Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 June 2013
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Healthy and Diseased Brain Networks View all 31 articles

Extraversion and neuroticism relate to topological properties of resting-state brain networks


Qing Gao* Qing Gao1*Qiang Xu Qiang Xu2Xujun Duan Xujun Duan2Wei Liao Wei Liao2Jurong Ding Jurong Ding2Zhiqiang Zhang Zhiqiang Zhang3Yuan Li Yuan Li4Guangming Lu and Huafu Chen* Guangming Lu3 and Huafu Chen2*
  • 1School of Mathematical Sciences, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
  • 2Key Laboratory for Neuroinformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
  • 3Department of Medical Imaging, Jinling Hospital, Clinical School, Medical College, Nanjing University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, China
  • 4School of Politics and Public Managements, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China

With the advent and development of modern neuroimaging techniques, there is an increasing interest in linking extraversion and neuroticism to anatomical and functional brain markers. Here, we aimed to test the theoretically derived biological personality model as proposed by Eysenck using graph theoretical analyses. Specifically, the association between the topological organization of whole-brain functional networks and extraversion/neuroticism was explored. To construct functional brain networks, functional connectivity among 90 brain regions was measured by temporal correlation using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data of 71 healthy subjects. Graph theoretical analysis revealed a positive association of extraversion scores and normalized clustering coefficient values. These results suggested a more clustered configuration in brain networks of individuals high in extraversion, which could imply a higher arousal threshold and higher levels of arousal tolerance in the cortex of extraverts. On a local network level, we observed that a specific nodal measure, i.e., betweenness centrality (BC), was positively associated with neuroticism scores in the right precentral gyrus (PreCG), right caudate nucleus, right olfactory cortex, and bilateral amygdala. For individuals high in neuroticism, these results suggested a more frequent participation of these specific regions in information transition within the brain network and, in turn, may partly explain greater regional activation levels and lower arousal thresholds in these regions. In contrast, extraversion scores were positively correlated with BC in the right insula, while negatively correlated with BC in the bilateral middle temporal gyrus (MTG), indicating that the relationship between extraversion and regional arousal is not as simple as proposed by Eysenck.

Introduction

In Eysenck's personality theory, he proposed three fundamental dimensions of personality: extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism (Eysenck, 1967; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985). It is now acknowledged that extraversion and neuroticism have their biological bases (Matthews and Gilliland, 1999), while the neuropsychology of the third dimension, psychoticism, has not been worked out in detail. The arousal theory of Eysenck (1967) related extraversion to arousability of the reticulo-cortical circuit and proposed a higher arousal threshold in cortex and higher levels of arousal tolerance in extraverts (Eysenck, 1967; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985; Fischer et al., 1998). Neuroticism, on the other hand, is associated with arousability of the limbic circuit, such that individuals with higher neuroticism scores have greater activation levels and lower thresholds within subcortical structures (Eysenck, 1990; Wei et al., 2012).

With the advent and development of modern neuroimaging techniques, there is increasing interest in exploring neuroanatomical or neurofunctional correlates of extraversion and neuroticism, to test the theoretically proposed biological explanation of the two fundamental dimensions. Neuroanatomical studies have found extraversion was associated with structural/anatomic variations in the middle and inferior frontal regions, fusiform gyrus, and insula (INS), whereas neuroticism was associated with variations in the orbitofrontal cortex, precentral gyrus (PreCG), and amygdala (AMYG) (Rauch et al., 2005; Omura et al., 2005b; Wright et al., 2006, 2007; Sollberger et al., 2009; DeYoung, 2010). In neurofunctional studies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments have also demonstrated that specific brain regions that are engaged during cognitive-affective tasks were associated with specific personality dimensions. For example, activations in the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, anterior cingulated cortex (ACC), and middle temporal gyrus (MTG) were correlated with extraversion (Canli et al., 2001; Eisenberger et al., 2005; Hutcherson et al., 2008; Tamura et al., 2012), while activations in the frontal cortex, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, and AMYG were related to neuroticism (Canli et al., 2001; Haas et al., 2007; Hooker et al., 2008; Harenski et al., 2009). In addition, a positron emission tomography (PET) study assessing resting regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) found that regions in ACC and temporal lobes were correlated with extraversion (Johnson et al., 1999). Extraversion was associated with regional cerebral glucose metabolism (rCMRglu) assessed by PET in right putamen, while neuroticism was associated with rCMRglu in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) (Kim et al., 2008). These studies indicated specialized, spatially distributed regions were associated with personality dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism, respectively, and provided neurobiological evidence for the hypothesized biological model of Eysenck's personality.

Instead of detection activation paradigms by task-based fMRI, resting-state fMRI studies observe intrinsic spontaneous fluctuations in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI signal while avoiding the constraints of task-based approaches (Raichle et al., 2001; Fox and Raichle, 2007; Raichle and Snyder, 2007; Adelstein et al., 2011). There is accumulating evidence for local characteristics of resting brain functions associated with personality dimensions using resting-state fMRI (Kunisato et al., 2011; Wei et al., 2011, 2012; Hahn et al., 2012). Using regional homogeneity (ReHo) approach, our prior study found ReHo was correlated negatively with extraversion in the MPFC, and correlated positively in INS, cerebellum, and cingulate gyrus; whereas neuroticism had negative correlation with ReHo in left middle frontal gyrus (Wei et al., 2011). In addition, by using other local characteristics, i.e., the fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (fALFF), our previous study found positive correlations between LFF amplitude at Slow-5 and extraversion in MPFC and PCU, and between LFF amplitude at Slow-5 and neuroticism in right PreCG; LFF amplitude at Slow-4 was negatively associated with extraversion and neuroticism in left hippocampus (HIP) and bilateral superior temporal cortex (STC), respectively (Wei et al., 2012). Table 1 summarizes the main results on the characteristics of resting brain functions associated with extraversion and neuroticism in recent resting-state fMRI studies.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The main results on the characteristics of resting brain functions associated with extraversion and neuroticism in recent resting-state fMRI studies.

