- 1Keenan Research Centre of the Li Ka Shing Knowledge Institute, St. Michael's Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 2Department of Surgery, Faculty of Medicine, Division of Neurosurgery, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 3Institute of Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 4Department of Physical Sciences, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 5Department of Medicine, Baycrest Geriatric Health Care Centre, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 6Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 7Toronto Rehabilitation Research Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 8Department of Medicine and Institute of Health Policy, Management and Evaluation, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 9Department of Medical Biophysics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Introduction: Non-invasive measurements of brain activity have an important role to play in understanding driving ability. The current study aimed to identify the neural underpinnings of human driving behavior by visualizing the areas of the brain involved in driving under different levels of demand, such as driving while distracted or making left turns at busy intersections.
Materials and Methods: To capture brain activity during driving, we placed a driving simulator with a fully functional steering wheel and pedals in a 3.0 Tesla functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) system. To identify the brain areas involved while performing different real-world driving maneuvers, participants completed tasks ranging from simple (right turns) to more complex (left turns at busy intersections). To assess the effects of driving while distracted, participants were asked to perform an auditory task while driving analogous to speaking on a hands-free device and driving.
Results: A widely distributed brain network was identified, especially when making left turns at busy intersections compared to more simple driving tasks. During distracted driving, brain activation shifted dramatically from the posterior, visual and spatial areas to the prefrontal cortex.
Conclusions: Our findings suggest that the distracted brain sacrificed areas in the posterior brain important for visual attention and alertness to recruit enough brain resources to perform a secondary, cognitive task. The present findings offer important new insights into the scientific understanding of the neuro-cognitive mechanisms of driving behavior and lay down an important foundation for future clinical research.
Introduction
Driving is an essential daily activity for many people, providing mobility, independence, and sometimes a source of livelihood. Previous research has reported factors that increase the risk of vehicle crashes and driving errors including fatigue and sleepiness after extended driving (Sagaspe et al., 2008) and alcohol consumption (De Boni et al., 2012). Research has also reported on neurological factors that impact driving ability, especially in individuals with cognitive impairments (Rapoport et al., 2007; Carr and Ott, 2010; MacDonald and Hébert, 2010; Nelson, 2010), that increase the risk of vehicle crashes (Rizzo, 2011). Health professionals have the responsibility to identify individuals with medical conditions that may detract from safe driving ability (Carr et al., 2006). However, to date there has been no consensus on the assessment of fitness to drive and it remains a significant challenge for clinicians to evaluate a patient's driving capacity (Eby and Molnar, 2010). Some individuals may be able to drive in simple, well-practiced circumstances, but may be incapable of driving safely when circumstances become more demanding or novel. There is very limited knowledge about how different levels of driving behavior are supported by normally functioning brains, and how different areas of the brain interact when performing various driving tasks.
Safe driving requires the ability to concentrate, to divide attention between multiple sensory events across visual and auditory modalities, and to make fast cognitive decisions in a complex and rapidly changing environment. The present study applied virtual reality (VR) technology in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) system to investigate how brain responses of healthy adults change across various driving scenarios. Using this novel setup allowed us to obtain a real-time, spatiotemporal profile of brain activity while driving in a safe environment. Previous fMRI driving research has identified brain areas responsive to different aspects of driving (Carvalho et al., 2006; Spiers and Maguire, 2007), including maintaining driving speed (Peres et al., 2000; Calhoun et al., 2002) and responding to uncertainties during driving (Callan et al., 2009). These studies commonly identified brain regions involving the motor, parietal, occipital, and cerebellar cortices responsible for various driving maneuvers (Walter et al., 2001; Calhoun et al., 2002; Uchiyama et al., 2003; Graydon et al., 2004; Calhoun and Pearlson, 2012) such as turning, reversing, and stopping (Spiers and Maguire, 2007) all of which require visual-spatial and visual-motor processes. Specific tasks such as monitoring other cars, processing traffic rules, and action planning, recruit the pre-supplementary motor area, the superior parietal and lateral occipital cortices, as well as the cerebellum (Spiers and Maguire, 2007). These additional activations in posterior brain regions have been attributed to the increased demands that driving places on vision and motor skills, as well as visuo-motor and visuo-spatial integration. Previous research examining driving performance with distractions has reported an associated reduction in brain resources (Just et al., 2008). However, existing studies have provided limited data related to real-world driving behaviors, such as driving with distraction.
