- 1Centre for Research on Ageing Health and Wellbeing, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
- 2Flinders Centre for Ageing Studies, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
Background: The association between dual sensory loss (DSL) and mental health has been well established. However, most studies have relied on self-report data and lacked measures that would enable researchers to examine causal pathways between DSL and depression. This study seeks to extend this research by examining the effects of DSL on mental health, and identify factors that explain the longitudinal associations between sensory loss and depressive symptoms.
Methods: Piecewise linear-mixed models were used to analyze 16-years of longitudinal data collected on up to five occasions from 1611 adults (51% men) aged between 65 and 103 years. Depressive symptoms were assessed by the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D). Vision loss (VL) was defined by corrected visual acuity >0.3 logMAR in the better eye, blindness, or glaucoma. Hearing loss (HL) was defined by pure-tone average (PTA) >25 dB in the better hearing ear. Analyses were adjusted for socio-demographics, medical conditions, lifestyle behaviors, activities of daily living (ADLs), cognitive function, and social engagement.
Results: Unadjusted models indicated that higher levels of depressive symptoms were associated with HL (B = 1.16, SE = 0.33) and DSL (B = 2.15, SE = 0.39) but not VL. Greater rates of change in depressive symptoms were also evident after the onset of HL (B = 0.16, SE = 0.06, p < 0.01) and DSL (B = 0.30, SE = 0.09, p < 0.01). The associations between depressive symptoms and sensory loss were explained by difficulties with ADLs, and social engagement.
Conclusion: Vision and HL are highly prevalent among older adults and their co-occurrence may compound their respective impacts on health, functioning, and activity engagement, thereby exerting strong effects on the mental health and wellbeing of those affected. There is therefore a need for rehabilitation programs to be sensitive to the combined effects of sensory loss on individuals.
Introduction
Sensory loss is a common experience for older adults and is one of the leading causes of non-fatal disease burden for Australians aged 65 years or older (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, 2007; Kiely et al., 2012b). Hearing loss (HL) in particular is the most prevalent chronic condition affecting the oldest old, yet is often under-recognized and undertreated (Lin, 2012). Age-related declines in sensory functions are gradual and initially unnoticed, but their impacts are substantial and wide ranging. For example, low levels of vision and hearing have been associated with poorer health (Crews and Campbell, 2004) and cognitive function (Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994; Anstey et al., 2003; Lin, 2012), loss of functional independence (Keller et al., 1999; Reuben et al., 1999; Wallhagen et al., 2001; Brennan et al., 2005), communication difficulties (Heine and Browning, 2004), reduced social engagement (Resnick et al., 1997; Crews and Campbell, 2004), poor quality of life (Chia et al., 2006; Fischer et al., 2009), and increased risk of falls (Lopez et al., 2011), hospitalization (Lee et al., 2005), and mortality (Mui et al., 1998; Lee et al., 2007; Karpa et al., 2010). All of these impacts are recognized as risk factors for depression and/or outcomes of depression. Unsurprisingly, a number of studies have consistently demonstrated a strong link between co-occurring vision and HL, often referred to as dual sensory loss (DSL), and depressive symptoms (Lupsakko et al., 2002; Capella-McDonnall, 2005, 2009, 2011a; Chou, 2008; Harada et al., 2008).
Although the association between DSL and mental health is well established, a number of research questions regarding their relationship remain unresolved. These include understanding the direct and indirect mental health consequences in relation to (a) clinical measures of DSL where vision and HL are defined by functional assessment rather than perceived sensory loss; (b) clarifying the relative independent contributions of hearing and vision loss (VL); and (c) identifying mediating factors that explain the association. For example, Capella-McDonnall (2009) has published longitudinal analysis of self-report data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) demonstrating that adults who reported VL prior to DSL experienced greater levels of depressive symptoms compared to those who did not report VL, but elevated levels of depressive symptoms were not evident among those who reported HL prior to DSL. Similar findings have also been reported for the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA), where increased risk of depression incidence was predicted by self-reported VL and DSL but not HL alone (Chou, 2008). It is unclear if DSL has an additional impact on mental health over and above the effects of a single sensory loss (Schneider et al., 2011). Together, these findings support the hypothesis that VL is the primary driver of the association between DSL and depression. However, older adults have a tendency to under report hearing problems in epidemiological surveys (Kiely et al., 2012a). Further, self-report measures may be subject to response endogeneity when examined in relation to depression – that is, adults with mental health problems may hold negative perceptions of their sensory abilities and so be more likely to report difficulties with their vision and hearing. These biases could mask the true nature of the association between the progression of sensory loss and depression. Many studies lack bio-psychosocial data that allow for the investigation of factors that may explain the association between DSL and depression, leaving authors to speculate about potential mechanisms and causal pathways (Brennan and Bally, 2007; Capella-McDonnall, 2009). Addressing these issues is important for the development and implementation of appropriately targeted rehabilitation programs.
