Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 March 2016
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience

The Effect of Aging in Inhibitory Control of Major Depressive Disorder Revealed by Event-Related Potentials

  • 1Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Dalian Medical University, Dalian, Liaoning, China
  • 2Translational Medicine Research Center of Nerve System Disease, Dalian, Liaoning, China

Elderly depressed patients manifest pronounced executive dysfunction compared with younger subjects with depressive disorder. Aging-related brain changes may result in executive dysfunction in geriatric depression. We investigated the neural correlates of inhibitory control processing in depressed subjects at different ages using event-related potentials (ERPs). A equiprobable visual Go/Nogo task was used in 19 young (27.4 ± 5.0 years) and 18 elderly (70.8 ± 6.9 years) depressed subjects and their age-matched healthy controls (20 young subjects, 26.2 ± 3.7 years, and 18 elderly subjects, 68.1 ± 4.8 years). The responses were based on two types of equilateral triangular figures of upright (Go) and inverted triangle (Nogo). The elderly subjects exhibited later N2 and P3 latencies, and larger Go-N2 and P3 amplitudes, compared with the younger subjects. Further, the elderly controls displayed smaller P3 in the central and parietal regions, and yielded larger Nogo-P3 amplitude in the frontal region compared with younger controls. While the young depressed patients yielded smaller P3 amplitude than the controls across frontal, central and parietal regions, elderly depressed patients yielded smaller P3 than the elderly controls only in the frontal region. Our results suggest that the inhibitory control subprocesses are differentially affected by depression and aging. The stimulus response speed and the effort intensity of inhibition control are specifically impaired in the elderly depressed patients. And the diminished amplitudes of frontal P3 in the elderly depression imply a frontal dysfunction mechanism.

Introduction

In addition to mood disorder, a broad range of cognitive deficits contributes to the susceptibility to and maintenance of major depressive disorder (MDD; Lee et al., 2012). Clinically, patients with MDD during advanced age show more pronounced executive dysfunction (Herrmann et al., 2007; Koenig et al., 2014), including a set of cognitive behaviors essential for normal mental processes such as decision-making, conflict resolution, error correction, and response inhibition (Alexopoulos, 2003). However, mild executive dysfunction occurs with normal aging (Keys and White, 2000; Brown et al., 2015), provoking questions about the concomitant effects of aging and depression on executive function. A neuropsychological study revealed that compared with depressed younger counterparts and healthy elderly subjects, depressed elderly patients had disproportionately poor scores in terms of inhibitory control and focused effort, while no age-depression interaction was found for tasks of selective or sustained attention (Lockwood et al., 2002). These studies imply that in addition to aging, other brain changes may be associated with development of executive dysfunction in geriatric depression. However, the neural basis of this disparity of executive dysfunction in adult depressed patient at different age group is unclear.

Inhibitory control, which involves initiation, active switching, and inhibition of overlearned responses, is a central component of executive function (Norman and Shallice, 1986). Neuroimaging studies have reported the involvement of prefrontal cortex (PFC), anterior cingulated cortex (ACC) and frontal limbic area in the inhibitory mechanism (Garavan et al., 1999; Luijten et al., 2014), which are reliably activated in subjects overcoming interference from incorrect but pre-potent response tendencies (Botvinick et al., 1999; Carter et al., 2000). Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI studies revealed structural and functional abnormalities of frontal lobe in depressed patients (Drevets et al., 1997; Ballmaier et al., 2004; Andreescu et al., 2013; Ironside et al., 2015; Wise et al., 2016) and normal aging adults (He et al., 2013). Further, the MDD patients showed abnormal activation in frontal and anterior cingulated areas during inhibitory processing (Matthews et al., 2009).

Event-related potentials (ERPs) provide enhanced temporal resolution of brain activities whereas fMRI techniques have limited use in studies of cognitive processes unfolding in the sub-second range. Inhibitory control processes can be examined by ERPs with Go/Nogo tasks (Falkenstein et al., 1999). Two major ERP components have been consistently linked with the inhibitory processes. First, the Nogo-N2 is regarded as a phasic negative shift in Nogo compared with Go trials, with a maximum amplitude over frontocentral scalp locations around 200 ms post-stimulus (Eimer, 1993). Second, a positive deflection of around 300 to 600 ms poststimulus with a larger amplitude than Go-P3 or P3b in the frontocentral area, is known as Nogo-P3 (Bokura et al., 2001). The Nogo-N2 is located within the ACC area and mediates the cognitive top-down inhibition mechanism to suppress the incorrect tendency to respond at a processing stage prior to motor execution (Falkenstein et al., 1999; Bokura et al., 2001). Nevertheless, the Nogo-N2 in Go/Nogo task also reflected conflict monitoring rather than response inhibition (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; Randall and Smith, 2011). The Nogo-P3 is likely to be related to motor inhibition (Bruin et al., 2001; Burle et al., 2004). However, the Nogo-P3 is not related to the inhibition itself, but to the cancellation of the planned response (Randall and Smith, 2011). Although the precise cognitive processes of Nogo-N2 and Nogo-P3 components remain unclear, they are implicated in inhibitory control mechanism (Huster et al., 2013).