From a functional integration perspective in the human brain, the multiple spatially distinct brain regions are functionally connected with coherent temporal dynamics (Friston et al., 1997; Sporns et al., 2000; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009), making up complex and reciprocal brain networks even when we are at rest (Greicius et al., 2003; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009). Such networks are thought to provide the physiological basis for information processing and mental representation (Canli, 2004; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). Furthermore, evidence for small-world attributes of brain networks has been reported in the relative studies (Sporns et al., 2004; Stam, 2004; Eguiluz et al., 2005; Achard et al., 2006; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009), indicating that small-world architectures in brain networks deviating from randomness reflect their specific functionality (Watts and Strogatz, 1998; Latora and Marchiori, 2001; Stam and Reijneveld, 2007; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; He and Evans, 2010). Since personality factors may well be related to the networks in the brain (Canli, 2004; Wilt and Revelle, 2009), the analysis of task-independent, resting-state functional connectivity may reveal the intrinsically organized functional brain networks (Biswal et al., 1995), and allow for a better understanding of the neurobiological bases of extraversion and neuroticism. The recent study by Adelstein et al. (2011) found that extraversion and neuroticism were encoded within resting-state functional connectivity between seed regions and the lateral paralimbic regions and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, respectively (Adelstein et al., 2011). However, the study was seed-based and lacked a network perspective on brain dynamics. In the present study, we hypothesized that the topological organization of the whole-brain functional networks would be associated with inter-individual variations in extraversion and neuroticism, and would link to Eysenck's cortical arousal theory of the two dimensions. To test our hypothesis, an exploratory analysis based on graph theory was thereby performed on the resting-state fMRI data of 71 healthy subjects, to detect the intrinsic resting-state functional connectivity mechanisms underlying the two personality dimensions.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We conducted the analysis with the same dataset in our previous study (Wei et al., 2011). Eighty-seven healthy right-handed subjects (48 males; age range: 17–36 yrs, mean age: 23.5 yrs) with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders participated in the study. The present study was approved by the local Medical Ethics Committee at Jinling Hospital, Nanjing University School of Medicine, and the informed written consents were obtained from all participants.

Personality Questionnaires

The revised Eysenck personality questionnaire short scale for Chinese (EPQ-RSC) (Eysenck, 1991; Qian et al., 2000) was used to assess personality dimensions of extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism of each subject before MRI scanning. Raw scores of the three dimensions were then converted into T-scores using the formula (Qian et al., 2000), respectively:

T=50+10×rawscoremeanSD,

where mean represents the mean value of the personality scores over all the subjects; SD is the standard deviation of the personality scores. We focused our analyses on extraversion and neuroticism whose resultant T-scores were used for calculating correlations with the brain network metrics.

Image Acquisition

Resting-state fMRI images were acquired using a single-shot, gradient-recalled echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence on a 3.0-T Siemens Trio scanner (Jinling Hospital, Nanjing, China). The acquisition parameters were: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, field of view (FOV) = 240 mm, image matrix size = 64 × 64, voxel size = 3.75 × 3.75 × 4 mm3, 30 transverse slices without slice gap, flip angle = 90°, and a total of 255 volumes for each subject.

Data Preprocessing

Data preprocessing was performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The first five volumes were discarded to ensure steady-state longitudinal magnetization. The remaining resting-state fMRI images were first corrected by the acquisition time delay among different slices, and then realigned to the first volume for head-motion correction. The dataset with translational or rotational parameters exceeding ±1 mm or ±1° would be excluded, according to our previous study on functional connectivity network (Liao et al., 2010). The images of remaining 71 participants were further spatially normalized into a standard stereotaxic space at 3 × 3 × 3 mm3, using the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template in SPM8. In order to avoid artificially introducing local spatial correlation, no spatial smoothing was applied, as previous studies suggested (Salvador et al., 2005; Achard et al., 2006; Achard and Bullmore, 2007; Liao et al., 2010). Since recent studies have showed that functional connectivity analysis is sensitive to gross head motion effects (Power et al., 2012; Van Dijk et al., 2012), we further evaluated the framewise displacement (FD) (Power et al., 2012) to express instantaneous head motion, and the threshold of 0.5 was suggested. The mean ± SD of FD over subjects was: 0.1080 ± 0.0159. Six subjects' FD values were beyond 0.5, but only in one frame for each subject. Scrubbing process was performed using toolbox “ArtRepair” in SPM8.

The mean time series of each ROI was corrected by a linear regression to remove the possible spurious variances including six head motion parameters acquired from the SPM8 preprocessing, the white matter (WM) and the ventricular brain signals averaged from a WM mask and a ventricular mask respectively (Fox et al., 2005; Salvador et al., 2005; Tian et al., 2006; Liao et al., 2010). The residuals of these regressions were temporally band-pass filtered (0.01 < f < 0.08 Hz) to reduce low-frequency drifts and physiological high-frequency respiratory and cardiac noise (Biswal et al., 1995), and linearly detrended for further functional connectivity and graph-theory analysis (Tian et al., 2006; Liao et al., 2010). The following approaches based on graph theory were performed by an in-house program coded in MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, MA).

Computation of Functional Connectivity Network

Node definition

To define the brain nodes, the anatomical parcellation was performed using the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) template, segmenting the images into 90 anatomical regions of interests (ROIs) (45 ROIs for each hemisphere). The representative time series in each ROI was obtained by averaging the fMRI time series across all voxels in the ROI.

Edge definition

To define the network edges, the residuals of the regression analysis were used to compute the Pearson's correlation, resulting in a 90 × 90 correlation matrix for each subject. A Fisher's r-to-z transformation was applied to the correlation matrices of all the subjects to improve the normality of the correlation coefficients (r) (Liu et al., 2008). The undirected edge eij between node i and node j is defined as:

eij={1when|rij|>T0otherwise

In general, if the absolute value of rij of a pair of brain regions, i and j, exceeds a predefined threshold T, an edge is assumed to exist; otherwise, no existence would be assumed (Liao et al., 2010).