This study aims to improve our understanding of the relationship between DSL and mental health by using clinical measures of vision and hearing to examine the long-term impacts of age-related declines in sensory function on depressive symptoms, and to identify mediating bio-psychosocial factors that explain the association. Longitudinal population-based data are used to investigate differences in levels of depressive symptoms between adults with and without sensory loss, and examine discontinuities in rates of change in depressive symptoms after the onset of a single sensory loss and DSL. We employ a similar analytic approach to Capella-McDonnall (2009), whose findings we build upon by defining DSL based on a clinical assessment of vision and hearing. Further, our analyses take into account an extensive range of covariates that have previously been shown to be associated with depression and/or sensory loss and have been hypothesized to be explanatory of the relationship. These covariates include socio-demographics, lifestyle behaviors, medical conditions, functional disability, and activity engagement. Given that HL is under-reported by older adults, and is related to other risk factors for depression, we hypothesize that audiometric HL, as well as VL, will be linked to increased levels of depressive symptoms, and that these associations between sensory loss and depressive symptoms will be explained by activity engagement and functional disability.
Materials and Methods
Sample
We report analyses of data from the Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging (ALSA), a representative population-based prospective study of older adults residing in the community and in residential aged care settings (Luszcz et al., 2007). ALSA drew a random sample of adults aged 70 years and older from the electoral roll for the Adelaide metropolitan area of South Australia in 1992 (voting is compulsory). In anticipation of lower response rates and higher mortality-based attrition, ALSA oversampled men aged 85 years and older. Additionally, spouses aged 65 years and older, or adults aged over 70 who were cohabiting with a sampled respondent were also recruited, resulting in a total baseline sample size of 2087 participants.
Data were collected via personal interview and clinical assessment on up to five occasions over a 16-years period (Wave 1 1992; Wave 2, 1994; Wave 3, 2000–2001; Wave 4 2002–2004; Wave 5 2008). The sample analyzed in this study comprised 1611 adults (51% men) aged between 65 and 103 years who completed the clinical assessment on at least one occasion. Due to attrition and wave non-response the panel was unbalanced across waves, and participants who returned to the study after a period of non-participation were retained for the analyses.
Measures
Depressive symptoms
The main outcome of this study, were assessed by the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). The CES-D is a 20-item instrument that is designed for use in epidemiological surveys to measure depressive symptoms experienced over the past week. Item responses are recorded on a four point Likert scale (0 = none of the time, 1 = some of the time, 2 = quite a bit of the time, 3 = all of the time). Total scores on the CES-D range between 0 and 60, with higher scores indicating greater levels of depressive symptoms and scores >16 reflecting probable depression (Anstey et al., 2007).
Sensory variables
Sensory variables included clinical assessment of vision and hearing. VL was defined by corrected distance (3 m) visual acuity >0.3 logMAR (equivalent to 20/40 or 6/12 vision) in the better eye, self-reported blindness, or glaucoma. Hearing was assessed by pure-tone audiometry. A pure-tone average (PTA) was calculated for frequencies of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz. HL was defined by a PTA >25 dB in the better ear. Participants were categorized into one of four sensory loss groups: no sensory loss (No SL), VL only, HL only, and DSL. Time was defined as years in study, and three variables reflecting time after the onset of sensory loss (post-time VL, post-time HL, and post-time DSL) were also created. These variables were used to assess differences in rates of change in CES-D scores before and after the onset of sensory loss.
Covariates
Socio-demographic variables included age, sex, education, marital status, and domicile. Age at baseline was mean centered at 78 years (range: 65–103). Education was defined by age left school and was mean centered at age 15. Marital status was coded as partnered (married or de facto), un-partnered (never married, separated), and widowed. Participants were classified as either living in the community (private residence) or in residential care (e.g., hostels, nursing homes, hospitals, or boarding houses). Life style factors included smoking status and alcohol use. Participants were classified as never smokers, former smokers, and current smokers. The coding of alcohol consumption was in line with current Australian National Health and Medical Research Council guidelines (NHMRC, 2009), and was defined by the average number of standard drinks consumed in a single session (abstain, two or fewer standard drinks, and more than two standard drinks). Medical conditions were obtained by self-report of clinician diagnoses and included: arthritis, hypertension, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, history of stroke, and cancer. Cognitive impairment was assessed by the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975).
Disability and daily functioning were defined by the number of reported difficulties (no difficulty, any difficulty) with activities of daily living (ADLS) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). ADLs included the following eight basic activities important for daily functioning: grooming, dressing, eating, bathing, toileting, moving inside the home, and transferring from bed to chair. IADLs included an additional 10 routine tasks: doing the laundry, light housework, heavy housework, home maintenance or gardening, meal preparation, using the telephone, managing money, using public transport, grocery shopping, and writing a letter. The number of difficulties with ADLs and IADLs were coded to reflect no difficulties, one difficulty, two difficulties, and three or more difficulties.
The Adelaide activity profile (AAP) was used to assess level of social and activity engagement. The AAP has been validated as a measure of lifestyle activities for older adults for use in epidemiological surveys (Clark and Bond, 1995; Bond and Clark, 1998; Isherwood et al., 2012). Social activity engagement was defined by responses to eight items requiring participants to report their level of participation in social activities over a 3-month period. Items included: attending religious services, outdoor social activities, organized social activities at a club or center, making telephone calls to friends and family, inviting people to visit at home, going on a drive or outing, performing voluntary or paid work. A measure of engagement in mentally stimulating activities was calculated based on levels of participation with the following four activities: spending time on a hobby, reading, watching TV or listening to the radio, and walking outside for 15 min or more. Higher scores reflected a greater level of engagement and scores were mean centered. A measure reflecting the specific role of HL in restricting social engagement was also included. Participants were asked “does hearing affect your personal or social life?” with responses coded on a four point scale (0 = never, 1 = seldom, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often).
All variables were time-varying and collected at each measurement occasion, with the exception of age at baseline, sex, and education.
