- 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
- 2Program in Neuroscience, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
- 3Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
While there is almost universal agreement amongst researchers that autism is associated with alterations in brain connectivity, the precise nature of these alterations continues to be debated. Theoretical and empirical work is beginning to reveal that autism is associated with a complex functional phenotype characterized by both hypo- and hyper-connectivity of large-scale brain systems. It is not yet understood why such conflicting patterns of brain connectivity are observed across different studies, and the factors contributing to these heterogeneous findings have not been identified. Developmental changes in functional connectivity have received inadequate attention to date. We propose that discrepancies between findings of autism related hypo-connectivity and hyper-connectivity might be reconciled by taking developmental changes into account. We review neuroimaging studies of autism, with an emphasis on functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of intrinsic functional connectivity in children, adolescents and adults. The consistent pattern emerging across several studies is that while intrinsic functional connectivity in adolescents and adults with autism is generally reduced compared with age-matched controls, functional connectivity in younger children with the disorder appears to be increased. We suggest that by placing recent empirical findings within a developmental framework, and explicitly characterizing age and pubertal stage in future work, it may be possible to resolve conflicting findings of hypo- and hyper-connectivity in the extant literature and arrive at a more comprehensive understanding of the neurobiology of autism.
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impaired social interaction and communication, repetitive behaviors, and restricted interests. According to the latest reports, ASD affects nearly 1 in 88 children, and the prevalence continues to grow (Investigators, 2012). The recognition of the increasing prevalence of ASD has placed a mandate on understanding its neurobiological foundations. As highlighted in several articles appearing in this special topic, one of the most well-documented observations in the autism literature is that the brains of individuals with the disorder exhibit aberrant functional connectivity or inter-regional communication (Belmonte et al., 2004). Functional connectivity as measured from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data is defined as “temporal correlations between remote neurophysiological events” (Friston, 1994). Functional connectivity is typically measured using one of three approaches: (1) regression analysis using a seed region of interest (Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005), (2) full or partial correlation analysis of multiple regions of interest (Ryali et al., 2012), or (3) independent component analysis (ICA) of the entire imaging dataset to identify spatial maps with common temporal profiles (Beckmann and Smith, 2004; Cole et al., 2010). These measures have been used to characterize large-scale networks in the human brain (Bressler and Menon, 2010; Sporns, 2011), and have paved the way for increasingly sophisticated investigations of brain connectivity in ASD (Kennedy and Adolphs, 2012).
Temporal correlations in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) fMRI signals are thought to arise from signal propagation and dynamical slowing down of fluctuations in anatomically constrained neural networks (Deco et al., 2013). Consistent with this, empirical studies using human ECoG have shown that slow (<0.1 Hz) spontaneous fluctuations of firing rate and gamma local field potentials are correlated with spontaneous fMRI fluctuations (Nir et al., 2008). Intrinsic functional connectivity measured during resting state fMRI may reflect a history of task-dependent coactivation, and likely serves to organize and coordinate neuronal activity, or might represent dynamic predictions about expected patterns of use (Fox and Raichle, 2007).
Several investigations have reported that functional connectivity between brain regions is weaker in high-functioning ASD, leading to long-distance cortical “under-connectivity” theories of autism (Courchesne and Pierce, 2005; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Schipul et al., 2011; Just et al., 2012). However, there is emerging evidence that challenges these models and suggests that functional connectivity between brain regions can be stronger in ASD (Uddin et al., 2013). It is not yet understood why such conflicting patterns of brain connectivity results are observed across different studies, and factors contributing to these heterogeneous findings have not been identified. A more nuanced account capturing patterns of both task-related and intrinsic hypo- and hyper-connectivity observed in autism is essential for characterizing aberrant brain organization in the disorder (Kana et al., 2011; Muller et al., 2011; Vissers et al., 2011). Recent attempts to provide explanations for the discrepant findings in the literature have delineated both methodological issues (Muller et al., 2011) and conceptual issues (Vissers et al., 2011). Here we propose that yet another source of inconsistency exists, namely that developmental changes in functional connectivity have received inadequate attention to date. We posit that discrepancies between findings of autism-related hypo-connectivity and hyper-connectivity might be reconciled by taking developmental stage into account.
The idea that critical periods of plasticity during brain development represent particularly vulnerable stages during which aberrant maturational process can occur is not a new one. In 2003 Rubenstein and Merzenich first introduced the theory that autism may arise from an increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in developing neural systems subserving sensory, mnemonic, social, and emotional processes. Hyperexcitability as a result of this imbalance has been hypothesized to contribute to poorly functionally differentiated and inherently unstable cortex in autism (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003). As summarized in a recent review characterizing autism as a critical period disorder, excessive plasticity at the wrong times could result in noisy and unstable processing, yet a brain that lacks appropriate levels of plasticity early in life might remain hyper- or hypo-connected and unresponsive to environmental changes early in life (LeBlanc and Fagiolini, 2011).
One of the earliest signs of autism is enlarged head circumference or macrocephaly (Lainhart et al., 1997). Infants and young children with ASD show signs of early brain overgrowth (Courchesne et al., 2003). Postmortem studies of children with ASD show that they have an overabundance or excess numbers of neurons in the prefrontal cortex (Courchesne et al., 2011). Animal models likewise provide evidence for hyper-connectivity at very early time points in development (Testa-Silva et al., 2011). There is a profound inconsistency between these observations and “under-connectivity” or hypo-connectivity theories that by and large do not account for the possibility of an early phase of neural hyper-connectivity in ASD.