Cognitive psychologists have proposed that the posterior attention system engages brain areas such as the occipital-parietal and posterior cingulate regions that are critical to visual-spatial orientation and integration functions. In contrast, the anterior attention system serves a higher-level attention function, engaging anterior portions of the frontal lobes such as the prefrontal and anterior cingulate regions, which are responsible for executive attentional control in more complex cognitive tasks associated with problem-solving and decision-making, especially during multi-tasking (Posner and Dehaene, 1994; Posner, 2012). Studies have used electroencephalography (EEG) techniques to estimate possible intracerebral sources associated with driving-related processing: deactivation of signals during fast driving related to a drop of cognitive control has been reported (Jancke et al., 2008); and visual event-related potentials (ERP) components were diminished and the attentional selection of target stimuli were less efficient during concurrent auditory dual-tasks (Gherri and Eimer, 2011). Magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies with driving simulators have reported multiple brain sources, including visual, parietal, and frontal areas that are engaged during visual attention to driving-related signals (traffic lights and direction signs), with increased attention demand in a dual-task audio condition modifying the neural processing of visual signals (Fort et al., 2010). Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy (FNIRS) has also been used to explore the neural underpinnings of driving behavior. Recent studies have suggested that the frontal lobes are sensitive to the type of driving task, with greater frontal lobe activity observed for externally directed driving behavior (following audio driving instructions) compared to internally-driven driving behavior (based on memory) (Liu et al., 2012). FNIRS data also suggest that parietal and occipital brain areas are responsible for spatial attention in the perception of VR space (Seraglia et al., 2011; also see the review of Calhoun and Pearlson, 2012).
The current study focused on identifying the underlying neural networks subserving different driving behaviors including distracted driving. Our immersive and novel VR setup (Kan et al., 2013) allowed us to investigate how the brains of healthy young adult drivers respond during various simulated driving conditions ranging in levels of complexity. This goal was achieved by using virtual driving technology combined with advanced neuroimaging techniques. Driving tasks were designed to require increasing levels of attentional processing demands and visual complexity. For example, the tasks included making turns at intersections with and without oncoming traffic, or driving under auditory distractions that mimic driving while talking on a hands-free cell phone or engaging in a conversation with a passenger. These simulated driving conditions allowed us to tap into the same neural processes associated with a more complete range of real-world driving behaviors. The cognitive manipulation during driving in the current study can be helpful in studying the underlying neural substrates for various driving behaviors, such as distracted driving. The knowledge of these neural substrates ultimately can help health professionals to more effectively assess driving competence in individuals with brain dysfunctions (e.g., using office-based, cost-effective tests), and will eventually contribute to the design of potential cognitive rehabilitation strategies.
We hypothesized that complex simulated driving conditions (such as making turns, particularly left turns at busy intersections while encountering oncoming traffic) would involve posterior brain activations including motor and occipital-parietal regions for visual-spatial and visual-motor integration. On the other hand, prefrontal activation would be involved in a distracted condition consisting of performing a secondary cognitive task during simulated driving, related to the executive function demands when attentional resources are divided across multiple tasks.
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
Ethical approval for the study was obtained on July 18th 2010, by the Research Ethics Board at Baycrest Hospital in Toronto, Canada. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participating in the study.
Participants
Participants were recruited through the university network via advertisement and emails. All participants were right-handed with normal or corrected vision. Participants without a valid driver's license, with a history of psychological or neurological illness, or with fMRI contraindications (such as having claustrophobia or ferromagnetic implants) were excluded. Sixteen participants (7 females and 9 males) between the ages of 20 and 30 years (Mean = 25.8, SD = 1.5) who were actively driving and with mean driving experience of 7.4 years (SD = 2.5) were studied.
Driving Simulation
We applied a novel approach using an immersive VR environment in a 3.0 Tesla MRI system to capture brain activity with high ecological validity (Kan et al., 2013). We applied driving hardware (steering wheel and foot pedals) custom built for compatibility with fMRI. The driving scenario was designed using STISIM Drive software (Systems Technology Inc., Hawthorne, CA). Participants viewed the simulation through a mirror attached to the head coil (see “Neuroimaging” section), which oriented on a projection screen illuminated by an LCD projector system projecting through a waveguide in the radiofrequency shield of the MRI room; participants also wore fMRI-compatible headphones (Silent Scan, Avotec, Inc., Stuart, FL) to hear the audio tasks.
