Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 November 2014
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Facing the Other: Novel Theories and Methods in Face Perception Research View all 34 articles

Spatio-temporal dynamics and laterality effects of face inversion, feature presence and configuration, and face outline

\r\nKsenija Marinkovic,*Ksenija Marinkovic1,2*Maureen G. CourtneyMaureen G. Courtney3Thomas WitzelThomas Witzel4Anders M. Dale,Anders M. Dale1,5Eric Halgren,Eric Halgren1,5
  • 1Department of Radiology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
  • 2Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
  • 3Cognitive Neuroimaging Laboratory, Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
  • 4Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Radiology Department at Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
  • 5Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA

Although a crucial role of the fusiform gyrus (FG) in face processing has been demonstrated with a variety of methods, converging evidence suggests that face processing involves an interactive and overlapping processing cascade in distributed brain areas. Here we examine the spatio-temporal stages and their functional tuning to face inversion, presence and configuration of inner features, and face contour in healthy subjects during passive viewing. Anatomically-constrained magnetoencephalography (aMEG) combines high-density whole-head MEG recordings and distributed source modeling with high-resolution structural MRI. Each person's reconstructed cortical surface served to constrain noise-normalized minimum norm inverse source estimates. The earliest activity was estimated to the occipital cortex at ~100 ms after stimulus onset and was sensitive to an initial coarse level visual analysis. Activity in the right-lateralized ventral temporal area (inclusive of the FG) peaked at ~160 ms and was largest to inverted faces. Images containing facial features in the veridical and rearranged configuration irrespective of the facial outline elicited intermediate level activity. The M160 stage may provide structural representations necessary for downstream distributed areas to process identity and emotional expression. However, inverted faces additionally engaged the left ventral temporal area at ~180 ms and were uniquely subserved by bilateral processing. This observation is consistent with the dual route model and spared processing of inverted faces in prosopagnosia. The subsequent deflection, peaking at ~240 ms in the anterior temporal areas bilaterally, was largest to normal, upright faces. It may reflect initial engagement of the distributed network subserving individuation and familiarity. These results support dynamic models suggesting that processing of unfamiliar faces in the absence of a cognitive task is subserved by a distributed and interactive neural circuit.

Introduction

Faces have captured a great deal of attention in the neuroimaging field, resulting in important insights into the brain networks that underlie material-specific processing. Based on neuroimaging evidence of right-dominant activity in the fusiform cortex that is greater to faces than other meaningful visual stimuli, this area has been termed the “fusiform face area” (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006), although the nature of its “face-specificity” has been debated (Gauthier et al., 1999; Halgren et al., 2000; Haxby et al., 2001; Haxby, 2006; Cowell and Cottrell, 2013).

Studies using temporally precise methodology such as ERPs (Event-Related Potentials) and MEG (Magnetoencephalography) reveal a face-sensitive deflection peaking at around 170 ms (N170 and its magnetic counterpart M170) estimated to that region (Lu et al., 1991; Halgren et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2000; Watanabe et al., 2003; Schweinberger et al., 2007; Eimer, 2011; Miki et al., 2011; Rossion and Jacques, 2011; Taylor et al., 2011). Intracranial studies confirm both the timing and the location of the primary generator of these potentials in the inferotemporal region (Allison et al., 1994; Halgren et al., 1994a; McCarthy et al., 1997; Puce et al., 1997; Barbeau et al., 2008) but also indicate that the face processing is subserved by a distributed network additionally comprising anterior temporal and prefrontal regions (Halgren et al., 1994b; Klopp et al., 1999; Marinkovic et al., 2000; Barbeau et al., 2008). Generators of face-induced N170 are highly consistent with the fMRI activity in the right fusiform gyrus (FG) (Puce et al., 1997) although fMRI studies also confirm engagement of distributed occipital, temporal, and frontal areas (Ishai et al., 2004; Chan and Downing, 2011).

Converging evidence suggests that faces are processed in a series of successive, but overlapping and mutually interactive processing stages engaging multiple brain areas. Following encoding in the posterior visual areas (at ~100 ms), activation peaks in the FG at about 170 ms after stimulus onset. At this time it is briefly phase locked with the activity in distributed association cortices primarily in ventral temporal and prefrontal regions (Klopp et al., 2000), suggesting that the face processing is mediated by a network of simultaneously active sources during the N170 stage. The N170 is followed by a deflection at ~240 ms (Barbeau et al., 2008) and subsequent activity that mediates integration with mnemonic, emotional, and other contributions in distributed areas, resulting in face recognition (Halgren et al., 1994a,b; Puce et al., 1999). This broad outline of the spatio-temporal activity pattern is consistent with the original model proposed by Bruce and Young (1986) which, in turn, serves as the foundation of the currently prevalent accounts (Halgren et al., 1994a; Haxby et al., 2002; Ishai, 2008; Behrmann and Plaut, 2013). Even though these models conceptualize face processing as being mostly sequential in nature, it is clear that this is an interactive process with overlapping, rather than discrete and temporally circumscribed stages (Halgren et al., 1994a,b; Barbeau et al., 2008; Behrmann and Plaut, 2013). They flexibly mediate structural encoding, familiarity, and retrieval of semantic information resulting in recognition, with an increasing degree of reliance on distributed and interactive circuits.

The goal of this study was to examine the spatio-temporal stages and the functional tuning of the areas engaged during face processing with an anatomically-constrained MEG method. This multimodal methodology combines whole-head high-density MEG and a distributed source modeling approach with high-resolution structural MRI and cortical reconstruction to estimate the anatomical distribution of the underlying neural networks in a time-sensitive manner (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Hämäläinen and Ilmoniemi, 1994; Dale et al., 1999, 2000; Fischl et al., 1999a). Our analysis focused on both the relative amplitudes and latencies of the deflections evoked by faces and other conditions, as well as the spatial pattern of estimated activation. In particular, we wished to examine the sensitivity of the M170 to presence and configuration of inner features, face inversion, and face outline. Some of these variables have been manipulated in other studies (Bentin et al., 1996; Eimer, 2000b; Tong et al., 2000; Macchi Cassia et al., 2006; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007; Harris and Nakayama, 2008; Rossion and Jacques, 2008; Liu et al., 2010; Nichols et al., 2010; Gao et al., 2013) but we aimed to explore these effects in a more comprehensive manner. We used grayscale photographs of unfamiliar faces and manipulated face orientation (upright vs. inverted), internal features and external outline (present or absent) and the relative feature configuration (canonical vs. rearranged) resulting in the following conditions: “normal—N,” “inverted—I,” correct facial features presented in an oval without the hairline (“oval—O”), unnaturally rearranged facial features within the natural face outline (“rearranged—R”), blank faces with natural outlines but with no features (“blank—B”). Visual control (C) stimuli were obtained by randomizing grayscale patches of the face images so that they no longer looked like faces while preserving the spatial frequency, luminance, and overall shape. We were especially interested in investigating the functional profile of the M170 and its sensitivity to the presence and absence of features and their arrangement. For instance, if it indeed reflects a face-encoding stage, then it will be responsive to the presence of facial features irrespective of the facial outline (Bentin et al., 1996; Eimer, 2000b; Tong et al., 2000; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007). Furthermore, by using methodology that provides reasonable spatial source estimates, we wished to examine the spatial characteristics of the M170. For instance, even though the right hemisphere (RH) dominance of the M170 has been established (Halgren et al., 2000; Rossion et al., 2003a; Kloth et al., 2006), contributions of the left hemisphere (LH) at this latency in the context of these manipulations are not clear.

