Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 02 September 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Hemispheric Bases for Emotion and Memory View all 9 articles

Hemispheric lateralization interrupted: material-specific memory deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy

  • 1Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
  • 3Golby Lab, A Surgical Brain Mapping Laboratory, Department of Neurosurgery, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA

The hemispheric lateralization of memory has largely been informed through the study of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy originating from medial temporal sources (mTLE). The material-specific model of memory relies on the basic framework that the left temporal lobe mediates verbal memories, while the right temporal lobe mediates non-verbal memories. Over the years, this model has been refined, and even challenged, as our understanding of the material-specific memory deficits in mTLE has been further elaborated in the neuropsychological and neuroimaging literature. The first goal of this mini-review is to highlight the major findings in the mTLE literature that have advanced and expanded our understanding of material-specific memory deficits in mTLE. Second, we will review how functional neuroimaging patterns of material-specific hemispheric lateralization in mTLE are being translated into the innovative clinical application of preoperative fMRI memory mapping.

Introduction

Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common form of localization-related epilepsy in adults, accounting for approximately 60% of cases (Hauser et al., 1991). The most common site of epileptogenesis in TLE originates from medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures, including the hippocampus and anterior subregions (including anterior parahippocampal cortex) (McMillan et al., 1987; Meencke and Veith, 1992; Wolf et al., 1993; Blume, 2006). Medial TLE (mTLE) is often poorly controlled with medications and class I evidence supports the use of surgical resections, specifically anterior temporal lobe resections (ATLR). Both the underlying disease focus and surgical management of mTLE can lead to significant disruption of function within regions and networks that are critically involved in memory function (Fisher et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2002; Sabsevitz et al., 2003; Stroup et al., 2003; Gleissner et al., 2004; Baxendale et al., 2006; Lineweaver et al., 2006; Binder et al., 2008; Hermann and Seidenberg, 2008). The study of memory deficits resulting from the underlying pathology and surgical management of mTLE has been the foundation for much of our understanding regarding memory function (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Penfield and Milner, 1958; Milner, 1970). One of the most influential frameworks describes the hemispheric specialization of memory based on verbal and non-verbal characteristics. The material-specific model dates back to Brenda Milner’s observation that post-operative lesions in the left MTL resulted in abnormalities in verbal memory, while right temporal lobe lesions caused reductions in memory for non-verbal material (assuming left cerebral language dominance) (Milner, 1966). Based on the foundation of material-specific memory deficits, our understanding of hemispheric lateralization of memory function has expanded over time (for a review, see Saling, 2009) and continues to be a heuristic employed in clinical practice and surgical decision making in mTLE (Baxendale, 2008). In fact, material-specific fMRI patterns of hemispheric asymmetry are currently being investigated as a means to estimate the risk for memory decline following ATLR in mTLE. Unlike other reviews on the material-specific memory in mTLE, we focus that later part of this mini-review on the translation of these imaging methods to preoperative clinical mapping.

yes

A number of neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated a relationship between material-specific memory impairment and lateralized MTL dysfunction in mTLE (Jones-Gotman, 1986; Seidenberg et al., 1996; Helmstaedter et al., 1997, 2003; Jones-Gotman et al., 1997, 2010; Gleissner et al., 1998; Pillon et al., 1999; Golby et al., 2002; Powell et al., 2005). However, inconsistencies have also been reported (for a review, see Saling, 2009) and our understanding of the functional specialization of temporal regions in material-specific memory and the participation of these functionally specialized nodes in a larger network framework of memory processes is still evolving. The variability in the material-specific findings in mTLE has also broadened our understanding of mTLE as a network disease that can affect the function of or connections between any number of nodes that are critical for memory. For instance, material-specific memory deficits in pre- and post-operative mTLE can result from focal dysfunction in subregions within the MTL, including the hippocampus and parahippocampal and entorhinal cortices, dysfunction within the lateral temporal lobe, and disconnections between MTL and ipsilateral cortical connections. mTLE has also been associated with patterns of disrupted functional localization and lateralization resulting from functional reorganization or compensatory regional involvement (Adcock et al., 2003; Thivard et al., 2005), which has allowed for the investigation of the functional implications of neuroplastic changes.

Material-Specific Memory Deficits and mTLE

The link between verbal memory deficits and the language-dominant mTLE is one of the most consistent patterns of material-specific memory deficits in mTLE (Jones-Gotman et al., 2000), although variability in verbal memory findings can be elicited by accounting for certain task-specific factors (Saling et al., 2002; Saling, 2009). In the non-verbal domain, evidence for a specific link between the right temporal lobe and spatial memory is considerably weaker. Some have argued that visual-spatial memory is a bilateral process, which may largely be mediated by the verbalizability of the information to be learned (van Asselen et al., 2006). To-date, pre- and post-operative ATLR studies using neuropsychological and neuroimaging techniques have produced a myriad of inconclusive data that support non-lateralized findings, including measures of navigation (Maguire et al., 1996; Jokeit et al., 2001), maze learning (Bohbot et al., 1998; Astur et al., 2002), scene recognition (Maguire and Cipolotti, 1998; Spiers et al., 2001a), plan drawing (Spiers et al., 2001b), abstract design (Piguet et al., 1994; Dige and Wik, 2001), and faces (Hermann et al., 1997; Reminger et al., 2004). Furthermore, a number of standard tests of visuospatial memory, including the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, commonly used in evaluation of mTLE, do not reliably distinguish between left and right mTLE (Lee et al., 2002; McConley et al., 2008).

Studies focused on understanding the inconsistencies in the patterns of material-specific memory deficits in mTLE have found evidence for functional dissociations based on the variable influence of extratemporal, intratemporal, and intra-MTL regional dysfunction and highlight the larger network context that can be disrupted by mTLE pathology and surgical management.