Graph Theoretical Analysis

Network metrics

The topological properties of the brain functional networks can be measured by both nodal and global network measures. In this study, we calculated the nodal measures including the degree Ki, the clustering coefficient Ci, the minimum path length Li, the efficiency Ei, and the betweenness centrality BCi of a node i; the global measures including the average degree K, the network efficiency involving the local efficiency Elocal and the global efficiency Eglobal, the characteristic path length L, the clustering coefficient of a network C, the normalized clustering coefficient γ, the normalized characteristic path length λ, and the small-worldness σ.

Degree. The degree at each node, Ki, i = 1, 2,…, 90, is defined as the number of nodes in a subgraph Gi, which is the graph including the nodes that are direct neighbors of node i. Briefly, Ki denotes to which extent the node is connected to the rest of the network (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Wang et al., 2010). A node with a higher degree has more connections (where each connection is counted once). The average degree K is the mean of Ki of all the nodes in the network.

Clustering coefficient. The absolute clustering coefficient Ci of a node is the ratio between the number of existing connections and the number of all possible connections in the subgraph Gi. Ci quantifies the level of local connectedness within a network (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; He and Evans, 2010)

Ci=eiKi(Ki1)/2,

where ei is the number of edges in the subgraph Gi. The clustering coefficient of the network C is the mean of Ci of all the nodes in the network.

Minimum path length. The nodal minimum path length Li is defined as the mean shortest absolute path length of node i to other nodes in a network (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009), which quantifies the level of routing efficiency or the capability for parallel information propagation of a network (Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; He and Evans, 2010; Liao et al., 2010)

Li=1N1ijGmin{Li,j},

where min {Li,j} is the shortest absolute path length between node i and node j, and the absolute path length is the number of edges included in the path connecting two nodes. The characteristic path length L is the mean of Li of all the nodes in the network.

Efficiency. The nodal efficiency Ei is the inverse of the harmonic mean of the length between node i and all other nodes in the network, to deal with the disconnected graphs, non-sparse graphs or both (Latora and Marchiori, 2001; Bassett and Bullmore, 2006; Wang et al., 2010)

Ei=1N1jGji1min{Li,j}.

The global efficiency Eglobal of the network is the mean of Ei of all the nodes in the network.

In the subgraph Gi, we can calculate the local efficiency of node i as:

Ei_local=1NGi(NGi1)j,kGijk1min{Lj,k}.

The local efficiency Elocal of the network is then similarly defined as the mean of Ei_local of all the nodes in the network (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

Betweenness centrality. The betweenness centrality BCi is defined as the fraction of all shortest paths in the network that pass through node i (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). BCi describes the central nodes that participate in many short paths within a network, and consequently act as important controls of information flow (Freeman, 1978)

BCi=1(N1)(N2)j,kGijkρj,k(i)ρj,k,

where ρj, k is the number of shortest paths between node j and k; ρj, k (i) is the number of shortest paths between j and k that pass through node i (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

Small-world parameters. Compared with random networks characterized by a low clustering coefficient and a typical short path length, networks with a small-world organization have a higher clustering coefficient and similar path length, i.e., γ = C/Crandom > 1, λ = L/Lrandom ≈ 1, namely normalized clustering coefficient and normalized characteristic path length, respectively (Watts and Strogatz, 1998). These two conditions can also be summarized into a quantitative measurement, σ = γ/λ > 1, namely small-worldness (Humphries et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2010). Crandom and Lrandom were calculated as the averaged clustering coefficient and characteristic path length of a set of 100 random networks with the same degree distribution as that of the examined functional connectivity network (Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; Liao et al., 2010). The random networks were generated based on a Markov-chain algorithm, according to our previous study (Liao et al., 2010).

Threshold selection

The threshold T was defined as the total number of edges in a graph divided by the maximum possible number of edges (Achard and Bullmore, 2007), namely wiring cost. We investigated the topological properties of brain functional network over a range of TminTTmax. (1) Tmin was selected by thresholding all networks to construct a sparse graph with the average degree K ≥ 2 × log (N) (here N = 90 represents the number of nodes); (2) Tmax was selected to ensure the small-worldness σ of the thresholded networks be larger than 1.1 for all participants (Liao et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2011). The resultant threshold range of 0.10 ≤ T ≤ 0.31 was used in our study. This range of sparsity allows the thresholded networks to be estimable for small-worldness and the number of spurious edges to be minimized (Watts and Strogatz, 1998; Achard and Bullmore, 2007; He et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2011).

Association Between Network Organization and Personality Dimensions

All the nodal and global measures were thresholded repeatedly over the range of 0.1 ≤ T ≤ 0.31 with an interval of 0.01, and the area under the curve (AUC) for each network metric was calculated, which provides a summarized scalar for topological characterization of brain networks independent of single threshold selection (Zhang et al., 2011). The partial correlation was then calculated between the AUC of each network metric and extraversion/neuroticism scores, with age and gender being covariates.

To assess the statistical significance of the correlation, the null distribution for each network metric was obtained by non-parametric permutation tests. Accordingly, 5000 subject specific random networks were generated at each threshold as null-model reference networks. The correlations between the AUC of each network metric and the personality scores were recalculated to obtain the null distribution. 1/number of regions was used as a false-positive correction, which implied that there was less than one false positive regional result per cortical map at this threshold (Lynall et al., 2010; Fornito et al., 2011).

Leave-One-Out Prediction

To test the validity of the significantly correlated measures in predicting personality scores of extraversion and neuroticism, a leave-one-out cross-validation strategy was applied. The significantly correlated measures acted as explanatory variables in the linear regression models to predict the personality scores. The predicted results of all the subjects were assessed by calculating the Pearson's correlation between the predicted values and the original values. The precision of individual prediction was assessed by the average of the absolute relative errors between the predicted and original scores.