A special case is presented by inverting face stimuli and we included this condition in our study. Impaired recognition of faces that are presented upside-down, relative to other objects (Valentine, 1988) has been termed the “face inversion effect” and is associated with larger amplitude and longer latency of the N170 (Rossion et al., 1999; Eimer, 2000a; Itier and Taylor, 2004a). fMRI studies, however, show that the inverted faces evoke either a smaller or equivalent activity in the FG than the upright faces (Kanwisher et al., 1998; Gauthier et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 1999). Moreover, some fMRI evidence suggests that inverted faces also recruit non-face (“object”) areas, evoking stronger responses more medially (Aguirre et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 1999). The dual route model suggests that inverted faces are additionally processed by the LH in a feature-based manner (Moscovitch et al., 1997; De Gelder and Rouw, 2001). This model was examined by comparing the M170 activity to inverted faces in the left and right fusiform cortices and in other engaged areas. The M170 is commonly followed by activity peaking at ~240 ms which is the earliest deflection that is reliably sensitive to face repetition and may reflect emergence of familiarity through learning (Tanaka et al., 2006; Schweinberger et al., 2007; Zimmermann and Eimer, 2013). We examined spatio-temporal characteristics of the M240 and its activity profile as a function of face orientation, features, and outline. Given that our primary focus of interest was the M170 and the relatively early processing stages that are relevant to the stimulus manipulations, we wished to minimize the semantic aspects of the processing. To that end, we used faces that were unfamiliar to our participants and employed a task of passive viewing with short presentation intervals.

Methods

Subjects

MEG recordings and structural MRI scans were obtained from 14 healthy right-handed male subjects between 22 and 29 years of age (mean = 24.21 ± 1.85). The subjects had no neurological impairments and no structural brain abnormalities were seen on their MRI scans. All subjects signed statements of consent that were approved by the relevant review board and were monetarily reimbursed for their participation.

Material

Participants viewed six different types of grayscale photos (examples are shown in Figure 1) including: normal upright faces (N), inverted faces (I), normal face features presented in an oval without hairline (O), faces with features that were rearranged into unnatural positions (R), blank faces without features but with normal hairline (B), and randomized visual control stimuli (C). The control stimuli consisted of random grayscale patches that no longer looked like faces but that preserved the spatial frequency, luminance, and overall shape. In an effort to ascertain that image manipulations did not cause potentially confounding changes in visual properties, a 2D spatial FFT was calculated across images. The control stimuli did not differ from normal faces in the mean power at low, middle or high spatial frequency bands (<5, 5–15, or 15–40 cycles per degree of visual angle, respectively). The stimulus set was comprised of the photos of six different Caucasian individuals that were not familiar to any of our subjects. All faces had neutral expression and were selected from a larger set used in prior studies (Marinkovic and Halgren, 1998). The six photographs were manipulated to obtain images across all six conditions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Group-based average dynamic statistical parametric maps of estimated activity to all six conditions on ventral surfaces at ~107 ms, ~160 ms, and at ~240 ms, showing estimates in ventral and lateral views. Early visual activity (at ~107 ms) is stronger to inverted faces and control stimuli. Inverted faces evoked the strongest M160 activity estimated to the fusiform gyrus, followed by oval, normal and rearranged images. Blank faces and randomized control faces evoked the weakest activity. The subsequent deflection, peaking at ~240ms was largest to normal faces in the ventral and anterolateral temporal areas bilaterally. Examples of the images are shown below. The individual in the photos consented to the publication of these images.

Task

During the MEG recording session the subjects were instructed to passively observe images that were presented in a randomized order on a computer-driven back-projection screen in front of the subject. Each image was presented for 225 ms at 1 s intervals on a gray background within a visual angle subtending 4° horizontal × 6° vertical. Each stimulus was repeated 16 times, yielding a total of 96 stimuli per condition.

Data Acquisition and Analysis

MEG signals were recorded from 204 channels (102 pairs of planar gradiometers) with a whole-head Neuromag Vectorview instrument (Elekta Neuromag) in a magnetically and electrically shielded room. The signals were recorded continuously with 601 Hz sampling rate and minimal filtering (0.1–200 Hz). Averages for each stimulus type were constructed from trials free of eyeblinks or other occasional artifacts. On average, 8.5 ± 4.4% trials were discarded. The position of magnetic coils attached to the skull, the main fiduciary points such as the nose, nasion and preauricular points, as well as a large array of random points spread across the scalp were digitized with 3Space Isotrak II system for subsequent precise co-registration with structural MRI images.

Each person's cortical surface was reconstructed from high-resolution T1-weighted MRI structural images (1.5T Picker Eclipse, Marconi Medical, Cleveland OH) and was subsampled to ~2500 dipole locations per hemisphere (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a). This cortical surface served as the solution space to constrain a noise-normalized minimum norm inverse solution, here termed anatomically-constrained MEG or aMEG. The forward solution was calculated using a boundary element model (Oostendorp and Van Oosterom, 1991). Using a linear estimation minimum norm approach with no constraints on dipole orientation (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Hämäläinen and Ilmoniemi, 1994), dipole strength power was estimated at each cortical location every 5 ms. The estimates were normalized by noise obtained from the average pre-stimulus baseline which reduced the point-spread function variability (Liu et al., 2002a), and resulted in a series of frames of dynamic statistical parametric maps (dSPMs) of estimated cortical activity (Dale et al., 2000). These noise-normalized estimates of the current dipole power for each location fit the F distribution and can be viewed as “brain movies” as they unfold in time. Group averages for each condition were obtained by aligning cortical folding patterns across all individuals and averaging their inverse estimates (Fischl et al., 1999b; Dale et al., 2000). Figure 1 presents the group average dSPMs of the overall activity patterns evoked by each stimulus condition at 107, 160, and 240 ms after stimulus onset. Estimated cortical activity is displayed on inflated views of an averaged cortical surface.

Whereas the movie snapshots represent estimated activity for the whole cortical surface at each time point, an alternative way of examining the data is to look at the timecourses (estimated noise-normalized dipole strength across time) for the selected regions of interest (ROIs). These waveforms represent estimated dipole strength moments in the cortical source space and are suitable for assessing the effects of stimulus conditions on both amplitude and latency (Marinkovic et al., 2003). In order to further explore activity timecourses and to ascertain statistical significance of the particular comparisons, ROIs were chosen for the relevant areas on the cortical surface based on the overall group average estimated activity. They included the posterior occipital cortex (Occ), the lateral FG, and ventrolateral anterior temporal cortex (aTL). The same group-based ROIs were used for all subjects in a manner blind to their individual activations by means of an automatic spherical morphing procedure (Fischl et al., 1999b). The ROIs contained 4.8 ± 2.3 vertices on average, corresponding to ~2.7 cm2 of the cortical surface. The noise-normalized dipole strength estimates were averaged across all cortical points contained in each ROI at each time point. These values obtained for each subject and task condition were used for the statistical analysis. Within-subject ANOVAs were employed to examine differences in activity among conditions at different latencies. In most cases it was possible to determine singular amplitude peaks within the three latency windows of interest. For the occipital activity peaking at ~107 ms (M107), peak amplitudes were detected within a 90–125 ms time window for each subject and task condition with an automatic algorithm. This made it possible to also examine task condition effects on peak latencies. Similarly, peak amplitude of the M160 in the right FG was identified within 130–190 ms time window for each subject and task condition. Activity in the left hemisphere at this latency was weaker and less consistent across subjects, making it difficult to detect amplitude peaks. Instead, average amplitudes were used to examine task condition effects on the activity within the 120–150 and 170–190 ms latency windows in the left FG. Within-subject ANOVA (Woodward et al., 1990) was used to statistically compare differences across conditions for each ROI and each of the three deflections. The Bonferroni method (Woodward et al., 1990) was used as a conservative protection against inflated p-values due to multiple comparisons and the adjusted p-values are reported unless specified differently.