Extratemporal Cortical Influences

Extratemporal neocortical and connectivity abnormalities in mTLE have been reported both contralateral and ipsilateral to the side of seizure onset (Oyegbile et al., 2004; McDonald et al., 2008; Mueller et al., 2010; Ji et al., 2013). Below we discuss three examples of extratemporal influence over material-specific findings in unilateral mTLE.

Frontal lobe language functions

In cases of language-dominant mTLE, extent of hemispheric disruption has the potential to impact language functioning or adequacy (Hermann et al., 1988, 1992). It has been suggested that the strong connections between the inferior frontal cortex and the MTL make frontal lobe language functions particularly sensitive to MTL pathology (Powell et al., 2004). In fact, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) evidence for reduced left hemisphere language connections in patients with left mTLE compared to both controls and right mTLE patients has been reported (Powell et al., 2007). Functionally, language adequacy has been shown to act as a significant confound when lateralizing verbal memory encoding (Hermann et al., 1988, 1992; Saling et al., 2002). That is, when controlling for language adequacy on a word-list learning task, performance does not correspond with side of seizure focus (right or left) (Hermann et al., 1988, 1992; Saling et al., 2002). Therefore, list-learning performance appears to be less specific to unilateral MTL pathology, and instead corresponds more strongly to extratemporal language processes. Interestingly, retention of verbal information over a delay period has been shown to be less influenced by language adequacy making it a stronger marker of MTL function in left mTLE (Hermann et al., 1988, 1992; Saling et al., 2002).

Frontal lobe memory functions

In addition to material-specific patterns of hemispheric asymmetry, neuroimaging studies have found evidence for hemispheric asymmetries within the frontal lobes that corresponds to the component stages of memory (encoding and retrieval) (for reviews, see Desgranges et al., 1998; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Lepage et al., 2000; Fletcher and Henson, 2001). The “hemispheric encoding and retrieval asymmetry” (HERA) model argues that there are observable hemispheric differences according to the stage of memory processing – encoding being preferentially associated with left frontal regions, and memory retrieval showing greater right-sided frontal activation regardless of material type (Desgranges et al., 1998; McDermott et al., 1999; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Lepage et al., 2000; Nyberg et al., 2000; Fletcher and Henson, 2001; Grady et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2003). Kennepohl et al. (2007) reported convincing evidence within the MTL that aligns with the HERA pattern – the left entorhinal cortex was significantly more active than the right regardless of material type during encoding trials. They found less convincing evidence for the lateralization of retrieval to the right MTL. The work of Kennepohl et al. (2007) raises the possibility that some variability associated with material-specific memory deficits in mTLE could be accounted for by other factors related to hemispheric asymmetries. Future studies in mTLE could be designed to take the HERA pattern into account to further deconstruct the functional specialization of MTL regions and their relationship to post-operative memory outcomes.

Parietal and striatal contributions

The neuroimaging literature suggests that a consistent network of brain regions participates in spatial navigation, including the hippocampus proper, the parietal lobe, striatum, occipitotemporal regions, cingulate cortex, and parahippocampal cortices (Aguirre et al., 1996, 1998; Maguire, 1997; Maguire et al., 1998; Jokeit et al., 2001). Two basic strategies in spatial navigation have been described – egocentric and allocentric (Iaria et al., 2003). Allocentric navigation strategies use landmarks and the spatial relationship to a target in order to create a visual map of the environment. Animal and human studies have found support to link allocentric navigation to MTL structures, including the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Morris et al., 1982; Bohbot et al., 1998, 2004). Egocentric navigation, which describes determining the position of targets in an environment relative to one’s body or position, has been localized to regions within the parietal lobe and striatum in animals and humans (Wiener, 1993; Weniger et al., 2009). Therefore, task-specific features and the extent of potential extratemporal mTLE-related pathology, mainly along MTL-parietal and MTL-striatal connections, may account for variability of visuospatial memory in mTLE.

Spatial navigation studies have also shown patterns of dynamic interaction between right and left temporal regions, and both left and right mTLE patients have demonstrated impairment (Maguire et al., 1996; Astur et al., 2002; Glikmann-Johnston et al., 2008; Canovas et al., 2011). Similarly, measures of object-location have found evidence for bilateral temporal contributions (Baxendale et al., 1998; Incisa della Rochetta et al., 2004; Kessels et al., 2004; Stepankova et al., 2004); although, most studies suggest that the ability to navigate, learn, and recall arbitrarily related objects and locations is right lateralized (Abrahams et al., 1997, 1999; Bohbot et al., 1998; Johnsrude et al., 1999; Duzel et al., 2003; Crane and Milner, 2005; Parslow et al., 2005; Diaz-Asper et al., 2006; Piekema et al., 2006). Overall, the potential bilateral nature of spatial memory may limit the detection of material-specific patterns, particularly during spatial navigation tasks.

Intratemporal Organization

Medial and lateral divisions of the temporal lobes are variably affected by mTLE and surgical resections (Fisher et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2002; Sabsevitz et al., 2003; Stroup et al., 2003; Gleissner et al., 2004; Baxendale et al., 2006; Lineweaver et al., 2006; Binder et al., 2008; Hermann and Seidenberg, 2008) and can influence the nature and extent of memory deficits. Below we discuss the findings related to the underlying semantic structure of verbal memory tasks and contributions of task-specific factors to material-specific verbal memory deficits in mTLE.

Semantic-arbitrary distinction

Performance on verbal memory tasks consistently varies relative to the degree the task draws on pre-existing semantic associations (Saling et al., 2002; Saling, 2009). Verbal paired-associate learning studies have shown that patients with language-dominant seizure foci are typically impaired when it comes to learning arbitrary word-pairs. This is known as the semantic-arbitrary distinction and is also supported by studies of story memory (Rausch and Babb, 1987; Saling et al., 1993). However, language-dominant MTL seizure foci do not reliably interfere with learning semantically related word-pairs (Saling et al., 1993, 2002). Similarly, both patients with right and left mTLE tend to be mildly impaired on story memory tasks, which are rich in semantic context (Saling et al., 1993, 2002). Findings related to the semantic-arbitrary distinction also extend to list-learning tasks that allow for semantic clustering (Helmstaedter et al., 1997; Saling et al., 2002), such that the presence of a semantic clustering component may mask a performance deficit on a list-learning task and interfere with the detection of memory impairment linked to underlying language-dominant MTL dysfunction.