The EEG literature has long reflected an understanding that stabilization and pruning of connections during development plays a central role in the development of cognitive and perceptual functions during critical periods early in life. Uhlhaas and colleagues summarize decades of work to hypothesize that “in ASDs abnormal brain maturation during early prenatal and postnatal periods results in cortical circuits that are unable to support the expression of high-frequency oscillations during infancy. These impaired oscillations might in turn reduce the temporal precision of coordinated firing patterns and thereby disturb activity-dependent circuit selection during further development” (Uhlhaas et al., 2010). These developmental perspectives from animal models and electrophysiological studies should be integrated into the fMRI community, which has struggled to reconcile inconsistent findings with regards to functional brain connectivity in ASD over the past several years.
There has been rapid progress in understanding changes in functional connectivity accompanying typical development with the advent of resting-state fMRI (Uddin et al., 2010). For example, it is now known that subcortical areas are more strongly functionally coupled with primary sensory, association, and paralimbic areas in children, whereas adults show stronger cortico-cortical functional connectivity between paralimbic, limbic, and association areas (Supekar et al., 2009). More generally, several studies have demonstrated that over development, functional brain networks shift from a local anatomical emphasis to a more distributed architecture (Fair et al., 2009; Kelly et al., 2009). It has recently been suggested that motion-related artifacts can have a significant impact on functional connectivity estimates (Power et al., 2012; Van Dijk et al., 2012) in such a way that makes it difficult to study developmental differences. While the appropriate treatment of motion-related artifacts is as yet an unresolved issue in the field (see Satterthwaite et al., 2012, 2013), findings from other imaging modalities including diffusion tensor imaging corroborate functional connectivity findings of increased integrity of long-distance connections with development (Supekar et al., 2010; Uddin et al., 2011). These and other insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience can and should inform theories of atypical development of functional connectivity in autism.
The majority of functional neuroimaging studies of autism have been conducted in adolescents or adults, in part due to practical limitations related to scanning very young children (Yerys et al., 2009). Evidence from these studies of older individuals generally supports the hypo-connectivity theory of autism. However, the lack of available empirical data from younger children with the disorder has made it difficult to test the extent to which the hypo-connectivity theory generalizes to younger age groups. Although calls for data sharing in autism research have been put forth in the past (Belmonte et al., 2008), only recently have large neuroimaging datasets been released. One recent grassroots data sharing initiative (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/abide/) has made pre-publication datasets of neuroimaging data collected from individuals between the ages of 6 and 60 available to researchers to facilitate and accelerate the discovery of the functional architecture of the autistic brain (Di Martino et al., 2013a). Still, at this time relatively little has been published addressing the issue of functional brain connectivity in young children with ASD.
The purpose of this review is to (1) summarize the current status of the field by highlighting key findings from studies using fMRI to examine task-related and intrinsic functional connectivity in individuals with ASD across various age groups, (2) reveal critical gaps in the literature which have led to an inconsistent characterization of functional connectivity in ASD, and (3) argue that a developmental perspective can help reconcile some extant contradictory findings, and is necessary for future progress in the field.
Functional Brain Connectivity in Autism: Review
Autism is a disorder with early life onset and variable developmental trajectory (Stefanatos, 2008). Functional neuroimaging studies of young children are thus especially critical for developing accurate models of the underlying neurobiology of the disorder. Thus, it is perhaps surprising that very few fMRI studies have addressed the question of how the brain is functionally organized in childhood ASD, at developmental stages more proximal to the onset of the disorder (Akshoomoff et al., 2002; Amaral, 2010). Below we survey fMRI studies of ASD examining task-based functional connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity with the goal of providing an overview of the existing literature and highlighting the dearth of developmental studies of functional connectivity in ASD.
Task-Based Functional Connectivity
Task-based functional connectivity measures the synchronization of activation levels between brain regions during the performance of a given cognitive task. Since the initial fMRI reports of hypo-connectivity in autism (Just et al., 2004), task-related reductions in inter-regional brain connectivity during language (Just et al., 2004; Mason et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2010), working memory (Koshino et al., 2005, 2008), mental imagery (Kana et al., 2006), executive functions (Just et al., 2007), cognitive control (Kana et al., 2007; Solomon et al., 2009; Agam et al., 2010), visuomotor coordination (Villalobos et al., 2005) and social cognition (Kleinhans et al., 2008; Kana et al., 2009) have been documented. However, reports of brain hyper-connectivity in ASD also exist in the domains of visuomotor processing (Mizuno et al., 2006; Turner et al., 2006), visual search (Shih et al., 2011), emotion processing (Welchew et al., 2005), memory (Noonan et al., 2009), and language (Shih et al., 2010). These findings are comprehensively reviewed elsewhere (Thai et al., 2009; Schipul et al., 2011; Vissers et al., 2011). A recent review of studies conducted mainly in adults highlights several methodological variables including concatenation of specific task blocks, the use of low-pass filtering, regression of main effects of task, and methods for selecting regions-of-interest that result in considerable heterogeneity between studies with respect to how functional connectivity is conceptualized and analyzed. The authors suggest that such variables may partially account for discrepancies in connectivity results, and that hypo-connectivity findings may be contingent upon these methodological choices (Muller et al., 2011). For example, Muller and colleagues surveyed 32 studies and found that the use of low-pass filtering of fMRI data more often produced results inconsistent with the general under-connectivity theory (Muller et al., 2011).
