Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 October 2013
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Neural processing of emotion in multimodal settings View all 25 articles

Avoidant symptoms in PTSD predict fear circuit activation during multimodal fear extinction

\r\nRebecca K. Sripada,*Rebecca K. Sripada1,2*Sarah N. GarfinkelSarah N. Garfinkel3Israel Liberzon\r\nIsrael Liberzon1
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
  • 2Veterans Affairs Center for Clinical Management Research, Department of Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton, UK

Convergent evidence suggests that individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) exhibit exaggerated avoidance behaviors as well as abnormalities in Pavlonian fear conditioning. However, the link between the two features of this disorder is not well understood. In order to probe the brain basis of aberrant extinction learning in PTSD, we administered a multimodal classical fear conditioning/extinction paradigm that incorporated affectively relevant information from two sensory channels (visual and tactile) while participants underwent fMRI scanning. The sample consisted of fifteen OEF/OIF veterans with PTSD. In response to conditioned cues and contextual information, greater avoidance symptomatology was associated with greater activation in amygdala, hippocampus, vmPFC, dmPFC, and insula, during both fear acquisition and fear extinction. Heightened responses to previously conditioned stimuli in individuals with more severe PTSD could indicate a deficiency in safety learning, consistent with PTSD symptomatology. The close link between avoidance symptoms and fear circuit activation suggests that this symptom cluster may be a key component of fear extinction deficits in PTSD and/or may be particularly amenable to change through extinction-based therapies.

Introduction

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a debilitating anxiety disorder that afflicts approximately 7 percent of the general population (Kessler et al., 2005). PTSD is characterized by three symptom clusters: reexperiencing, hyperarousal, and avoidance symptoms (APA, 2000). The avoidance cluster includes avoidance of internal and external reminders of the trauma, failure to recall important aspects of the trauma, loss of interest in significant activities, subjective detachment or estrangement from others, restricted range of affect, and sense of foreshortened future (APA, 2000). Some studies suggest that avoidance symptoms track the diagnosis of PTSD better than either of the other two symptom clusters (North et al., 1999). In addition, clinical research indicates that for individuals with PTSD, avoidance symptoms may be the most detrimental symptoms to psychosocial functioning (Hendrix et al., 1998; Riggs et al., 1998; Ruscio et al., 2002; Kuhn et al., 2003; Samper et al., 2004; Lauterbach et al., 2007; Solomon and Mikulincer, 2007; Malta et al., 2009) and quality of life (Lunney and Schnurr, 2007; Schnurr and Lunney, 2008). Furthermore, early avoidance symptoms may predict subsequent PTSD development (Bryant et al., 2000; North et al., 2012). These multiple lines of evidence suggest that avoidant symptoms might signify a key process in PTSD pathophysiology.

In parallel, convergent evidence suggests that PTSD is associated with various abnormalities in fear associated learning, including greater acquisition of conditioned fear, overgeneralization of conditioning, impaired inhibitory learning, and impaired extinction (Orr et al., 2000; Lissek et al., 2005; Milad et al., 2008, 2009; Jovanovic et al., 2009, 2010; Rougemont-Bucking et al., 2011; Mahan and Ressler, 2012; Lommen et al., 2013). It has been suggested that deficits in fear associated learning may play a role in the development (see Lommen et al., 2013) and maintenance (Mahan and Ressler, 2012) of PTSD, and that abnormalities in the extinction and/or retention of conditioned fear may be particularly salient for the persistence of fear memories in PTSD (Milad et al., 2008, 2009). Few studies to date have probed the neural circuitry underlying fear extinction deficits in PTSD, but the existing evidence suggests key roles for amygdala, hippocampus, and vmPFC in this process (Milad et al., 2005, 2007, 2009; Mahan and Ressler, 2012).

Conceptually, deficits in fear-associated learning have been hypothesized to contribute to the development and maintenance of reexperiencing and hyperarousal symptoms. However, the link between fear-associated learning deficits and other key components of PTSD pathophysiology, such as avoidance symptoms, is not well understood. Animal models suggest that avoidance may stem from fear extinction deficits. For instance, Chen et al. (2012) demonstrated that rats displaying greater fear after conditioning go on to exhibit greater behavioral avoidance over a 4 week period. It is also possible that avoidance symptoms may exacerbate fear extinction deficits by reducing the frequency with which individuals come in contact with feared stimuli, thus providing less opportunity for extinction to occur. For instance, socially anxious individuals with more severe avoidance in early treatment experience greater subsequent fear in later treatment (Aderka et al., 2013). However, no research has investigated the neurobiological underpinnings of this phenomenon.

In order to probe the brain basis of the link between avoidance and aberrant extinction learning in PTSD, we administered a multimodal classical fear conditioning/extinction paradigm that incorporated affectively relevant information from two sensory channels (visual and tactile) in an fMRI environment. Mild shock was used as the unconditioned stimulus (US), and colored lights were used as the conditioned stimuli (CS+ and CS-). We have previously demonstrated that PTSD patients exhibit impaired extinction recall and greater return of extinguished fear when trauma-relevant stimuli are presented, and that these abnormalities are particularly associated with avoidance symptoms (Garfinkel et al., unpublished). Thus, the current study sought to investigate whether individual differences in avoidance symptoms might influence extinction learning. We hypothesized that in response to conditioned stimuli (CS’s) and context, individuals with more severe avoidance symptoms would demonstrate greater activity in brain networks related to emotion processing and fear expression.