Driving Tasks
Prior to fMRI, participants underwent an hour-long training session in an fMRI simulator to practice simulated driving. The tasks included straight driving (“Straight Driving,” Figure 1A), turning at intersections with and without oncoming traffic, or driving while performing audio tasks. Each participant performed six simple right-hand turns (“Right Turn”) and six left-hand turns (“Left-Turn,” Figure 1B) with no traffic. To increase driving complexity, participants encountered six left turns with a stream of oncoming traffic (“Left Turn + Traffic,” Figure 1C), which required participants to decide when to turn safely. In conditions of distracted driving, participants were presented with concurrent audio tasks consisting of general knowledge true or false questions (e.g., “a triangle has four sides”) during straight driving (“Straight + Audio”; six times) as well as in the demanding turns (“Left Turn + Traffic + Audio”; six times). Participants answered the questions by pressing corresponding buttons embedded on the steering wheel (similar to modern vehicle designs for answering hands-free devices or volume controls). The experimental protocol is shown in Figure 2. Straight driving served as the control condition (baseline) interspersed between other specific tasks in pseudo-random order. Each task was introduced by a voice recording (e.g., “At the intersection, turn left”) approximately 5 s prior to the task, similar to navigation instructions with a Global Positioning System. Participants were asked to follow the traffic rules and to drive as close to the posted speed limit as possible. Each driving task was separated by at least 15 s of straight driving (baseline/controls) to maintain separation to limit overlapping of the fMRI hemodynamic response signals.
Figure 1. Representative STISIM screenshots of simulated driving conditions used in the fMRI. Rural scenery was chosen to minimize the potential confounding variations from using complex visual backgrounds. (A) Straight driving; (B) Left turn at intersection with no traffic; (C) Left turn with oncoming traffic.
Neuroimaging
Participants received motion training to operate the driving controls with minimal head motion and practiced on four training runs that introduced all driving conditions. During testing, participants were placed in the MRI system with the driving hardware in a comfortable position. A high-resolution anatomical scan was acquired first, followed by three fMRI runs with simulated driving. The driving scenarios were triggered synchronously with the fMRI time series data collection. Each run was approximately 9–9.5 min. Images were acquired using a research-dedicated whole body 3.0 Tesla MRI system (Magnetom TIM Trio software version b15, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). The high-resolution anatomical scan was acquired with T1-weighted, 3D magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo imaging (MPRAGE; echo time (TE) = 2.63 ms, 160 slices, thickness = 1.0 mm, gap = 0 mm, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 192 mm, matrix = 256 × 192, yielding 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels). Functional MRI was undertaken using T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (EPI; repetition time (TR) = 2 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 70 degrees, 32 slices, thickness = 4.5 mm, gap = 0 mm, FOV = 200 × 200 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, yielding 3.13 × 3.13 × 4.5 mm voxels).
Data Analysis
The first 10 s of scanning for each driving run was discarded to allow for equilibration effects. Using AFNI freeware (Cox, 1996), time series data were corrected for physiological movement due to respiration, corrected for slice timing effects, and co-registered to the 16th time point of the first run. The dataset was spatially smoothed using a 5 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel and normalized by the run-wise mean of each voxel. Statistical brain activation maps were calculated using a General Linear Model (GLM) by convolving a stimulus-timing file with a variable-shape hemodynamic response function (HRF) with seven regressors (for an expected HRF of 14 s, one regressor was used per TR). Estimated head motion parameters, in six degrees of freedom as determined from the co-registration procedure indicated above, were included as nuisance regressors as well as a 4th order polynomial for baseline detrending. The resulting GLM parameter estimates were summed, transformed into Talairach brain atlas space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988), spatially smoothed using a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel, and the maps for all 16 subjects were entered into a within-subjects, random effects ANOVA. Finally, t-statistic maps for all conditions were thresholded using a false discovery rate method (Genovese et al., 2002) at a level of q = 0.05, to correct for multiple statistical comparisons. For interpretation, the final group activation maps were overlaid on the average of all 16 anatomical images, which were also transformed into Talairach brain atlas space. To disentangle the visuo-motor and higher cognitive functions involved in driving, straight driving was used as the control condition in comparison with all other driving conditions. The statistical comparisons of interest across group activation maps were, therefore: distracted straight driving vs. control; right turns vs. control; left turns vs. control; complex turns vs. control; and distracted complex turns vs. control.