In contrast, arbitrary paired-associate learning has been correlated with imaging findings in left mesial temporal structures, specifically the perirhinal cortex (Lillywhite et al., 2007) and the arbitrary-semantic effect has been confirmed in left mTLE patients following standard en bloc resections (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1996; Helmstaedter et al., 1997; Saling et al., 2002). More specifically, a lateral to medial gradient has been reported, such that medial structures have been shown to play a critical role in forming arbitrary associations, but are not necessary if semantic associations are pre-existing (Saling et al., 2002). In the pre-existing semantic conditions, cortical or lateral regions are recruited due to their involvement in housing semantic stores (Lillywhite et al., 2007; Saling, 2009).

Intra-MTL Regional Specialization

The exact nature of the discrete functional contributions of MTL structures to memory processes remains a matter of intense debate, particularly within the literature on component processes of recognition memory (Aggleton and Brown, 2006; Eichenbaum et al., 2007, 2012; Squire et al., 2007; Henke, 2010). Below we discuss the predominant models of intra-MTL functional specialization during encoding and recognition.

Item-in-context model of encoding

Based on the collection of findings from the recollection and familiarity literature (for reviews, see Davachi, 2006; Diana et al., 2007; Eichenbaum et al., 2007), Eichenbaum et al. (2012) proposed a model of episodic memory encoding or “item-in-context” memory. Work in both animals and humans, argues that cortical inputs along the ventral “what” stream first process information regarding objects and events in perirhinal and lateral entorhinal regions, while information regarding spatial context from the dorsal “where” stream is processed by the medial entorhinal and parahippocampal cortex (Suzuki and Amaral, 1994; Furtak et al., 2007; Kerr et al., 2007; van Strien et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2011). Encoding of episodic memories is then proposed to involve the binding or convergence of object and context information from these regions within the hippocampus. Retrieval of episodic memories when cued by either an object or context would reactivate the bound representation within the hippocampus and retrieve the corresponding item or context information by reactivating the “what” and “where” streams (Eichenbaum et al., 2012). The item-in-context model of encoding is consistent with the idea of a medial-lateral gradient of temporal lobe functional specialization discussed above, wherein medial structures are necessary to form novel or arbitrary associations.

Recollection and familiarity

Most agree that recognition memory consists of two processes: recollection and familiarity. Recollection refers to the recognition of a stimulus that is bound to contextual details, while familiarity describes an awareness of a stimulus in the absence of contextual details from the original encoding episode (for reviews, see Yonelinas and Levy, 2002; Skinner and Fernandes, 2007). The dual process model suggests that these two processes are mediated by distinct MTL systems; with recollection linked to hippocampal function and familiarity associated with the perirhinal cortex (Aggleton and Brown, 1999, 2006; Yonelinas and Levy, 2002; Eichenbaum et al., 2007). The study of surgical mTLE patients has made a strong case for the dual process model. In fact, Bowles et al. (2007, 2010) have demonstrated a double dissociation, wherein standard ATLR mTLE patients exhibited isolated impairments in recollection and sparing of familiarity processes, while patient NB, who underwent a resection localized to the perirhinal and entorhinal cortices, displayed a selective familiarity impairment (Bowles et al., 2007, 2010). A number of functional imaging studies have also reported that activation of the hippocampus occurs with episodic recollection and memory for associations and context, whereas activation of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex is linked with familiarity (for reviews, see Davachi, 2006; Diana et al., 2007; Eichenbaum et al., 2007). Bowles et al. (2010) also showed that overall recognition performance was comparable between the ATLR patients and patient NB, which provides evidence against the main counterargument to the dual process model that states the functional specialization of MTL regions is primarily related to memory strength (Squire et al., 2004, 2007; Wais et al., 2008).

fMRI Memory Mapping for Preoperative Prediction of Memory Outcomes

Mapping brain memory patterns using Blood oxygen dependent (BOLD) fMRI has been gaining momentum in clinical practice as a means to estimate the risk for memory decline following ATLR in mTLE. The foundation for clinical fMRI mapping is based on the patterns of material-specific findings discussed above, as well as patterns of neuroplasticity and reorganization and their relationship to outcome measures. While there are still methodological and even material-specific challenges (Jansen et al., 2009; Binder, 2011), preoperative fMRI memory mapping has the potential to be a promising addition to the current methods of outcome prediction and as a non-invasive alternative to the intracarotid amytal test (IAT).

yes

Neuroimaging studies in mTLE have revealed functionally asymmetric patterns of activation within MTL structures contralateral to the seizure foci during material-specific encoding (Golby et al., 2002; Powell et al., 2007; Banks et al., 2012; Alessio et al., 2013; Sidhu et al., 2013). Functional asymmetries have been associated with variability in neural reorganization within different populations of mTLE patients (Mechanic-Hamilton et al., 2009), with greater asymmetries in mTLE patients with an early age of onset compared to patients with later onset (corresponding to the literature on language lateralization in mTLE). However, it is unclear how well patterns of contralateral activation reflect compensatory activation, as performance levels do not always equate between control subjects and patients (Richardson et al., 2006; Powell et al., 2007). As a result, some have argued that contralateral activation is more appropriately regarded as a marker of network disruption (Powell et al., 2007).

Predictive studies of memory outcomes following ATLR have generally relied on two perspectives to guide decision making (Chelune et al., 1991; Chelune, 1995). The functional reserve hypothesis proposes that the level of functioning of the hippocampus contralateral to the seizure focus will determine the memory outcome. In contrast, the functional adequacy model predicts that post-operative memory outcome will be inversely related to the level of preoperative functioning of the tissue to be resected (Chelune, 1995; Chelune and Najm, 2000).