Recognizing and documenting methodological issues is a critical first step toward synthesizing findings in the “functional connectivity in autism” literature and identifying robust and replicable results. Overall, task-based functional connectivity studies largely support the hypo-connectivity theory, however, the majority of these report results from older adolescents and adults. Additionally, task-based approaches produce results that cannot be easily generalized to other cognitive states, and differences between groups in task performance can make interpretation of hypo- and hyper-connectivity results difficult. The emergence of resting-state fMRI as a means for characterizing the intrinsic functional architecture of the brain, unconfounded by task and behavioral effects, has facilitated data collection from younger typically developing (TD) children and children with ASD (Uddin et al., 2010).
Resting-State Functional Connectivity
Since the initial demonstration by Biswal and colleagues that coherent spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in BOLD signal can be detected within functional systems in the absence of task performance (Biswal et al., 1995), the use of resting-state fMRI in neuroscience has grown exponentially. Applications in clinical neuroscience have been particularly useful, and have provided insights into systems-level cortical and subcortical anomalies of functional connectivity in neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD, Castellanos and Proal, 2012) and schizophrenia (Yu et al., 2012). Surprisingly, however, there are relatively few published resting-state functional connectivity studies examining individuals with ASD. Extant studies have by and large focused on adolescents or adults with the disorder, with a few notable exceptions.
The earliest published intrinsic functional connectivity study of autism was conducted by Cherkassky and colleagues, who used a seed region-of-interest (ROI) approach to demonstrate functional hypo-connectivity in anterior–posterior connections in adolescents and adults with ASD (Cherkassky et al., 2006). Using an ROI-based approach in another study, Kennedy and colleagues demonstrated disrupted intrinsic connectivity of the default mode network (DMN), but not the dorsal attention network, in a group of adolescents and adults with autism (Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008). Others have replicated this finding of reduced DMN connectivity in adults (Monk et al., 2009), as well as adolescents (Weng et al., 2010) with ASD. Similar findings of decreased functional connectivity of the DMN in adults with ASD have been obtained using data-driven ICA approaches (Assaf et al., 2010), as well as studies combining both seed correlation and ICA (von dem Hagen et al., 2013). Whole-brain connectivity approaches have also provided evidence of hypo-connectivity of social processing-related brain circuits in adolescents with ASD (Gotts et al., 2012), though a recent systematic investigation using both ROI-based and ICA-based analytic approaches found very few examples of functional hypo-connectivity in adults with ASD compared with age-matched control participants (Tyszka et al., 2013). A study of adolescents and adults revealed decreased intrinsic functional connectivity of the insular cortex in high-functioning ASD (Ebisch et al., 2010). As summarized in Table 1, the studies that have reported group differences in the direction of autism-related intrinsic hypo-connectivity were all conducted in either adolescent or adult high-functioning (average or above average IQ) samples. The nascent literature on childhood ASD, in contrast, paints a very different picture.
In a group of children between 7 and 14 years of age, Di Martino and colleagues found that children with ASD exhibit functional hyper-connectivity compared with TD peers. They found increased functional connectivity between striatal subregions and heteromodal association and limbic cortices including insula and superior temporal gyrus (Di Martino et al., 2011). Recently, we demonstrated that children aged 7–12 with autism exhibit hyper-connectivity of several major large-scale brain networks important for cognitive functions. A widely-used method for comparing brain networks between groups is dual regression ICA. Dual regression employs a set of spatial maps derived from the initial group ICA in a linear model fit against each individual fMRI dataset, resulting in matrices describing the temporal dynamics of the corresponding networks for each subject (Beckmann et al., 2005; Filippini et al., 2009). Using this approach, we found that children with ASD exhibited greater functional connectivity than TD children within the DMN, salience, fronto-temporal, motor, and visual networks (Uddin et al., 2013). This somewhat surprising hyper-connectivity result also emerged using complementary analytic approaches and was replicated in several independent datasets (Supekar et al., 2012), and by other groups examining wider age ranges (6–17-year-olds) (Washington et al., 2013). Further, we have found that even within the DMN, hypo- vs. hyper-connectivity results can be observed in children with ASD depending on the precise anatomical location of ROIs within the posterior medial cortex (Lynch et al., 2013). Another recent study of children aged 9–18 found mixed patterns of hypo- and hyper-connectivity between ROIs across the entire brain (Rudie et al., 2013). Rudie and colleagues also report data from graph theoretical analyses demonstrating that while “small worldness” was similar between groups, network level reductions in modularity and clustering as well as shorter characteristic path lengths were observed in children and adolescents with ASD. Some reports of hypo-connectivity between specific ROIs in children with ASD have been published recently (Dinstein et al., 2011; Abrams et al., 2013).
These recent findings raise an important question: If childhood autism is characterized by functional hyper-connectivity, and adults with autism exhibit functional hypo-connectivity, why has the field been slow to examine this important developmental discontinuity? One possible explanation is that the hypo-connectivity theory of ASD has been so dominant that investigators finding contradictory findings have been reluctant to publish their results. This idea has been discussed in a recent Simons Foundation blog (http://sfari.org/news-and-opinion/specials/2013/connectivity/guest-blog-negative-results). A survey of presentations at recent meetings of the International Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR) and the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) suggests that this may in fact be the case. Deen and colleagues conducted ROI-based analyses on data collected from children aged ~13 with ASD and TD control participants. In a poster presented at IMFAR in 2011, they report: “A number of group differences were found in both directions, with no trend toward more differences in the direction of TD>ASD … in the ROI analysis, 19 correlations were stronger in the TD group, while 38 were stronger in the ASD group” (Deen and Pelphrey, 2011). In an HBM poster from the 2012 meeting, You and colleagues reported using a “connectivity degree”—computed by counting, for each voxel, the number of voxels meeting a correlation threshold of r > 0.25 inside (local) and outside (distant) its neighborhood defined as a sphere of 14 mm radius (Sepulcre et al., 2010)—to find that degree of functional connectivity was higher in 7–13-year-old children with ASD than TD children (You et al., 2012). These initial findings of functional hyper-connectivity in children with ASD are only now beginning to surface, and may have been initially received with skepticism due to their inconsistency with the hypo-connectivity theory.
