthy humans. Exp. Brain Res. 182, 109–117. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0975-3
Costa, P. T. J., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO-PI-R professional manual: revised NEO personality and NEO Five- Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Psychol. Assess. 4, 5–13. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5
Cuypers, K., Thijs, H., and Meesen, R. L. J. (2014). Optimization of the transcranial magnetic stimulation protocol by defining a reliable estimate for corticospinal excitability. PLoS ONE 9:e86380. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086380
Ehrsson, H. H. (2009). How many arms make a pair? Perceptual illusion of having an additional limb. Perception 38, 310–312. doi: 10.1068/p6304
Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). “The concept of body ownership and its relation to multisensory integration,” in The New Handbook of Multisensory Processes, ed. B. E. Stein (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 775–792.
Ehrsson, H. H., Rosén, B., Stockselius, A., Ragnö, C., Köhler, P., Lundborg, G., et al. (2008). Upper limb amputees can be induced to experience a rubber hand as their own. Brain 131, 3443–3452. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn297
Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., and Passingham, R. E. (2004). That’s my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305, 875–877. doi: 10.1126/science.1097011
Ehrsson, H. H., Wiech, K., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., and Passingham, R. E. (2007). Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 9828–9833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610011104
Facchini, S., Romani, M., Tinazzi, M., and Aglioti, S. M. (2002). Time-related changes of excitability of the human motor system contingent upon immobilisation of the ring and little fingers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 367–375. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00009-3
Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., and Olivier, E. (2005). Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others’ action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 213–218. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.013
Farmer, H., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., and Tsakiris, M. (2012). Beyond the colour of my skin: how skin colour affects the sense of body-ownership. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1242–1256. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.011
Gandevia, S. C., and Rothwell, J. C. (1987). Knowledge of motor commands and the recruitment of human motoneurons. Brain 110(Pt 5), 1117–1130. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.5.1117
Gillies, M., and Spanlang, B. (2010). Comparing and evaluationg real-time character engines for virtual environments. Presence 19, 95–117. doi: 10.1162/pres.19.2.95
González-Franco, M., Peck, T. C., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and Slater, M. (2013). A threat to a virtual hand elicits motor cortex activation. Exp. Brain Res. 23, 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3800-1
Guterstam, A., Gentile, G., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2010). The Invisible hand illusion: multisensory integration leads to the embodiment of a discrete volume of empty space. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1078–1099. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a_00393
Guterstam, A., Petkova, V. I., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). The illusion of owning a third arm. PLoS ONE 6:e17208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017208
Hohwy, J., and Paton, B. (2010). Explaining away the body: experiences of supernaturally caused touch and touch on non-hand objects within the rubber hand illusion. PLoS ONE 5:e9416. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009416
Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B. A., Peterson, M. J., et al. (2006). Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1169–1176. doi: 10.1038/nn1758
Kamke, M. R., Hall, M. G., Lye, H. F., Sale, M. V., Fenlon, L. R., Carroll, T. J., et al. (2012). Visual attentional load influences plasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 7001–7008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1028-12.2012
Kamke, M. R., Ryan, A. E., Sale, M. V., Campbell, M. E. J., Riek, S., Carroll, T. J., et al. (2014). Visual spatial attention has opposite effects on bidirectional plasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 1475–1480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1595-13.2014
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O’Connor, M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature 409:305. doi: 10.1038/35053167
Kilteni, K., Bergstrom, I., and Slater, M. (2013). Drumming in immersive virtual reality: the body shapes the way we play. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 19, 597–605. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2013.29
Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., Koerding, K., and Slater, M. (2015). Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:141. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00141
Kilteni, K., Normand, J.-M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., and Slater, M. (2012). Extending body space in immersive virtual reality: a very long arm illusion. PLoS ONE 7:e40867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040867
Langer, N., Hänggi, J., Müller, N. A., Simmen, H. P., and Jäncke, L. (2012). Effects of limb immobilization on brain plasticity. Neurology 78, 182–188. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823fcd9c
Lewis, E., Lloyd, D. M., and Farrell, M. J. (2012). The role of the environment in eliciting phantom-like sensations in non-amputees. Front. Psychol. 3:600. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00600
Liepert, J., Tegenthoff, M., and Malin, J. P. (1995). Changes of cortical motor area size during immobilization. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 97, 382–386. doi: 10.1016/0924-980X(95)00194-P
Llobera, J., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., and Slater, M. (2013). The relationship between virtual body ownership and temperature sensitivity. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20130300. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.0300
Maister, L., Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., and Tsakiris, M. (2013). Experiencing ownership over a dark-skinned body reduces implicit racial bias. Cognition 128, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.002
Maister, L., Slater, M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., and Tsakiris, M. (2014). Changing bodies changes minds: owning another body affects social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.001
Makin, T. R., Scholz, J., Filippini, N., Slater, D. H., Tracey, I., Johansen-berg, H., et al. (2013). Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat. Commun. 4:1570. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2571
Martini, M., Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2015). The body fades away: investigating the effects of transparency of an embodied virtual body on pain threshold and body ownership. Sci. Rep. 5:13948. doi: 10.1038/srep13948
Maselli, A., and Slater, M. (2013). The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:83. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00083
May, A. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 475–482. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.002