The functional adequacy model is supported by findings that demonstrate mTLE patients with better preoperative memory functioning are at greater risk for significant memory declines, than patients with low average or poor preoperative memory functioning (Chelune et al., 1991; Hermann et al., 1995; Helmstaedter and Elger, 1996; Jokeit et al., 1997; Davies et al., 1998; Stroup et al., 2003; Gleissner et al., 2004; Baxendale et al., 2006, 2007; Lineweaver et al., 2006; Bonelli et al., 2010). fMRI studies have also been supportive of the functional adequacy model, suggesting that patients with greater ipsilateral activation compared to contralateral MTL activation have greater memory decline following temporal lobectomy (Binder et al., 2008; Frings et al., 2008; Powell et al., 2008; Bonelli et al., 2010; Dupont et al., 2010). Interestingly, a recent DTI study has also shown that stronger connectivity between a default mode network node in the posterior cingulate cortex and the epileptogenic hippocampus, compared to the contralateral hippocampus, has also been linked to greater post-operative memory decline, suggesting a broader network adequacy model (McCormick et al., 2013).

The functional reserve hypothesis has received less support; however, recent neuroimaging work has found evidence for a more nuanced understanding of the functional reserve hypothesis based on anterior and posterior divisions of the MTL (Bonelli et al., 2010). Support for the functional adequacy model was found in patterns of anterior MTL activation, such that the greater asymmetry toward the left was associated with greater decline in verbal memory (Bonelli et al., 2010). However, support for a functional reserve hypothesis was found in patterns of activation in the posterior MTL ROI such that greater asymmetry toward the left was associated less verbal memory decline after ATLR (Bonelli et al., 2010). The authors interpreted the posterior findings as intrahemispheric or intra-MTL reorganization of anterior MTL function. Recent data also suggests that the functional reserve and adequacy models may apply differently in the context of right or left MTL seizure foci: memory outcomes for patients with left mTLE may be best predicted by well-functioning right MTL, while patients with right mTLE might be more dependent on the extent of remaining memory function with ipsilateral MTL structures (Banks et al., 2012).

Summary

The underlying focal pathology and surgical management of mTLE has informed much of our understanding about the material-specific lateralization of memory function. As research methodologies and cognitive theories have been developed, their application to the study of memory deficits in mTLE has refined and even challenged the model of material-specificity. Within the verbal domain, recent work has highlighted the importance of accounting for language adequacy and pre-existing semantic associations. The association between non-verbal deficits and right mTLE has been less consistent than the link between verbal memory and left mTLE, but tasks such as object-location binding have demonstrated promise. Finally, mTLE patients demonstrate aberrant lateralization of activation patterns on functional neuroimaging studies during material-specific memory tasks. These patterns are now being investigated as potential tools for clinical mapping to estimate post-operative memory outcomes following ATLRs.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Funding support given to Alexandra Golby through NIH P41EB015898, R21 CA156943, and R21 NS075728.

References

Abrahams, S., Morris, R. G., Polkey, C. E., Jarosz, J. M., Cox, T. C., Graves, M., et al. (1999). Hippocampal involvement in spatial and working memory: a structural MRI analysis of patients with unilateral mesial temporal lobe sclerosis. Brain Cogn. 41, 39–65. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1095

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Abrahams, S., Pickering, A., Polkey, C. E., and Morris, R. G. (1997). Spatial memory deficits in patients with unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia 35, 11–24. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00051-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adcock, J. E., Wise, R. G., Oxbury, J. M., Oxbury, S. M., and Matthews, P. M. (2003). Quantitative fMRI assessment of the differences in lateralization of language-related brain activation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage 18, 423–438. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00013-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Brown, M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 425–444. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002034 discussion 444-489,

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aggleton, J. P., and Brown, M. W. (2006). Interleaving brain systems for episodic and recognition memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 10, 455–463. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aguirre, G. K., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., and D’Esposito, M. (1996). The parahippocampus subserves topographical learning in man. Cereb. Cortex 6, 823–829. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.6.823

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., and D’Esposito, M. (1998). Neural components of topographical representation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 839–846. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.839

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Alessio, A., Pereira, F. R., Sercheli, M. S., Rondina, J. M., Ozelo, H. B., Bilevicius, E., et al. (2013). Brain plasticity for verbal and visual memories in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy and hippocampal sclerosis: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 186–199. doi:10.1002/hbm.21432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Astur, R. S., Taylor, L. B., Mamelak, A. N., Philpott, L., and Sutherland, R. J. (2002). Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav. Brain Res. 132, 77–84. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00399-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Banks, S. J., Sziklas, V., Sodums, D. J., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2012). fMRI of verbal and nonverbal memory processes in healthy and epileptogenic medial temporal lobes. Epilepsy Behav. 25, 42–49. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxendale, S. (2008). The impact of epilepsy surgery on cognition and behavior. Epilepsy Behav. 12, 592–599. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.12.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., Harkness, W., and Duncan, J. (2006). Predicting memory decline following epilepsy surgery: a multivariate approach. Epilepsia 47, 1887–1894. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00810.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., Harkness, W., and Duncan, J. (2007). The role of the intracarotid amobarbital procedure in predicting verbal memory decline after temporal lobe resection. Epilepsia 48, 546–552. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00940.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baxendale, S. A., Thompson, P. J., and Van Paesschen, W. (1998). A test of spatial memory and its clinical utility in the pre-surgical investigation of temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Neuropsychologia 36, 591–602. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00163-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Binder, J. R. (2011). Functional MRI is a valid noninvasive alternative to Wada testing. Epilepsy Behav. 20, 214–222. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Binder, J. R., Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, T. A., Raghavan, M., and Mueller, W. M. (2008). Use of preoperative functional MRI to predict verbal memory decline after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia 49, 1377–1394. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01625.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blume, W. T. (2006). The progression of epilepsy. Epilepsia 47(Suppl. 1), 71–78. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00665.x