Results

Inspection of the overall activity indicates that the earliest activity is estimated to the occipital region at ~107 ms (M107) after stimulus onset. It propagates anteriorly via the ventral visual stream to the predominantly right ventral temporal areas peaking at ~160 ms (M160), and further on to the anterior ventrolateral temporal and prefrontal regions at ~240 ms (M240). Group-average dSPM estimates are shown in Figure 1 for the activity at 107, 160, and 240 ms. Timecourses derived from the relevant ROIs are shown in Figure 2, and graphs of mean estimated activity across all conditions in Figure 3. Table 1 summarizes main results across the ROIs and peak latencies.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Group-based average time courses of the estimated noise-normalized dipole strengths to all six conditions in selected cortical locations. The earliest activity was estimated to the occipital region at ~107 ms after stimulus onset and was strongest to inverted and control images. At ~160 ms, inverted faces elicited the strongest activity in the right-lateralized ventral temporal area, centered on the fusiform gyrus. Canonically oriented stimuli with inner features irrespective of their arrangement elicited identical activity at ~160 ms on the right. Inverted faces additionally elicited the immediately subsequent deflection at ~180 ms on the left. The M240 was largest to normal, upright faces in the anterior temporal areas bilaterally, possibly reflecting the initial engagement of the network subserving individuation, acquired familiarity, and recognition.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Upper panel: group average noise-normalized dipole strengths expressed as dSPM F-values for the ROIs in the fusiform cortex bilaterally and in the left anterior temporal area representing the successive processing stages: no activity difference in the left fusiform 120–150 ms across conditions; strongest activity to inverted faces in the right fusiform (140–170 ms) and in the left anterior temporal area (160–190 ms). Lower panel: noise-normalized peak amplitude dipole strength estimates in the anterior temporal areas bilaterally within 220–270 ms. Normal, upright faces elicit the strongest activity in both hemispheres, possibly reflecting acquired familiarity processing.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. ANOVA results for the main effects and condition contrasts carried out for M107, M160, and M240 response amplitudes and peak latencies.

The early occipital response peaks at 107 ms with a very similar amplitude and profile in both hemispheres. This observation was confirmed with an ANOVA of the peak amplitude (within 90–125 ms timewindow) with the factors of hemisphere and condition type. There was no main effect of laterality [F(1, 13) = 0.29, p > 0.5] and no laterality x condition interaction [F(5, 65) = 0.99, p > 0.45], so the results were pooled across both hemispheres. The main effect of Condition [F(5, 65) = 8.0, p < 0.0001] results from a greater peak amplitude to inverted faces and control stimuli [F(1, 13) = 12.9, p < 0.05] as compared to all other stimuli. The peak latency (107 ms) does not differ between the two hemispheres, F(1, 13) = 0.61 p > 0.45), but the peak latency to inverted faces (111 ms) is longer than the latency to all other stimuli (106 ms), F(1, 13) = 11.1, p < 0.05.

The subsequent deflection (M160) is right-dominant and is estimated to the fusiform cortex (Figure 1). ANOVA of the peak amplitude (within 130–190 ms time window) indicates that the right M160 is uniquely sensitive to condition differences as shown by the significant main effect, F(5, 65) = 8.4, p < 0.0001. Inverted faces evoke the greatest activity amplitude than all other stimuli, F(1, 13) = 14.8, p < 0.01, followed by other stimuli that include facial features such as the oval, normal, and rearranged faces (Figures 13). Activity to normal, upright faces does not differ from the activity to faces with rearranged features, or to normal features presented in an oval. That is, the canonically oriented stimuli containing inner features regardless of their arrangement elicit activity that appears to be very similar at ~160 ms latency. Blank facial outlines with no features elicit the weakest activity, F(1, 13) = 21.5, p < 0.01.

At around this latency, activity estimated to the left FG is much weaker overall (Figures 1, 2). Since the peak patterns at this latency in the left hemisphere are not consistent or always clearly distinguishable across subjects, the condition effects are examined by averaging response amplitudes within the specified latency windows. The first average deflection peaking at 140 ms (average amplitude within 120–150 ms) is not differentiated by any of the stimulus characteristics, as indicated by the lack of main effect, F(1, 13) = 0.1, ns (Figures 2, 3). However, the main effect of the deflection peaking at 180 ms (average amplitude within 170–190 ms latency window), F(5, 65) = 2.5, p < 0.05, reflected its sensitivity to inversion. This deflection tends to be greater to inverted than all other stimuli, F(1, 13) = 4.0, p < 0.07 (uncorrected). A similar pattern but with a more robust effect of inversion is observed in the left aTL at this latency (Figure 2), with a significant main effect of condition, F(5, 65) = 9.6, p < 0.001. In the aTL, inverted faces elicit greater activity than all other stimuli at ~180 ms, F(1, 13) = 22.5, p < 0.001. Therefore, inverted faces selectively engage the left ventral temporal cortex with slightly longer peak latency than the right-dominant fusiform area.

The M160 is followed by another peak at ~240 ms (M240) after stimulus onset (Figures 13). The strongest M240 is elicited by normal faces, especially along the ventral stream, including the left FG and aTL bilaterally. ANOVA of the peak amplitudes within 210–250 ms latency in the left FG revealed a main effect of condition, F(5, 65) = 2.5, p < 0.05, with a tendency for normal faces eliciting greater activity than all other stimuli, F(1, 13) = 8.4, p < 0.07. In the left anterior ventrolateral temporal cortex, the activity to normal faces was also stronger than to all other stimuli overall, F(1, 13) = 11.4, p < 0.05, although it did not differ from the stimuli with features presented within the oval. The peak latency (239 ± 16 ms) did not differ across conditions with the exception of a longer peak latency trend for the inverted faces (246 ms), F(1, 13) = 7.9, p < 0.09. Finally, as on the left, the activity to normal faces in the right aTL was greater than to other stimuli within 220–270 ms time window, F(1, 13) = 9.8, p < 0.05. The peak latency (255 ± 20 ms) was longer on the right than on the left, F(1, 13) = 36.1, p < 0.001.

Discussion

Our results support models proposing that face processing unfolds in successive, but overlapping and mutually dependent spatio-temporal stages in the ventral visual stream. The incoming face stimuli are analyzed for their visual characteristics at ~100 ms in the occipital visual areas as indexed by M107. Structural encoding of the face-specific aspects takes place in the FG at ~160 ms (M160) especially on the right, with the exception of the inverted faces that additionally activate anteroventral temporal cortex on the left. Subsequent, presumably more integrative processing, engages distributed inferoventral and anterolateral temporal areas at ~240 ms (M240) bilaterally. These latencies of face-related activity peaks have been observed in other MEG studies (Schweinberger et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2011) and confirmed with iEEG (Barbeau et al., 2008), lending further support to similar stages proposed by other models (Bruce and Young, 1986; Halgren et al., 1994a; Haxby et al., 2000).