Material and Methods

Subjects

Fifteen right-handed OEF/OIF veterans with PTSD were recruited from the Ann Arbor Veterans Affairs PTSD Clinic. Mean age was 27.3 (SD = 4.5). Ten patients were married and five were single. Twelve patients were Caucasian, one was Asian, one was African-American, and one was Hispanic. Participants were included as part of a larger sample (Sripada et al., 2012a, 2012b; Garfinkel et al., unpublished) that also included healthy combat-exposed controls. All participants received comprehensive psychiatric assessment with the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) and the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al., 1995). Mean CAPS score was 75.9 (SD = 17.2). All combat exposure (including index trauma for PTSD participants) took place within 5 years prior to study enrollment. Clinical interviews were performed by experienced masters- or doctoral-level clinicians with extensive training in the CAPS, at a subspecialty clinic specializing in PTSD. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) psychosis, (b) history of traumatic brain injury, (c) alcohol or substance abuse or dependence in the past 3 months, (d) any psychoactive medication other than sleep aids, (e) left-handedness, (f) presence of ferrous-containing metals within the body, and (g) claustrophobia. Seven participants also met diagnostic criteria for depression, and one had comorbid panic disorder; however, PTSD was always the primary diagnosis. Two participants were using low-dose trazodone as a sleep aid; no other psychiatric medications were permitted. After a complete description of the study was provided to the participants, written informed consent was obtained. The study was approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Michigan Medical School and the Ann Arbor VA Healthcare System. All procedures took place between August 2008 and July 2010.

Task

Participants were fear conditioned in a modified version of Milad et al.’s (2005, 2007) paradigm. The CS’s were colored lights (pink and blue), presented on a background of an office or library setting (context). The US was an electric shock (500 ms duration pulse sequence) delivered to the index and middle fingers, titrated individually to the level defined as “highly annoying but not painful” (Orr et al., 2000).

Habituation, fear acquisition, and fear extinction all occurred within the scanner in three separate functional runs. Prior to each run, participants were informed that they could receive a shock at any time (see Milad et al., 2007). Habituation involved 12 presentations of the context plus light pairings, and ensured that participants became familiar with stimuli and contexts. During fear acquisition, one context (either the office or library) was presented, counterbalanced on a between-subjects basis. This context remained on the screen for 2–7 s, followed by activation of the light (either pink or blue) for a further 2–7 s, ensuring that the epoch for each context and context + light paring amounted to 9 s in total. For the CS+, the US was delivered at 60% contingency (10 out of 16 trials), to coincide with CS offset. The other CS was presented 16 times, and was never associated with shock (forming the CS-). The 16 CS- trials were interleaved with the 16 CS+ trials. Each trial was followed by the presentation of a white fixation cross on a black background, jittered for a duration of 12–18 s. Fear extinction followed fear acquisition, and involved a switch in context (from office to library or vice versa). During extinction, the stimuli formerly associated with shock (CS+) were presented in the absence of shock (CS+E), interspersed with presentations of the CS- (see Figure 1). 16 presentations of the CS+E were interleaved with 16 CS- presentations. The 32 trials of fear acquisition and 32 trials of extinction learning were blocked into the first 16 (early) trials and the last 16 (late) trials. In order to isolate successfully acquired (fully learned) conditioning, we restricted our analysis of the acquisition phase to the late acquisition phase. Conversely, to maintain a focus on extinction learning rather than extinction retention, our analysis of the extinction phase was restricted to early extinction.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Fear conditioning and extinction procedures. Fear conditioning contingencies were established during acquisition, followed by extinction of the CS+ (to form CS+E).

fMRI Data Acquisition

Scans were collected on a 3.0 Tesla General Electric Signa® ExciteTM scanner (Milwaukee, WI, USA). After subjects were positioned in the scanner, a T1-weighted low resolution structural image was acquired approximately parallel to the AC-PC line [gradient recall echo sequence (GRE), repetition time (TR) = 250 ms, echo time (TE) = 5.7 ms, flip angle (FA) = 90°, 2 averages, field of view (FOV) = 22 cm, matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 3 mm, 40 axial slices to cover the whole brain], which was identical to the prescription of the functional acquisitions. Functional images were acquired with a T2*-weighted, reverse spiral acquisition sequence (gradient recall echo, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90°, FOV = 22 cm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 3 mm with no gap, 40 axial slices to cover the whole brain, acquisition voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm) which has been shown to minimize signal drop-out in regions such as ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex that are vulnerable to susceptibility artifact (Glover and Law, 2001). The intermediate template and fMRI images were acquired using a GE Quadrature sending and receiving head coil. Four initial volumes were discarded from each run to allow for equilibration of the scanner signal. A high-quality T1-weighted structural image was obtained with a 3-D volume inversion recovery fast spoiled gradient recalled echo (IR-FSPGR) protocol (TR = 12.3 ms, TE = 5.2 ms, FA = 9°, TI = 650 ms, FOV = 26 cm, matrix = 256 × 256 for in-plane resolution of 1 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm with no gap, 160 contiguous axial slices to cover the whole brain), using an eight-channel GE phase array receiving head coil. E-prime was used to present stimuli (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). Participants wore glasses with built-in mirrors (NordicNeuro Labs) in order to view the projected stimuli inside the scanner.