Moseley, G. L., Olthof, N., Venema, A., Don, S., Wijers, M., Gallace, A., et al. (2008). Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the illusory ownership of an artificial counterpart. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 13169–13173. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803768105
Newport, R., and Gilpin, H. R. (2011). Multisensory disintegration and the disappearing hand trick. Curr. Biol. 21, R804–R805. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.044
Ngomo, S., Leonard, G., and Mercier, C. (2012). Influence of the amount of use on hand motor cortex representation: effects of immobilization and motor training. Neuroscience 220, 208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.06.018
Nogueira-campos, A. A., Alice, L., Oliveira, S., Della-maggiore, V., Esteves, P. O., Rodrigues, E. D. C., et al. (2014). Corticospinal excitability preceding the grasping of emotion-laden stimuli. PLoS ONE 9:e94824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094824
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Pascual-Leone, A., Peris, M., Tormos, J. M., Pascual, A. P., and Catalá, M. D. (1996). Reorganization of human cortical motor output maps following traumatic forearm amputation. Neuroreport 7, 2068–2070. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199609020-2
Peck, T. C., Seinfeld, S., Aglioti, S. M., and Slater, M. (2013). Putting yourself in the skin of a black avatar reduces implicit racial bias. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 779–787. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.016
Petkova, V. I., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS ONE 3:e3832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003832
Pitcher, J. B., Ogston, K. M., and Miles, T. S. (2003). Age and sex differences in human motor cortex input-output characteristics. J. Physiol. 546, 605–613. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.029454
Rohde, M., Wold, A., Karnath, H.-O., and Ernst, M. O. (2013). The human touch: skin temperature during the rubber hand illusion in manual and automated stroking procedures. PLoS ONE 8:e80688. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080688
Rosén, B., Ehrsson, H. H., Antfolk, C., Cipriani, C., Sebelius, F., and Lundborg, G. (2009). Referral of sensation to an advanced humanoid robotic hand prosthesis. Scand. J. Plast. Reconstr. Surg. Hand Surg. 43, 260–266. doi: 10.3109/02844310903113107
Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016
Rossini, P. M., Barker, A. T., Berardelli, A., Caramia, M. D., Caruso, G., Cracco, R. Q., et al. (1994). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90029-9
Rossini, P. M., Rossi, S., Tecchio, F., Pasqualetti, P., Finazzi-Agrò, A., and Sabato, A. (1996). Focal brain stimulation in healthy humans: motor maps changes following partial hand sensory deprivation. Neurosci. Lett. 214, 191–195. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12940-2
Ruge, D., Muggleton, N., Hoad, D., Caronni, A., and Rothwell, J. C. (2014). An unavoidable modulation? Sensory attention and human primary motor cortex excitability. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 1–9. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12651
Salerno, S., Zamagni, E., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Farnè, A., and Frassinetti, F. (2012). Increases of corticospinal excitability in self-related processing. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2716–2721. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08176.x
Schmalzl, L., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). Experimental induction of a perceived “telescoped” limb using a full-body illusion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:34. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00034
Schmalzl, L., Kalckert, A., Ragnö, C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). Neural correlates of the rubber hand illusion in amputees: a report of two cases. Neurocase 20, 407–420. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2013.791861
Schmalzl, L., Thomke, E., Ragnö, C., Nilseryd, M., Stockselius, A., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). “Pulling telescoped phantoms out of the stump”: manipulating the perceived position of phantom limbs using a full-body illusion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:121. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00121
Slater, M., Spanlang, B., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., and Blanke, O. (2010). First person experience of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 5:e10564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010564
Sohn, Y. H., Dang, N., and Hallett, M. (2003). Suppression of corticospinal excitability during negative motor imagery. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 2303–2309. doi: 10.1152/jn.00206.2003
Spanlang, B., Normand, J.-M., Borland, D., Kilteni, K., Giannopoulos, E., Pomes, A., et al. (2014). How to build an embodiment lab: achieving body representation illusions in virtual reality. Front. Robot. AI. Virtual Environ. 1:9. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2014.00009
Stefan, K., Wycislo, M., and Classen, J. (2004). Modulation of associative human motor cortical plasticity by attention. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 66–72. doi: 10.1152/jn.00383.2003
Stinear, C. M. (2010). “Corticospinal facilitation during motor imagery,” in The Neurophysiological Foundations of Mental and Motor Imagery, eds A. Guillot and C. Collet (Oxford: University Press), 47–61.
Tecchia, F. (2010). A flexible framework for wide-spectrum VR development. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 19, 302–312. doi: 10.1162/PRES_a_00002
Théoret, H., Kobayashi, M., Merabet, L., Wagner, T., Tormos, J. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2004). Modulation of right motor cortex excitability without awareness following presentation of masked self-images. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 54–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.01.003
Tremblay, F., Tremblay, L. E., and Colcer, D. E. (2001). Modulation of corticospinal excitability during imagined knee movements. J. Rehabil. Med. 33, 230–234. doi: 10.1080/165019701750419635
Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 48, 703–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034
Uddin, L. Q., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., and Iacoboni, M. (2006). rTMS to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 65–71. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl003
Werhahn, K. J., Mortensen, J., Kaelin-Lang, A., Boroojerdi, B., and Cohen, L. G. (2002). Cortical excitability changes induced by deafferentation of the contralateral hemisphere. Brain 125, 1402–1413. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf140
Keywords: illusory amputation, body ownership, corticospinal excitability, transcranial magnetic stimulation, virtual reality
Citation: Kilteni K, Grau-Sánchez J, Veciana De Las Heras M, Rodríguez-Fornells A and Slater M (2016) Decreased Corticospinal Excitability after the Illusion of Missing Part of the Arm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:145. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00145
Received: 19 November 2015; Accepted: 21 March 2016;
Published: 14 April 2016.
Edited by:
Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USAReviewed by:
Alessandro Farne, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, FranceBrian S. Baum, Regis University, USA
Copyright © 2016 Kilteni, Grau-Sánchez, Veciana De Las Heras, Rodríguez-Fornells and Slater. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Mel Slater, melslater@ub.edu