There are a few published ERPs reports describing inhibitory control processes in MDD patients using a Go/Nogo paradigm (Kaiser et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2007; Ruchsow et al., 2008; Katz et al., 2010). In a study of middle-aged adult depression (mean age = 40.0 years), a reduction in early frontotemporal positivity in the N2 time window (polarity-inverted N2) has been reported in an auditory Go/Nogo task (Kaiser et al., 2003). In addition, depressed patients (mean age = 40.1 years) in partial remission showed a specifically reduced Nogo-P3 while the Nogo-N2 was unaffected when performing a hybrid flanker Go/Nogo paradigm (Ruchsow et al., 2008). In a previous study, we measured ERPs from the subjects with late-life depression (mean age = 68.7 years) and matched controls using a simple visual Go/Nogo task, and found a larger Nogo-N2 and smaller Nogo-P3 in the depressed group (Zhang et al., 2007). Katz et al. (2010), however, found a strongly reduced Nogo-N2 enhancement in depressed elderly participants (mean age = 73.4 years), with data pointing to more posterior areas of the middle frontal gyrus. The inconsistent findings across these studies might be due to age-related effects, medication effects, differences in adopted paradigms and severity of depression, warranting further investigation.

Given neuropsychological tests have revealed that interaction between aging and depression minimizes executive function and compromises inhibitory control in patients with MDD (Lockwood et al., 2002), we hypothesized that aging and depression would interactively modulate ERPs. To our knowledge, the effects of aging and depression on inhibitory functioning were not simultaneously investigated via neuroelectrophysiological studies. Accordingly, the aim of the present study was to analyze ERPs presentation of subjects with MDD in different age groups, with a particular focus on the inhibitory control processes via Go/Nogo task. To control the effects of aging and depression on the ERPs waves, young adults and elderly MDD patients were included along with their gender-, education-, and age-matched controls.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty young adults (age range of 21–36 years) and 21 elderly adults (age range of 60–80 years) with MDD were recruited from the First Affiliated Hospital of Dalian Medical University. All the patients met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria of MDD single episode (296.3×) based on a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV by two experienced psychiatrists. Twenty young adults and 20 elderly adults with no current or past axis I psychiatric diagnosis were recruited by advertisement and served as control groups. The young controls and MDDs as well as the elderly controls and MDDs were matched for age, gender, and education. All participants were right-handed and naive to antidepressants before this study. The exclusion criteria for patient and control participants were: (1) head injury or neurological disorders; (2) history of substance abuse; (3) other medical illnesses compromising the central nervous system; (4) the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Cockrell and Folstein, 1988) scores below 24; and (5) hearing, vision, or motor impairment that precluded the behavioral task. Severity of depression was evaluated by a Chinese version of the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale of Depression (HRSD-17; Hamilton, 1960), and severity of co-morbid anxiety was indicated by a Chinese version of the 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAM-A; Hamilton, 1959). One young patient, three elderly patients, and two elderly controls were excluded because of insufficient number of correct artifact-free trials. The final sample consisted of 19 young patients and 18 elderly patients, 20 young controls and 18 elderly controls (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical demographic and behavioral profile (Mean SD).

The study protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of Dalian Medical University (KY2013–36). All the subjects signed written informed consent after the experiment was fully explained.

Stimuli and Task

An uncued visual Go/Nogo task (Zhang et al., 2007) was presented on a computer monitor by STIM-2 Software. The whole task consisted of 200 stimuli. Upright or inverted equilateral triangular figures were used as the stimuli, which were white on a black background, presented pseudo-randomly with equal probability on the screen (light degree = 60 cd/m2), and with a viewing distance of 150 cm. The stimulus size was approximately 2.8° horizontally and vertically. The duration of the stimulus was 50 ms and the inter-trial interval was set at 950 ms. Participants were instructed to press a button with the left or right thumb as quickly as possible for each upright triangle (Go), but inhibit their response upon seeing inverted triangles (Nogo). The thumb used was counterbalanced among the subjects (a half subjects used their left thumb and another half used their right thumb). The speed of response was stressed, as the subjects had to respond within 600 ms, and responses beyond this window were deemed incorrect. All the subjects underwent a short training session containing 20 stimuli to ensure that they understood the task correctly. The reaction times (RTs) for Go stimuli and response accuracy were measured.

EEG Recording and ERP Analysis

Electroencephalography (EEG) was continuously recorded from Ag/AgCl electrodes in an elastic cap (10/20 system) with a left mastoid reference. The EEG recording sites were: FP1, FP2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FT7, FC3, FCz, FC4, FT8, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, TP7, CP3, CPz, CP4, TP8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, Oz, and O2. The electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded using two pairs of electrodes, one placed above and below the right eye, and another 10 mm from the lateral canthi. The electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ throughout the experiment. The EEG was amplified using a Neuroscan NuAmps system. Amplifier settings included a bandpass of 0.1~100 Hz and a sampling rate of 1000 Hz.