Results
Brain Activations
Significant brain activations are summarized below, reported relative to the straight driving control condition. Brain activation images for each task are shown in Figures 3 and 4. Peak locations of brain activity are reported in Table 1. All reported results were in contrast with the baseline (control condition: straight driving). Behavioral results can be seen in Appendix.
Figure 3. Brain activations from the bottom to the top of the brain (left to right figures) of participants when performing various simulated driving conditions. (A) The right-turn condition showed minimal activation in the brain; (B) Left-turn showed more activation in the posterior brain regions; (C) The left-turns with oncoming traffic generated larger significant activations of multiple bilateral regions in the mid-posterior brain areas. See details in the “Results” section.
Figure 4. Brain activations associated with distracted driving. (A) Straight driving with a cognitive-distraction, audio task. (B) The demanding, left-turn condition with oncoming traffic plus the cognitive distraction.
Regular Driving
Right turn (Figure 3A). Minimal significant activation was observed in this simple task, including the somatosensory association (postcentral gyrus), parietal (including precuneus), and visual cortices (lingual gyrus).
Left turn (Figure 3B). More activation was detected in this condition, which was observed in the premotor cortex, somatosensory area, visual and parietal cortices, as well as the cerebellum.
Left turn + traffic (Figure 3C). This condition showed larger significant activations of multiple bilateral regions in the mid-posterior brain, including motor and premotor areas, visual, parietal, and somatosensory regions, and the cerebellum.
Distracted driving
Straight + audio (Figure 4A). Significant activations were found in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) bilaterally, in addition to auditory cortex. Other activations were detected in the parietal lobes. Decreased activation in occipital-visual regions was observed.
Left turn + traffic + audio (Figure 4B). In addition to auditory, motor, somatosensory, visual, parietal, and cerebellar regions, significant additional activations were detected in anterior brain areas bilaterally, mainly in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the frontal polar region.
Behavioral Results
Driving performance showed an effect of speed differences among undistracted driving conditions (p < 0.05; Repeated Measure ANOVA; Table A1); post-hoc comparisons showed that the mean speed in left-turn-traffic was the slowest (29.4 km/hr, SD = 4.3 km/hr) compared to left turns (26.8 km/hr, SD = 5.2 km/hr) and right turns (24.0 km/hr, SD = 3.3 km/hr). Left turn speed was significantly slower than right turn speed, which was the fastest among all conditions significantly (p < 0.05, Least Significant Difference post-hoc tests; Table A2). The speed in the distracted left-turn-traffic condition was not significantly different from the left turn and left-turn-traffic conditions and was only slower than right turns (p < 0.05). Average speed during straight driving (58.57 km/hr, SD = 3.36 km/hr) was not significantly different from that of distracted straight driving (58.69 km/hr, SD = 2.34 km/hr) (Table A3). Lane position during straight driving (2.35 km/hr, SD = 0.31 km/hr) was not significantly different from that of distracted straight driving (2.51, SD = 0.42 km/hr) (Table A3). Average response accuracy to the audio distraction task was 87% (range = 50–100%, above the chance level).
Discussion
The current study extends previous research by using an immersive fMRI-compatible driving simulator to examine how the human brain responds to various driving conditions, and by characterizing the effects of cognitive distraction on driving. First, we observed that the patterns of brain activation depend on the type of simulated driving task. Performing right turns, the simplest task, generated minimal activation relative to the control condition (Figure 3A). Making left turns, without oncoming traffic, the participants showed activations in the posterior brain, including visual-parietal and motor areas (Figure 3B), suggesting that cognitive resources involving visuospatial and motor coordination are required for making left turns. Performing the more demanding left turns at busy intersections, where in the real world most serious crashes occur (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2009; Choi, 2010), produced larger activations in the posterior network, along with additional activation of the cingulate cortex, an area important for cognitive-response selection and alertness (Vogt et al., 2004) (Figure 3C).