CrossRef Full Text

Bohbot, V. D., Iaria, G., and Petrides, M. (2004). Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. Neuropsychology 18, 418–425. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.418

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bohbot, V. D., Kalina, M., Stepankova, K., Spackova, N., Petrides, M., and Nadel, L. (1998). Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex. Neuropsychologia 36, 1217–1238. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00161-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonelli, S. B., Powell, R. H., Yogarajah, M., Samson, R. S., Symms, M. R., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2010). Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection. Brain 133, 1186–1199. doi:10.1093/brain/awq006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bowles, B., Crupi, C., Mirsattari, S. M., Pigott, S. E., Parrent, A. G., Pruessner, J. C., et al. (2007). Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporal-lobe resection that spares the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 16382–16387. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705273104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bowles, B., Crupi, C., Pigott, S., Parrent, A., Wiebe, S., Janzen, L., et al. (2010). Double dissociation of selective recollection and familiarity impairments following two different surgical treatments for temporal-lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 48, 2640–2647. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cabeza, R., and Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 1–47. doi:10.1162/08989290051137585

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canovas, R., Leon, I., Serrano, P., Roldan, M. D., and Cimadevilla, J. M. (2011). Spatial navigation impairment in patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy: evidence from a new virtual reality-based task. Epilepsy Behav. 22, 364–369. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.07.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chelune, G., and Najm, I. M. (2000). “Risk factors associated with post-surgical decrements in memory,” in Epilepsy Surgery, eds H. Luders and Y. Comair (Philadelphia: Lippincott), 497–504.

Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus functional reserve: predicting memory functions following temporal lobectomy. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 10, 413–432. doi:10.1093/arclin/10.5.413

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chelune, G. J., Naugle, R. I., Luders, H., and Awad, I. A. (1991). Prediction of cognitive change as a function of preoperative ability status among temporal lobectomy patients seen at 6-month follow-up. Neurology 41, 399–404. doi:10.1212/WNL.41.3.399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crane, J., and Milner, B. (2005). What went where? Impaired object-location learning in patients with right hippocampal lesions. Hippocampus 15, 216–231. doi:10.1002/hipo.20043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 693–700. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davies, K. G., Bell, B. D., Bush, A. J., and Wyler, A. R. (1998). Prediction of verbal memory loss in individuals after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia 39, 820–828. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1157.1998.tb01175.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desgranges, B., Baron, J. C., and Eustache, F. (1998). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory: the role of the frontal lobes, the hippocampal formation, and other areas. Neuroimage 8, 198–213. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0359

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 11, 379–386. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diaz-Asper, C. M., Dopkins, S., Potolicchio, S. J. Jr., and Caputy, A. (2006). Spatial memory following temporal lobe resection. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 1462–1481. doi:10.1080/13803390500434359

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dige, N., and Wik, G. (2001). A visual learning and memory test for preoperative evaluation of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Int. J. Neurosci. 111, 167–174. doi:10.3109/00207450108994228

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dupont, S., Duron, E., Samson, S., Denos, M., Volle, E., Delmaire, C., et al. (2010). Functional MR imaging or Wada test: which is the better predictor of individual postoperative memory outcome? Radiology 255, 128–134. doi:10.1148/radiol.09091079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duzel, E., Habib, R., Rotte, M., Guderian, S., Tulving, E., and Heinze, H. J. (2003). Human hippocampal and parahippocampal activity during visual associative recognition memory for spatial and nonspatial stimulus configurations. J. Neurosci. 23, 9439–9444.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Eichenbaum, H., Sauvage, M., Fortin, N., Komorowski, R., and Lipton, P. (2012). Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1597–1608. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123–152. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328

CrossRef Full Text

Fisher, R. S., Vickrey, B. G., Gibson, P., Hermann, B., Penovich, P., Scherer, A., et al. (2000). The impact of epilepsy from the patient’s perspective I. Descriptions and subjective perceptions. Epilepsy Res. 41, 39–51. doi:10.1016/S0920-1211(00)00128-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fletcher, P. C., and Henson, R. N. (2001). Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain 124, 849–881. doi:10.1093/brain/124.5.849

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frings, L., Wagner, K., Halsband, U., Schwarzwald, R., Zentner, J., and Schulze-Bonhage, A. (2008). Lateralization of hippocampal activation differs between left and right temporal lobe epilepsy patients and correlates with postsurgical verbal learning decrement. Epilepsy Res. 78, 161–170. doi:10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2007.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Furtak, S. C., Wei, S. M., Agster, K. L., and Burwell, R. D. (2007). Functional neuroanatomy of the parahippocampal region in the rat: the perirhinal and postrhinal cortices. Hippocampus 17, 709–722. doi:10.1002/hipo.20314

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gleissner, U., Helmstaedter, C., and Elger, C. E. (1998). Right hippocampal contribution to visual memory: a presurgical and postsurgical study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 65, 665–669. doi:10.1136/jnnp.65.5.665

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gleissner, U., Helmstaedter, C., Schramm, J., and Elger, C. E. (2004). Memory outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: one-year follow-up. Epilepsia 45, 960–962. doi:10.1111/j.0013-9580.2004.42203.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glikmann-Johnston, Y., Saling, M. M., Chen, J., Cooper, K. A., Beare, R. J., and Reutens, D. C. (2008). Structural and functional correlates of unilateral mesial temporal lobe spatial memory impairment. Brain 131, 3006–3018. doi:10.1093/brain/awn213

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Golby, A. J., Poldrack, R. A., Illes, J., Chen, D., Desmond, J. E., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Memory lateralization in medial temporal lobe epilepsy assessed by functional MRI. Epilepsia 43, 855–863. doi:10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.20501.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., Beig, S., and Craik, F. I. (2001). An examination of the effects of stimulus type, encoding task, and functional connectivity on the role of right prefrontal cortex in recognition memory. Neuroimage 14, 556–571. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0865