Analyses
In line with standard model building procedures for testing discontinuities in intercepts and change as described by Singer and Willett (2003), we conducted a series of piecewise linear-mixed models to test for the longitudinal effects of sensory loss on depressive symptoms over time. In an initial step we tested an unconditional means model (an intercept only model not adjusted for any covariates) and unconditional growth model (adjusted only for time in study), which served as base comparison models for subsequent analyses. We then added to the unconditional growth model fixed and random effects for the three dummy-coded indicators of sensory loss (VL, HL, and DSL), and the three post-time sensory loss variables (time-post VL, time-post HL, and time-post DSL). The purpose of this model was to assess discontinuities in levels and rates of change in CES-D scores as a function of sensory loss. Specifically, the time-varying indicators of sensory loss reflect differences in levels of depressive symptoms between participants with a sensory loss relative to those with no sensory loss. The time in study variable reflects rates of change in depressive symptoms for participants with no sensory loss (or prior to the first observed instance of sensory loss). The three post-time sensory loss variables reflect differences in linear rates of change in depressive symptoms for participants during times that they are experiencing sensory loss, relative to participants who are not experiencing a sensory loss. The level 1 (individual level) and level 2 (population average level) equations tested for the sensory loss adjusted piecewise linear-mixed model with fixed and random effects for all variables are:
Level 1:
Level 2:
Where, π represents the estimated CES-D score for a given time-varying covariate for person i, and ε represents the level 1 residual (unexplained variance). In the level 2 equation β represents the population average (fixed effect) association between a given variable and CES-D score, and U represents the population average (random effect) variance component for a given variable. Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) indices tests of Chi square differences were used to assess the optimal model fit relative to the unconditional means and unconditional growth model. To reduce model complexity and facilitate model convergence, random effects that did not contribute to the overall model fit were excluded from subsequent multivariate adjusted models.
To identify explanatory variables that attenuate the longitudinal associations between sensory loss and depression, two series of analyses were conducted. First, covariates were added individually to age, sex, and education adjusted models. These analyses tested the extent to which a single variable set explained the association between sensory loss and CES-D scores without taking into consideration the shared effects of other factors. Second, to assess how each variable set explained the association independently of the effects of other factors, each set of variables were added sequentially to the optimal sensory loss adjusted model in the following sequence:
• Model 1 added time invariant socio-demographic variables (e.g., age at baseline, sex, education).
• Model 2 added time-varying socio-demographic variables reflecting participants’ current circumstances (marital status and domicile).
• Model 3 added life style behaviors (e.g., smoking and alcohol consumption).
• Model 4 added medical conditions (e.g., diabetes, cardiovascular disease).
• Model 5 added cognitive function (MMSE).
• Model 6 added markers of disability (e.g., ADLs and IADLs).
• Model 7 added activity engagement variables (social engagement, mental engagement, hearing related social limitations).
There were 1550 participants with complete data on all covariates, representing a loss of 4% of the available sample (n = 1611) due to item non-response. We therefore employed multiple imputation to reduce bias attributable to this item level missing data (Schafer and Graham, 2002; Graham et al., 2007; Graham, 2009; Janssen et al., 2010). We imputed 20 datasets under the missing at random (MAR) assumption which maintains that after controlling for observed variables, the remaining missing data is not associated with any unobserved factors and can be considered completely random. The imputation model included all variables from the full analytic model, plus auxiliary variables (self-rated health, country of origin, preferred language, career occupation, PTA, visual acuity, and individual scale items). Data were not imputed for participants who had missing data for an entire wave due to attrition or mortality. Results from the multiple imputation analyses were compared to complete case analyses.
To illustrate the effects of sensory loss progression on depressive symptoms estimates from the sensory loss adjusted model were used to graph the predicted trajectories in CES-D scores over 16-years for four hypothetical cases who experienced: (i) no SL at any time, (ii) VL from year 4, (iii) HL from year 4, (iv) DSL from year 7. All analyses were conducted using Stata 11 statistical software (StataCorp, 2009).
Results
The baseline sample characteristics for each sensory loss group are presented in Table 1. There were 1246 participants (mean age = 78) with data on sensory loss and depressive symptoms at baseline. A number of participants who did not complete the clinical assessment at baseline did so at later waves, resulting in a total sample size of 1611 participants. Sensory loss was highly prevalent, particularly among the oldest old. Only 10.5% of participants aged 85–94 were identified with no sensory loss, and all participants aged 95 years and older experienced some degree of sensory loss. Tests of bivariate associations indicated that adults with DSL were more likely to be male, older, widowed, residing in residential care settings, have lower levels of education, abstain from alcohol consumption, rate their health as fair or poor, have been diagnosed with diabetes and experience a greater number of difficulties with ADLs and IADLs. In contrast, single sensory loss did not show strong discrimination for many of these variables. HL was more prevalent among men than women, and increased slightly with age, while the prevalence of VL actually decreased among older age-groups reflecting the inflow of older participants to DSL. Over the course of the study, on average 21.1% of the sample had no sensory loss, 6.5% had VL only, 47.2% had HL only, and 25.2% had DSL. A total of 584 participants were identified with DSL on at least one occasion. For the majority of incident DSL cases, onset of HL preceded VL. Attrition and mortality were not the only sources of missing data, there were 373 (23%) participants who completed the clinical assessment of sensory functioning and CES-D but had item level missing data for other covariates selected for inclusion in the multivariate analyses. The means and standard deviations in CES-D scores by loss group at each measurement wave are presented in Table 2. The intra-class correlation for CES-D scores was 0.49 (standard error = 0.02), indicating that roughly equal proportions of the unexplained variance could be attributed to between and within person differences.