Second, a significant shift in activation from the posterior to the anterior brain was observed when driving became distracted. Compared to straight driving, auditory distraction during straight driving significantly activated not only auditory areas but also the prefrontal cortices (mainly in the ventral lateral prefrontal cortex regions; Figure 4A), while decreased activation in posterior brain regions was evident. These findings support the study hypothesis in that undistracted driving, even in attentionally demanding conditions, engaged the posterior brain system, while driving under cognitive distractions activated the anterior brain system. Consistently, when the more challenging maneuver (turning left at a busy intersection) was performed under cognitive distraction, the anterior network was additionally engaged (Figure 4B), predominantly in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex/frontal pole. These regions are associated with executive functions including attention and working memory processes, and processing thoughts and decision-making critical for multitasking (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000).
To substantiate the observed shift from occipital to frontal brain activations, particularly in the prefrontal areas when comparing the left-turn-traffic condition to the left-turn-traffic plus cognitive distraction, we extracted mean BOLD percentage change values for each subject from the activated occipital and prefrontal regions of interest, and conducted post-hoc tests of the differences between task conditions using Matlab (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA). Results confirmed a significant decrease in the mean occipital activation [from 0.59 to 0.36%, p = 0.001, paired samples t(15) = 4.01] and an increase in the mean prefrontal activation [from 0.11 to 0.39%, p = 0.016, paired samples t(15) = −2.72].
This anterior-vs.-posterior shift in BOLD signals reflects changing reactions of the brain, and highlights the effect of distracted driving and the role of the anterior frontal region, an area that has been associated with impulsiveness (e.g., Beeli et al., 2008) critical to driving. The pattern of increased frontal activation accompanied by cognitive distraction has been previously observed in participants performing a visual event detection task while passively watching a driving video under auditory distraction (Hsieh et al., 2009), as well as during performing divided attention, dual-tasks involving both visual and auditory modalities compared to performing single-modality tasks (Schubert and Szameitat, 2003; Johnson and Zatorre, 2006). One important caveat is that the prefrontal activity observed when the participants were performing simultaneous driving and auditory tasks may not be entirely associated with the distraction, but may be at least partly related to auditory attention needed for the secondary task. Future studies will be required to analyze this issue in more detail.
Supporting the findings and interpretation of the present work, an observation of decreased activation in parietal-visual areas and impaired driving performance in a dual-task driving condition involving concurrent language comprehension has also been previously reported (Just et al., 2008). We provided data within a single study demonstrating a response shift between these brain areas from the posterior to the anterior networks related to driving distraction. This interplay between anterior and posterior brain regions is possibly related to a competition for limited resources and attentional reallocation between the anterior, executive attention in multitasking and the posterior, visual-response attention system (Rees et al., 1997; Wickens, 2008). The brain may face a “bottleneck” when multiple tasks simultaneously compete for shared and limited resources, constraining available resources for individual tasks (Just et al., 2001; Dux et al., 2006). This view suggests that with cognitive distraction during driving, to support mental processing in the anterior brain, resources of the posterior brain important for visual alertness and visual attention were sacrificed.
The current finding has important implications regarding distracted driving. While changes in driving performance observed in the undistracted conditions (slowing down from right turns to left turns and traffic) were parallel to the results of brain activations in the posterior brain (increases in activated areas), brain activity shifted to the anterior network when there was no behavioral change from the undistracted to the distracted condition. Eye-tracking studies have shown that hands-free conversations using cell phones impair attention to visual inputs (Strayer et al., 2003). These distracted drivers experience “inattention blindness”: their field of view narrows (Maples et al., 2008), and they tend to “look at” but not “see” the information in their driving environment (Strayer, 2007), and miss visual cues important for safe driving (Jacobson and Gostin, 2010). As a result, epidemiological findings of real-world collisions show that drivers using hands-free phones are just as likely to experience vehicle crashes as those using hand-held devices (Redelmeier and Tibshirani, 1997; McEvoy et al., 2005). The present study provides neuroimaging evidence supporting previous behavioral observations suggesting that multitasking while driving may potentially compromise visual attention and alertness due to a reduction in brain activation supporting critical visual processing areas, even without significant behavioral changes. Therefore the dangers introduced by distracted driving should be regarded in terms of increased cognitive distractions (i.e., using hands-free cell phones) rather than motor distractions (i.e., physically holding a device) (Strayer and Johnston, 2001).