M107—Sensitivity to Low-Level Visual Features

In the present study, the initial activity peak (M107) in the occipital area is greater to inverted and randomized control faces in comparison to other stimulus categories. Other ERP and MEG studies have also reported larger peak at ~100 ms to inverted faces (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 1998; Itier and Taylor, 2002, 2004a; Schweinberger et al., 2007; Meeren et al., 2008) and to randomized control faces (Halgren et al., 2000) in comparison to normal faces. Based on such findings, it has been proposed that stimulus categorization takes place at ~100 ms based on holistic perception of a face (Liu et al., 2002b; Itier and Taylor, 2004b). However, other evidence suggests that the activity differences may be merely due to low-level visual differences. MEG studies indicate that the mid-occipital M100 amplitude is increased as a function of parametrically varied pixel noise (Tarkiainen et al., 2002) and spatial frequency (Tanskanen et al., 2005). Similarly, the fMRI-BOLD signal is larger to visually randomized faces in retinotopic areas (Lerner et al., 2001). This evidence is consistent with the idea that the observed categorical differentiation at ~100 ms is based on low-level visual characteristics rather than a holistic percept (Rossion and Caharel, 2011; Cauchoix et al., 2014). Nevertheless, this deflection may represent an initial step in the face-sensitive analysis of the global visual characteristics with the purpose of tuning and facilitating subsequent processing (Halgren et al., 1994a; Itier and Taylor, 2004a). All of our stimuli belong to the face-like category, but those that deviate more from a global face template based on their shape (inverted faces) or texture and contour (randomized control stimuli) evoke the strongest M107 activity in the occipital area (Figure 2). Based on its sensitivity to low-level features, this initial stage may serve as a domain-specific gate, “flagging” stimuli that deviate in orientation or shape (Portin et al., 1999; Tsao and Livingstone, 2008) and allowing for a fast visual categorization (Crouzet and Thorpe, 2011). This stage may facilitate subsequent structural encoding stage which is represented in the FG at 160 ms, carrying out further refinement (Rossion and Caharel, 2011).

M160—Global Face Encoding

This stage is reflected in a strongly right-lateralized M160 deflection which was greatest to inverted faces. All other face-like stimuli (normal, oval, and rearranged) evoked similar, intermediate-level activity in the fusiform cortex, whereas blank and randomized control faces evoked the weakest activity (Figure 2). This suggests that the face representation formed at this stage is based on a roughly face-like template that contains basic visual elements of a face: oval-shaped contour in an upright position with contrasting facial features regardless of whether they are spaced appropriately. Although the M160 representation lacks precision allowing for individuation at this stage, the stimuli that were most face-like evoked stronger activity than the blank faces and control stimuli which carry very little visual information needed for subsequent recognition. Our data are consistent with previous suggestions that this deflection reflects the operation of a face-encoding processing stage (Halgren et al., 1994a; Bentin et al., 1996; Puce et al., 1999; Eimer, 2000b; Downing et al., 2001; Bentin and Carmel, 2002), akin to the structural encoding (“face detection”) module originally proposed by Bruce and Young (1986). In contrast to M107 which is sensitive to gross visual characteristics, the M160 deflection (presumably analogous to N170 in the ERP literature) is larger to stimuli that broadly resemble faces and can be processed further for familiarity. Consistent with other evidence, the M160 is responsive to the presence of facial features in the veridical or rearranged configuration irrespective of the facial outline (Bentin et al., 1996; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007). The M160 is attenuated to blank faces that lack internal features and to randomized control stimuli, confirming other similar findings at this latency in the FG (Eimer, 2000b; Tong et al., 2000). The finding that the right-lateralized M160 is similar in amplitude to stimuli containing inner features irrespective of their configuration could represent a process broadly generalizable to other types of visual stimuli such as words. For instance, ventral temporal cortex on the left is comparably activated by real and pseudowords, but not by other control stimuli (Cohen et al., 2002). In other words, the presence of the requisite features even if they are in unnatural locations may be necessary and sufficient for initial acceptance of a stimulus as possibly representing a face. This aspect of the face processor may be useful in situation when faces are seen in non-habitual orientations (for example, when the observed face is on a person lying on her side) and/or when much of the face is obscured by a hat or hair).

The N170 is largely insensitive to familiarity or repetition and consequently unresponsive to individuation (Marinkovic and Halgren, 1998; Puce et al., 1999; Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Eimer, 2000a; Anaki et al., 2007; Schweinberger et al., 2007; Barbeau et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2011; Rivolta et al., 2012), providing additional evidence for its role in global face encoding (Bentin et al., 1996). In contrast, the process of individuation and recognition is subserved at the subsequent stage at ~240 ms, located downstream in temporal cortices bilaterally. During the M160, the face-like features may be extracted by a domain-specific mechanism, permitting formation of a unitary and holistic representation of a face (Tanaka and Farah, 1993; Bentin and Golland, 2002; Schiltz and Rossion, 2006; Jacques and Rossion, 2009). This representation may be projected to distributed association cortices for further mnemonic, semantic, and emotional processing, resulting in the integration of the recognition process, as suggested by face-selective broadband coherence in intracranial EEG between the fusiform and distributed cortical areas (Klopp et al., 2000). The M160 was estimated to the right-dominant ventral temporal area, in the FG. Indeed, intracranial recordings confirm that the primary generators of the N170 deflection are in the fusiform area (Allison et al., 1994; Halgren et al., 1994a; McCarthy et al., 1997; Puce et al., 1997; Barbeau et al., 2008), in agreement with neuroimaging evidence (Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006).

Face Inversion Engages Dual-Route Processing

The M160 in the right fusiform cortex to inverted faces had a larger amplitude and longer peak latency than all other stimuli, replicating results of numerous other ERP and MEG studies (Bentin et al., 1996; Eimer, 2000a; Rossion et al., 1999, 2000; Liu et al., 2000; Sagiv and Bentin, 2001; Itier and Taylor, 2002, 2004a; Watanabe et al., 2003; Kloth et al., 2006; Honda et al., 2007). In the left ventral temporal cortex, the immediately preceding deflection peaked at ~140 ms and was insensitive to any manipulation (Figures 2, 3). However, the immediately subsequent deflection peaking at ~180 ms on the left was selectively elicited by inverted faces (Figure 2) in a manner similar to the right M160. Clearly, the M160 is not maximal to optimal stimuli (i.e., normal, upright faces) but to inverted stimuli that deviate from the canonical orientation. At this point, the inverted faces have been classified as faces and need to engage additional resources to continue being processed for recognition. Even though at this latency the overall activity is much weaker in the LH overall, the deflection at 180 ms is elicited selectively by inverted faces. This indicates that they may uniquely engage bilateral ventral temporal cortices, supporting a dual route model (Moscovitch et al., 1997; De Gelder and Rouw, 2001; Rhodes et al., 2004), as well as the related idea that inverted faces recruit other mechanisms in addition to the right fusiform region (Aguirre et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 1999; Rossion et al., 2000; Yovel and Kanwisher, 2005; Epstein et al., 2006; Rossion, 2009). Despite a clear RH dominance in face processing, some evidence suggests that the LH contributes significantly to processing inverted faces. Behavioral studies using divided visual field methodology show the RH advantage in discriminating upright, but not inverted faces (Hillger and Koenig, 1991; Cattaneo et al., 2013), indicating left hemisphere engagement during processing of inverted faces. Similarly, split-brain monkeys show the face inversion effect when the stimuli are presented to the RH, but not to the LH (Vermeire and Hamilton, 1998). The face recognition deficit in prosopagnosic patients is more pronounced with bilateral lesions (Barton, 2008), possibly resulting from a disruption in interhemispheric communication which is critical for integrated perceptual decisions. Furthermore, relatively spared processing of inverted faces in prosopagnosia (Farah, 1996; De Gelder and Rouw, 2001) could be explained by a model of bilateral engagement of a more general system for visual objects (Aguirre et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 1999). Finally, MEG studies (Dobel et al., 2008, 2011) reported that individuals with congenital prosopagnosia manifested a decreased M170 and a strongly reduced gamma power in the left fusiform cortex, confirming left hemisphere involvement in normal face processing. This observation is confirmed by an fMRI study showing decreased activation in the left FG in congenital prosopagnosic patients (Dinkelacker et al., 2011). Therefore, it appears that by disturbing canonical face processing, face inversion creates suboptimal conditions for face recognition (Rossion, 2008), resulting in bilateral engagement of the ventral visual stream. This effect is not unique inasmuch as the N170 is similarly augmented to contrast inversion and misaligned face halves (Itier and Taylor, 2002; Letourneau and Mitchell, 2008; Jacques and Rossion, 2010) which may also rely on additional visual processing mechanisms. Furthermore, engagement of additional resources in the non-dominant hemisphere by visually deviating stimuli may be a more general principle generalizing beyond faces. For instance, even though left-dominance of language processing has been firmly established (Price, 2010), the right hemisphere is selectively engaged by unpronounceable non-words (Marinkovic et al., 2014). Similarly, the right ventral occipitotemporal cortex is more strongly activated by words in the less fluent language in bilingual speakers (Leonard et al., 2010).