Table 2. Frequency counts and CES-D means and standard deviations (SD) for each sensory loss group by wave.
A series of models were tested to identify the optimal parameterization relative to the unconditional growth model (Table 3). Residual plots confirmed that the assumptions of linear regression were met, and transformations of CESD score did not alter the substantive findings. Broadly, the inclusion of sensory loss variables as fixed effects improved model fit (Model A). The addition of random effects for DSL and time-post DSL made further substantial improvements in model fit (Model C) while the addition of random effects for HL and time-post HL made a small but significant improvement in model fit (Model D). Thus, the effects of HL and DSL on levels and rates of change in CES-D scores varied across individuals. Although the fit indices indicated that the inclusion of VL did not contribute to the overall model fit, this variable was retained in subsequent analyses to distinguish this group from participants with no sensory loss.
Table 3. Model fit indices for competing models piecewise linear-mixed models of change in CES-D scores as a function of sensory loss (n = 1611).
The estimates from sensory loss adjusted analysis indicated that participants identified with DSL (β = 2.17, SE = 0.39, p < 0.001) and HL only (β = 1.14, SE = 0.32, p < 0.001) had elevated levels of depressive symptoms compared to those with no sensory loss. Participants with DSL had significantly higher levels of CES-D scores compared to those with HL (Mean Difference = 1.03, SE = 0.34, p = 0.003). However, there was no reliable difference in levels of CES-D scores between those identified with VL only and those identified with no sensory loss (p = 0.91). There was an estimated annual rate of change in CES-D scores of 0.12 (SE = 0.04, p = 0.004) units per year among adults not experiencing a sensory loss. An additional increase in linear rates of change in CES-D scores occurred after the first instance of HL (β = 0.17, SE = 0.06, p = 0.006) and DSL (β = 0.23, SE = 0.09, p < 0.001). There was no difference in rates of change in CES-D scores after the first instance of VL (p = 0.47) relative to adults with no sensory loss. Figure 1 provides a graphical illustration of these differences in levels and rates of change in CES-D scores before and after the first occurrence that a sensory loss was observed. There was also an association between the degree of HL and depressive symptoms scores, with higher PTA thresholds predicting higher CESD scores (results not tabled).
Figure 1. Mean trajectories estimated from unadjusted linear-mixed models, depicting discontinuities in levels and rates of change of CES-D scores as a function of sensory loss for four hypothetical scenarios: no sensory loss at any time (gray dash), vision loss only from year 4 (black dash), hearing loss only from year 4 (gray line), dual sensory loss from year 7 (black line).
To assess the extent to which the associations between sensory loss and CESD-D scores were explained by the effects of other factors, covariates were first added individually to age, sex, and education adjusted Model D (Table 4) and then in a series of hierarchical steps (Table 5). After adjusting for activity engagement, none of the sensory loss variables (DSL, HL, VL, time-post DSL, time-post HL) reliably predicted differences in levels or rates of change in CES-D scores. The associations between sensory loss and CES-D were also attenuated after the inclusion of ADLs and IADLs.
Table 4. Parameter estimates from piecewise linear-mixed models testing predictors of CES-D scores in Model G, adjusted for covariates, from 20 multiply imputed datasets (n = 1611).
Table 5. Parameter estimates from piecewise linear-mixed models testing predictors of CES-D score, from 20 multiply imputed datasets (n = 1611).
Discussion
Previous research has established a link between perceived sensory loss and poor mental health, though combined VL and HL has not consistently been reported to have additional burden over and above the effects of a single sensory loss (Capella-McDonnall, 2009; Schneider et al., 2011). Understanding the pathways between DSL and depressive symptoms in late-life is important for the health management and care of older adults. However, no studies have examined long-term changes in depressive symptoms in relation to the progression of vision and HL using clinical measures of sensory function, while at the same time taking into account a rich set of contextual and potentially mediating factors.
Regarding incident sensory loss, the present findings illustrated that the first observed occurrence of HL and DSL coincided with a jump in the level of depressive symptoms. In contrast, no increase in the level of depressive symptoms was observed at the first observed occurrence of VL. Notably, the elevation in depressive symptoms associated with DSL was almost twice that of HL indicating that co-occurring problems with vision and hearing contribute additional burden to mental health over and above the effects of a single sensory loss. There was also a greater increase in depressive symptoms over time after the onset of DSL and HL, compared to adults with VL or no sensory loss. Consistent with previous studies (Capella-McDonnall, 2009) we observed considerable variability between individuals in the way that the progression of DSL impacted on mental health profiles.