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., and Kurland, L. T. (1991). Prevalence of epilepsy in Rochester, Minnesota: 1940-1980. Epilepsia 32, 429–445. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1157.1991.tb04675.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helmstaedter, C., and Elger, C. E. (1996). Cognitive consequences of two-thirds anterior temporal lobectomy on verbal memory in 144 patients: a three-month follow-up study. Epilepsia 37, 171–180. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1157.1996.tb00009.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helmstaedter, C., Grunwald, T., Lehnertz, K., Gleissner, U., and Elger, C. E. (1997). Differential involvement of left temporolateral and temporomesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn. 35, 110–131. doi:10.1006/brcg.1997.0930

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helmstaedter, C., Kurthen, M., Lux, S., Reuber, M., and Elger, C. E. (2003). Chronic epilepsy and cognition: a longitudinal study in temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann. Neurol. 54, 425–432. doi:10.1002/ana.10692

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 523–532. doi:10.1038/nrn2850

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hermann, B., and Seidenberg, M. (2008). “Memory impairment and its cognitive context in epilepsy,” in Behavioral Aspects of Epilepsy: Principles and Practice, eds D. L. Schacter, G. L. Holmes, and K.-N. Dga (New York: Demos), 147–153.

Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Haltiner, A., and Wyler, A. R. (1992). Adequacy of language function and verbal memory performance in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex 28, 423–433. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80152-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Haltiner, A., and Wyler, A. R. (1995). Relationship of age at onset, chronologic age, and adequacy of preoperative performance to verbal memory change after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia 36, 137–145. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1157.1995.tb00972.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Schoenfeld, J., and Davies, K. (1997). Neuropsychological characteristics of the syndrome of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch. Neurol. 54, 369–376. doi:10.1001/archneur.1997.00550160019010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., Steenman, H., and Richey, E. T. (1988). The interrelationship between language function and verbal learning/memory performance in patients with complex partial seizures. Cortex 24, 245–253. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(88)80033-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iaria, G., Petrides, M., Dagher, A., Pike, B., and Bohbot, V. D. (2003). Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: variability and change with practice. J. Neurosci. 23, 5945–5952.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Incisa della Rochetta, A., Samson, S., Ehrle, N., Denos, M., Hasboun, D., and Baulad, M. (2004). Memory for visuospatial location following selective hippocampal sclerosis: the use of different coordinate systems. Neuropsychology 18, 15–28. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jansen, A., Sehlmeyer, C., Pfleiderer, B., Sommer, J., Konrad, C., Zwitserlood, P., et al. (2009). Assessment of verbal memory by fMRI: lateralization and functional neuroanatomy. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 111, 57–62. doi:10.1016/j.clineuro.2008.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ji, G. J., Zhang, Z., Zhang, H., Wang, J., Liu, D. Q., Zang, Y. F., et al. (2013). Disrupted causal connectivity in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. PLoS ONE 8:e63183. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063183

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J., and Anderson, A. W. (2003). FMRI evidence for an organization of prefrontal cortex by both type of process and type of information. Cereb. Cortex 13, 265–273. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.3.265

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnsrude, I. S., Owen, A. M., Crane, J., Milner, B., and Evans, A. C. (1999). A cognitive activation study of memory for spatial relationships. Neuropsychologia 37, 829–841. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00136-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jokeit, H., Ebner, A., Holthausen, H., Markowitsch, H. J., Moch, A., Pannek, H., et al. (1997). Individual prediction of change in delayed recall of prose passages after left-sided anterior temporal lobectomy. Neurology 49, 481–487. doi:10.1212/WNL.49.2.481

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jokeit, H., Okujava, M., and Woermann, F. G. (2001). Memory fMRI lateralizes temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 57, 1786–1793. doi:10.1212/WNL.57.10.1786

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones-Gotman, M. (1986). Right hippocampal excision impairs learning and recall of a list of abstract designs. Neuropsychologia 24, 659–670. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(86)90005-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones-Gotman, M., Harnadek, M. C., and Kubu, C. S. (2000). Neuropsychological assessment for temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 27(Suppl. 1), S39–S43. discussion S50-32,

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jones-Gotman, M., Smith, M. L., Risse, G. L., Westerveld, M., Swanson, S. J., Giovagnoli, A. R., et al. (2010). The contribution of neuropsychology to diagnostic assessment in epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 18, 3–12. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.02.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones-Gotman, M., Zatorre, R. J., Olivier, A., Andermann, F., Cendes, F., Staunton, H., et al. (1997). Learning and retention of words and designs following excision from medial or lateral temporal-lobe structures. Neuropsychologia 35, 963–973. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00024-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kennepohl, S., Sziklas, V., Garver, K. E., Wagner, D. D., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2007). Memory and the medial temporal lobe: hemispheric specialization reconsidered. Neuroimage 36, 969–978. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerr, K. M., Agster, K. L., Furtak, S. C., and Burwell, R. D. (2007). Functional neuroanatomy of the parahippocampal region: the lateral and medial entorhinal areas. Hippocampus 17, 697–708. doi:10.1002/hipo.20315

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessels, R. P., Hendriks, M., Schouten, J., Van Asselen, M., and Postma, A. (2004). Spatial memory deficits in patients after unilateral selective amygdalohippocampectomy. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 907–912. doi:10.1017/S1355617704106140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, T. M., Yip, J. T., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2002). Memory deficits after resection from left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: a meta-analytic review. Epilepsia 43, 283–291. doi:10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.09901.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lepage, M., Ghaffar, O., Nyberg, L., and Tulving, E. (2000). Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 506–511. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.1.506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lillywhite, L. M., Saling, M. M., Briellmann, R. S., Weintrob, D. L., Pell, G. S., and Jackson, G. D. (2007). Differential contributions of the hippocampus and rhinal cortices to verbal memory in epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 10, 553–559. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lineweaver, T. T., Morris, H. H., Naugle, R. I., Najm, I. M., Diehl, B., and Bingaman, W. (2006). Evaluating the contributions of state-of-the-art assessment techniques to predicting memory outcome after unilateral anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia 47, 1895–1903. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00807.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maguire, E. A. (1997). Hippocampal involvement in human topographical memory: evidence from functional imaging. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 352, 1475–1480. doi:10.1098/rstb.1997.0134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maguire, E. A., Burke, T., Phillips, J., and Staunton, H. (1996). Topographical disorientation following unilateral temporal lobe lesions in humans. Neuropsychologia 34, 993–1001. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(96)00022-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maguire, E. A., and Cipolotti, L. (1998). Selective sparing of topographical memory. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 65, 903–909. doi:10.1136/jnnp.65.6.903