M240—Emergence of Familiarity via Repetition

Extensive imaging evidence obtained with hemodynamic methods has been commonly interpreted in the context of dedicated face-processing modules particularly in the fusiform area (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006). However, spatio-temporally sensitive methods impose the idea of distributed and partly sequential processing encompassing mutually dependent and overlapping areas whereby the face-relevant information is increasingly refined in the posterior-to-anterior direction, reaching identity/semantic networks in the anterior temporal and inferior prefrontal cortices (Halgren et al., 1994a,b, 2000; Puce et al., 1999; Barbeau et al., 2008). Faces are processed by the ventral processing stream similar to other visual stimuli. Subsequent to an early engagement of the striate cortex (M107), ventral occipito-temporal areas support an intermediate material-specific processing stage (M160) providing structural representations to downstream distributed associative areas for processing of identity and emotional expression (Bruce and Young, 1986; Klopp et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2002b). In contrast to M107 and M160 that were larger to inverted faces, the normal, upright faces evoked the largest M240 estimated to the ventral and anterior temporal areas bilaterally, in agreement with other MEG reports (Schweinberger et al., 2007). The M240 deflection engages distributed anterior temporal cortices and may index familiarity detection and recognition, supporting previous iEEG findings (Barbeau et al., 2008). Furthermore, recent evidence shows that the (presumably analogous) N250 is sensitive not only to familiarity (Caharel et al., 2014), but that it emerges to previously unfamiliar faces as a result of repetition and, consequently, familiarization (Tanaka et al., 2006; Schweinberger et al., 2007; Pierce et al., 2011; Zimmermann and Eimer, 2013). Even though we did not manipulate repetition in a condition-specific manner, the present results are consistent with the idea that this deflection may reflect access to recognition units and activation of a memory trace for the particular face that has become familiar with repetition (Zimmermann and Eimer, 2013). Our localization estimates and the observation of the sensitivity of the inferior and anterior temporal cortices to face orientation and identity are supported by fMRI studies (Sugiura et al., 2001; Rotshtein et al., 2005; Kriegeskorte et al., 2007; Nasr and Tootell, 2012; O'Neil et al., 2013) and are further confirmed with single cell recordings in non-human primates (Freiwald and Tsao, 2010). Similarly, lesion studies report that anterior temporal lesions result in face recognition impairments (Glosser et al., 2003; Barton, 2008; Gainotti and Marra, 2011). Thus, it appears that familiarity detection stage depends on the anterior temporal structures, and possibly specifically perirhinal cortex (Allison et al., 1994; Halgren et al., 1994a; Henson et al., 2003).

Even though the estimated M240 sources in our study are bilaterally distributed, the overall activity is left-dominant. It is generally accepted that the left hemisphere is essential for semantic domain especially in language tasks whereas the right hemisphere subserves face processing (Dien, 2009). Right hemisphere bias for faces has been widely reported and accepted (De Renzi et al., 1994; Kanwisher et al., 1997). However, even during face processing left hemisphere may play a dominant role in storage and retrieval of semantic face attributions as indicated by lesion (Glosser et al., 2003; Snowden et al., 2004) and imaging evidence (Griffith et al., 2006). Given that in our study only photographs of previously unknown faces were used, the connection with semantic system is speculative. Nevertheless, an increase in M240 resulting from repeated exposure to upright, normal faces may partially stem from initial engagement of the network supporting person-specific information (Gainotti and Marra, 2011; Zimmermann and Eimer, 2013). These semantic face attributions may be represented in the left hemisphere as is the case with left-lateralized N360 to famous faces (Barbeau et al., 2008). Baron and Osherson (2011) used face stimuli in a visual categorization task and showed that the left anterior temporal lobe was especially sensitive to combinatorial face categorization. Importance of the left hemisphere is supported by reports of prosopagnosia resulting from ventral lesions in the left hemisphere (Verstichel and Chia, 1999; Vuilleumier et al., 2003). Furthermore, Dinkelacker et al. (2011) showed decreased fMRI activation in the left FG in congenital prosopagnosic individuals. Similarly, a MEG study found weaker activity overall in the left occipitotemporal areas in congenital prosopagnosic patients (Dobel et al., 2008). Nevertheless, the overwhelming evidence suggests that face processing depends on distributed bilateral contributions (Farah, 1990; Haxby et al., 2000; Verosky and Turk-Browne, 2012) even in the case of emotional face processing (Fusar-Poli et al., 2009).

The anterolateral and ventral temporal regions may be essential for bringing together the configural representation of the face stimuli with the identity-relevant representations as part of a distributed network (Avidan et al., 2013; O'Neil et al., 2014). iEEG recordings show coherence between the FG and distributed association areas at ~200 ms to faces (Klopp et al., 2000) and functional connectivity studies support this finding (O'Neil et al., 2014). This transitional entrainment may represent a widespread projection for further processing. The M240 may thus represent the familiarity detection stage as an initial step in accessing the person identity/semantic system that exists for the famous faces or personal acquaintances, followed by the full-fledged recognition percept laden with emotional, mnemonic, and other associations (Halgren et al., 1994a). Since the participants in our experiment were engaged in passive viewing of unfamiliar faces and were not asked to make any explicit judgments, we interpret the M240 as an index of familiarity with caution. Nevertheless, people excel at making attributions about unfamiliar faces such as age, gender, attractiveness, intelligence, etc. (Bruce and Young, 1986) and the M240 may index a familiarity detection stage within a generic face processing stream.