Overall, we found that impaired visual functioning was not associated with poorer mental health, except when combined with HL. On this evidence, audiometric HL appears to be the main driver of the association between clinically defined DSL and increased depressive symptoms. This contrasts with previous studies that have used self-report data and identified perceived VL, rather than perceived HL, as being more strongly associated with increased risk of depression or greater propensity to report depressive symptoms (Crews and Campbell, 2004; Capella-McDonnall, 2005, 2009; Chou, 2008). Crews and Campbell (2004) also found that adults who report VL have a greater number of difficulties with ADLs than do adults who report HL. These discrepancies with our findings most likely reflect differences in the assessment, measurement, and definition of sensory loss, demonstrating that self-report measures operate differently to clinical measures. Further, adults with severe VL and specific eye disease such as age-related macular degeneration have been consistently reported to be at increased risk of depression (Brody et al., 2001; Casten et al., 2004; Evans et al., 2007; Popescu et al., 2012; Eramudugolla et al., 2013). This literature may also seem at odds with the present findings. However, these studies investigated vision in isolation, and did not include measures of hearing function. Our null finding only applies to VL in the absence of HL. Given the high prevalence of HL among older adults, there were relatively few adults with “VL only” in our study and they tended to be younger. Moreover, recent evidence on the association between visual acuity (a main component of our VL variable) and depression has been mixed. Eramudugolla et al. (2013) reported an association between depression and low contrast visual acuity but not high contrast visual acuity, and in a nationally representative survey of US adults Zhang et al. (2013) found that self-reported VL independently predicted increased depression risk, but visual acuity did not.
A key outcome of this study was the identification of explanatory factors that attenuate the association between sensory loss and depressive symptoms, and can be targeted by interventions. Virtually all the variables examined weakened the relationship. Most notably, participation levels in socially engaging and mentally stimulating activities fully explained the increased depressive symptoms experienced by adults with sensory loss. The next strongest mediators of the association were difficulties with daily functioning. This evidence supports the notion that maintaining an active and engaged lifestyle by participating in meaningful activities can mitigate the adverse impacts caused by functional limitations and frailty, and is important for older adults’ health and wellbeing. Capella-McDonnall (2011a) hypothesizes that increased dependence on others and greater need for assistance due to age-related sensory loss can lead to an externalized shift in locus of control and poorer self-concept. Thus, older adults may be at increased risk of depression if they do not have sufficient resources available to cope with the restricted independence and communication difficulties caused by sensory declines. Future research should investigate the role of personal resources such as perceived control.
Previous studies investigating mediators of perceived sensory loss and mental health have been mixed. Cross-sectional analysis of community-dwelling adults in the USA found that self-reported sensory loss reliably predicted depressive symptoms independently of markers of socio-economic position, health, social support, activity engagement, and disability (Capella-McDonnall, 2005). On the other hand, Chou (2008) reported that after adjusting for mobility impairment, informal support and limitations with daily functioning, VL remained an independent predictor of depression risk but HL and DSL did not. Not only do these cross-sectional findings conflict with each other, neither pattern is consistent with our longitudinal findings. This may reflect the differences of self-reports compared to clinical measures of function, or the choice of covariates.
Other longitudinal studies, though based on self-report data, have provided findings more in line with ours. For example, volunteer programs were shown to provide protective effects against depression for adults with self-reported DSL (Capella-McDonnall, 2011a) and physical status has been identified as a moderator of this association (Capella-McDonnall, 2011b). Similar analyses have also shown that adults adapt to their sensory loss and after a period of adjustment experience an improvement in their level of depressive symptoms. (Capella-McDonnall, 2009). Unfortunately the small number of repeated observations (t = 5) and high proportion of missing data at later waves precluded modeling of quadratic slopes after the onset of sensory loss in our study. This would have facilitated testing of adaption to sensory loss. The selection, optimization, and compensation model (Baltes et al., 1999) and assimilative and accommodative coping model (Rothermund and Brandtstadter, 2003) are useful frameworks for conceptualizing successful adaption to sensory loss as they emphasize the importance of shifting personal goals and active interventions in response to loss. Factors hypothesized to enable the adaption process would include cognitive resources (Heyl and Wahl, 2011), sense of control, use of sensory aids, social and environmental support, and changes in expectations (Brennan and Bally, 2007).
As outlined, most of the research on DSL has been primarily reliant on epidemiological survey data that has been used to provide descriptive population estimates derived from self-report measures (Brennan and Bally, 2007; Schneider et al., 2011), which are argued to be more ecologically valid, providing an assessment of the perceived impact of sensory loss (Heine et al., 2013). While analyses of self-report data make an important contribution to our understanding of how perceptions of sensory loss relate to healthy aging, they should not be conflated with processes associated with functional abilities. Self-report data has limited validity as a measure of impairment as perceptions of sensory loss are influenced by individual differences in health expectations – which are shaped by social norms (Sargent-Cox et al., 2008). Because declining hearing is a common experience in late life, many older adults perceive hearing difficulties to be a normal part of the aging process and so are less likely to report a HL, even in severe cases (Kiely et al., 2012a). Further, adults with poor mental health are more likely to report difficulties with vision and hearing, which introduces response bias and makes it difficult to draw clear conclusions regarding the direction of the association between self-reported DSL and depressive symptoms. We found that the association between DSL and depression was partly explained by self-reports that hearing limitations restricted a person’s social life. This along with the finding of Zhang et al. (2013) could be interpreted as showing that self-reported sensory loss is more relevant when investigating their impacts on wellbeing. By using clinical measures we were able to ascertain factors that underlie the association between sensory impairment and depressive symptoms, and therefore identify circumstances that might lead to reports of difficulties with sensory functioning.
There is no standard or clear definition of DSL (Saunders and Echt, 2007; Smith et al., 2008). While we applied cut-points for mild impaired vision (visual acuity >0.3 logMAR) and hearing (PTA >25 dB in the better hearing ear) that conformed to common international standards, it is possible that when defining DSL the ideal thresholds may change depending on the interaction between sensory modalities. We also recognize that VL and HL occur along a continuum and the common practice of defining ranges of sensory impairment based on arbitrary, though conventional, thresholds overlooks the complex interactions that occur between different sensory domains and may conceal some effects.