Preprocessing of fMRI Data

An initial series of preprocessing steps was carried out. First, we removed k-space outliers in raw data that were two standard deviations away from the mean and substituted them with the average value from neighboring voxels. Next, a B0 field map was used in the reconstruction of the images to remove the distortions that resulted from magnetic field inhomogeneity [IEEE-TIME, 10:629-637, 1991]. The variance due to physiological responses (i.e., cardiac and respiratory sources) was removed using regression (Glover et al., 2000). Additional preprocessing and image analysis was performed in SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk). The T2 overlay was co-registered to the functional images, and then the high-resolution T1 image was co-registered to overlay. T1 images were normalized to the scalped T1 template and the functional volumes were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using the previously computed transformation matrix. Images were smoothed using an isotropic 8 mm full-width-half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.

Analysis

fMRI comparisons of interest were implemented as linear contrasts. Realignment parameters were added as covariates of no interest at the first level. Z-score images from individual analyses were entered into second-level random-effects analyses (one-sample and two-sample t-tests) implemented in SPM5. Second-level maps were thresholded at p < 0.001, cluster-level corrected for multiple comparisons via family wise error correction. Regions of interests (ROIs) were selected from a systematic review of fMRI fear conditioning studies (Sehlmeyer et al., 2009), and defined using the automated anatomical labeling atlas (AAL). They included amygdala, hippocampus, vmPFC (bilateral medial orbital frontal gyrus), dmPFC (bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus), and insula. Only the clusters within the regions of interest that survived family wise error small volume correction were extracted and used for further analysis. Symptom severity was assessed via the CAPS, which consists of three subscales (reexperiencing symptoms, avoidance symptoms, and hyperarousal symptoms) that are summed for a total score. Bivariate correlations were computed between CAPS score and the extracted BOLD signal during fear acquisition and fear extinction.

Results

Subjects were successfully fear conditioned, as reported elsewhere (Garfinkel et al., unpublished). As predicted, fear conditioning activated a network of fear processing regions in response to CS+ presentation in both PTSD patients and Combat-exposed Controls. There were no between-group differences in brain activation patterns or skin conductance responses during the conditioning phase (Garfinkel et al., unpublished).

Correlations with Avoidance Symptoms

Fear Acquisition

To explore whether avoidance symptoms were associated with differential neural activation patterns during conditioning, the CAPS avoidance subscale was entered as a regressor in a whole brain analysis during the fear acquisition phase. During CS+ (as compared to CS-) greater CAPS avoidance was associated with greater activity in right hippocampus ([33, -27, -9], k = 9, z = 3.62, p = 0.025, SVC; see Table 1). This correlation remained significant after controlling for levels of reexperiencing and hyperarousal symptoms. No other significant associations were observed.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Correlations with CAPS avoidance symptoms.

Fear Extinction

To explore whether avoidance symptoms were associated with differential activation patterns during extinction learning, the CAPS avoidance subscale was entered as a regressor in a whole brain analysis during the fear extinction phase. During context presentation prior to CS presentation (as compared to fixation), avoidance was associated with greater activity in left hippocampus ([-21, -30, -3], k = 3, z = 3.42, p = 0.047, SVC), left insula ([-48, 6, -9], k = 21, z = 3.52, p = 0.05, SVC), and right amygdala ([24, 3, -24], k = 1, z = 3.22, p = 0.025, SVC; see Figure 2). Correlations with insula and amygdala remained significant after controlling for other PTSD symptom clusters. During CS+E (as compared to CS-), greater avoidance was associated with greater activity in right amygdala ([24, 3, -24], k = 5, z = 3.89, p = 0.002, SVC), vmPFC ([3, 45, -6], k = 14, z = 3.35, p = 0.05, SVC), dmPFC ([-6, 30, 57], k = 36, z = 3.77, p = 0.049, SVC), left insula ([-39, -18, -3], k = 2, z = 3.45, p = 0.05, SVC), and left hippocampus ([-24, -21, -12], k = 17, z = 3.79, p = 0.011, SVC; see Figure 3). The correlation with left hippocampus remained significant after controlling for other PTSD symptom clusters. Immediately following CS+E, during the period that involved shock administration while in the acquisition phase, greater avoidance was associated with greater activity in right amygdala ([27, 3, -27], k = 9, z = 3.62, p = 0.007, SVC), right insula ([42, 6, -6], k = 11, z = 3.66, p = 0.038, SVC), right hippocampus ([36, -30, -12], k = 26, z = 3.9, p = 0.009, SVC), left anterior insula ([-39, 15, -12], k = 70, z = 4.35, p = 0.004, SVC), left posterior insula ([-36, -18, 6], k = 52, z = 3.63, p = 0.042, SVC), and a trend for vmPFC ([33, -27, -9], k = 11, z = 3.4, p = 0.06, SVC; see Figure 4). Correlations with left and right insula remained significant after controlling for other PTSD symptom clusters. Whole brain activations are reported in Table 1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. During context presentation in the early extinction phase, CAPS avoidance symptoms were associated with increased activation in (A) left insula (y = 2) and (B) right amygdala (y = 7). During context presentation, CAPS total symptoms were associated with increased activation in (C) right hippocampus (x = 15), (D) left hippocampus (x = -22), and (E) left amygdala (y = -1). Activations presented at p < 0.00005. CAPS, clinician-administered PTSD scale.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. During CS+ presentation in the early extinction phase, CAPS avoidance symptoms were associated with increased activation in (A) left insula (y = -18), (B) right amygdala (y = 5), (C) vmPFC (x = 3), (D) dmPFC (x = -8), and (E) left hippocampus (x = -24). Activations presented at p < 0.00005. vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. During fixation after CS+ in the early extinction phase, CAPS avoidance symptoms were associated with increased activation in (A) left insula (y = -15), (B) right amygdala (y = 3), and (C) vmPFC (x = -6). During fixation after CS+, CAPS total symptoms were associated with increased activation in (D) right hippocampus (x = 36), (E) right insula and right amygdala (y = 0). Activations presented at p < 0.00005.