Previous studies have reported measures that may be able to predict those who passed on-road assessments from those who failed (Baldock et al., 2006). These tests include, for example, The Trail Making Test-B (Richardson and Marottoli, 2003; Staplin et al., 2003; Whelihan et al., 2005), Ergovision Movement Perception Test (De Raedt and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, 2000), UFOV (De Raedt and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, 2000), Complex Reaction Time Task (De Raedt and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, 2000), Paper Folding Task (De Raedt and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, 2000), Dot Counting (De Raedt and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, 2000), WMS Visual Reproduction (Richardson and Marottoli, 2003), and Computerized Visual Attention Test Single Task (Mathias and Lucas, 2009). It is clinically important to understand the specific cognitive functions required in different driving circumstances in order to identify “borderline” drivers or those with “restricted” capacities who may be at risk in certain driving conditions. Recent evidence shows that smaller gray matter volume in frontal brain regions was associated with lower executive function capacity and a proclivity to risky driving (Sakai et al., 2012). Given the current finding that prefrontal areas were only significantly activated during the distracted driving conditions, we propose that future assessment should consider using neurocognitive tests that tap into executive, frontal-lobe functions and divided attention for evaluating “fitness for distracted driving.” Based on the current study, it can be inferred that damage to the anterior brain regions may result in specific or restricted impairment (i.e., this type of patient may be unfit to drive safely in distracted circumstances), whereas damage to the posterior brain areas involving the visual, spatial, and motor abilities would significantly compromise general fitness to drive. This implication is also critical for establishing potential restricted driving licensing (Marshall et al., 2002).
In addition, while regular driving (e.g., simple right/left turn without traffic) relies on more learned and automatic processes that activate a driving network in the posterior brain, the mid-cingulate cortex was only differentially activated when the driving conditions became more demanding (left turn with oncoming traffic). This brain region has a major role to play in response selection (Paus et al., 1993; Vogt et al., 2004), attention-for-action (Posner et al., 1988), and working memory (Petit et al., 1998); therefore neurocognitive tests associated with selective attention, response inhibition, and visual-motor abilities can be targeted to evaluate the ability to drive safely in demanding circumstances, even without distraction.
A limitation of the present study is the use of young drivers, which may reduce the generalizability to older populations. Another limitation is that by using simulated driving we were unable to replicate the potential anxiety associated with driving under conditions of increasing complexity, given that there is no real crash risk. Although the use of simulated driving during fMRI may not perfectly generalize to real-world driving, it allows for the investigation of complex driving conditions that are not usually tested during on-road assessments (i.e., left turns during peak traffic or driving while talking on a cell phone). Indeed, previous research suggests that there is a significant correlation between on-road test performance and performance in the driving simulator (Freund et al., 2002). Lundqvist and colleagues have suggested that the predictive ability of driving simulators in assessing actual driving behavior was superior to that of on-road driving tests, partially due to the well-controlled complexity levels in the simulated driving scenarios (Lundqvist et al., 2000). Driving simulators provide a more standardized environment (Michon, 1989; Ranney, 1994) compared to on-road driving tests, offering an ecologically valid and safe way to study human driving behavior in a variety of challenging conditions (Grealy et al., 1999).
Conclusions
The present study provides new neuroimaging data of the complex brain activity associated with distracted driving and driving under different levels of complexity. We found that brain activations during driving rely on areas important for various cognitive functions including the posterior visual-spatial attentional system vs. the anterior, frontal-lobe functions in multitasking and divided attention. For most people, driving involves highly practiced skills that generally draw on automatic or practiced abilities relying on a posterior network, and does not heavily require the anterior frontal system for more effortful mental processing. However, there are potentially many distractions present during driving (Editorial, 2001), including conversations with passengers, or unpredictable events that increase the risk of crashes. The current findings may be applied to future research on clinical populations with various brain disorders (e.g., Ott et al., 2000; Bedard et al., 2011) to determine how brain damage affects the ability to adapt to daily driving tasks. The shift in brain activation indicates that the assessment of fitness to drive should consider different levels of driving demands and more selective evaluations of driving ability (e.g., in different traffic conditions, or driving while conversing with the examiner). As the brain has limited cognitive resources, this fundamental constraint limits the capacity during driving to carrying out any other cognitive operations such as language comprehension (Newman et al., 2007). Lastly, automobile manufacturers also have a responsibility to improve safety by refraining from installing various communication devices in vehicles, or by installing deactivation systems if drivers attempt to use the devices while the car is in motion (Jacobson and Gostin, 2010). More research is needed to determine if intervention programs (e.g., Devos et al., 2009) that apply simulator protocols with attention training can improve fitness to drive, for example, in improving left turns at busy intersections, or reducing vehicle collision risks for certain brain damaged populations.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a Personnel Award from the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada and an Early Researcher Award from the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation to Dr. Tom A. Schweizer. Dr. Naglie is supported by the University of Toronto Mary Trimmer Chair in Geriatric Medicine. We would also like to thank Monica Maher, Megan Hird, and Meishan Yan for their assistance in the preparation of the manuscript, and Annette Weekes-Holder for performing fMRI of the volunteers.