Results

Descriptive Statistics of the Personality Dimensions

Table 2 describes the scores of the three personality dimensions from the EPQ-RSC questionnaire, and Table 3 describes the correlations across the scores of the three dimensions. As two dimensions concerned in the present study, extraversion had a moderate negative correlation with neuroticism (r = −0.238, p = 0.046). The result was concordant with many prior studies, suggesting an inverse relationship between extraversion and neuroticism (Rusting and Larsen, 1997; Wright et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2008). Therefore, we added extraversion (or neuroticism) scores as covariate when calculating the partial correlation between neuroticism (or extraversion) and the AUC of each network metric, to obtain effects that were uniquely driven by each personality dimension.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of the three personality dimensions of 71 participants.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between scores of the three personality dimensions.

The Associations Between Network Metrics and Extraversion

Among all the global measures of the network calculated in the present study, only the AUC of normalized clustering coefficient γ showed significant correlation with extraversion (Figure 1). As for the nodal measures, results indicated that only the AUC of BCi showed significant correlations with extraversion. Extraversion significantly increased with BCi in left INS, while significantly decreased with BCi in bilateral MTG. Figure 2 demonstrates the brain regions showing significant correlations between their BCi and extraversion scores along with the corresponding correlation coefficients.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The correlation between the area under the curve (AUC) of γ and extraversion scores (p < 1.90). AUC was calculated over the range of 0.1 ≤ T ≤ 0.31 with an interval of 0.01.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The brain regions showing significantly correlations between AUC of BCi and extraversion scores (p < 1.90). AUC was calculated over the range of 0.1 ≤ T ≤ 0.31 with an interval of 0.01. The cyan color represents the negative correlations, while the magenta color represents the positive correlations. INS, insular; L, left; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; R, right.

Figure 3 depicts the topological characteristics of network metrics which have significant associations with extraversion, as a function of wiring cost thresholds. The asterisk indicates the threshold where the significant correlation between the metric and extraversion was detected (permutation testing, p < 1.90). The inset figure indicates the correlation between the metric and extraversion at wiring cost = 0.22.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The topological characteristics of network metrics which have significant associations with extraversion, as a function of wiring cost thresholds. The asterisk indicates the threshold where the significant correlation between the metric and extraversion was detected (permutation testing, p < 1/90). The inset figure indicates the correlation between the metric and extraversion at wiring cost = 0.22.

The Associations Between Network Metrics and Neuroticism

No global measures showed significant correlation with neuroticism. Significant correlations were revealed in the AUC of BCi, too. Neuroticism scores showed increased significant correlation with BCi in right PreCG, right olfactory cortex (OLF), right caudate nucleus (CAU), and bilateral AMYG. No significantly negative correlation was found. Figure 4 indicates the brain regions showing significant correlations between BCi and neuroticism scores along with the corresponding correlation coefficients.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The brain regions showing significantly correlations between AUC of BCi and neuroticism scores (p < 1.90). AUC was calculated over the range of 0.1 ≤ T ≤ 0.31 with an interval of 0.01. AMYG, amygdala; CAU, caudate nucleus; L, left; OLF, olfactory cortex; PreCG, precentral gyrus; R, right.

Figure 5 depicts the topological characteristics of BCi as a function of wiring cost thresholds, in the brain regions whose BCi values have significant associations with neuroticism. The asterisk also indicates the threshold where the significant correlation between the metric and neuroticism was detected (permutation testing, p < 1.90). The inset figure indicates the correlation between the metric and neuroticism at wiring cost = 0.22.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The topological characteristics of BCi as a function of wiring cost thresholds, in the brain regions whose BCi values have significant associations with neuroticism. The asterisk also indicates the threshold where the significant correlation between the metric and neuroticism was detected (permutation testing, p < 1/90). The inset figure indicates the correlation between the metric and neuroticism at wiring cost = 0.22.

The Prediction of Personality Scores by Leave-One-Out Approach

Figure 6 shows the predicted and original pairs of extraversion (Figure 6A) and neuroticism (Figure 6B) scores, respectively. The Pearson's correlation coefficients of the predicted and original personality scores were 0.536 (p = 0.146 × 10−7) for extraversion, and 0.547 (p = 0.784 × 10−8) for neuroticism. The precisions of individual prediction were 11.4% for extraversion and 21.7% for neuroticism.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The correlations between the predicted and original personality scores. (A) Extraversion scores (r = 0.536, p = 0.146 × 10−7); (B) neuroticism scores (r = 0.547, p = 0.784 × 10−8).

Discussion

Methodological Considerations

The present study differed from our previous studies in both hypothesis and analysis methods. In the previous studies, the purpose was to identify the associations between the personality dimensions and the local synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity (Wei et al., 2011), or between the personality dimensions and the fLFF in individual brain regions (Wei et al., 2012). Thereby the analysis method as well as the results obtained was at the functional segregation level.

Since the multiple spatially distinct brain regions are functionally connected with coherent temporal dynamics, the topological properties of the brain functional networks may predict individual differences in the two fundamental personality dimensions. To test this hypothesis, in the present study, we applied the graph theory method to explore the correlation between the network metrics in the resting-state brain network and the personality dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism at the functional integration level. To the best of our knowledge, the present study is among the first demonstrations of an association between personality dimensions and the properties of the resting-state functional network.

Extraversion and the Network Metrics

The present results showed that compared to individuals with lower extraversion scores, individuals with higher extraversion scores had larger AUC of normalized clustering coefficient γ. γ quantifies the extent of local cliquishness or local efficiency of information transfer of a network (He and Evans, 2010). Our results indicated that the whole functional brain networks of individuals with higher extraversion scores were more clustered than these of individuals with lower extraversion scores.