Conclusion

Faces are highly relevant visual objects engaging a multi-stage cascade of mutually dependent and overlapping distributed activity in the ventral visual stream with flexible downstream allocation. An initial analysis of the low-level visual characteristics takes place in the occipital region at ~100 ms. Its sensitivity to low-level visual features and deviation in orientation or shape and texture may facilitate fast initial categorization. The subsequent activity of the predominantly right ventral temporal area (centered on the posterior FG) at ~160 ms may index the face detection stage by subserving structural encoding necessary for downstream individuation and recognition. Additional engagement of the left ventral temporal area at ~180 ms by inverted faces is consistent with the dual route model and spared processing of inverted faces in prosopagnosia. The M240 may index engagement of the familiarity processing network in bilateral, distributed anteroventral temporal areas. Thus, our data support dynamic models of face processing that suggest that face perception is subserved by a distributed and interactive neural circuit (Bruce and Young, 1986; Halgren et al., 1994a,b; Puce et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 2000; Klopp et al., 2000; De Gelder and Rouw, 2001; Rossion et al., 2003b; Ishai et al., 2005; Barbeau et al., 2008; Nasr and Tootell, 2012; Cauchoix et al., 2014).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Rupali P. Dhond, Sharelle Baldwin, Jeremy Jordin, Dave Post, Brendan Cox, Kim Paulson, Jeffrey Lewine, and Bruce Fischl. This work was supported by NIH (NS18741, AA016624, RR14075, P01 HD033113).

References

Aguirre, G. K., Singh, R., and D'esposito, M. (1999). Stimulus inversion and the responses of face and object-sensitive cortical areas. Neuroreport 10, 189–194. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199901180-00036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, T., Ginter, H., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Puce, A., Luby, M., et al. (1994). Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 821–825.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Anaki, D., Zion-Golumbic, E., and Bentin, S. (2007). Electrophysiological neural mechanisms for detection, configural analysis and recognition of faces. Neuroimage 37, 1407–1416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avidan, G., Tanzer, M., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Liu, N., Ungerleider, L. G., and Behrmann, M. (2013). Selective dissociation between core and extended regions of the face processing network in congenital prosopagnosia. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1565–1578. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbeau, E. J., Taylor, M. J., Regis, J., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P., and Liegeois-Chauvel, C. (2008). Spatio temporal dynamics of face recognition. Cereb. Cortex 18, 997–1009. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, S. G., and Osherson, D. (2011). Evidence for conceptual combination in the left anterior temporal lobe. Neuroimage 55, 1847–1852. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.066

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, J. J. (2008). Structure and function in acquired prosopagnosia: lessons from a series of 10 patients with brain damage. J. Neuropsychol. 2, 197–225. doi: 10.1348/174866407X214172

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrmann, M., and Plaut, D. C. (2013). Distributed circuits, not circumscribed centers, mediate visual recognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 210–219. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., and Carmel, D. (2002). Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier. Cognition 85, 197–202. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00102-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., and Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 35–54. doi: 10.1080/026432900380472

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., and Golland, Y. (2002). Meaningful processing of meaningless stimuli: the influence of perceptual experience on early visual processing of faces. Cognition 86, B1–B14. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00124-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, V., and Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 77(Pt 3), 305–327. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caharel, S., Ramon, M., and Rossion, B. (2014). Face familiarity decisions take 200 msec in the human brain: electrophysiological evidence from a go/no-go speeded task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 81–95. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00451

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattaneo, Z., Renzi, C., Bona, S., Merabet, L. B., Carbon, C. C., and Vecchi, T. (2013). Hemispheric asymmetry in discriminating faces differing for featural or configural (second-order relations) aspects. Psychon. Bull. Rev 21, 363–369. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0484-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cauchoix, M., Barragan-Jason, G., Serre, T., and Barbeau, E. J. (2014). The neural dynamics of face detection in the wild revealed by MVPA. J. Neurosci. 34, 846–854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3030-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A. W., and Downing, P. E. (2011). Faces and eyes in human lateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:51. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, L., Lehericy, S., Chochon, F., Lemer, C., Rivaud, S., and Dehaene, S. (2002). Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain 125, 1054–1069. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf094

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowell, R. A., and Cottrell, G. W. (2013). What evidence supports special processing for faces? A cautionary tale for fMRI interpretation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1777–1793. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00448

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crouzet, S. M., and Thorpe, S. J. (2011). Low-level cues and ultra-fast face detection. Front. Psychol. 2:342. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00342

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D., et al. (2000). Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81138-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., and Sereno, M. I. (1993). Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Gelder, B., and Rouw, R. (2001). Beyond localisation: a dynamical dual route account of face recognition. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 107, 183–207. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00024-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., Perani, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Silveri, M. C., and Fazio, F. (1994). Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined to the right hemisphere–an MRI and PET study and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia 32, 893–902. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90041-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dien, J. (2009). A tale of two recognition systems: implications of the fusiform face area and the visual word form area for lateralized object recognition models. Neuropsychologia 47, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinkelacker, V., Gruter, M., Klaver, P., Gruter, T., Specht, K., Weis, S., et al. (2011). Congenital prosopagnosia: multistage anatomical and functional deficits in face processing circuitry. J. Neurol. 258, 770–782. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5828-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobel, C., Junghofer, M., and Gruber, T. (2011). The role of gamma-band activity in the representation of faces: reduced activity in the fusiform face area in congenital prosopagnosia. PLoS ONE 6:e19550. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019550

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobel, C., Putsche, C., Zwitserlood, P., and Junghofer, M. (2008). Early left-hemispheric dysfunction of face processing in congenital prosopagnosia: an MEG study. PLoS ONE 3:e2326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002326

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downing, P., Liu, J., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). Testing cognitive models of visual attention with fMRI and MEG. Neuropsychologia 39, 1329–1342. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00121-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (2000a). Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 145–158. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00038-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (2000b). The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. Neuroreport 11, 2319–2324. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200007140-00050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (2011). “The face-sensitive N170 component of the event-related brain potential,” in The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception, eds A. J. Calder, G. Rhodes, M. Johnson, and J. V. Haxby (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 329–344.

Epstein, R. A., Higgins, J. S., Parker, W., Aguirre, G. K., and Cooperman, S. (2006). Cortical correlates of face and scene inversion: a comparison. Neuropsychologia 44, 1145–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us About Normal Vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

Farah, M. J. (1996). Is face recognition ‘special’? Evidence from neuropsychology. Behav. Brain Res. 76, 181–189. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00198-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999a). Cortical surface-based analysis. II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B., and Dale, A. M. (1999b). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Freiwald, W. A., and Tsao, D. Y. (2010). Functional compartmentalization and viewpoint generalization within the macaque face-processing system. Science 330, 845–851. doi: 10.1126/science.1194908

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusar-Poli, P., Placentino, A., Carletti, F., Allen, P., Landi, P., Abbamonte, M., et al. (2009). Laterality effect on emotional faces processing: ALE meta-analysis of evidence. Neurosci. Lett. 452, 262–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.01.065

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainotti, G., and Marra, C. (2011). Differential contribution of right and left temporo-occipital and anterior temporal lesions to face recognition disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00055

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Goldstein, A., Harpaz, Y., Hansel, M., Zion-Golumbic, E., and Bentin, S. (2013). A magnetoencephalographic study of face processing: M170, gamma-band oscillations and source localization. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1783–1795. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Anderson, A. W., Skudlarski, P., and Gore, J. C. (1999). Activation of the middle fusiform ‘face area’ increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 568–573. doi: 10.1038/9224