The SCAN 4.3 Software was used for off-line analysis. The EEG was re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids. The EOG artifacts were corrected using the method proposed by Semlitsch et al. (1986). The EEG was segmented into the epoch from 200 ms pre-stimulus to 800 ms post-stimulus and the baseline corrected to the mean amplitude 200 ms before the stimulus. The trials contaminated with artifacts greater than ± 100 μV were rejected before averaging. Single trials were visually inspected and those containing muscular or other artifact were excluded manually. We removed the trials with response times shorter than 100 ms, as they were assumed to reflect non-deliberate behavior. The artifact-free EEG epochs with correct response for Go and Nogo conditions were averaged respectively. Only the trials with correct response (button-press) for the Go condition and correct inhibition (no button-press) for the Nogo condition were averaged, and at least 50 trials were available for each subject and condition. The numbers of trials remaining for each group were shown in Table 2. For further analysis, the ERPs data were digitally low-pass filtered at 16 Hz using a zero phase-shift filter (squared Butterworth FIR filter, 24 dB/octave).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The number of trials remaining for averaging for each group (Mean (SD)).

Visual inspection of the grand average waveforms revealed typical N2 and P3 components in all the groups. Based on previous studies (Falkenstein et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2007; Ruchsow et al., 2008), the grand average waves and topographies for each experimental condition, the peak amplitudes and latencies of N2 were measured at the Fz and Cz sites, and the peak amplitudes and latencies of P3 was measrue at the Fz, Cz and Pz sites. Stimulus-locked time windows of 150–250 ms were selected for N2 and 250–420 ms for P3. Local minima (N2) or maxima (P3) were used for peak picking, manual intervention was possible during the process to ensure that the computer did not make anomalous peak selections. To avoid N2 component potentially distorting P3 component, peak-to-peak analysis (subtracting the peak amplitude and latency of N2 from P3) were performed at frontal and central sites (Fz and Cz).

Statistical Analysis

The behavioral data (i.e., RTs, hit rate (HR) and false alarm rate (FAR)) were analyzed using univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA). The between-subject variables included age (young, elderly) and depression (depressed, control). Errors caused by delayed responses (missing the RTs deadline) were not considered. Accuracy of target signal detection was assessed by calculating sensory discriminability (d′) from hit and false rates (Swets et al., 1978).

For the ERPs data, mixed design repeated measures ANOVA (rmANOVA) were conducted, including the between-subjects factors of age (young, elderly) and depression (depressed, control), and within-subject factors of trial-type (Go vs. Nogo) and electrode-site (Fz and Cz for N2, Fz, Cz and Pz for P3). Global analyses were followed by restricted between- and within-subject analyses for interpretation of significant interactions. The Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were adopted where appropriate (Jennings and Wood, 1976). Post hoc comparisons were conducted using the Bonferroni procedure. The correlation between the ERPs and clinical and behavioral data was assessed using the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient.

Results

Clinical Data and Behavioral Performance

Table 1 presents clinical and behavioral data of all groups. There was no difference in age, gender proportion and educational levels between young depressed and control subjects, and between elderly depressed and control subjects (all p values > 0.1). Also, the episodes did not differ statistically between young and elderly depressed patients (p > 0.05). Significant differences in HRSD-17 and HAM-A (all p values < 0.05), but not in MMSE (p > 0.05) were observed between patients and controls.

The young subjects showed shorter RTs, higer HR, lower FAR and higher d′ value than the elderly subjects for significant age main effects (F(1,71) = 12.45, p = 0.001; F(1,71) = 4.22, p = 0.044; F(1,71) = 9.29, p = 0.036; F(1,71) = 6.90, p = 0.011, respectively). In addition, the ANOVA revealed significant depression main effects for RTs (F(1,71) = 4.11, p = 0.046, control < depression) and d′ (F(1,71) = 8.22, p = 0.005, control > depression), but not for HR and FAR (all p values > 0.05). There was no significant interaction between age and depression in any behavioral measures (all p values > 0.05).

Event-Related Potentials

Figure 1 illustrate the grand-average ERPs over Fz, Cz and Pz sites in response to Go and Nogo trials. The topographic maps of the N2 and P3 time windows for each type trial are presented in Figure 2. For the sake of brevity, other ERPs components were not reported, and the effects not involving “age” or “depression” were not analyzed in detail.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Grand average event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by Go and Nogo stimuli in four groups. The young controls manifested larger P3 than young depressed patients across frontal (Fz), central (Cz) and parietal (Pz) sites. In elderly subjects, only Fz site showed differences between depressed and controls. The elderly subjects showed prolonged N2 and P3 latency and enhanced Go-N2 amplitude. The elderly controls elicited smaller P3 at central (Cz) and parietal (Pz) sites, whereas larger Nogo-P3 at Fz site compared with young controls.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Two-deminsional scalp topographic distributions of N2 and P3 elicited by Go and Nogo stimuli in four groups (YC, young control; YD, young depression; EC, elderly control; ED, elderly depression). The peak latencies of N2 and P3 was selected across the four groups.