In Eysenck's biological theory of extraversion, extraverts have a higher threshold for cortical arousal, as they are assumed to be chronically “under-aroused” (Eysenck, 1967; Tran et al., 2006; Wilt and Revelle, 2009). It is this chronic intrinsic under-arousal which is thought to drive highly extraverted people to engage in typically extraverted behaviors in order to enhance their low arousal states (Eysenck, 1994; Kehoe et al., 2012). Thereby extraverts should, on average, respond more and faster than introverts (in order to increase their arousal) during performance tasks (Eysenck, 1994; Wilt and Revelle, 2009). Furthermore, this under-arousability enables extraverts to tolerate much higher levels of arousal than introverts, who withdraw to avoid further increases in arousal which they find difficult to withstand (Eysenck, 1967, 1994; Kehoe et al., 2012).

From a network perspective, compared to random network which has low clustering coefficient and short path length, small-world network has higher clustering coefficient than a random network. The lower γ in the brain networks of introverts suggested that functional brain networks in introverts had a trend toward a randomized configuration. Random networks have less modularized information processing or fault tolerance compared with small-world networks (Latora and Marchiori, 2001; Zhang et al., 2011). A higher arousal threshold in cortex and higher level of arousal tolerance in extraverts could be interpreted by more clustered configuration in functional brain networks of extraverts.

As one of the nodal centrality metrics, increased BCi suggested the strengthened roles of coordinating whole-brain networks (Zhang et al., 2011). The brain regions with relatively higher BCi participate more in information transition and consequently act as important controls of information flow. The greater activation levels and lower arousal thresholds in these regions with higher BCi may seem conceivable.

BCi in left INS was found to be positively correlated with extraversion, whereas BCi values in bilateral MTG were found to be negatively correlated with extraversion in this study.

INS is thought to play a central role for one's emotional processing (Iaria et al., 2008; Lamm and Singer, 2010) and is involved in various neuropsychiatric diseases such as mood disorders, depressive disorders, and panic disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2006; Nagai et al., 2007; Fitzgerald et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2010; Guo et al., 2011). There were many studies providing direct evidence for a link between INS activation and extraversion or extraversion-related dimension (Johnson et al., 1999; Omura et al., 2005a; Sollberger et al., 2009; Kehoe et al., 2012; Tamura et al., 2012). For example, a PET study showed the correlation between blood flow of INS and extraversion (Johnson et al., 1999). A fMRI study revealed that extraversion correlated with neural responses to positive word stimuli in bilateral INS (Omura et al., 2005a). A recent fMRI study suggested that INS activity may “mediate” the development of extraversion (Tamura et al., 2012). Our previous study found a positive association between extraversion and ReHo in INS. Positive correlation has also been found between extraversion and gray matter volume of INS in a morphometric study (Sollberger et al., 2009). The relation between BCi and extraversion scores detected in INS in the present study further demonstrated that the regional characteristics of INS suggested individual differences in extraversion dimension. However, the positive correlation was unexpected according to Eysenck's predictions about this dimension. The expected negative correlations were revealed between extraversion and BCi in bilateral MTG.

MTG was found to participate in both emotional processing and encoding tasks (Critchley et al., 2000; Dolcos et al., 2004; Olson et al., 2007). Abnormal functions of middle temporal areas in patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) were reported both in resting MRI (Wolf et al., 2011) and emotional task MRI studies (Guitart-Masip et al., 2009). In normal late adolescents, fMRI study found correlations of age and extraversion with neural activation in MTG (Tamura et al., 2012). Our previous study also showed a negative association between extraversion and ReHo in MTG (Wei et al., 2011). These results demonstrated that MTG played a role in extraversion and the dysfunction of MTG was associated with personality disorders. Our study further showed the BCi of bilateral MTG was negatively correlated with extraversion scores, suggesting that extraverts demonstrated lower levels of cortical arousal and higher arousal thresholds in bilateral MTG. This is consistent with Eysenck's arousal hypothesis of extraversion.

The results on the relationship between extraversion and arousal in specific brain regions some of which contradict Eysenck's predictions about this dimension while some of which agree with it have also been found in other studies (Kehoe et al., 2012; Wei et al., 2012). This may imply that the relationship between extraversion and arousal is a more complex story than that originally proposed by Eysenck (Kehoe et al., 2012). Researchers may face more complicated situations when looking into various brain regions. Further research is required to elucidate if and how arousal processing differences are a central feature of extraversion (Kehoe et al., 2012).

Neuroticism and the Betweenness Centrality

The other main finding of this study was significantly positive associations between BCi and neuroticism scores in brain regions including right OLF, right CAU, and bilateral AMYG.

According to Eysenck's biological theory of personality, high levels of neuroticism are theorized to reflect increased reactivity of the limbic system (Eysenck, 1991, 1994; Kehoe et al., 2012). Our results provided some supporting evidence for this hypothesis, with individuals high in neuroticism showing higher BCi values in right OLF, right CAU and bilateral AMYG. The limbic system supports a variety of functions involving motivation, emotion, learning, and memory. Especially, AMYG has been associated with anxiety and mood disorders, for which neuroticism is a risk factor (Haas et al., 2007). Individual differences in neuroticism, a dimension characterized by experiences of negative emotion, anxiety, and emotional lability (Cunningham et al., 2010), have been shown to correlate with greater AMYG activation during unpleasant picture stimuli (Harenski et al., 2009), during fear learning (Hooker et al., 2008), during trials of high emotional conflict (Haas et al., 2007), and greater amygdala-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex connectivity while viewing angry and fearful facial expressions (Cremers et al., 2010). All these studies suggested a greater activation levels in AMYG during stimuli in individuals with high neuroticism, which were concordant with Eysenck's arousal theory of neuroticism. Here, our results further demonstrated that in resting-state functional brain networks, individuals high in neuroticism exhibited higher BCi values in AMYG as well as in OLF and CAU, suggesting greater activation levels and lower arousal thresholds in these regions in the resting brains of high neuroticism subjects.