Correlations with Caps Total

To explore whether total PTSD symptoms were associated with differential activation patterns during fear acquisition and extinction learning, correlations were also computed between BOLD signal and CAPS total score. During extinction, in response to context presentation prior to CS, CAPS total was associated with greater activity in right hippocampus (2 clusters: [15, -36, 0], k = 4, z = 3.71, p = 0.017, SVC; [18, -33, -2], k = 2, z = 3.42, p = 0.039, SVC), left hippocampus (2 clusters: [-24, -30, -3], k = 23, z = 3.93, p = 0.011, SVC; [-27, -15, -12], k = 2, z = 3.45, p = 0.042, SVC), and left amygdala ([-30, 0, -27], k = 1, z = 3.21, p = 0.021, SVC; see Table 2; Figure 2). Left (p = 0.015, SVC) and right (p = 0.032, SVC) hippocampal activity was also significantly correlated with the sum of reexperiencing and hyperarousal symptom clusters (i.e., total CAPS score minus avoidance subscale). Immediately following CS+E, CAPS total was associated with greater activity in right amygdala (2 clusters: [33, -3, -27], k = 4, z = 3.35, p = 0.015, SVC; [27, 3, -27], k = 1, z = 3.10, p = 0.029, SVC), right hippocampus (2 clusters: [36, -9, -27], k = 37, z = 3.83, p = 0.012, SVC; [21, -36, 6], k = 6, z = 3.31, p = 0.05, SVC), and right insula (2 clusters: [45, 0, -6], k = 32, z = 4.39, p = 0.003, SVC; [33, -15, 6], k = 4, z = 3.59, p = 0.044, SVC; see Figure 4). Whole brain activations are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Correlations with CAPS total symptoms.

Correlations with Other Symptom Clusters

For completeness, we also conducted an exploratory analysis to investigate whether reexperiencing or hyperarousal symptoms were associated with unique activation patterns during fear acquisition and extinction learning. During extinction, in response to context presentation prior to CS, CAPS hyperarousal was associated with greater activity in left hippocampus ([-24, -24, -12], k = 27, z = 4.17, p = 0.004, SVC). Reexperiencing symptoms were not associated with differential activation in any ROI.

Time Course of ROI Activation

We implemented a series of repeated-measures ANOVAs to investigate the degree to which the different stages of extinction (context presentation, CS+E presentation, and immediate aftermath of CS+E presentation) activated key limbic and prefrontal regions. This analysis revealed that dmPFC activation varied by stage (F(26,2) = 3.82, p = 0.035). Post-hoc LSD tests showed that dmPFC exhibited greater activation during context processing (M = 0.26, SD = 0.67) than during CS+E presentation (M = -0.47, SD = 1.12; p = 0.048) or during the immediate aftermath of CS+E presentation (M = -0.16, SD = 0.72; p = 0.045). No other ROI demonstrated significantly different activation across stages.

Discussion

In this fMRI study, we investigated the neural underpinnings of the link between fear extinction learning and avoidant symptoms in PTSD. We found that amongst individuals with PTSD, greater avoidance symptomatology was associated with greater activation in emotion processing circuits in response to conditioned cues and contextual information, both during fear acquisition and fear extinction. This pattern was observed during presentation of context immediately prior to the CS (i.e., “context alone”), during the presentation of the CS+E, and immediately following the CS+E. Correlations with insula, amygdala, and hippocampus survived after controlling for other PTSD symptom clusters. Heightened responses to previously conditioned stimuli in individuals with more avoidant symptoms or more severe PTSD could indicate a deficiency in safety learning, consistent with PTSD symptomatology. The close link between avoidance symptoms and fear circuit activation suggests that this symptom cluster may be a key component of fear extinction deficits in PTSD and/or may be particularly amenable to change through extinction-based therapies.

The multimodal nature of this experiment enhanced its applicability to PTSD. Research demonstrates that multimodal trauma experiences may exacerbate PTSD symptoms, but also that multimodal treatment can enhance efficacy. For instance, extending the duration of context presentation (including tactile, visual, and olfactory cues) before foot-shock administration (in mice) increases generalization and avoidance (Sauerhofer et al., 2012). This finding was interpreted to suggest that fostering multimodal learning enhances conditioning (Sauerhofer et al., 2012). Conversely, emerging evidence suggests that individuals with high avoidance may benefit more from treatment when it incorporates multisensory trauma cues, which provides less opportunity for further avoidance (Rizzo et al., 2009; Norrholm and Jovanovic, 2010). For instance, in virtual reality exposure therapy, patients are immersed in simulations of trauma-relevant environments that allow for precise control of stimulus conditions. Directly delivering these multimodal cues can help circumvent clinical avoidance. Changes in PTSD symptom clusters, and particularly symptoms in the avoidant cluster, may be the key mechanism of PTSD treatment efficacy (Monson et al., 2012). Our paradigm is inherently multimodal because it involves manipulation of context (visual) and conditioned cues (tactile). Thus, it may be a more effective PTSD probe than unimodal paradigms. That avoidance symptoms were extensively correlated with brain activation while the other symptom clusters were not suggests that the multimodal nature of the paradigm was particularly effective at drawing out avoidant tendencies.