N2 Amplitudes

As shown in Figure 1, Nogo stimuli evoked larger N2 component than Go stimuli in all the groups. Analysis of the N2 amplitude revealed a significant main effect for trial-type (−2.65 ± 7.63 μV for Nogo vs. 0.10 ± 7.64 μV for Go; F(1,71) = 61.42, p < 0.001). As illustrated in Figure 1, young subjects showed smaller N2 compared with the elderly subjects in Go trials, whereas Nogo-N2 amplitude did not differ between young and elderly subjects. ANOVA yielded significant trial-type × age interaction effects (F(1,71) = 5.33, p = 0.024). Depressed subjects demonstrated mildly larger Nogo-N2 amplitude than that of control groups, but the difference was not significant (F(1,71) = 0.92, p = 0.341). No significant main effects and interactive effects were seen in between-subjects analysis in N2 amplitude (all p values > 0.1).

N2 Latencies

In regard to the N2 latency, there was a significant main effect of trial-type (F(1,71) = 12.16, p = 0.001). As expected, Nogo stimuli yielded delayed N2 component than Go stimuli (217.16 ± 33.73 ms vs. 207.73 ± 32.48 ms). As shown in Figure 1, the elderly participants displayed later N2 peaks than the young, and ANOVA also revealed a main effect of age (221.20 ± 31.41 ms vs. 204.37 ± 31.41 ms; F(1,71)= 2.37, p = 0.012). The main effect of depression, trial-type × age and age × depression interaction were not significant (all p values > 0.5).

P3 Amplitudes

As illustrated in Figure 1, the Nogo-P3 was larger than Go-P3 in frontal and central regions, whereas Go-P3 was larger than Nogo-P3 in the parietal region of the four groups. The within-subject analysis yielded significant main effects of trial-type (9.84 ± 8.76 μV for Nogo vs. 11.01 ± 5.05 μV for Go; F(1,71)= 11.24, p = 0.001) and electrode-site (F(1,71) = 31.47, p < 0.001) as well as a trial-type × electrode-site interaction which indicated the discrepancy of Go-P3 and Nogo-P3 distribution (F(1,71) = 86.52, p < 0.001). Post hoc analysis demonstrated that Nogo-P3 amplitude was significantly larger than Go-P3 in Fz (11.74 ± 4.73 μV vs. 10.54 ± 4.54 μV) and Cz (11.11 ± 4.65 μV vs. 11.51 ± 4.88 μV) sites, whereas Go-P3 was significantly larger than Nogo-P3 in the Pz (10.98 ± 4.89 μV vs. 6.67 ± 4.21 μV) site (all p values < 0.05).

The between-subject analysis revealed main effects of age (11.29 ± 4.84 μV for young subjects vs. 9.48 ± 4.83 μV for elderly subjects; F(1,71) = 4.62, p = 0.035) and depression ((9.35 ± 4.96 μV for depression subjects vs. 11.46 ± 4.69 μV for controls; F(1,71) = 6.33, p = 0.014), these main effects were qualified by age × electrode-site (F(1.41,100.3) = 9.51, p = 0.001), depression × electrode-site (F(1.41,100.3) = 3.50, p = 0.049), and age × depression × electrode-site interactions (F(1.41,100.3) = 6.83, p = 0.005). To explain these interactions, further restricted ANOVAs were carried out. First, the ANOVA was performed on the young and elderly subjects separately to highlight the different pattern of P3 in depression and controls (Figures 1, 3). In the young subjects, a main effect of depression (12.46 ± 4.43 μV for young control vs. 10.07 ± 4.96 μV for young depressed; F(1,37) = 4.32, p = 0.045) was observed (Figure 3), no other main effects or interactions reached significant level) all p values > 0.05). In the elderly subjects, however, there was no main effect of depression (F(1,37) = 2.22, p = 0.146). A depression × electrode-site interaction was shown in elderly subjects (F(1.42, 71) = 10.46, p < 0.001), and post hoc analysis indicated that the elderly depressed group yielded smaller P3 at frontal (Fz) site compared with the elderly controls (9.00 ± 4.77 μV vs. 13.24 ± 4.21 μV, p < 0.05; Figure 3). Second, the ANOVA was calculated in the control and depressed subjects separately to highlight the differences in the P3 pattern between the young and elderly subjects (Figures 1, 3). The P3 amplitude in young controls was significantly larger than in elderly controls (12.46 ± 4.43 μV vs. 10.37 ± 4.76 μV, F(1,36) = 4.12, p = 0.049). Further, the main effect was qualified by age × electrode site (F(1.33,50) = 16.06, p < 0.001) and age × trial-type × electrode-site interactions (F(1.5,54.5) = 4.94, p = 0.019). Post hoc analysis revealed that the elderly controls displayed smaller P3 in the central (Cz; 9.64 ± 4.31 μV vs. 12.69 ± 3.72 μV) and parietal (Pz; 7.23 ± 4.36 μV vs. 11.55 ± 4.81 μV) sites (all p values < 0.05), but larger Nogo-P3 in the frontal (Fz) site (13.80 ± 4.42 μV vs. 11.79 ± 3.81 μV) compared with young controls (all p values < 0.05). In the depressed subjects, no age main effect was observed (all p values > 0.1).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The main effect and interaction effects of P3 amplitude. Error bars represent standard errors.