Interestingly, most significantly correlated brain regions were located in the right hemisphere, suggesting the lateralization of these regions with regard to neuroticism. Electroencephalogram (EEG) researchers have found that dimensions related to the withdrawal aspect of neuroticism are associated with greater activation of right frontal lobe relative to left (Zuckerman, 2005; Shackman et al., 2009). The brain's right hemisphere appears to be preferentially involved in emotions and motivational states associated with withdrawal, whereas left hemisphere is preferentially involved in approach (Davidson, 2002; DeYoung, 2010). Our results provided neurofunctional evidence from the point of view of functional brain networks in resting-state.

The Prediction of Personality Scores

In the present study, the leave-one-out cross-validation strategy was applied to test the validity of the significantly correlated measures in prediction personality scores. Our results showed very strong correlations between the original and predicted personality scores. The results suggested that statistically the functional brain network measures detected in our study did play roles in the prediction of personality. However, when considering the precision of individual prediction, the averages of the absolute relative errors for the prediction were 11.4% (range: 0.3–42.3%) for extraversion and 21.7% (range: 0.2–48.0%) for neuroticism. This implied the network measures alone were not able to precisely predict the individual personality. There have been some studies trying to predict individual personality from subjects' social behaviors (Bai et al., 2012) or emotional cognition tasks (Jackson, 2005). These studies combined with our findings shed light on the future work of precise prediction of personality, and further suggested that to integrate the information of individual's outer behaviors and inner topological properties of resting-state functional brain networks may give new clues to the neuropsychology of personality dimensions.

Limitations

Since this was an exploratory study, no prior hypothesis for the relation of a certain measure to personality was proposed. The false-positive correction used in the present study, which was 1/number of regions, was not as conservative as a Bonferroni or false discovery rate (FDR) correction (Lynall et al., 2010). Thereby type I error was not able to be strongly controlled in our analyses. In addition, whether extraversion/neuroticism is correlated with intelligence is still under controversial. Studies on the Big five model of personality and intelligence found associations between extraversion/neuroticism and intelligence. Extraversion and intelligence were found to be significantly negatively correlated (Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997; Wolf and Ackerman, 2005); however, further study argued extraversion might be related to some aspects of intelligence test-taking, rather than to actual intelligence (DeYoung, 2011). Neuroticism exhibited a small but reliable negative correlation with intelligence (Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997) though; this correlation was likely to be due to the mediation by test anxiety (Moutafi et al., 2006). Higher-order traits may exist above the Big Five, but they do not appear to be related to intelligence (DeYoung et al., 2008). In Eysenck's personality theory, he asserted that intelligence is unrelated to personality (Eysenck, 1994). A study assessing personality by EPQ_R demonstrated that affective dimensions of personality are independent of intelligence (Gray et al., 2005). Since the present study aimed to test the Eysenck's personality theory of cortical arousal on extraversion and neuroticism, personality traits was asserted to be unrelated to intelligence. Further investigations of the relations between intelligence and extraversion/neuroticism are required in future studies.

Conclusions

By applying the graph theoretical analysis to the resting-state fMRI data, the present study found that the normalized clustering coefficient values of the whole-brain functional networks were positively correlated with extraversion scores, suggesting an association between extraversion and the global network measure which quantifies the clustered configuration in the brain network. The more clustered configuration in brain functional network of extraverts may result in a higher arousal threshold in cortex and higher levels of arousal tolerance. However, extraversion scores were positively correlated with BCi in right insula, while negatively correlated with BCi in bilateral MTG, indicating that the relationship between extraversion and regional arousal is not as simple as that proposed by Eysenck. On the other hand, neuroticism scores showed consistently positive associations with BCi in specific brain regions in the PreCG and limbic system, providing some supporting evidence for Eysenck's biological theory of neuroticism. Furthermore, the right lateralization of these regions with regard to neuroticism gave neurofunctional evidence to the preferential involvement of brain's right hemisphere in emotions and motivational states associated with withdrawal aspect of neuroticism.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Natural Science Foundation of China (61035006, 91132721, and 61125304); Postdoctoral Science Foundation of China (20100481378) and Special Postdoctoral Science Foundation of China (2012T50772). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

Achard, S., and Bullmore, E. (2007). Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 3:e17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Achard, S., Salvador, R., Whitcher, B., Suckling, J., and Bullmore, E. (2006). A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J. Neurosci. 26, 63–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ackerman, P. L., and Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychol. Bull. 121, 219–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., Deyoung, C. G., Zuo, X. N., Kelly, C., et al. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture. PLoS ONE 6:e27633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027633

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bai, S., Zhu, T., and Cheng, L. (2012). Big-five personality prediction based on user behaviors at social network sites. Comput. Soc. arXiv:1204.4809.

Bassett, D. S., and Bullmore, E. (2006). Small-world brain networks. Neuroscientist 12, 512–523. doi: 10.1177/1073858406293182

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.3410/f.714597885.790202808

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: learning from individual differences in emotion processing. J. Pers. 72, 1105–1132. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00292.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 33–42. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.33

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cremers, H. R., Demenescu, L. R., Aleman, A., Renken, R., Van Tol, M. J., Van Der Wee, N. J., et al. (2010). Neuroticism modulates amygdala-prefrontal connectivity in response to negative emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage 49, 963–970. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Critchley, H., Daly, E., Phillips, M., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., et al. (2000). Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 9, 93–105.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cunningham, W. A., Arbuckle, N. L., Jahn, A., Mowrer, S. M., and Abduljalil, A. M. (2010). Aspects of neuroticism and the amygdala: chronic tuning from motivational styles. Neuropsychologia 48, 3399–3404. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 68–80. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01328-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 4, 1165–1180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x

CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G. (ed.). (2011). Intelligence and Personality. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., Seguin, J. R., and Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Externalizing behavior and the higher order factors of the Big Five. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 947–953. doi: 10.1037/a0013742

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dolcos, F., Labar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2004). Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron 42, 855–863. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00289-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eguiluz, V. M., Chialvo, D. R., Cecchi, G. A., Baliki, M., and Apkarian, A. V. (2005). Scale-free brain functional networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 94, 018102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.018102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality from a controlled processing perspective: an fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 169–181. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.2.169

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

Eysenck, H. J. (ed.). (1990). Biological Dimensions of Personality. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales (EPS Adult). London: Hodder and Stoughton.