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glosser, G., Salvucci, A. E., and Chiaravalloti, N. D. (2003). Naming and recognizing famous faces in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 61, 81–86. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000073621.18013.E1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffith, H. R., Richardson, E., Pyzalski, R. W., Bell, B., Dow, C., Hermann, B. P., et al. (2006). Memory for famous faces and the temporal pole: functional imaging findings in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 9, 173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2006.04.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halgren, E., Baudena, P., Heit, G., Clarke, J. M., and Marinkovic, K. (1994a). Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. I. Depth-recorded potentials in the human occipital, temporal and parietal lobes [corrected]. J. Physiol. Paris 88, 1–50. doi: 10.1016/0928-4257(94)90092-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halgren, E., Baudena, P., Heit, G., Clarke, J. M., Marinkovic, K., and Chauvel, P. (1994b). Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 2. Depth-recorded potentials in the human frontal and Rolandic cortices. J. Physiol. Paris 88, 51–80. doi: 10.1016/0928-4257(94)90093-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halgren, E., Raij, T., Marinkovic, K., Jousmaki, V., and Hari, R. (2000). Cognitive response profile of the human fusiform face area as determined by MEG. Cereb. Cortex 10, 69–81. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.1.69

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, M. S., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1994). Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 32, 35–42. doi: 10.1007/BF02512476

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, A., and Nakayama, K. (2008). Rapid adaptation of the m170 response: importance of face parts. Cereb. Cortex 18, 467–476. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V. (2006). Fine structure in representations of faces and objects. Nat Neurosci 9, 1084–1086. doi: 10.1038/nn0906-1084

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., and Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293, 2425–2430. doi: 10.1126/science.1063736

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 59–67. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01330-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Clark, V. P., Schouten, J. L., Hoffman, E. A., and Martin, A. (1999). The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80690-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henson, R. N., Cansino, S., Herron, J. E., Robb, W. G., and Rugg, M. D. (2003). A familiarity signal in human anterior medial temporal cortex? Hippocampus 13, 301–304. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillger, L. A., and Koenig, O. (1991). Separable mechanisms in face processing: evidence from hemispheric specialization. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.1.42

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honda, Y., Watanabe, S., Nakamura, M., Miki, K., and Kakigi, R. (2007). Interhemispheric difference for upright and inverted face perception in humans: an event-related potential study. Brain Topogr. 20, 31–39. doi: 10.1007/s10548-007-0028-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A. (2008). Let's face it: it's a cortical network. Neuroimage 40, 415–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Pessoa, L., Bikle, P. C., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2004). Repetition suppression of faces is modulated by emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 9827–9832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403559101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Schmidt, C. F., and Boesiger, P. (2005). Face perception is mediated by a distributed cortical network. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.05.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itier, R. J., and Taylor, M. J. (2002). Inversion and contrast polarity reversal affect both encoding and recognition processes of unfamiliar faces: a repetition study using ERPs. Neuroimage 15, 353–372. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0982

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itier, R. J., and Taylor, M. J. (2004a). Effects of repetition learning on upright, inverted and contrast-reversed face processing using ERPs. Neuroimage 21, 1518–1532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itier, R. J., and Taylor, M. J. (2004b). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb. Cortex 14, 132–142. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacques, C., and Rossion, B. (2009). The initial representation of individual faces in the right occipito-temporal cortex is holistic: electrophysiological evidence from the composite face illusion. J. Vis. 9, 8.1–8.16. doi: 10.1167/9.6.8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacques, C., and Rossion, B. (2010). Misaligning face halves increases and delays the N170 specifically for upright faces: implications for the nature of early face representations. Brain Res. 1318, 96–109. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., Tong, F., and Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition 68, B1–B11. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00035-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., and Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klopp, J., Halgren, E., Marinkovic, K., and Nenov, V. (1999). Face-selective spectral changes in the human fusiform gyrus. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 676–682. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(98)00039-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klopp, J., Marinkovic, K., Chauvel, P., Nenov, V., and Halgren, E. (2000). Early widespread cortical distribution of coherent fusiform face selective activity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 286–293. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200012)11:4<286::AID-HBM80>3.0.CO;2-R

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kloth, N., Dobel, C., Schweinberger, S. R., Zwitserlood, P., Bolte, J., and Junghofer, M. (2006). Effects of personal familiarity on early neuromagnetic correlates of face perception. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 3317–3321. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05211.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriegeskorte, N., Formisano, E., Sorger, B., and Goebel, R. (2007). Individual faces elicit distinct response patterns in human anterior temporal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 20600–20605. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705654104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, M. K., Brown, T. T., Travis, K. E., Gharapetian, L., Hagler, D. J. Jr., Dale, A. M., et al. (2010). Spatiotemporal dynamics of bilingual word processing. Neuroimage 49, 3286–3294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, Y., Hendler, T., Ben-Bashat, D., Harel, M., and Malach, R. (2001). A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 287–297. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.4.287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Letourneau, S. M., and Mitchell, T. V. (2008). Behavioral and ERP measures of holistic face processing in a composite task. Brain Cogn. 67, 234–245. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.01.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., Palva, J. M., Sams, M., Hietanen, J. K., Aronen, H. J., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1998). Face-selective processing in human extrastriate cortex around 120 ms after stimulus onset revealed by magneto- and electroencephalography. Neurosci. Lett. 253, 147–150. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00586-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, A. K., Dale, A. M., and Belliveau, J. W. (2002a). Monte Carlo simulation studies of EEG and MEG localization accuracy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 16, 47–62. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2002b). Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 910–916. doi: 10.1038/nn909

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2010). Perception of face parts and face configurations: an FMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 203–211. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21203

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Higuchi, M., Marantz, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2000). The selectivity of the occipitotemporal M170 for faces. Neuroreport 11, 337–341. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200002070-00023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, S. T., Hamalainen, M. S., Hari, R., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Lounasmaa, O. V., Sams, M., et al. (1991). Seeing faces activates three separate areas outside the occipital visual cortex in man. Neuroscience 43, 287–290. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90293-W

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macchi Cassia, V., Kuefner, D., Westerlund, A., and Nelson, C. A. (2006). Modulation of face-sensitive event-related potentials by canonical and distorted human faces: the role of vertical symmetry and up-down featural arrangement. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1343–1358. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinkovic, K., Dhond, R. P., Dale, A. M., Glessner, M., Carr, V., and Halgren, E. (2003). Spatiotemporal dynamics of modality-specific and supramodal word processing. Neuron 38, 487–497. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00197-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinkovic, K., and Halgren, E. (1998). Human brain potentials related to the emotional expression, repetition, and gender of faces. Psychobiology 26, 348–356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinkovic, K., Rosen, B. Q., Cox, B., and Hagler, D. J. Jr. (2014). Spatio-temporal processing of words and nonwords: Hemispheric laterality and acute alcohol intoxication. Brain Res. 1558, 18–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.02.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinkovic, K., Trebon, P., Chauvel, P., and Halgren, E. (2000). Localised face processing by the human prefrontal cortex: face-selective intracerebral potentials and post-lesion deficits. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 187–199. doi: 10.1080/026432900380562