P3 Latencies

As shown in Figure 1, elderly subjects yielded delayed P3 peaks compared with younger subjects. The between-subject analysis yielded significant age main effect (315.12 ± 37.35 ms for young vs. 350.42 ± 49 ms for elderly; F(1,71) = 33.93, p < 0.001). The within-subject analysis revealed significant main effects of the trial-type (F(1,71) = 57.35, p < 0.001) and electrode site (F(1.38,97.9) = 24.09, p < 0.001). The Nogo-P3 showed later peak latencies than the Go-P3 (352.12 ± 40.75 ms vs. 328.31 ± 40.91 ms). Posterior P3 showed earlier peaks than the anterior sites (339.13 ± 44.35 ms for Fz site, 337.24 ± 46.08 ms for Cz site and 319.86 ± 47.46 ms for Pz). The trial-type effect (Go/Nogo effect) was further modulated by trial-type × age interaction (F(1,71) = 11.07, p = 0.001). Significant age main effects were observed when ANOVAs were performed on the Go- and Nogo-P3 latency separately (young < elderly, all p values < 0.05). Remarkably, a depression main effect was found for the Nogo-P3 latency (340.49 ± 35.90 ms for controls vs. 355.20 ± 50.63 ms for depressed subjects; F(1,71) = 5. 80, p = 0.027) but not for the Go-P3 latencies (F(1,71) = 0.42, p > 0.5).

N2/P3 Peak-to-peak Amplitudes and Latencies

For the N2/P3 peak-to-peak amplitudes, the ANOVA showed significant main effects of depression (13.38 ± 5.49 μV for controls vs. 11.60 ± 4.13 μV for depressed subjects; F(1,71) = 4.96, p = 0.029) and trial-type (14.08 ± 5.37 μV for Nogo vs. 10.93 ± 4.60 μV for Go, F(1,71) = 43.37, p < 0.001). Similar to the analysis of the P3 amplitudes, the depression main effects of N2/P3 peak-to-peak amplitudes were further qualified by depression × electrode-site (F(1,71) = 8.31 p = 0.004) and age × depression × electrode-site interactions (F(1,71) = 9.12, p = 0.004). For the N2/P3 peak-to-peak latencies, the between-subject analysis yielded significant age main effect (115.87 ± 39.86 ms for young vs. 137.25 ± 48.09 ms for elderly; F(1,71) = 7.80, p = 0.007). The within-subject analysis revealed significant main effects of the trial-type (136.12 ± 43.41 ms for Nogo vs. 116.65 ± 44.87 ms for Go, F(1,71) = 13.97, p < 0.001). No significant age × depression interaction effect was shown on both N2/P3 peak-to-peak amplitudes and latencies (all p values > 0.1).

Correlation Analysis

Correlation analysis between the clinical scale and ERPs measures (Nogo-N2 and Nogo-P3 in Fz site, Go-P3 in Pz site) in the young and the elderly depressed groups, respectively, demonstrated no significant correlation between HRSD-17 or HAM-A scores and ERPs measures (all p values > 0.05).

Discussion

In the present study, the behavioral performance and ERPs components were assessed in young and elderly adults with MDD and their age-, gender-, and education-matched controls using a visual Go/Nogo task. The effects of depression and aging on the behavior and electrophysiology were analyzed. The younger subjects showed shorter RTs, higher HR, lower FAR and higher d′ indices than those of the elderly. The patients with MDD demonstrated longer RT and lower d′ than in the controls. All the subjects displayed a distinct Go/Nogo effect, i.e., Nogo stimuli elicited larger frontal N2 and larger frontal P3 compared with Go stimuli. The Go-P3 was mainly distributed in the parietal region, whereas the Nogo-P3 showed “anteriorization” effects (Figure 2), replicating previous studies (Falkenstein et al., 1999; Bokura et al., 2001; Zhang et al., 2007; Randall and Smith, 2011). The young depressed patients yielded smaller P3 amplitude than young controls across frontal, central and parietal region (Figure 1). The elderly depressed patients yielded smaller P3 than the elderly controls only in the frontal region (Figures 1, 3). The elderly subjects showed delayed N2 and P3 latencies, and larger N2 (only for Go-N2) and P3 amplitude, compared with the young subjects (Figure 1). Further, the elderly controls elicited smaller P3 in the central and parietal regions, and yielded larger Nogo-P3 in the frontal region compared with young controls (Figures 1, 3).

Effects of Age

The elderly subjects yielded longer N2 latency and larger Go-N2 compared to young participants, consistent with the decreased HR and d′ index in elderly subjects. Hence, the altered N2 latency in geriatric subjects may primarily be age-related, suggesting deficient pre-motor processes underlying response activation and inhibition in aged subjects, consistent with studies involving healthy elderly adults (Mudar et al., 2015) and depressed elderly subjects (Katz et al., 2010). Given the Go-N2 component may be associated with stimulus-response or response activation (Gajewski et al., 2008), the enhanced Go-N2 amplitudes in the elderly participants indicated that additional processing resources were required to activate the response signal in elderly subjects.