Eysenck, H. J. (ed.). (1994). Personality: Biological Foundations. London: Academic Press.

Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, M. W. (eds.). (1985). Personality and Individual Differences: a Natural Science Approach. New York, NY; Plenum Press.

Fischer, H., Wik, G., and Fredrikson, M. (1998). Extraversion, Neuroticism and brain function: a PET study of personality. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 23, 345–352. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00027-5

CrossRef Full Text

Fitzgerald, P. B., Laird, A. R., Maller, J., and Daskalakis, Z. J. (2008). A meta-analytic study of changes in brain activation in depression. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 683–695. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20426

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fornito, A., Yoon, J., Zalesky, A., Bullmore, E. T., and Carter, C. S. (2011). General and specific functional connectivity disturbances in first-episode schizophrenia during cognitive control performance. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freeman, L. C. (1978). Centrality in social networks: conceptual clarification. Soc. Netw. 1, 215–239. doi: 10.1016/0378-8733(78)90021-7

CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., and Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 6, 218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gray, J. R., Burgess, G. C., Schaefer, A., Yarkoni, T., Larsen, R. J., and Braver, T. S. (2005). Affective personality differences in neural processing efficiency confirmed using fMRI. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 182–190. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.2.182

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guitart-Masip, M., Pascual, J. C., Carmona, S., Hoekzema, E., Berge, D., Perez, V., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of impaired emotional discrimination in borderline personality disorder: an fMRI study. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 1537–1545. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.08.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guo, W. B., Sun, X. L., Liu, L., Xu, Q., Wu, R. R., Liu, Z. N., et al. (2011). Disrupted regional homogeneity in treatment-resistant depression: a resting-state fMRI study. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1297–1302. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haas, B. W., Omura, K., Constable, R. T., and Canli, T. (2007). Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 249–256. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.249

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hahn, T., Dresler, T., Ehlis, A. C., Pyka, M., Dieler, A. C., Saathoff, C., et al. (2012). Randomness of resting-state brain oscillations encodes Gray's personality trait. Neuroimage 59, 1842–1845. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harenski, C. L., Kim, S. H., and Hamann, S. (2009). Neuroticism and psychopathy predict brain activation during moral and nonmoral emotion regulation. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 9, 1–15. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.1.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

He, Y., Chen, Z., and Evans, A. (2008). Structural insights into aberrant topological patterns of large-scale cortical networks in Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurosci. 28, 4756–4766. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0141-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

He, Y., and Evans, A. (2010). Graph theoretical modeling of brain connectivity. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 23, 341–350. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32833aa567

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Miyakawa, A., Knight, R. T., and D'Esposito, M. (2008). The influence of personality on neural mechanisms of observational fear and reward learning. Neuropsychologia 46, 2709–2724. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Humphries, M. D., Gurney, K., and Prescott, T. J. (2006). The brainstem reticular formation is a small-world, not scale-free, network. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 503–511. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3354

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hutcherson, C. A., Goldin, P. R., Ramel, W., McRae, K., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Attention and emotion influence the relationship between extraversion and neural response. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 71–79. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iaria, G., Committeri, G., Pastorelli, C., Pizzamiglio, L., Watkins, K. E., and Carota, A. (2008). Neural activity of the anterior insula in emotional processing depends on the individuals' emotional susceptibility. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 363–373. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jackson, C. J. (2005). How preferred ear for listening moderates emotional cognitions in the prediction of personality. Laterality 10, 305–320. doi: 10.1080/13576500442000102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, D. L., Wiebe, J. S., Gold, S. M., Andreasen, N. C., Hichwa, R. D., Watkins, G. L., et al. (1999). Cerebral blood flow and personality: a positron emission tomography study. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 252–257.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kehoe, E. G., Toomey, J. M., Balsters, J. H., and Bokde, A. L. (2012). Personality modulates the effects of emotional arousal and valence on brain activation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 858–870. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr059

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, S. H., Hwang, J. H., Park, H. S., and Kim, S. E. (2008). Resting brain metabolic correlates of neuroticism and extraversion in young men. Neuroreport 19, 883–886. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328300080f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kunisato, Y., Okamoto, Y., Okada, G., Aoyama, S., Nishiyama, Y., Onoda, K., et al. (2011). Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state. Neurosci. Lett. 492, 109–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.067

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 579–591. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0251-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Latora, V., and Marchiori, M. (2001). Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 87, 198701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.87.198701

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liao, W., Zhang, Z., Pan, Z., Mantini, D., Ding, J., Duan, X., et al. (2010). Altered functional connectivity and small-world in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. PLoS ONE 5:e8525. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008525

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu, Y., Liang, M., Zhou, Y., He, Y., Hao, Y., Song, M., et al. (2008). Disrupted small-world networks in schizophrenia. Brain 131, 945–961. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu, Z., Xu, C., Xu, Y., Wang, Y., Zhao, B., Lv, Y., et al. (2010). Decreased regional homogeneity in insula and cerebellum: a resting-state fMRI study in patients with major depression and subjects at high risk for major depression. Psychiatry Res. 182, 211–215. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.03.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lynall, M. E., Bassett, D. S., Kerwin, R., McKenna, P. J., Kitzbichler, M., Muller, U., et al. (2010). Functional connectivity and brain networks in schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 30, 9477–9487. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0333-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matthews, G., and Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H.J. Eysenck and J.A. Gray: a comparative review. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 26, 583–626. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00158-5