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maguire, E. A., Frith, C. D., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., and O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where things are parahippocampal involvement in encoding object locations in virtual large-scale space. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 61–76. doi:10.1162/089892998563789

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McConley, R., Martin, R., Palmer, C. A., Kuzniecky, R., Knowlton, R., and Faught, E. (2008). Rey Osterrieth complex figure test spatial and figural scoring: relations to seizure focus and hippocampal pathology in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 13, 174–177. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McCormick, C., Quraan, M., Cohn, M., Valiante, T. A., and McAndrews, M. P. (2013). Default mode network connectivity indicates episodic memory capacity in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 54, 809–818. doi:10.1111/epi.12098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDermott, K. B., Buckner, R. L., Petersen, S. E., Kelley, W. M., and Sanders, A. L. (1999). Set- and code-specific activation in frontal cortex: an fMRI study of encoding and retrieval of faces and words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 631–640. doi:10.1162/089892999563698

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McDonald, C. R., Ahmadi, M. E., Hagler, D. J., Tecoma, E. S., Iragui, V. J., Gharapetian, L., et al. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of memory and language impairments in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 71, 1869–1876. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000327824.05348.3b

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McMillan, T. M., Powell, G. E., Janota, I., and Polkey, C. E. (1987). Relationships between neuropathology and cognitive functioning in temporal lobectomy patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 50, 167–176. doi:10.1136/jnnp.50.2.167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mechanic-Hamilton, D., Korczykowski, M., Yushkevich, P. A., Lawler, K., Pluta, J., Glynn, S., et al. (2009). Hippocampal volumetry and functional MRI of memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 16, 128–138. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.07.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meencke, H. J., and Veith, G. (1992). Migration disturbances in epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. Suppl. 9, 31–39. discussion 39-40.

Milner, B. (1966). “Amnesia following operation on the temporal lobes,” in Amnesia, eds C. Whitty and O. Zangwill (London: Butterworth), 109–133.

Milner, B. (1970). “Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain,” in Biology of Memory, eds P. Kh and D. Broadbent (New York: Academic Press), 29–50.

Morris, R. G., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N., and O’Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297, 681–683. doi:10.1038/297681a0

CrossRef Full Text

Mueller, S. G., Laxer, K. D., Barakos, J., Cheong, I., Finlay, D., Garcia, P., et al. (2010). Involvement of the thalamocortical network in TLE with and without mesiotemporal sclerosis. Epilepsia 51, 1436–1445. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02413.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyberg, L., Persson, J., Habib, R., Tulving, E., Mcintosh, A. R., Cabeza, R., et al. (2000). Large scale neurocognitive networks underlying episodic memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 163–173. doi:10.1162/089892900561805

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Keefe, J., and Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1

CrossRef Full Text

Oyegbile, T. O., Dow, C., Jones, J., Bell, B., Rutecki, P., Sheth, R., et al. (2004). The nature and course of neuropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 62, 1736–1742. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000125186.04867.34

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parslow, D. M., Morris, R. G., Fleminger, S., Rahman, Q., Abrahams, S., and Recce, M. (2005). Allocentric spatial memory in humans with hippocampal lesions. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 118, 123–147. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Penfield, W., and Milner, B. (1958). Memory deficit produced by bilateral lesions in the hippocampal zone. AMA Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 79, 475–497. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1958.02340050003001

CrossRef Full Text

Piekema, C., Kessels, R. P., Mars, R. B., Petersson, K. M., and Fernandez, G. (2006). The right hippocampus participates in short-term memory maintenance of object-location associations. Neuroimage 33, 374–382. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piguet, O., Saling, M. M., O’Shea, M. F., Berkovic, S. F., and Bladin, P. F. (1994). Rey figure distortions reflect nonverbal recall differences between right and left foci in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 9, 451–460. doi:10.1016/0887-6177(94)90007-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pillon, B., Bazin, B., Deweer, B., Ehrle, N., Baulac, M., and Dubois, B. (1999). Specificity of memory deficits after right or left temporal lobectomy. Cortex 35, 561–571. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70819-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Powell, H. W., Koepp, M. J., Richardson, M. P., Symms, M. R., Thompson, P. J., and Duncan, J. S. (2004). The application of functional MRI of memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: a clinical review. Epilepsia 45, 855–863. doi:10.1111/j.0013-9580.2004.41603.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Powell, H. W., Koepp, M. J., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P. A., Salek-Haddadi, A., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2005). Material-specific lateralization of memory encoding in the medial temporal lobe: blocked versus event-related design. Neuroimage 27, 231–239. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Powell, H. W., Parker, G. J., Alexander, D. C., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P. A., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., et al. (2007). Abnormalities of language networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage 36, 209–221. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.028

CrossRef Full Text

Powell, H. W., Richardson, M. P., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P. A., Thompson, P. J., Duncan, J. S., et al. (2008). Preoperative fMRI predicts memory decline following anterior temporal lobe resection. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 79, 686–693. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2007.115139

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rausch, R., and Babb, T. L. (1987). “Evidence for memory specialization within the mesial temporal lobe of man,” in Fundamental Mechanisms of Human Brain Function, ed. J. Engel Jr. (New York: Raven Press), 103–109.