The elderly subjects displayed delayed P3 latency and N2/P3 peak-to-peak latency in both Go and Nogo trails compared with the controls, in line with a previous study (Vallesi, 2011). The prolonged P3 latency was also consistent with the defective behavioral data in the elderly groups, indicating that the elderly depressed participants were slower than the young participants in their responses to the stimuli.

The young controls displayed more robust P3 in the central and parietal regions but smaller frontal Nogo-P3 than those in the elderly controls. The parietal P3 (P3b) amplitude reflects the context-updating operations and subsequent memory storage and its latency reflecting stimulus evaluation time, which are essential for the cognitive processes (Polich, 2007). The frontal Nogo-P3 reflects the monitoring of successful outcome of the inhibition process (Schmajuk et al., 2006). Neurocognitive studies have reported altered patterns of brain activity associated with executive function in aged vs. younger adults (Turner and Spreng, 2012). Age-related activity during inhibitory control was observed in the right inferior frontal gyrus, and the elderly adults engaged the dorsolateral PFC as well as supplementary motor cortex and left inferior parietal lobule during working memory (Turner and Spreng, 2012). Therefore, the present behavioral and ERPs outcomes confirmed that altered cerebral cortex activity generally mediated normal aging during executive processing.

The elderly controls showed larger frontal Nogo-P3 than the young controls in this study, consistent with a previous study (Vallesi et al., 2009). However, no significant correlation of subject’s age with the Nogo-P3 amplitude was observed (Fallgatter et al., 1999). Mudar et al. revealed significantly reduced Nogo-P3 amplitudes in the aged compared with younger adults (Mudar et al., 2015). This discrepancy suggests a nonlinear relationship between inhibitory strength and Nogo-P3 amplitude considering that the tasks differed across these studies. Therefore, further investigations into mechanisms modulating the inhibitory strength are needed to test this hypothesis.

Effects of Depression

The depressed subjects demonstrated mildly enhanced Nogo-N2 but did not reach significant level, supporting a previous study showing an unaffected Nogo-N2 (Ruchsow et al., 2008), but inconsistent with the results showing more negative Nogo-N2 in depressed groups (Kaiser et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2007). In Kaiser et al.’s (2003) study, all the patients were middle-aged adults (mean age = 40.0 years) and treated with antidepressant medication at the time of the experiment, and the task they used was a modification of the auditory oddball paradigm. Our previous research (Zhang et al., 2007) adopted a same paradigm to the present study in elderly subjects, but eleven patients were taking antidepressant. Several studies demonstrated that medication per se has an impact on ERP amplitudes (e. g. Johannes et al., 2001). Therefore the divergence might be attributed to the differences in processing demands and the baseline demographic and medical profile across these studies.

The between-subject analysis revealed main effects of depression in P3 amplitude as well as N2/P3 peak-peak amplitude (depression < control). Additionally, our results revealed a depression main effect for the the Nogo-P3 latency (depression > control). While Nogo-P3 reflects response inhibition, Go-P3 reflects response activation, the activation of elements in an event categorization network that is controlled by the joint operation of attention and working memory (Kok, 2001). Therefore these results consistent with the clinical facts that depressed patients manifest both general attention impairment and executive dysfunction (Kindermann et al., 2000; Alexopoulos, 2003; Lee et al., 2012).

Effects of Interactions

In the present study, the ERP measurements did not yield any first-order age × depression interaction, but showed significant age × depression × electrode-site interactions on P3 amplitudes and N2/P3 peek-to-peek amplitudes. The young depressed subjects manifested decreased P3 amplitude across frontal, central and parietal regions compared with the young controls, but in the aged subjects, depressed patients showed diminished P3 magnitude only in the frontal region, especially for the Nogo-P3. Diminished Nogo-P3 has been reported in young depressed (Ruchsow et al., 2008) and elderly depressed patients (Zhang et al., 2007). The Nogo-P3 originates within the ACC (Fallgatter et al., 2004; Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2013), midcingulate cortex (MCC) and the pre-central region as well as the insulae (Huster et al., 2010). It is responsible for response monitoring and is connected to brain structures that regulate mood, emotion, thought and visceral responses (Schneider-Hassloff et al., 2015). Indices of ACC dysfunction are associated with adverse outcomes of geriatric depression (Alexopoulos et al., 2008). Importantly, the present study showed altered cerebral pattern of inhibitory between young and elderly depressed patients. The amplitude of P3 diminished more “anteriorly” in geriatric depressed patients than in young depressed patients, implying a mechanism of frontal dysfunction in geriatric depression.

Numerous studies suggested that executive dysfunction occurred in a considerable number of older individuals with MDD (Alexopoulos et al., 2000, 2008; Kindermann et al., 2000; Lockwood et al., 2002; Alexopoulos, 2003; Herrmann et al., 2007). Previous findings provide indirect support for the role of frontostriatal-limbic abnormalities in the executive dysfunction of geriatric depression (Kindermann et al., 2000; Hannestad et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2007; Bobb et al., 2012). However, given the potential contribution of age on the brain regions related to executive function (He et al., 2013; Brown et al., 2015), and the dependence of different subcomponents of inhibitory control on different brain circuits (Huster et al., 2010, 2013), the present results suggest variable age- and depression-related deficits in the timing or rate of decline in function. Our results suggest that the response stimulus evaluation speed (indexed as Go-P3 latency) and the intensity of inhibition monitoring or evaluating process (indexed as frontal P3 amplitude) are impaired in the elderly depressed patients, whereas the pre-motor inhibition processes (indexed as N2 component) are modulated by aging but less likely by depressed status.