Limitations of this study include biases introduced by attrition and non-response, diminished sample size at later waves, and lack of clinical assessment of eye disease such as age-related macular degeneration that have previously been linked to depression. Despite the inclusion of adults residing in institutions and oversampling of older men, the ALSA is still subject to selective attrition due to poor health which could introduce biases (Anstey and Luszcz, 2002). Although ALSA interviewers were trained to collect data from older adults with functional limitations, severe loss of vision and hearing are still likely to hinder survey completion. Consequently, it is likely that our sample represents a healthy population of older adults and estimates on the prevalence and impact of sensory loss should be considered conservative.
Due to the long time intervals between measurement occasions it was not possible to precisely model the timing of sensory loss onset, so the first observed occurrence of a sensory loss can only be interpreted to indicate that visual acuity or hearing function crossed a threshold commonly used to define mild impairment at some time in the year(s) between measurement occasions. It is likely that the progression of sensory loss will differ across individuals and could occur suddenly or unfold gradually over time. Such complexities could not be captured in the present analyses. Moreover, we did not examine the severity of sensory loss and used basic identifiers of impaired visual and auditory function. VL was defined by impaired visual acuity or presence of glaucoma and blindness. Other aspects of vision that were not assessed in this study, such as poor contrast sensitivity, reduced peripheral vision, and macular degeneration, could be more important for physical functioning and mental health. Similarly, our use of PTA in the better hearing ear disregards much of the richness of data available in an audiogram and we lacked measures of central auditory functioning. Although we have replaced a poor marker of HL (self-reported hearing difficulties) with a more ideal measure of hearing function (PTA >25 dB), it is possible that at the same time we have applied a narrower definition of VL (visual acuity >0.3 logMAR and eye disease) that does not adequately reflect broader aspects of visual functioning that would otherwise be captured by self-report. Future research would extend existing findings by investigating other measures of hearing and vision, such as speech recognition, peripheral vision, and contrast sensitivity (e.g., Fischer et al., 2009). This would enable a rigorous definition of DSL, as well as allowing for greater sophistication in our understanding of the co-morbidity of vision and hearing impairment and associated functional limitations. These limitations notwithstanding, the strengths of this study are the use of population-based longitudinal data, use of multiple imputation, clinical measures of sensory loss, and the inclusion of an extensive range of covariates in our analyses.
In summary, VL and HL are highly prevalent among older adults and their co-occurrence may compound their respective impacts on health and functioning, thereby exerting strong effects on the mental health and wellbeing of those affected. Visual cues can provide an important compensatory mechanism for hearing impaired adults, and, conversely, auditory cues for adults with visual impairment. Our findings indicate that VL alone (in the absence of HL) has little effect on depressive symptoms, but does accentuate the effects of HL. Importantly, the association was explained by levels of participation in social activities and daily functioning.
The principal contribution of audiometric HL to increased depressive symptoms suggests that hearing assistive technologies and hearing rehabilitation programs may help to reduce the impacts of DSL. This evidence base can inform health policy which is needed to encourage improved awareness and screening of sensory loss and co-morbidities among older adults. There is a need for health professionals and care practitioners to be sensitive to the combined effects of vision and HL on mental health and wellbeing. Our findings point to the important roles that functional independence and activity engagement can play in mitigating these adverse impacts.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging (ALSA) was initially funded by a grant from the US National Institute of Health (Grant No. AG 08523-02), with additional funding from the South Australian government, Flinders University, and other NGOs. Subsequent funding has been provided by the Australian Research Council (ARC-LP 0669272, ARC-LP 100200413, ARC-DP 0879152, and ARC-DP 130100428) and the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC, 179839 and 229922). Anstey is support by NHMRC fellowship # 1002560. We wish to thank the participants in ALSA, who have given their time over many years, and gratefully acknowledge the work of the project team at the Flinders Centre for Ageing Studies, Flinders University, Adelaide, who carried out the ALSA and provided data for this paper. We also acknowledge the ARC Centre of Excellence in Population Ageing Research (project # CE110001029).
References
Anstey, K. J., Hofer, S. M., and Luszcz, M. A. (2003). A latent growth curve analysis of late-life sensory and cognitive function over 8 years: evidence for specific and common factors underlying change. Psychol. Aging 18, 714–726. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.714
Anstey, K. J., and Luszcz, M. A. (2002). Selective non-response to clinical assessment in the longitudinal study of aging: implications for estimating population levels of cognitive function and dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 704–709. doi:10.1002/gps.651
Anstey, K. J., Von Sanden, C., Sargent-Cox, K. A., and Luszcz, M. A. (2007). Prevalence and risk factors for depression in a longitudinal, population-based study including individuals in the community and residential care. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 497–505. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e31802e21d8
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2007). The burden of disease and injury in Australia 2003. Canberra: AIHW.