The current study suggests that avoidance symptoms are associated with hyperactivity in a variety of regions key to emotion processing and extinction learning (Sehlmeyer et al., 2009), including hippocampus, amygdala, insula, and medial prefrontal regions. In our data, avoidance symptoms were associated with greater hippocampal activity across both fear acquisition and fear extinction phases. Correlations between avoidance and hippocampal activity were observed during the presentation of context alone, during the presentation of previously conditioned cues, and immediately following the presentation of previously conditioned cues (during the period in which participants were shocked while in the acquisition phase). These findings are consistent with the role of hippocampus in contextual information processing (Maren et al., 2013) and with its role in “binding” contextual information with fear cues (Fanselow, 2000; Maren, 2001). Hippocampal activation was also associated with greater overall symptom severity, even after controlling for avoidance symptoms. This finding could help explain previous reports that PTSD patients exhibit greater hippocampal activity than healthy controls during fear acquisition and extinction learning (Bremner et al., 2005) or during non-fear related encoding (Werner et al., 2009). Interestingly, Milad et al. (2009) report reduced hippocampal activity in PTSD patients during extinction recall, which typically occurs the day after the conditioning and extinction phases. Conversely, in the present study, we found associations with avoidance during extinction learning, during the first 16 trials of extinction. This may reflect enhanced encoding or processing of conditioned associations formed during the acquisition phase in individuals with higher avoidance. This would suggest that higher avoidance is not only related to the expression of acquired fear, but also to fear learning. It is also possible that higher hippocampal activation in high avoidance patients reflects emotional rather than memory processing. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that anterior hippocampal regions in humans (which are analogous to ventral hippocampal regions in rodents; Moser and Moser, 1998), are involved in affect processing (Fanselow and Dong, 2010).

Avoidance symptom severity was also positively associated with amygdala and insula activity. These associations were present during context alone, during the presentation of previously conditioned cues, and immediately following conditioned cues. This too is consistent with previous animal and human findings. During extinction, high-anxious rats show hyperactivation (increased c-Fos expression) of the central nucleus of the amygdala (Muigg et al., 2008). Similarly, neuroimaging studies of individuals with PTSD report amygdala hyperactivity (Milad et al., 2009) and insula hyperactivity (Bremner et al., 2005) during extinction learning. Greater amygdala and insula activation during extinction is also correlated with trait anxiety (Barrett and Armony, 2009; Sehlmeyer et al., 2011). Insula and amygdala are key regions in salience detection and anticipation of negative events (Armony and LeDoux, 1997; Paulus and Stein, 2006). These regions are also associated with negative emotion production in PTSD, more generally (Shin and Liberzon, 2010). Greater activity in these emotion generation regions could thus reflect hyperactive fear responding to signals previously paired with negative outcomes. It could also reflect a failure to encode safety signals, or failure to adapt to or integrate new contextual information into previously learned contingencies (Liberzon and Sripada, 2008; Garfinkel and Liberzon, 2009).

We also found that avoidance symptom severity correlated with greater dmPFC and vmPFC activation. Greater dmPFC activity is consistent with previous findings of dmPFC/dACC hyperactivity in PTSD. For instance, Milad et al. (2009) report greater dACC activity during extinction recall in patients with PTSD, and Rougemont-Bucking et al. (2011) report exaggerated dACC activation in response to context presentation during late conditioning and early extinction. Other studies support that dmPFC/dACC hyperactivity in PTSD is also present during cognitive interference tasks such as oddball tasks (Bryant et al., 2005; Felmingham et al., 2009), Stroop tasks (Shin et al., 2007) and the Multi-Source Interference Task (Shin et al., 2011). The greater vmPFC activity found in highly avoidant patients, on the other hand, seemingly diverges from some previous reports of hypoactive vmPFC in PTSD (Bremner et al., 2005; Milad et al., 2009; Rougemont-Bucking et al., 2011). However, Barrett and Armony (2009) report that greater trait anxiety is associated with greater vmPFC activity during extinction. In our data, it is possible that vmPFC hyperactivity could represent a compensatory response to down-regulate amygdala activity. More broadly, the relationship between greater symptom severity and widespread hyperactivity across fear and emotion circuitry suggests that PTSD symptomatology is associated with greater neural reactivity during extinction learning. This hyperactivity may give rise to aberrant extinction retention.

Our findings suggest that PTSD symptoms and avoidance symptoms in particular are associated with exaggerated fear circuit activity. Previous fear conditioning studies in PTSD have largely focused on reexperiencing and hyperarousal symptom clusters. Reexperiencing symptoms have been demonstrated to be associated with greater fear-potentiated startle during fear acquisition and extinction (Glover et al., 2011; Norrholm et al., 2011). Hyperarousal symptoms, too, are associated with exaggerated fear responding (Jovanovic et al., 2010; Glover et al., 2011; Norrholm et al., 2011). One study reported that in response to script-driven imagery, avoidance symptoms were negatively correlated with vmPFC/rACC activation (Hopper et al., 2007). To our knowledge, however, ours is the first study to demonstrate a link between avoidance symptoms and greater reactivity to cue and context processing. Studies using animal models can provide important insight into the link between avoidance and fear extinction. Some investigators have suggested that the construct of avoidance involves both non-associative novelty fear, which is ameliorated by habituation, and stimulus-specific associative fear, which is ameliorated by extinction training (Pamplona et al., 2011). Generalized avoidance behavior in mice is reduced through both habituation and through extinction training (Costanzi et al., 2011; Pamplona et al., 2011), suggesting that both novelty fear and stimulus-specific fear contribute to avoidance behavior. Our data is consistent with this dual conceptualization, since it demonstrates that avoidance is associated with exaggerated limbic responding to both context (novel) and cues (conditioned). Individuals with greater avoidance symptoms may be more sensitive to both types of stimuli.