CrossRef Full Text

Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., and Tsaousis, I. (2006). Is the relationship between intelligence and trait neuroticism mediated by test anxiety? Pers. Indiv. Differ. 40, 587–597. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.08.004

CrossRef Full Text

Nagai, M., Kishi, K., and Kato, S. (2007). Insular cortex and neuropsychiatric disorders: a review of recent literature. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 387–394. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., and Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130, 1718–1731. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Omura, K., Aron, A., and Canli, T. (2005a). Variance maps as a novel tool for localizing regions of interest in imaging studies of individual differences. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 252–261. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.2.252

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Omura, K., Todd Constable, R., and Canli, T. (2005b). Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism. Neuroreport 16, 1905–1908. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186596.64458.76

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2006). An insular view of anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59, 2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Qian, M., Wu, G., Zhu, R., and Zhang, S. (2000). Development of the revised eysenck personality questionnaire short scale for Chinese (EPQ-RSC). Acta. Psychol. Sinica 32, 317–323.

Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., and Snyder, A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37, 1083–1090. discussion: 1097–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauch, S. L., Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Quinn, B. T., Fischl, B., and Pitman, R. K. (2005). Orbitofrontal thickness, retention of fear extinction, and extraversion. Neuroreport 16, 1909–1912. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186599.66243.50

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rusting, C. L., and Larsen, R. J. (1997). Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: a test of two theoretical models. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 22, 607–612. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00246-2

CrossRef Full Text

Salvador, R., Suckling, J., Coleman, M. R., Pickard, J. D., Menon, D., and Bullmore, E. (2005). Neurophysiological architecture of functional magnetic resonance images of human brain. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1332–1342. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1500–1506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sollberger, M., Stanley, C. M., Wilson, S. M., Gyurak, A., Beckman, V., Growdon, M., et al. (2009). Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 47, 2812–2827. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sporns, O., Chialvo, D. R., Kaiser, M., and Hilgetag, C. C. (2004). Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 418–425. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sporns, O., Tononi, G., and Edelman, G. M. (2000). Connectivity and complexity: the relationship between neuroanatomy and brain dynamics. Neural Netw. 13, 909–922. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00053-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stam, C. J. (2004). Functional connectivity patterns of human magnetoencephalographic recordings: a ‘small-world’ network? Neurosci. Lett. 355, 25–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.063

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stam, C. J., and Reijneveld, J. C. (2007). Graph theoretical analysis of complex networks in the brain. Nonlinear Biomed. Phys. 1, 3. doi: 10.1186/1753-4631-1-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tamura, M., Moriguchi, Y., Higuchi, S., Hida, A., Enomoto, M., Umezawa, J., et al. (2012). Neural network development in late adolescents during observation of risk-taking action. PLoS ONE 7:e39527. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039527

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, L., Jiang, T., Wang, Y., Zang, Y., He, Y., Liang, M., et al. (2006). Altered resting-state functional connectivity patterns of anterior cingulate cortex in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 400, 39–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.02.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tran, Y., Craig, A., Boord, P., Connell, K., Cooper, N., and Gordon, E. (2006). Personality traits and its association with resting regional brain activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 60, 215–224. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Den Heuvel, M. P., Stam, C. J., Kahn, R. S., and Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance. J. Neurosci. 29, 7619–7624. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Dijk, K. R., Sabuncu, M. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2012). The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59, 431–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, J., Zuo, X., and He, Y. (2010). Graph-based network analysis of resting-state functional MRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:16. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watts, D. J., and Strogatz, S. H. (1998). Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature 393, 440–442. doi: 10.1038/30918

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wei, L., Duan, X., Yang, Y., Liao, W., Gao, Q., Ding, J. R., et al. (2011). The synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity predicts extraversion and neuroticism. Brain Res. 1419, 68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wei, L., Duan, X., Zheng, C., Wang, S., Gao, Q., Zhang, Z., et al. (2012). Specific frequency bands of amplitude low-frequency oscillation encodes personality. Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22176. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilt, J., and Revelle, W. (2009). Extraversion. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Wolf, M. B., and Ackerman, P. L. (2005). Extraversion and intelligence: a meta-analytic investigation. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 39, 531–542. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.020

CrossRef Full Text

Wolf, R. C., Sambataro, F., Vasic, N., Schmid, M., Thomann, P. A., Bienentreu, S. D., et al. (2011). Aberrant connectivity of resting-state networks in borderline personality disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 36, 402–411. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100150

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wright, C. I., Feczko, E., Dickerson, B., and Williams, D. (2007). Neuroanatomical correlates of personality in the elderly. Neuroimage 35, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wright, C. I., Williams, D., Feczko, E., Barrett, L. F., Dickerson, B. C., Schwartz, C. E., et al. (2006). Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1809–1819. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj118

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, J., Wang, J., Wu, Q., Kuang, W., Huang, X., He, Y., et al. (2011). Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zuckerman, M. (ed.). (2005). Psychobiology of Personality. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Keywords: resting-state, functional magnetic resonance imaging, graph topological properties, extraversion, neuroticism

Citation: Gao Q, Xu Q, Duan X, Liao W, Ding J, Zhang Z, Li Y, Lu G and Chen H (2013) Extraversion and neuroticism relate to topological properties of resting-state brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00257

Received: 29 November 2012; Accepted: 22 May 2013;
Published online: 11 June 2013.

Edited by:

Yong He, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Henrik Walter, Charité Universitätsmedizin, Germany
R. Matthew Hutchison, Western University, Canada

Copyright © 2013 Gao, Xu, Duan, Liao, Ding, Zhang, Li, Lu and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Qing Gao, School of Mathematical Sciences, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, No. 4, Section 2, N. Jianshe Rd., Chengdu 610054, China e-mail: qingqing.gao@gmail.com;
Huafu Chen, Key Laboratory for Neuroinformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, No. 4, Section 2, N. Jianshe Rd., Chengdu 610054, China e-mail: chenhf@uestc.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.