Reminger, S. L., Kaszniak, A. W., Labiner, D. M., Littrell, L. D., David, B. T., Ryan, L., et al. (2004). Bilateral hippocampal volume predicts verbal memory function in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 5, 687–695. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2004.06.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., Duncan, J. S., and Dolan, R. J. (2006). Memory fMRI in left hippocampal sclerosis: optimizing the approach to predicting postsurgical memory. Neurology 66, 699–705. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000201186.07716.98

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, T. A., Spanaki, M. V., Possing, E. T., Morris, G. L. III, et al. (2003). Use of preoperative functional neuroimaging to predict language deficits from epilepsy surgery. Neurology 60, 1788–1792. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000068022.05644.01

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saling, M. M. (2009). Verbal memory in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: beyond material specificity. Brain 132, 570–582. doi:10.1093/brain/awp012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saling, M. M., Berkovic, S. F., O’Shea, M. F., Kalnins, R. M., Darby, D. G., and Bladin, P. F. (1993). Lateralization of verbal memory and unilateral hippocampal sclerosis: evidence of task-specific effects. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 15, 608–618. doi:10.1080/01688639308402582

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saling, M. M., O’Shea, M. F., Weintrob, D. L., Wood, A. G., Reutens, D. C., and Berkovic, S. (2002). “Medial and lateral contributions to verbal memory: evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy,” in Frontiers of Memory, eds A. Yamadori, K. R. T. Fujii, and K. Suzuki (Sendai: Tohoku University Press), 151–158.

Scoville, W. B., and Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 20, 11–21. doi:10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11

CrossRef Full Text

Seidenberg, M., Hermann, B. P., Dohan, F. C. Jr., Wyler, A. R., Perrine, A., and Schoenfeld, J. (1996). Hippocampal sclerosis and verbal encoding ability following anterior temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia 34, 699–708. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00146-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sidhu, M. K., Stretton, J., Winston, G. P., Bonelli, S., Centeno, M., Vollmar, C., et al. (2013). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study mapping the episodic memory encoding network in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 136, 1868–1888. doi:10.1093/brain/awt099

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Skinner, E. I., and Fernandes, M. A. (2007). Neural correlates of recollection and familiarity: a review of neuroimaging and patient data. Neuropsychologia 45, 2163–2179. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spiers, H. J., Burgess, N., Hartley, T., Vargha-Khadem, F., and O’Keefe, J. (2001a). Bilateral hippocampal pathology impairs topographical and episodic memory but not visual pattern matching. Hippocampus 11, 715–725. doi:10.1002/hipo.1087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spiers, H. J., Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., Baxendale, S. A., Hartley, T., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2001b). Unilateral temporal lobectomy patients show lateralized topographical and episodic memory deficits in a virtual town. Brain 124, 2476–2489. doi:10.1093/brain/124.12.2476

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E., and Clark, R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 279–306. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130

CrossRef Full Text

Squire, L. R., Wixted, J. T., and Clark, R. E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 872–883. doi:10.1038/nrn2154

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stepankova, K., Fenton, A. A., Pastalkova, E., Kalina, M., and Bohbot, V. D. (2004). Object-location memory impairment in patients with thermal lesions to the right or left hippocampus. Neuropsychologia 42, 1017–1028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stroup, E., Langfitt, J., Berg, M., Mcdermott, M., Pilcher, W., and Como, P. (2003). Predicting verbal memory decline following anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL). Neurology 60, 1266–1273. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000058765.33878.0D

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suzuki, W. A., and Amaral, D. G. (1994). Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 350, 497–533. doi:10.1002/cne.903500402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thivard, L., Hombrouck, J., Du Montcel, S. T., Delmaire, C., Cohen, L., Samson, S., et al. (2005). Productive and perceptive language reorganization in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage 24, 841–851. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Asselen, M., Kessels, R. P., Neggers, S. F., Kappelle, L. J., Frijns, C. J., and Postma, A. (2006). Brain areas involved in spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 44, 1185–1194. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Strien, N. M., Cappaert, N. L., and Witter, M. P. (2009). The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 272–282. doi:10.1038/nrn2614

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wais, P. E., Mickes, L., and Wixted, J. T. (2008). Remember/know judgments probe degrees of recollection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 400–405. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, Q., Gao, E., and Burkhalter, A. (2011). Gateways of ventral and dorsal streams in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 1905–1918. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3488-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weniger, G., Ruhleder, M., Wolf, S., Lange, C., and Irle, E. (2009). Egocentric memory impaired and allocentric memory intact as assessed by virtual reality in subjects with unilateral parietal cortex lesions. Neuropsychologia 47, 59–69. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wiener, S. I. (1993). Spatial and behavioral correlates of striatal neurons in rats performing a self-initiated navigation task. J. Neurosci. 13, 3802–3817.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wolf, H. K., Campos, M. G., Zentner, J., Hufnagel, A., Schramm, J., Elger, C. E., et al. (1993). Surgical pathology of temporal lobe epilepsy. Experience with 216 cases. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 52, 499–506. doi:10.1097/00005072-199309000-00008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yonelinas, A. P., and Levy, B. J. (2002). Dissociating familiarity from recollection in human recognition memory: different rates of forgetting over short retention intervals. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 575–582. doi:10.3758/BF03196315

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: material-specific, memory, hemispheric lateralization, temporal lobe epilepsy, fMRI

Citation: Willment KC and Golby A (2013) Hemispheric lateralization interrupted: material-specific memory deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:546. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00546

Received: 28 June 2013; Accepted: 19 August 2013;
Published online: 02 September 2013.

Edited by:

Ruth E. Propper, Montclair State University, USA

Reviewed by:

Tad Brunye, US Army NSRDEC; Tufts University, USA
Giorgio Bonmassar, Massachusetts General Hospital, USA

Copyright: © 2013 Willment and Golby. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexandra Golby, Department of Neurosurgery, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, 75 Francis Street, Boston, MA 02115, USA e-mail: agolby@partners.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.