The relationship between fear extinction deficits and avoidance symptoms in PTSD might be bidirectional. Previous studies suggest that extinction deficits can lead to the development of avoidance symptoms, and conversely that pre-existent “higher avoidance” can be a contributor to extinction deficits. In support of the first hypothesis, greater fear in response to aversive stimuli is associated with greater levels of subsequent avoidance in rats (Chen et al., 2012). Additionally, pre-trauma deficits in extinction learning are associated with greater risk for developing PTSD after trauma in Dutch soldiers (Lommen et al., 2013). Thus, our findings of greater activity in fear circuits could reflect a mechanism by which individuals develop greater avoidance symptomatology, and provide additional support for the notion of harnessing fear extinction for the purpose of effective avoidance reduction. Alternatively, avoidance could precede fear extinction deficits, in that avoidance symptoms could result in greater fear responding (or amygdala hyperactivity) when confronted with fear-related stimuli that are usually avoided. Theoretical models of PTSD suggest that chronic avoidance leads to greater intensity of avoided cognitions and emotions (Hayes et al., 1999). Additionally, Aderka et al. (2013) recently reported that fear and avoidance predict each other during cognitive-behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder. Our findings could also reflect resistance to extinction in individuals with greater avoidance. There is evidence to suggest that individuals with greater avoidance have poorer response to CBT (Taylor et al., 2001) and greater rates of attrition (Glynn et al., 1999), though other studies have found that avoidant coping predicts better response to exposure therapy (Leiner et al., 2012). Furthermore, avoidance symptoms may not be as responsive to trauma-focused treatment as other PTSD symptom clusters (Glynn et al., 1999). Longitudinal studies are needed to determine whether fear circuit hyperactivity during extinction is better understood as a risk factor for avoidance symptoms or as a consequence of these symptoms.

Our study had several limitations. First, our paradigm is multimodal in that it involves both visual and tactile cues. However, the conditioned stimuli were primarily visual in nature. Thus, future studies on the relationship between PTSD symptoms and fear conditioning abnormalities could use additional modalities, such as olfactory cues, to further probe the multimodal nature of the link between avoidance symptoms and fear extinction. Second, the design used in this study, i.e., fear conditioning followed by fear extinction, does not allow us to clearly disambiguate the effects of extinction learning from the potential effects of differential recall of CS+ memory trace. For example, the amygdala hyperactivity we observed during extinction may reflect either extinction learning or recall of the CS+ conditioning (see Quirk and Mueller, 2008). Similarly, greater hippocampal activity during both conditioning and extinction learning in avoidant individuals could indicate overconsolidation of fear during conditioning, greater recall of conditioning in the extinction phase, or overgeneralization of fear expression into a neutral context. Future studies could use on-line expectancy ratings to help distinguish between these alternatives. A full factorial design utilizing both new and previously viewed contexts in conjunction with new and previously viewed cues would also be helpful in distinguishing novelty fear from the effects of conditioning. From a clinical perspective, however, PTSD symptoms could be similarly exacerbated by either deficient extinction learning or excessive acquisition-related fear. As such, this phase of extinction may provide a valuable target for research on treatment-enhancing approaches.

In conclusion, our results demonstrate that individuals with greater levels of avoidance exhibit hyperactivation in brain regions involved in fear expression during the presentation of previously conditioned cues and contextual information. This represents a potential brain-based mechanism contributing to the maintenance of fear memories in PTSD patients. Our findings suggest that ameliorating impaired inhibition of fear is an important treatment target for PTSD, in particular for PTSD patients with high levels of avoidance.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this article was supported by grants from the Michigan Institute for Clinical and Health Research (U028028) to SG, from the National Institute of Mental Health (R24 MH075999) to IL, and from the Telemedicine and Advanced Technology Research Center (W81XWH-08-2-0208) to IL.

References

Aderka, I. M., Mclean, C. P., Huppert, J. D., Davidson, J. R., and Foa, E. B. (2013). Fear, avoidance and physiological symptoms during cognitive-behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 352–358. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

APA, (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Armony, J. L., and LeDoux, J. E. (1997). How the brain processes emotional information. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 821, 259–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48285.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barrett, J., and Armony, J. L. (2009). Influence of trait anxiety on brain activity during the acquisition and extinction of aversive conditioning. Psychol. Med. 39, 255–265. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003516

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., et al. (1995). The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. J. Trauma. Stress 8, 75–90. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490080106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bremner, J. D., Vermetten, E., Schmahl, C., Vaccarino, V., Vythilingam, M., Afzal, N., et al. (2005). Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Med. 35, 791–806. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003290

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bryant, R. A., Felmingham, K. L., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M., Peduto, A. S., Rennie, C., et al. (2005). Neural networks of information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bryant, R. A., Marosszeky, J. E., Crooks, J., Baguley, I., and Gurka, J. (2000). Coping style and post-traumatic stress disorder following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 14, 175–180. doi: 10.1080/026990500120826

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, X., Li, Y., Li, S., and Kirouac, G. J. (2012). Early fear as a predictor of avoidance in a rat model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 226, 112–117. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Costanzi, M., Cannas, S., Saraulli, D., Rossi-Arnaud, C., and Cestari, V. (2011). Extinction after retrieval: effects on the associative and nonassociative components of remote contextual fear memory. Learn. Mem. 18, 508–518. doi: 10.1101/lm.2175811