Developmental Model of Functional Brain Connectivity in ASD
We propose that the discrepancies between the adult ASD and childhood ASD findings with respect to whole-brain functional connectivity may be reconciled by considering critical developmental factors such as the onset of puberty, which signals the beginning of adolescence and has a major impact on brain structure and function. Puberty typically begins between 9 and 12 years of age, and creates a surge of hormones that trigger rapid physical growth, sexually dimorphic alterations in facial structure, metabolic changes, and several social, behavioral, and emotional changes (Crone and Dahl, 2012). Studies of brain development in animals suggest that the hormonal events surrounding puberty exert significant effects on brain maturation (Cahill, 2006). Relatively few neuroimaging studies have explored the role of puberty in human brain development (Blakemore et al., 2010; Crone and Dahl, 2012; Galvan et al., 2012), though it was noted long ago that measurements of peak gray matter volume coincide with the onset of puberty (Giedd et al., 1999; Blakemore, 2012).
The age-related discontinuity in the autism neuroimaging literature between findings from children and adults coincides with this developmental period. As summarized in Table 1, studies of children under the age of 12 (presumably predominantly pre-pubertal) find considerable evidence for functional hyper-connectivity in ASD, whereas the studies reporting data collected from adolescents and adults (presumably predominantly post-pubertal) reveal functional hypo-connectivity in ASD. A schematic model of this proposed developmental shift is depicted in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Schematic model of two scenarios that could explain a developmental shift from intrinsic hyper-to hypo-connectivity in ASD. In scenario 1 (solid red line), the ASD group shows a less steep developmental increase in functional connectivity over the age span compared with the TD group. In scenario 2 (dashed red line), the ASD group shows anomalous patterns of connectivity across the pubertal period. Resting-state functional connectivity MRI studies provide evidence for widespread hyper-connectivity in children with ASD in contrast to hypo-connectivity observed in adolescents and adults with ASD. To reconcile these findings, it will be necessary to conduct longitudinal studies that span the developmental period surrounding puberty (gray oval). ASD, autism spectrum disorders; TD, typical development.
A growing body of literature documents age-related increases in white matter volume (Lenroot and Giedd, 2006), which may be related to increases in long-range functional connectivity from childhood through adolescence and into adulthood (Fair et al., 2008; Kelly et al., 2009). Recent reports of strengthening of structural and functional connectivity with age have shed light on typical developmental processes (Hagmann et al., 2010; Supekar et al., 2010; Uddin et al., 2011). Similar developmental studies of brain connectivity in ASD do not yet exist. In concert with studies of the effects of puberty on typical brain development, this work will help to explain the developmental shift that is suggested by the existing literature of functional connectivity in autism.
Challenges and Gaps in the Literature
Lack of Longitudinal Data and Data From Younger Participants
The most critical gap in the literature on functional brain connectivity in ASD is the lack of longitudinal studies tracking the same individuals as they progress from pre- to post-pubertal stages of development (Wass, 2011). There are a few longitudinal findings from structural neuroimaging studies spanning the developmental period discussed in this review. One report found significantly greater decreases in gray matter volume in children with autism scanned at two time points (age ~11 and at 30-month follow-up) compared with TD children (Hardan et al., 2009).
While very few studies have examined functional connectivity in young children and toddlers with autism (Dinstein et al., 2011), some have started to use structural measures to examine high-risk infants, including siblings of children with autism. Wolff and colleagues report that infants with ASD showed higher fractional anisotropy (FA) of most fiber tracts at 6 months followed by a slower change over time relative to infants without ASD such that by 24 months of age, the infants with ASD had lower FA values (Wolff et al., 2012). This study suggests that aberrant development of white matter may precede the manifestation of autistic symptoms in the first year of life, and highlights the importance of longitudinal data and data from young children and infants with the disorder.
Lack of Pubertal Stage Assessment
As highlighted throughout, a potentially informative way of stratifying a sample would be to group individuals by pubertal stage to examine brain maturation as a function of sexual maturity in ASD. Explicit characterization of pubertal stage in research participants can be accomplished in one of several ways. The most widely used tool is the Tanner scale for assessing pubertal development. Tanner staging characterizes individuals along a puberty scale from 1 to 5 on the basis of pubic hair and breast development in females, and pubic hair and genital development in males (Tanner and Whitehouse, 1976). A physical exam carried out by a trained clinician is the typical mode of administration. While there are several limitations to Tanner staging (including ethnic homogeneity of the scale), the test is the current gold standard for puberty assessment. Self-report versions of the scale have also been developed [e.g., the Petersen Developmental Scale (PDS) (Petersen et al., 1988)]. Hormonal assays can also in principle be used to assess pubertal stage, but practical considerations limit their utility Blakemore et al. (2010). Adopting one of these approaches to pubertal assessment when studying adolescents will likely contribute to clarity and interpretability of neuroimaging findings in this population.
Insufficient Characterization of Heterogeneity
One significant obstacle to understanding the brain basis of ASD is the fact that the disorder (indeed, disorders) encompasses a wide range of abilities and levels of functioning. Almost no functional brain imaging data is available from individuals who are considered “low-functioning.” Additionally, because of the 4:1 male:female ratio in diagnosis (Werling and Geschwind, 2013), males with the disorder are much more prevalent and therefore receive the majority of attention from researchers. As a consequence, very little is known about gender-specific functional connectivity differences associated with the disorder. It has recently been shown that individuals with variants of the MET gene show differential patterns of resting-state functional connectivity, such that differences between ASD and controls were moderated by genotype (Rudie et al., 2012). This study highlights the important point that studies of disorders characterized by considerable heterogeneity, such as ASD, may need to be particularly mindful of potential genetic differences within their samples.