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., and Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 471–507. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.471
Bond, M. J., and Clark, M. S. (1998). Clinical applications of the Adelaide activities profile. Clin. Rehabil. 12, 228–237. doi:10.1191/026921598676962688
Brennan, M., and Bally, S. J. (2007). Psychosocial adaptations to dual sensory loss in middle and late adulthood. Trends Amplif. 11, 281–300. doi:10.1177/1084713807308210
Brennan, M., Horowitz, A., and Su, Y. P. (2005). Dual sensory loss and its impact on everyday competence. Gerontologist 45, 337–346. doi:10.1093/geront/45.3.337
Brody, B. L., Gamst, A. C., Williams, R. A., Smith, A. R., Lau, P. W., Dolnak, D., et al. (2001). Depression, visual acuity, comorbidity, and disability associated with age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology 108, 1893–1900. doi:10.1016/S0161-6420(01)00754-0
Capella-McDonnall, M. (2009). The effects of developing a dual sensory loss on depression in older adults: a longitudinal study. J. Aging Health 21, 1179–1199. doi:10.1177/0898264309350077
Capella-McDonnall, M. (2011a). The effect of productive activities on depressive symptoms among older adults with dual sensory loss. Res. Aging 33, 234–255. doi:10.1177/0164027511399106
Capella-McDonnall, M. (2011b). Physical status as a moderator of depressive symptoms among older adults with dual sensory loss. Rehabil. Psychol. 56, 67–76. doi:10.1037/a0022696
Capella-McDonnall, M. E. (2005). The effects of single and dual sensory loss on symptoms of depression in the elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 855–861. doi:10.1002/gps.1368
Casten, R. J., Rovner, B. W., and Tasman, W. (2004). Age-related macular degeneration and depression: a review of recent research. Curr. Opin. Ophthalmol. 15, 181–183. doi:10.1097/01.icu.0000120710.35941.3f
Chia, E.-M., Mitchell, P., Rochtchina, E., Foran, S., Golding, M., and Wang, J. J. (2006). Association between vision and hearing impairments and their combined effects on quality of life. Arch. Ophthalmol. 124, 1465–1470. doi:10.1001/archopht.124.10.1465
Chou, K. L. (2008). Combined effect of vision and hearing impairment on depression in older adults: evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. J. Affect. Disord. 106, 191–196. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2007.05.028
Clark, M. S., and Bond, M. J. (1995). The Adelaide activities profile: a measure of the life-style activities of elderly people. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 7, 174–184. doi:10.1007/BF03324332
Crews, J. E., and Campbell, V. A. (2004). Vision impairment and hearing loss among community-dwelling older Americans: implications for health and functioning. Am. J. Public Health 94, 823–829. doi:10.2105/AJPH.94.5.823
Eramudugolla, R., Wood, J., and Anstey, K. J. (2013). Co-morbidity of depression and anxiety in common age-related eye disease: a population-based study of 662 adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:56. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2013.00056
Evans, J. R., Fletcher, A. E., and Wormald, R. P. (2007). Depression and anxiety in visually impaired older people. Ophthalmology 114, 283–288. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2006.10.006
Fischer, M. E., Cruickshanks, K. J., Klein, B. E., Klein, R., Schubert, C. R., and Wiley, T. L. (2009). Multiple sensory impairment and quality of life. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 16, 346–353. doi:10.3109/09286580903312236
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 549–576. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530
Graham, J. W., Olchowski, A. E., and Gilreath, T. D. (2007). How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prev. Sci. 8, 206–213. doi:10.1007/s11121-007-0070-9
Harada, S., Nishiwaki, Y., Michikawa, T., Kikuchi, Y., Iwasawa, S., Nakano, M., et al. (2008). Gender difference in the relationships between vision and hearing impairments and negative well-being. Prev. Med. 47, 433–437. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.06.011
Heine, C., Browning, C., Cowlishaw, S., and Kendig, H. (2013). Trajectories of older adults’ hearing difficulties: examining the influence of health behaviors and social activity over 10 years. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 13, 911–918. doi:10.1111/ggi.12030
Heine, C., and Browning, C. J. (2004). The communication and psychosocial perceptions of older adults with sensory loss: a qualitative study. Ageing Soc. 24, 113–130. doi:10.1017/S0144686X03001491
Heyl, V., and Wahl, H.-W. (2011). Managing daily life with age-related sensory loss: cognitive resources gain in importance. Psychol. Aging 27, 510–521. doi:10.1037/a0025471
Isherwood, L. M., King, D. S., and Luszcz, M. A. (2012). A longitudinal analysis of social engagement in late-life widowhood. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 74, 211–229. doi:10.2190/AG.74.3.c
Janssen, K. J. M., Donders, A. R. T., Harrell, F. E. Jr., Vergouwe, Y., Chen, Q., Grobbee, D. E., et al. (2010). Missing covariate data in medical research: to impute is better than to ignore. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 63, 721–727. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.12.008
Karpa, M. J., Gopinath, B., Beath, K., Rochtchina, E., Cumming, R. G., Wang, J. J., et al. (2010). Associations between hearing impairment and mortality risk in older persons: the Blue Mountains Hearing Study. Ann. Epidemiol. 20, 452–459. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.03.011
Keller, B. K., Morton, J. L., Thomas, V. S., and Potter, J. F. (1999). The effect of visual and hearing impairments on functional status. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 1319–1325.