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fanselow, M. S. (2000). Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behav. Brain Res. 110, 73–81. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00186-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fanselow, M. S., and Dong, H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Felmingham, K. L., Williams, L. M., Kemp, A. H., Rennie, C., Gordon, E., and Bryant, R. A. (2009). Anterior cingulate activity to salient stimuli is modulated by autonomic arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 173, 59–62. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garfinkel, S. N., and Liberzon, I. (2009). Neurobiology of PTSD: a review of neuroimaging findings. Psychiatr. Ann. 39, 370–381. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20090527-01

CrossRef Full Text

Glover, E. M., Phifer, J. E., Crain, D. F., Norrholm, S. D., Davis, M., Bradley, B., et al. (2011). Tools for translational neuroscience: PTSD is associated with heightened fear responses using acoustic startle but not skin conductance measures. Depress. Anxiety 28, 1058–1066. doi: 10.1002/da.20880

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glover, G. H., and Law, C. S. (2001). Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts. Magn. Reson. Med. 46, 515–522. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1222

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glover, G. H., Li, T. Q., and Ress, D. (2000). Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 162–167. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::AID-MRM23>3.0.CO;2-E

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glynn, S. M., Eth, S., Randolph, E. T., Foy, D. W., Urbaitis, M., Boxer, L., et al. (1999). A test of behavioral family therapy to augment exposure for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 67, 243–251. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.67.2.243

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., and Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change. New York, USA: Guilford Press.

Hendrix, C. C., Erdmann, M. A., and Briggs, K. (1998). Impact of Vietnam veterans’ arousal and avoidance on spouses’ perceptions of family life. Am. J. Fam. Ther. 26, 115–128. doi: 10.1080/01926189808251092

CrossRef Full Text

Hopper, J. W., Frewen, P. A., Van Der Kolk, B. A., and Lanius, R. A. (2007). Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 713–725. doi: 10.1002/jts.20284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jovanovic, T., Norrholm, S. D., Blanding, N. Q., Davis, M., Duncan, E., Bradley, B., et al. (2010). Impaired fear inhibition is a biomarker of PTSD but not depression. Depress. Anxiety 27, 244–251. doi: 10.1002/da.20663

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jovanovic, T., Norrholm, S. D., Fennell, J. E., Keyes, M., Fiallos, A. M., Myers, K. M., et al. (2009). Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity. Psychiatry Res. 167, 151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kuhn, E., Blanchard, E. B., and Hickling, E. J. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder and psychosocial functioning within two samples of MVA survivors. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 1105–1112. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00071-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lauterbach, D., Bak, C., Reiland, S., Mason, S., Lute, M. R., and Earls, L. (2007). Quality of parental relationships among persons with a lifetime history of posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 161–172. doi: 10.1002/jts.20194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leiner, A. S., Kearns, M. C., Jackson, J. L., Astin, M. C., and Rothbaum, B. O. (2012). Avoidant coping and treatment outcome in rape-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 80, 317–321. doi: 10.1037/a0026814

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liberzon, I., and Sripada, C. S. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy of PTSD: a critical review. Prog. Brain Res. 167, 151–169. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)67011-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lissek, S., Powers, A. S., Mcclure, E. B., Phelps, E. A., Woldehawariat, G., Grillon, C., et al. (2005). Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 1391–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lommen, M. J., Engelhard, I. M., Sijbrandij, M., Van Den Hout, M. A., and Hermans, D. (2013). Pre-trauma individual differences in extinction learning predict posttraumatic stress. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lunney, C. A., and Schnurr, P. P. (2007). Domains of quality of life and symptoms in male veterans treated for posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 955–964. doi: 10.1002/jts.20269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mahan, A. L., and Ressler, K. J. (2012). Fear conditioning, synaptic plasticity and the amygdala: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Neurosci. 35, 24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malta, L. S., Levitt, J. T., Martin, A., Davis, L., and Cloitre, M. (2009). Correlates of functional impairment in treatment-seeking survivors of mass terrorism. Behav. Ther. 40, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.12.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maren, S. (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maren, S., Phan, K. L., and Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Lasko, N. B., Chang, Y., Rauch, S. L., and Pitman, R. K. (2008). Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: results of a twin study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 1075–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milad, M. R., Quinn, B. T., Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Fischl, B., and Rauch, S. L. (2005). Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 10706–10711. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502441102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milad, M. R., Wright, C. I., Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., Quirk, G. J., and Rauch, S. L. (2007). Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 446–454. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Monson, C. M., Macdonald, A., Vorstenbosch, V., Shnaider, P., Goldstein, E. S., Ferrier-Auerbach, A. G., et al. (2012). Changes in social adjustment with cognitive processing therapy: effects of treatment and association with PTSD symptom change. J. Trauma. Stress 25, 519–526. doi: 10.1002/jts.21735

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moser, M. B., and Moser, E. I. (1998). Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 608–619. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<608::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muigg, P., Hetzenauer, A., Hauer, G., Hauschild, M., Gaburro, S., Frank, E., et al. (2008). Impaired extinction of learned fear in rats selectively bred for high anxiety – evidence of altered neuronal processing in prefrontal-amygdala pathways. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 2299–2309. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06511.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Norrholm, S. D., and Jovanovic, T. (2010). Tailoring therapeutic strategies for treating posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 6, 517–532. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S10951

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Norrholm, S. D., Jovanovic, T., Olin, I. W., Sands, L. A., Karapanou, I., Bradley, B., et al. (2011). Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 556–563. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