Reports of relationships between efficiency of functional brain networks and IQ (van den Heuvel et al., 2009) as well as between regional node properties and IQ (Wu et al., 2013) are beginning to emerge. Important directions for future work include assessing interactions between diagnostic category, IQ, and functional connectivity measures. One can speculate that a unique developmental trajectory might exist for children with ASD on the low-functioning end of the spectrum, compared with high-functioning ASD and typical development.
Direct Comparisons Between Task-Based and Resting-State Functional Connectivity
Both task-based and resting-state fMRI have been applied to the study of functional connectivity in ASD. However, to date no empirical work has investigated both types of measures in the same individual. It is clear that methodological choices in both task- and resting-state approaches can affect outcomes in autism neuroimaging studies (Muller et al., 2011). Further, it is increasingly recognized that intrinsic and evoked brain states interact in complex and unpredictable ways (He, 2013). As the field moves closer toward understanding the ways in which task-based and resting-state measures can meaningfully capture brain dynamics, it will continue to inform functional connectivity theories of autism and allow investigators to more confidently predict the conditions under which aberrant brain connectivity in ASD will manifest.
Relationships Between Functional and Structural Connectivity
As the focus of the current review is to summarize findings from the fMRI functional connectivity literature, and structural findings have recently been reviewed elsewhere (Schipul et al., 2011; Vissers et al., 2011), we have included only a limited discussion of the links between structural and functional connectivity here. Relationships between functional and structural connectivity are complex, even in the neurotypical adult brain (Damoiseaux and Greicius, 2009), and these relationships undergo significant changes with development (Supekar et al., 2010; Uddin et al., 2011). In a previously published review of structural connectivity changes in ASD (Vissers et al., 2011), it is noted by the authors that few studies exist simultaneously examining functional and structural changes in ASD. To our knowledge, there are only three reports that do so. In a study by Rudie and colleagues, the authors report that structural connectivity (measured by FA) between the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex did not show significant differences between ASD and TD children (Rudie et al., 2012). This group has also recently shown that there are no significant differences between groups with respect to structure-function correlations assessed at the whole brain level (Rudie et al., 2013). Finally, it was recently shown that in adults with ASD, reduced functional and structural connectivity can be observed in the right temporo-parietal junction and left frontal lobe (Mueller et al., 2013). The dearth of studies examining structure-function relationships and their development in ASD leaves several open questions that will need to be addressed by future multimodal imaging approaches.
Assessing Whole Brain vs. Region-Specific Patterns of Functional Connectivity
An important area for future work will be to understand functional connectivity abnormalities in ASD at the global level, across the whole brain, as well as in specific functional networks or sets of nodes. There is already evidence to suggest that in children with the disorder, widespread hyper-connectivity can be observed (Supekar et al., 2012; Uddin et al., 2013) alongside both hypo-connectivity (Abrams et al., 2013; Lynch et al., 2013) and hyper-connectivity (Di Martino et al., 2011) between subsets of specific regions. The immediate challenge will be to develop metrics to more systematically assess region-specific and large-scale patterns of connectivity and apply them uniformly to different age groups of individuals with ASD and TD controls.
Clinical Implications: Brain-Based Biomarkers
One of the goals of functional imaging of neurodevelopmental disorders is to quantify brain connectivity in ways that may eventually be used to develop brain-based biomarkers for objectively identifying children with disorders. Anderson and colleagues demonstrate that functional connectivity based classifiers perform more accurately on datasets from younger individuals (<20 years of age) with ASD (Anderson et al., 2011). These findings underscore the importance of understanding age-related changes in functional connectivity in ASD, as they have clear implications for the development of increasingly sophisticated approaches to diagnosis and evaluation of response to treatment. Functional connectivity measures can also aid in understanding unique and shared neural markers in ASD and comorbid conditions such as ADHD (Di Martino et al., 2013b). Our recent demonstration of high levels of classification accuracy based on examination of specific intrinsic large-scale networks in 7–12 year-old children highlights the utility of using data from narrower developmental windows to identify potential biomarkers for the disorder (Uddin et al., 2013).
Summary and Future Directions
Inadequate attention to critical age-related developmental stages has impeded our understanding of functional brain connectivity in ASD. Here we have (1) reviewed the emerging literature on intrinsic functional brain connectivity in ASD, (2) identified results of hypo- and hyper-connectivity as being partially attributable to the age of participants examined, and (3) proposed that longitudinal studies examining pre- and post-pubertal individuals with ASD are sorely needed to resolve current controversies regarding the nature of brain connectivity abnormalities in the disorder. A developmental perspective will contribute greatly to future research efforts in autism neuroimaging.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge Carl Feinstein for insightful discussions. This work was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health Career Development Award [K01MH092288] to Lucina Q. Uddin, as well as grants from the Singer Foundation, the Stanford Institute for Neuro-Innovation and Translational Neurosciences, and the National Institutes of Health [DC011095 and MH084164] to Vinod Menon. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH or the NIH.
Abbreviations
ASD, Autism spectrum disorder; fMRI, Functional magnetic resonance imaging; DMN, Default mode network; ICA, Independent component analysis.