Kiely, K. M., Gopinath, B., Mitchell, P., Browning, C. J., and Anstey, K. J. (2012a). Evaluating a dichotomized measure of self-reported hearing loss against gold standard audiometry: prevalence estimates and age bias in a pooled national dataset. J. Aging Health 24, 439–458. doi:10.1177/0898264311425088
Kiely, K. M., Gopinath, B., Mitchell, P., Luszcz, M., and Anstey, K. J. (2012b). Cognitive, health, and sociodemographic predictors of longitudinal decline in hearing acuity among older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 997–1003. doi:10.1093/gerona/gls066
Lee, D. J., Gomez-Marin, O., Lam, B. L., Zheng, D. D., Arheart, K. L., Christ, S. L., et al. (2007). Severity of concurrent visual and hearing impairment and mortality – the 1986-1994 National Health Interview Survey. J. Aging Health 19, 382–396. doi:10.1177/0898264307300174
Lee, D. J., Lam, B. L., Gomez-Marin, O., Zheng, D. D., and Caban, A. J. (2005). Concurrent hearing and visual impairment and morbidity in community-residing adults – the National Health Interview Survey, 1986 to 1996. J. Aging Health 17, 531–546. doi:10.1177/0898264305277956
Lin, F. R. (2012). Hearing loss in older adults: who’s listening? JAMA 307, 1147–1148. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.321
Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol. Aging 9, 339–355.
Lopez, D., McCaul, K. A., Hankey, G. J., Norman, P. E., Almeida, O. P., Dobson, A. J., et al. (2011). Falls, injuries from falls, health related quality of life and mortality in older adults with vision and hearing impairment – is there a gender difference? Maturitas 69, 359–364. doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2011.05.006
Lupsakko, T., Mantyjarvi, M., Kautiainen, H., and Sulkava, R. (2002). Combined hearing and visual impairment and depression in a population aged 75 years and older. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 808–813. doi:10.1002/gps.689
Luszcz, M. A., Giles, L., Eckermann, S., Edwards, P., Browne-Yung, K., and Hayles, C. (2007). The Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing: 15 years of ageing in South Australia. Adelaide: South Australian Department of Families and Communities.
Mui, S., Reuben, D., Damesyn, M., Greendale, G., and Moore, A. (1998). Sensory impairment as a predictor of 10-year mortality and functional impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 46, 19–20.
NHMRC. (2009). Australian Guidelines to Reduce Health Risks from Drinking Alcohol. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.
Popescu, M. L., Boisjoly, H. L. N., Schmaltz, H., Kergoat, M.-J., Rousseau, J., Moghadaszadeh, S., et al. (2012). Explaining the relationship between three eye diseases and depressive symptoms in older adults. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 2308–2313. doi:10.1167/iovs.11-9330
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306
Resnick, H. E., Fries, B. E., and Verbrugge, L. M. (1997). Windows to their world: the effect of sensory impairments on social engagement and activity time in nursing home residents. J. Gerontol. BPsychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 52, S135–S144. doi:10.1093/geronb/52B.3.S135
Reuben, D. B., Mui, S., Damesyn, M., Moore, A. A., and Greendale, G. A. (1999). The prognostic value of sensory impairment in older persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 930–935.
Rothermund, K., and Brandtstadter, J. (2003). Coping with deficits and losses in later life: from compensatory action to accommodation. Psychol. Aging 18, 896–905. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.896
Sargent-Cox, K. A., Anstey, K. J., and Luszcz, M. A. (2008). Determinants of self-rated health items with different points of reference: implications for health measurement of older Adults. J. Aging Health 20, 739–761. doi:10.1177/0898264308321035
Saunders, G. H., and Echt, K. V. (2007). An overview of dual sensory impairment in older adults: perspectives for rehabilitation. Trends Amplif. 11, 243–258. doi:10.1177/1084713807308365
Schafer, J. L., and Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol. Methods 7, 147–177. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147
Schneider, J. M., Gopinath, B., McMahon, C. M., Leeder, S. R., Mitchell, P., and Wang, J. J. (2011). Dual sensory impairment in older age. J. Aging Health 23, 1309–1324. doi:10.1177/0898264311408418
Singer, J. D., and Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, S. L., Bennett, L. W., and Wilson, R. H. (2008). Prevalence and characteristics of dual sensory impairment (hearing and vision) in a veteran population. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 45, 597–609. doi:10.1682/JRRD.2007.02.0023
Wallhagen, M. I., Strawbridge, W. J., Shema, S. J., Kurata, J., and Kaplan, G. A. (2001). Comparative impact of hearing and vision impairment on subsequent functioning. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 49, 1086–1092. doi:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49213.x
Zhang, X., Bullard, K. M., Cotch, M. F., Wilson, M. R., Rovner, B. W., McGwin, G. Jr., et al. (2013). Association between depression and functional vision loss in persons 20 years of age or older in the United States, NHANES 2005-2008. JAMA Ophthalmol. 131, 573–581. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2013.2597
Keywords: depression, mental health, dual sensory loss, aging, hearing loss, visual impairment, Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging
Citation: Kiely KM, Anstey KJ and Luszcz MA (2013) Dual sensory loss and depressive symptoms: the importance of hearing, daily functioning, and activity engagement. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:837. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00837
Received: 15 May 2013; Accepted: 19 November 2013;
Published online: 16 December 2013.
Edited by:
Bamini Gopinath, University of Sydney, AustraliaReviewed by:
Catherine McMahon, Macquarie University, AustraliaWalter Wittich, MAB-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre, Canada
Copyright: © 2013 Kiely, Anstey and Luszcz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Kim M. Kiely, Centre for Research on Ageing Health and Wellbeing, Australian National University, Building 62A Eggleston Road, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia e-mail: kim.kiely@anu.edu.au