North, C. S., Nixon, S. J., Shariat, S., Mallonee, S., Mcmillen, J. C., Spitznagel, E. L., et al. (1999). Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing. JAMA 282, 755–762. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.8.755

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

North, C. S., Oliver, J., and Pandya, A. (2012). Examining a comprehensive model of disaster-related posttraumatic stress disorder in systematically studied survivors of 10 disasters. Am. J. Public Health 102, e40–e48. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300689

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Peri, T., and Pitman, R. K. (2000). De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 290–298. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.290

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pamplona, F. A., Henes, K., Micale, V., Mauch, C. P., Takahashi, R. N., and Wotjak, C. T. (2011). Prolonged fear incubation leads to generalized avoidance behavior in mice. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 354–360. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2006). An insular view of anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Quirk, G. J., and Mueller, D. (2008). Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Riggs, D. S., Byrne, C. A., Weathers, F. W., and Litz, B. T. (1998). The quality of the intimate relationships of male Vietnam veterans: problems associated with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 11, 87–101. doi: 10.1023/A:1024409200155

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzo, A., Reger, G., Gahm, G., Difede, J., and Rothbaum, B. O. (2009). “Virtual reality exposure therapy for combat-related PTSD,” in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Basic Science and Clinical Practice, eds P. J. Shiromani, T. M. Keane, and J. E. Ledoux (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press), 375–399. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60327-329-9_18

CrossRef Full Text

Rougemont-Bucking, A., Linnman, C., Zeffiro, T. A., Zeidan, M. A., Lebron-Milad, K., Rodriguez-Romaguera, J., et al. (2011). Altered processing of contextual information during fear extinction in PTSD: an fMRI study. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 17, 227–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00152.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruscio, A. M., Weathers, F. W., King, L. A., and King, D. W. (2002). Male war-zone veterans’ perceived relationships with their children: the importance of emotional numbing. J. Trauma. Stress 15, 351–357. doi: 10.1023/A:1020125006371

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Samper, R. E., Taft, C. T., King, D. W., and King, L. A. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and parenting satisfaction among a national sample of male vietnam veterans. J. Trauma. Stress 17, 311–315. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000038479.30903.ed

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sauerhofer, E., Pamplona, F. A., Bedenk, B., Moll, G. H., Dawirs, R. R., Von Horsten, S., et al. (2012). Generalization of contextual fear depends on associative rather than non-associative memory components. Behav. Brain Res. 233, 483–493. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schnurr, P. P., and Lunney, C. A. (2008). Exploration of gender differences in how quality of life relates to posttraumatic stress disorder in male and female veterans. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 45, 383–393. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2007.06.0099

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sehlmeyer, C., Dannlowski, U., Schoning, S., Kugel, H., Pyka, M., Pfleiderer, B., et al. (2011). Neural correlates of trait anxiety in fear extinction. Psychol. Med. 41, 789–798. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sehlmeyer, C., Schoning, S., Zwitserlood, P., Pfleiderer, B., Kircher, T., Arolt, V., et al. (2009). Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 4:e5865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005865

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20), S22–S33; quiz 34–57.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Shin, L. M., Bush, G., Milad, M. R., Lasko, N. B., Brohawn, K. H., Hughes, K. C., et al. (2011). Exaggerated activation of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex during cognitive interference: a monozygotic twin study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 979–985. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.09121812

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shin, L. M., Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Handwerger, K., Cannistraro, P. A., Wright, C. I., et al. (2007). Dorsal anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 701–712. doi: 10.1002/jts.20231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shin, L. M., and Liberzon, I. (2010). The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 169–191. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.83

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Solomon, Z., and Mikulincer, M. (2007). Posttraumatic intrusion, avoidance, and social functioning: a 20-year longitudinal study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75, 316–324. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.2.316

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sripada, R. K., King, A. P., Garfinkel, S. N., Wang, X., Sripada, C. S., Welsh, R. C., et al. (2012a). Altered resting-state amygdala functional connectivity in men with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 241–249. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sripada, R. K., King, A. P., Welsh, R. C., Garfinkel, S. N., Wang, X., Sripada, C. S., et al. (2012b). Neural dysregulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence for disrupted equilibrium between salience and default mode brain networks. Psychosom. Med. 74, 904–911. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318273bf33

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, S., Fedoroff, I. C., Koch, W. J., Thordarson, D. S., Fecteau, G., and Nicki, R. M. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder arising after road traffic collisions: patterns of response to cognitive-behavior therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69, 541–551. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.3.541

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Werner, N. S., Meindl, T., Engel, R. R., Rosner, R., Riedel, M., Reiser, M., et al. (2009). Hippocampal function during associative learning in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 309–318. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: fear conditioning, avoidance, posttraumatic stress disorder, fMRI, neuroimaging, amygdala, hippocampus

Citation: Sripada RK, Garfinkel SN and Liberzon I (2013) Avoidant symptoms in PTSD predict fear circuit activation during multimodal fear extinction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:672. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00672

Received: 14 June 2013; Accepted: 26 September 2013;
Published online: 17 October 2013.

Edited by:

Martin Klasen, Rheinisch-Westfälische Technische Hochschule, Aachen University, Germany

Reviewed by:

Tanja Jovanovic, Emory University, USA
Gregory J. Quirk, University of Puerto Rico School of Medicine, Puerto Rico

Copyright © 2013 Sripada, Garfinkel and Liberzon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rebecca K. Sripada, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Road, 2702 Rachel Upjohn Building, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA e-mail: rekaufma@umich.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.