References
Abrams, D. A., Lynch, C. J., Cheng, K. M., Phillips, J., Supekar, K., Ryali, S., et al. (2013). Underconnectivity between voice-selective cortex and reward circuitry in children with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 12060–12065. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302982110
Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J., and Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage 52, 336–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.010
Akshoomoff, N., Pierce, K., and Courchesne, E. (2002). The neurobiological basis of autism from a developmental perspective. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 613–634. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402003115
Amaral, D. G. (2010). The promise and the pitfalls of autism research: an introductory note for new autism researchers. Brain Res. 1380, 3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.077
Anderson, J. S., Druzgal, T. J., Froehlich, A., DuBray, M. B., Lange, N., Alexander, A. L., et al. (2011). Decreased interhemispheric functional connectivity in autism. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1134–1146. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq190
Assaf, M., Jagannathan, K., Calhoun, V. D., Miller, L., Stevens, M. C., Sahl, R., et al. (2010). Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage 53, 247–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067
Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 360, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634
Beckmann, C. F., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821
Belmonte, M. K., Allen, G., Beckel-Mitchener, A., Boulanger, L. M., Carper, R. A., and Webb, S. J. (2004). Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J. Neurosci. 24, 9228–9231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-04.2004
Belmonte, M. K., Mazziotta, J. C., Minshew, N. J., Evans, A. C., Courchesne, E., Dager, S. R., et al. (2008). Offering to share: how to put heads together in autism neuroimaging. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 2–13. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0352-2
Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409
Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage 61, 397–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080
Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., and Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 926–933. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21052
Bressler, S. L., and Menon, V. (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004
Cahill, L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 477–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909
Castellanos, F. X., and Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007
Cherkassky, V. L., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2006). Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. Neuroreport 17, 1687–1690. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239956.45448.4c
Cole, D. M., Smith, S. M., and Beckmann, C. F. (2010). Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:8. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008
Courchesne, E., Carper, R., and Akshoomoff, N. (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. JAMA 290, 337–344. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.3.337
Courchesne, E., Mouton, P. R., Calhoun, M. E., Semendeferi, K., Ahrens-Barbeau, C., Hallet, M. J., et al. (2011). Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. JAMA 306, 2001–2010. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1638
Courchesne, E., and Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.001
Crone, E. A., and Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636–650. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313
Damoiseaux, J. S., and Greicius, M. D. (2009). Greater than the sum of its parts: a review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 525–533. doi: 10.1007/s00429-009-0208-6
Deco, G., Ponce-Alvarez, A., Mantini, D., Romani, G. L., Hagmann, P., Corbetta, M. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity emerges from structurally and dynamically shaped slow linear fluctuations. J. Neurosci. 33, 11239–11252.
Deen, B., and Pelphrey, K. A. (2011). “Large-Scale Cortical Functional Connectivity in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders,” in Paper presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, San Diego, CA.
Di Martino, A., Kelly, C., Grzadzinski, R., Zuo, X. N., Mennes, M., Mairena, M. A., et al. (2011). Aberrant striatal functional connectivity in children with autism. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.029
Di Martino, A., Yan, C. G., Li, Q., Denio, E., Castellanos, F. X., Alaerts, K., et al. (2013a). The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol. Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78. [Epub ahead of print].
Di Martino, A., Zuo, X. N., Kelly, C., Grzadzinski, R., Mennes, M., Schvarcz, A., et al. (2013b). Shared and distinct intrinsic functional network centrality in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.011. [Epub ahead of print].
Dinstein, I., Pierce, K., Eyler, L., Solso, S., Malach, R., Behrmann, M., et al. (2011). Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers with autism. Neuron 70, 1218–1225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.018
Ebisch, S. J., Gallese, V., Willems, R. M., Mantini, D., Groen, W. B., Romani, G. L., et al. (2010). Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula regions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1013–1028. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21085
Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Dosenbach, N. U., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Barch, D. M., et al. (2008). The maturing architecture of the brain's default network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 4028–4032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800376105
Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., et al. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5:e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381
Filippini, N., MacIntosh, B. J., Hough, M. G., Goodwin, G. M., Frisoni, G. B., Smith, S. M., et al. (2009). Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 7209–7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106
Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102
Friston, K. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107
Galvan, A., Van Leijenhorst, L., and McGlennen, K. M. (2012). Considerations for imaging the adolescent brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 293–302. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.02.002
Geschwind, D. H., and Levitt, P. (2007). Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158
Gotts, S. J., Simmons, W. K., Milbury, L. A., Wallace, G. L., Cox, R. W., and Martin, A. (2012). Fractionation of social brain circuits in autism spectrum disorders. Brain 135, 2711–2725. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws160
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100
Hagmann, P., Sporns, O., Madan, N., Cammoun, L., Pienaar, R., Wedeen, V. J., et al. (2010). White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in the late developing human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 19067–19072. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009073107
Hardan, A. Y., Libove, R. A., Keshavan, M. S., Melhem, N. M., and Minshew, N. J. (2009). A preliminary longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of brain volume and cortical thickness in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 320–326. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.024
He, B. J. (2013). Spontaneous and task-evoked brain activity negatively interact. J. Neurosci. 33, 4672–4682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2922-12.2013.
Investigators. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders–autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR. Surveill. Summ. 61, 1–19.