No significant correlation between ERPs measurements and the HRSD-17 was found, consistent with two previous studies (Kaiser et al., 2003; Ruchsow et al., 2008) but in contrast to another study (Zhang et al., 2007). These findings suggest that the inhibitory control deficit might be a disease-dependent feature in MDD patients.

This study is limited by the cross-sectional design involving distinct age groups. Therefore, further work is needed to investigate the role of age as a continuous variable across a larger number of participants. The feasibility of ERPs as biological markers of recovery in longitudinal studies needs to be evaluated in the future.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that the subprocesses of inhibitory control are differentially affected by aging and depression. The pre-motor inhibition processes are modulated by aging but less likely by depressed status. Specifically, the stimulus response speed and the effort intensity of inhibition control are impaired in the elderly depressed patients. And the diminished amplitudes of frontal P3 in geriatric depression imply a frontal dysfunction mechanism.

Author Contributions

BWZ and JX carried out the studies, participated in collecting data, and drafted the manuscript. YC performed the statistical analysis and participated in its design. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81401486, 81101113 and 81171412).

References

Alexopoulos, G. S. (2003). Role of executive function in late-life depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 18–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alexopoulos, G. S., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Latoussakis, V., Kanellopoulos, D., and Murphy, C. F. (2008). Anterior cingulate dysfunction in geriatric depression. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 347–355. doi: 10.1002/gps.1939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexopoulos, G. S., Meyers, B. S., Young, R. C., Kalayam, B., Kakuma, T., Gabrielle, M., et al. (2000). Executive dysfunction and long-term outcomes of geriatric depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 285–290. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.3.285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatry Press.

Google Scholar

Andreescu, C., Tudorascu, D. L., Butters, M. A., Tamburo, E., Patel, M., Price, J., et al. (2013). Resting state functional connectivity and treatment response in late-life depression. Psychiatry Res. 214, 313–321. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballmaier, M., Toga, A. W., Blanton, R. E., Sowell, E. R., Lavretsky, H., Peterson, J., et al. (2004). Anterior cingulate, gyrus rectus and orbitofrontal abnormalities in elderly depressed patients: an MRI-based parcellation of the prefrontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 99–108. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobb, D. S., Jr. Adinoff, B., Laken, S. J., McClintock, S. M., Rubia, K., Huang, H. W., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of successful response inhibition in unmedicated patients with late-life depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 1057–1069. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e318235b728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., and Kobayashi, S. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2224–2232. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00691-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 402, 179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S. W., Johnson, T. M., Sohl, M. E., and Dumas, M. K. (2015). Executive attentional resources in timing: effects of inhibitory control and cognitive aging. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 1063–1083. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruin, K. J., Wijers, A. A., and van Staveren, A. S. (2001). Response priming in a Go/Nogo task: do we have to explain the Go/Nogo N2 effect in terms of response activation instead of inhibition? Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 1660–1671. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00601-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burle, B., Vidal, F., and Bonnet, M. (2004). Electroencephalographic nogo potentials in a no-movement context: the case of motor imagery in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 360, 77–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., et al. (2000). Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 1944–1948. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cockrell, J. R., and Folstein, M. F. (1988). Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Psychopharmacol. Bull. 24, 689–692.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C., Price, J. L., Simpson, J. R., Jr. Todd, R. D., Reich, T., Vannier, M., et al. (1997). Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 386, 824–827. doi: 10.1038/386824a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (1993). Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task. Biol. Psychol. 35, 123–138. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(93)90009-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., and Hohnsbein, J. (1999). ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta. Psychol. 101, 267–291. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallgatter, A. J., Ehlis, A. C., Seifert, J., Strik, W. K., Scheuerpflug, P., Zillessen, K. E., et al. (2004). Altered response control and anterior cingulate function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder boys. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 973–981. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallgatter, A. J., Mueller, T. J., and Strik, W. K. (1999). Age-related changes in the brain electrical correlates of response control. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 833–838. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00022-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajewski, P. D., Stoerig, P., and Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP—correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Res. 1189, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., and Stein, E. A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 96, 8301–8306. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Rosa, J. J., Inuggi, A., Blasi, V., Cursi, M., Annovazzi, P., Comi, G., et al. (2013). Response competition and response inhibition during different choice-discrimination tasks: evidence from ERP measured inside MRI scanner. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 37–47. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1959). The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 32, 50–55. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannestad, J., Taylor, W. D., McQuoid, D. R., Payne, M. E., Krishnan, K. R., Steffens, D. C., et al. (2006). White matter lesion volumes and caudate volumes in late-life depression. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 21, 1193–1198. doi: 10.1002/gps.1640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, X., Qin, W., Liu, Y., Zhang, X., Duan, Y., Song, J., et al. (2013). Age-related decrease in functional connectivity of the right fronto-insular cortex with the central executive and default-mode networks in adults from young to middle age. Neurosci. Lett. 544, 74–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.03.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, L. L., Goodwin, G. M., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2007). The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychol. Med. 37, 1693–1702. doi: 10.1017/s0033291707001134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., and Herrmann, C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87, 217–233. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Westerhausen, R., Pantev, C., and Konrad, C. (2010). The role of the cingulate cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1260–1271. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ironside, M., O’Shea, J., Cowen, P. J., and Harmer, C. J. (2015). Frontal cortex stimulation reduces vigilance to threat: implications for the treatment of depression and anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.012 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, J. R., and Wood, C. C. (1976). Letter: the epsilon-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. Psychophysiology 13, 277–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb00116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannes, S., Wieringa, B. M., Nager, W., Dengler, R., and Münte, T. F. (2001). Oxazepam alters action monitoring. Psychopharmacology 155, 100–106. doi: 10.1007/s002130100680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, S., Unger, J., Kiefer, M., Markela, J., Mundt, C., and Weisbrod, M. (2003). Executive control deficit in depression: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task. Psychiatry Res. 122, 169–184. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00004-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, R., De Sanctis, P., Mahoney, J. R., Sehatpour, P., Murphy, C. F., Gomez-Ramirez, M., et al. (2010). Cognitive control in late-life depression: response inhibition deficits and dysfunction of the anterior cingulate cortex. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 1017–1025. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181d695f2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keys, B. A., and White, D. A. (2000). Exploring the relationship between age, executive abilities and psychomotor speed. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 6, 76–82. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700611098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindermann, S. S., Kalayam, B., Brown, G. G., Burdick, K. E., and Alexopoulos, G. S. (2000). Executive functions and P300 latency in elderly depressed patients and control subjects. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 8, 57–65. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200002000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, A. M., Bhalla, R. K., and Butters, M. A. (2014). Cognitive functioning and late-life depression. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 461–467. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201990559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. S., Hermens, D. F., Porter, M. A., and Redoblado-Hodge, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. J. Affect Disord 140, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockwood, K. A., Alexopoulos, G. S., and van Gorp, W. G. (2002). Executive dysfunction in geriatric depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 1119–1126. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luijten, M., Machielsen, M. W., Veltman, D. J., Hester, R., de Haan, L., and Franken, I. H. (2014). Systematic review of ERP and fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control and error processing in people with substance dependence and behavioural addictions. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 39, 149–169. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, S., Simmons, A., Strigo, I., Gianaros, P., Yang, T., and Paulus, M. (2009). Inhibition-related activity in subgenual cingulate is associated with symptom severity in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 172, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mudar, R. A., Chiang, H. S., Maguire, M. J., Spence, J. S., Eroh, J., Kraut, M. A., et al. (2015). Effects of age on cognitive control during semantic categorization. Behav. Brain Res. 287, 285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., van den Wildenberg, W., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 3, 17–26. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.1.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior. USA: Springer.