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., and Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 605–610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meeren, H. K., Hadjikhani, N., Ahlfors, S. P., Hamalainen, M. S., and De Gelder, B. (2008). Early category-specific cortical activation revealed by visual stimulus inversion. PLoS ONE 3:e3503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miki, K., Takeshima, Y., Watanabe, S., Honda, Y., and Kakigi, R. (2011). Effects of inverting contour and features on processing for static and dynamic face perception: an MEG study. Brain Res. 1383, 230–241. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., and Behrmann, M. (1997). What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 555–604. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasr, S., and Tootell, R. B. (2012). Role of fusiform and anterior temporal cortical areas in facial recognition. Neuroimage 63, 1743–1753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, D. F., Betts, L. R., and Wilson, H. R. (2010). Decoding of faces and face components in face-sensitive human visual cortex. Front. Psychol. 1:28. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Neil, E. B., Barkley, V. A., and Kohler, S. (2013). Representational demands modulate involvement of perirhinal cortex in face processing. Hippocampus 23, 592–605. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Neil, E. B., Hutchison, R. M., Mclean, D. A., and Kohler, S. (2014). Resting-state fMRI reveals functional connectivity between face-selective perirhinal cortex and the fusiform face area related to face inversion. Neuroimage 92, 349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostendorp, T., and Van Oosterom, A. (1991). The potential distribution generated by surface electrodes in inhomogeneous volume conductors of arbitrary shape. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 38, 409–417. doi: 10.1109/10.81559

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, L. J., Scott, L. S., Boddington, S., Droucker, D., Curran, T., and Tanaka, J. W. (2011). The n250 brain potential to personally familiar and newly learned faces and objects. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:111. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portin, K., Vanni, S., Virsu, V., and Hari, R. (1999). Stronger occipital cortical activation to lower than upper visual field stimuli. Neuromagnetic recordings. Exp. Brain Res. 124, 287–294. doi: 10.1007/s002210050625

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 62–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puce, A., Allison, T., and McCarthy, G. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. III: effects of top-down processing on face-specific potentials. Cereb. Cortex 9, 445–458. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.445

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puce, A., Allison, T., Spencer, S. S., Spencer, D. D., and McCarthy, G. (1997). Comparison of cortical activation evoked by faces measured by intracranial field potentials and functional MRI: two case studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 298–305.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Jaquet, E., Winkler, C., and Clifford, C. W. (2004). Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms. Curr. Biol. 14, 2119–2123. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivolta, D., Palermo, R., Schmalzl, L., and Williams, M. A. (2012). Investigating the features of the m170 in congenital prosopagnosia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:45. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 128, 274–289. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B. (2009). Distinguishing the cause and consequence of face inversion: the perceptual field hypothesis. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 132, 300–312. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.08.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Caharel, S. (2011). ERP evidence for the speed of face categorization in the human brain: disentangling the contribution of low-level visual cues from face perception. Vision Res. 51, 1297–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Delvenne, J. F., Debatisse, D., Goffaux, V., Bruyer, R., Crommelinck, M., et al. (1999). Spatio-temporal localization of the face inversion effect: an event- related potentials study. Biol. Psychol. 50, 173–189. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00013-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Despland, P., Bruyer, R., Linotte, S., et al. (2000). The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: an electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. Neuroreport 11, 69–74. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200001170-00014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Jacques, C. (2008). Does physical interstimulus variance account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the human brain? Ten lessons on the N170. Neuroimage 39, 1959–1979. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Jacques, C. (2011). “The N170: understanding the time-course of face pereception in the human brain,” in The Oxford Handbook of ERP Components, eds S. Luck and E. Kappenman (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 115–142.

Rossion, B., Joyce, C. A., Cottrell, G. W., and Tarr, M. J. (2003a). Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex. Neuroimage 20, 1609–1624. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., and Crommelinck, M. (2003b). The functionally defined right occipital and fusiform “face areas” discriminate novel from visually familiar faces. Neuroimage 19, 877–883. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00105-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotshtein, P., Henson, R. N., Treves, A., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2005). Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 107–113. doi: 10.1038/nn1370

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagiv, N., and Bentin, S. (2001). Structural encoding of human and schematic faces: holistic and part- based processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 937–951. doi: 10.1162/089892901753165854

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiltz, C., and Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex. Neuroimage 32, 1385–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweinberger, S. R., Kaufmann, J. M., Moratti, S., Keil, A., and Burton, A. M. (2007). Brain responses to repetitions of human and animal faces, inverted faces, and objects: an MEG study. Brain Res. 1184, 226–233. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., and Neary, D. (2004). Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain 127, 860–872. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh099

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiura, M., Kawashima, R., Nakamura, K., Sato, N., Nakamura, A., Kato, T., et al. (2001). Activation reduction in anterior temporal cortices during repeated recognition of faces of personal acquaintances. Neuroimage 13, 877–890. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0747

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, J. W., Curran, T., Porterfield, A. L., and Collins, D. (2006). Activation of preexisting and acquired face representations: the N250 event-related potential as an index of face familiarity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1488–1497. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1488

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, J. W., and Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 46, 225–245. doi: 10.1080/14640749308401045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanskanen, T., Nasanen, R., Montez, T., Paallysaho, J., and Hari, R. (2005). Face recognition and cortical responses show similar sensitivity to noise spatial frequency. Cereb. Cortex 15, 526–534. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarkiainen, A., Cornelissen, P. L., and Salmelin, R. (2002). Dynamics of visual feature analysis and object-level processing in face versus letter-string perception. Brain 125, 1125–1136. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M. J., Bayless, S. J., Mills, T., and Pang, E. W. (2011). Recognising upright and inverted faces: MEG source localisation. Brain Res. 1381, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch, M., Weinrib, O., and Kanwisher, N. (2000). Response properties of the human fusiform face area. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 257–280. doi: 10.1080/026432900380607

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., and Livingstone, M. S. (2008). Mechanisms of face perception. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 411–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 79, 471–491. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02747.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeire, B. A., and Hamilton, C. R. (1998). Inversion effect for faces in split-brain monkeys. Neuropsychologia 36, 1003–1014. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00054-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verosky, S. C., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2012). Representations of facial identity in the left hemisphere require right hemisphere processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1006–1017. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00196

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verstichel, P., and Chia, L. (1999). [Difficulties in face identification after lesion in the left hemisphere]. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 155, 937–943.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., Mohr, C., Valenza, N., Wetzel, C., and Landis, T. (2003). Hyperfamiliarity for unknown faces after left lateral temporo-occipital venous infarction: a double dissociation with prosopagnosia. Brain 126, 889–907. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg086

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, S., Kakigi, R., and Puce, A. (2003). The spatiotemporal dynamics of the face inversion effect: a magneto- and electro-encephalographic study. Neuroscience 116, 879–895. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00752-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, J. A., Bonett, D. G., and Brecht, M. L. (1990). Introduction to Linear Models and Experimental Design. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Google Scholar

Yovel, G., and Kanwisher, N. (2005). The neural basis of the behavioral face-inversion effect. Curr. Biol. 15, 2256–2262. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, F. G., and Eimer, M. (2013). Face learning and the emergence of view-independent face recognition: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia 51, 1320–1329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zion-Golumbic, E., and Bentin, S. (2007). Dissociated neural mechanisms for face detection and configural encoding: evidence from N170 and induced gamma-band oscillation effects. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1741–1749. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: magnetoencephalography, faces, fusiform gyrus, temporal cortex, laterality, dual route model, face inversion

Citation: Marinkovic K, Courtney MG, Witzel T, Dale AM and Halgren E (2014) Spatio-temporal dynamics and laterality effects of face inversion, feature presence and configuration, and face outline. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:868. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00868

Received: 30 April 2014; Accepted: 08 October 2014;
Published online: 10 November 2014.

Edited by:

Mark A. Williams, Macquarie University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Christian Dobel, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Germany
Emmanuel J. Barbeau, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, France

Copyright © 2014 Marinkovic, Courtney, Witzel, Dale and Halgren. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ksenija Marinkovic, Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, 5500 Campanile Dr. San Diego, CA 92182–4611, USA e-mail: kmarinkovic@mail.sdsu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.