Jones, T. B., Bandettini, P. A., Kenworthy, L., Case, L. K., Milleville, S. C., Martin, A., et al. (2010). Sources of group differences in functional connectivity: an investigation applied to autism spectrum disorder. Neuroimage 49, 401–414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.051
Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., Kana, R. K., and Minshew, N. J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb. Cortex 17, 951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006
Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., and Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 127(Pt 8), 1811–1821. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh199
Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., Malave, V. L., Kana, R. K., and Varma, S. (2012). Autism as a neural systems disorder: a theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1292–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., and Just, M. A. (2006). Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain 129(Pt 9), 2484–2493. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl164
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., and Just, M. A. (2009). Atypical frontal-posterior synchronization of Theory of Mind regions in autism during mental state attribution. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 135–152. doi: 10.1080/17470910802198510
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., and Just, M. A. (2007). Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.004
Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E., and Moore, M. S. (2011). Disrupted cortical connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum disorders. Phys. Life Rev. 8, 410–437. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.001
Kelly, A. M., Di Martino, A., Uddin, L. Q., Shehzad, Z., Gee, D. G., Reiss, P. T., et al. (2009). Development of anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to early adulthood. Cereb. Cortex 19, 640–657. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn117
Kennedy, D. P., and Adolphs, R. (2012). The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 559–572. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006
Kennedy, D. P., and Courchesne, E. (2008). The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. Neuroimage 39, 1877–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052
Kleinhans, N. M., Richards, T., Sterling, L., Stegbauer, K. C., Mahurin, R., Johnson, L. C., et al. (2008). Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. Brain 131, 1000–1012. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm334
Koshino, H., Carpenter, P. A., Minshew, N. J., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage 24, 810–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028
Koshino, H., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., and Just, M. A. (2008). fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cereb. Cortex 18, 289–300. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm054
Lainhart, J. E., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Landa, R., Santangelo, S. L., Coon, H., et al. (1997). Macrocephaly in children and adults with autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 282–290. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00019
LeBlanc, J. J., and Fagiolini, M. (2011). Autism: a “critical period” disorder? Neural Plast. 2011, 921680. doi: 10.1155/2011/921680
Lenroot, R. K., and Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 718–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001
Lynch, C. J., Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., and Menon, V. (2013). Default mode network in childhood autism: posteromedial cortex heterogeneity and relationship with social deficits. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.013
Mason, R. A., Williams, D. L., Kana, R. K., Minshew, N., and Just, M. A. (2008). Theory of Mind disruption and recruitment of the right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism. Neuropsychologia 46, 269–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.018
Mizuno, A., Villalobos, M. E., Davies, M. M., Dahl, B. C., and Muller, R. A. (2006). Partially enhanced thalamocortical functional connectivity in autism. Brain Res. 1104, 160–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.064
Monk, C. S., Peltier, S. J., Wiggins, J. L., Weng, S. J., Carrasco, M., Risi, S., et al. (2009). Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage 47, 764–772. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.069
Mueller, S., Keeser, D., Samson, A. C., Kirsch, V., Blautzik, J., Grothe, M., et al. (2013). Convergent findings of altered functional and structural brain connectivity in individuals with high functioning autism: a multimodal MRI Study. PLoS ONE 8:e67329. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067329
Muller, R. A., Shih, P., Keehn, B., Deyoe, J. R., Leyden, K. M., and Shukla, D. K. (2011). Underconnected, but how? A survey of functional connectivity MRI studies in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2233–2243. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq296
Nir, Y., Mukamel, R., Dinstein, I., Privman, E., Harel, M., Fisch, L., et al. (2008). Interhemispheric correlations of slow spontaneous neuronal fluctuations revealed in human sensory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1100–1108. doi: 10.1038/nn.2177
Noonan, S. K., Haist, F., and Muller, R. A. (2009). Aberrant functional connectivity in autism: evidence from low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations. Brain Res. 1262, 48–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.076
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., and Boxer, A. (1988). Self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17, 117–133. doi: 10.1007/BF01537962
Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59, 2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018
Rubenstein, J. L., and Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2, 255–267. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x
Rudie, J. D., Brown, J. A., Beck-Pancer, D., Hernandez, L. M., Dennis, E. L., Thompson, P. M., et al. (2013). Altered functional and structural brain network organization in autism. Neuroimage Clin. 2, 79–94. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2012.11.006
Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Brown, J. A., Beck-Pancer, D., Colich, N. L., Gorrindo, P., et al. (2012). Autism-associated promoter variant in MET impacts functional and structural brain networks. Neuron 75, 904–915. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.010
Ryali, S., Chen, T., Supekar, K., and Menon, V. (2012). Estimation of functional connectivity in fMRI data using stability selection-based sparse partial correlation with elastic net penalty. Neuroimage 59, 3852–3861. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.054
Satterthwaite, T. D., Elliott, M. A., Gerraty, R. T., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Calkins, M. E., et al. (2013). An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage 64, 240–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052
Satterthwaite, T. D., Wolf, D. H., Loughead, J., Ruparel, K., Elliott, M. A., Hakonarson, H., et al. (2012). Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage 60, 623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063
Schipul, S. E., Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2011). Inter-regional brain communication and its disturbance in autism. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:10. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00010
Sepulcre, J., Liu, H., Talukdar, T., Martincorena, I., Yeo, B. T., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). The organization of local and distant functional connectivity in the human brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 6:e1000808. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000808
Shih, P., Keehn, B., Oram, J. K., Leyden, K. M., Keown, C. L., and Muller, R. A. (2011). Functional differentiation of posterior superior temporal sulcus in autism: a functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 270–277. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.040
Shih, P., Shen, M., Ottl, B., Keehn, B., Gaffrey, M. S., and Muller, R. A. (2010). Atypical network connectivity for imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 48, 2931–2939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.035
Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S. J., Ursu, S., Ravizza, S., Cummings, N., Ly, S., et al. (2009). The neural substrates of cognitive control deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47, 2515–2526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.019
Sporns, O. (2011). From simple graphs to the connectome: networks in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 62, 881–886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.085
Stefanatos, G. A. (2008). Regression in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol. Rev. 18, 305–319. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9073-y
Supekar, K., Musen, M., and Menon, V. (2009). Development of large-scale functional brain networks in children. PLoS Biol. 7:e1000157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000157
Supekar, K., Uddin, L. Q., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., Gaillard, W., Kenworthy, L., et al. (2012). “Widespread brain hyper-connectivity in children with autism,” in Paper Presented at the Organization for Human Brain Mapping, (Beijing).