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, W. M., and Smith, J. L. (2011). Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2400–2407. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchsow, M., Groen, G., Kiefer, M., Beschoner, P., Hermle, L., Ebert, D., et al. (2008). Electrophysiological evidence for reduced inhibitory control in depressed patients in partial remission: a Go/Nogo study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 68, 209–218. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmajuk, M., Liotti, M., Busse, L. and Woldorff, M. G. (2006). Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults. Neuropsychologia 44, 384–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider-Hassloff, H., Straube, B., Nuscheler, B., Wemken, G., and Kircher, T. (2015). Adult attachment style modulates neural responses in a mentalizing task. Neuroscience 303, 462–473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., and Presslich, O. (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23, 695–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swets, J. A., Green, D. M., Getty, D. J., and Swets, J. B. (1978). Signal detection and identification at successive stages of observation. Percept. Psychophys. 23, 275–289. doi: 10.3758/bf03199711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, G. R., and Spreng, R. N. (2012). Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 826.e1–826.e13. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A. (2011). Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: a Go/Nogo event-related potential study. Neurosci. Lett. 487, 313–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., Stuss, D. T., McIntosh, A. R., and Picton, T. W. (2009). Age-related differences in processing irrelevant information: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia 47, 577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, T., Radua, J., Nortje, G., Cleare, A. J., Young, A. H., and Arnone, D. (2016). Voxel-based meta-analytical evidence of structural disconnectivity in major depression and bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 293–302. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, B. W., Zhao, L., and Xu, J. (2007). Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in late-life depression: a Go/Nogo study. Neurosci. Lett. 419, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, aging, inhibitory control, event-related potentials, N2, P3

Citation: Zhang B-W, Xu J and Chang Y (2016) The Effect of Aging in Inhibitory Control of Major Depressive Disorder Revealed by Event-Related Potentials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:116. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00116

Received: 30 September 2015; Accepted: 01 March 2016;
Published: 30 March 2016.

Edited by:

Chiang-shan R. Li, Yale University, USA

Reviewed by:

Andrew Chang, McMaster University, Canada
Peter Manza, Stony Brook University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Zhang, Xu and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jing Xu, eHVqaW5nX2RvY0BhbGl5dW4uY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.