Supekar, K., Uddin, L. Q., Prater, K., Amin, H., Greicius, M. D., and Menon, V. (2010). Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children. Neuroimage 52, 290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.009
Tanner, J. M., and Whitehouse, R. H. (1976). Clinical longitudinal standards for height, weight, height velocity, weight velocity, and stages of puberty. Arch. Dis. Child. 51, 170–179. doi: 10.1136/adc.51.3.170
Testa-Silva, G., Loebel, A., Giugliano, M., de Kock, C. P., Mansvelder, H. D., and Meredith, R. M. (2011). Hyperconnectivity and slow synapses during early development of medial prefrontal cortex in a mouse model for mental retardation and autism. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1333–1342. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr224
Thai, N. J., Longe, O., and Rippon, G. (2009). Disconnected brains: what is the role of fMRI in connectivity research? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 73, 27–32.
Turner, K. C., Frost, L., Linsenbardt, D., McIlroy, J. R., and Muller, R. A. (2006). Atypically diffuse functional connectivity between caudate nuclei and cerebral cortex in autism. Behav. Brain Funct. 2, 34. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-34
Tyszka, J. M., Kennedy, D. P., Paul, L. K., and Adolphs, R. (2013). Largely typical patterns of resting-state functional connectivity in high-functioning adults with autism. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht040. [Epub ahead of print].
Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., Lynch, C. J., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., Feinstein, C., et al. (2013). Salience network-based classification and prediction of symptom severity in children with autism. JAMA Psychiatry. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.104. [Epub ahead of print].
Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., and Menon, V. (2010). Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: insights from resting-state FMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:21. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00021
Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K. S., Ryali, S., and Menon, V. (2011). Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development. J. Neurosci. 31, 18578–18589. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-11.2011
Uhlhaas, P. J., Roux, F., Rodriguez, E., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., and Singer, W. (2010). Neural synchrony and the development of cortical networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.002
van den Heuvel, M. P., Stam, C. J., Kahn, R. S., and Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance. J. Neurosci. 29, 7619–7624. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009
Van Dijk, K. R., Sabuncu, M. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2012). The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59, 431–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044
Villalobos, M. E., Mizuno, A., Dahl, B. C., Kemmotsu, N., and Muller, R. A. (2005). Reduced functional connectivity between V1 and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. Neuroimage 25, 916–925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.022
Vissers, M. E., Cohen, M. X., and Geurts, H. M. (2011). Brain connectivity and high functioning autism: a promising path of research that needs refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral links. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 604–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.003
von dem Hagen, E. A. H., Stoyanova, R. S., Baron-Cohen, S., and Calder, A. J. (2013). Reduced functional connectivity within and between ‘social’ resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss053. [Epub ahead of print].
Washington, S. D., Gordon, E. M., Brar, J., Warburton, S., Sawyer, A. T., Wolfe, A., et al. (2013). Dysmaturation of the default mode network in autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22252. [Epub ahead of print].
Wass, S. (2011). Distortions and disconnections: disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain Cogn. 75, 18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.005
Welchew, D. E., Ashwin, C., Berkouk, K., Salvador, R., Suckling, J., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2005). Functional disconnectivity of the medial temporal lobe in Asperger's syndrome. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 991–998. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.028
Weng, S. J., Wiggins, J. L., Peltier, S. J., Carrasco, M., Risi, S., Lord, C., et al. (2010). Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 1313, 202–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.057
Werling, D. M., and Geschwind, D. H. (2013). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 26, 146–153. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548
Wolff, J. J., Gu, H., Gerig, G., Elison, J. T., Styner, M., Gouttard, S., et al. (2012). Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 589–600. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11091447
Wu, K., Taki, Y., Sato, K., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Takeuchi, H., et al. (2013). Topological organization of functional brain networks in healthy children: differences in relation to age, sex, and intelligence. PLoS ONE 8:e55347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055347
Yerys, B. E., Jankowski, K. F., Shook, D., Rosenberger, L. R., Barnes, K. A., Berl, M. M., et al. (2009). The fMRI success rate of children and adolescents: typical development, epilepsy, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3426–3435. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20767.
You, X., Norr, M., Murphy, E., Sokoloff, J., Yerys, B., Gaillard, W., et al. (2012). “Atypical development of local and distant intrinsic functional connectivity in ASD,” in Paper Presented at the Organization for Human Brain Mapping, (Beijing).
Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, brain development, functional connectivity, puberty, fMRI
Citation: Uddin LQ, Supekar K and Menon V (2013) Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:458. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00458
Received: 18 April 2013; Accepted: 22 July 2013;
Published online: 07 August 2013.
Edited by:
Rajesh K. Kana, University of Alabama at Birmingham, USAReviewed by:
Qingbao Yu, The Mind Research Network, USAR. Matthew Hutchison, Western University, Canada
Timothy A. Keller, Carnegie Mellon University, USA
Diane L. Williams, Duquesne University, USA
Copyright © 2013 Uddin, Supekar and Menon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Lucina Q. Uddin, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, 401 Quarry Rd., Stanford, CA 94305-5719, USA e-mail: lucina@stanford.edu