References
Baldock, M. R., Mathias, J. L., McLean, A. J., and Berndt, A. (2006). Self-regulation of driving and its relationship to driving ability among older adults. Accid. Anal. Prev. 38, 1038–1045.
Bedard, M., Riendeau, J., Weaver, B., and Clarkson, A. (2011). Roadwise review has limited congruence with actual driving performance of aging drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 43, 2209–2214.
Beeli, G., Casutt, G., Baumgartner, T., and Jäncke, L. (2008). Modulating presence and impulsiveness by external stimulation of the brain. Behav. Brain Funct. 4:33. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-4-33
Calhoun, V. D., and Pearlson, G. D. (2012). A selective review of simulated driving studies: combining paradigms, analysis approaches, and future direction. Neuroimage 59, 25–35.
Calhoun, V. D., Pekar, J. J., McGinty, V. B., Adali, T., Watson, T. D., and Pearlson, G. D. (2002). Different activation dynamics in multiple neural systems during simulated driving. Hum. Brain Mapp. 16, 158–167.
Callan, A. M., Osu, R., Yamagishi, Y., Callan, D. E., and Inoue, N. (2009). Neural correlates of resolving uncertainty in driver's decision making. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2804–2812.
Carr, D. B., Meuser, M., and Morris, J. C. (2006). Driving retirement: the role of the physician. CMAJ 175, 601–602.
Carr, D. B., and Ott, B. R. (2010). The older adult driver with cognitive impairment. JAMA 303, 1632–1641.
Carvalho, K. N., Pearlson, G. D., Astur, R. S., and Calhoun, V. D. (2006). Simulated driving and brain imaging: combining behavior, brain activity, and virtual reality. CNS Spectr. 11, 52–62.
Choi, E. (2010). Crash Factors in Intersection-Related Crashes: An On-Scene Perspective. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Transportation. DOT HS 811 366. Accessed March 27, 2012. Available online at: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/Pubs/811366.pdf
Christoff, K., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28, 168–186.
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173.
De Boni, R., do Nascimento Silva, P. L., Bastos, F. I., Pechansky, F., and de Vasconcellos, M. T. (2012). Reaching the hard-to-reach: a probability sampling method for assessing prevalence of driving under the influence after drinking in alcohol outlets. PLoS ONE 7:e34104. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034104
De Raedt, R., and Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, I. (2000). The relationship between cognitive/neuropsychological factors and car driving performance in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 48, 1664–1668.
Devos, H., Akinwuntan, A. E., Nieuwboer, A., Tant, M., Truijen, S., de Wit, L., et al. (2009). Comparison of the effect of two driving retraining programs on on-road performance after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 23, 699–705.
Dux, P. E., Ivanoff, J., Asplund, C. L., and Marois, R. (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron 52, 1109–1120.
Eby, D. W., and Molnar, L. J. (2010). Driving fitness and cognitive impairment: issues for physicians. JAMA 303, 1642–1643.
Fort, A., Martin, R., Jacquet-Andrieu, A., Combe-Pangaud, C., Foliot, G., Daligault, S., et al. (2010). Attentional demand and processing of relevant visual information during simulated driving: a MEG study. Brain Res. 1363, 117–127.
Freund, B., Gravenstein, S., and Ferris, R. (2002). Evaluating driving performance of cognitively impaired and healthy adults: A pilot study comparing on-road testing and driving simulation. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 50, 1309–1310.
Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., and Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878.
Gherri, E., and Eimer, M. (2011). Active listening impairs visual perception and selectivity: a study of auditory dual-task costs on visual attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 832–844.
Graydon, F. X., Young, R., Benton, M. D., Genrik, R. J. II., Posse, S., Hsieh, L., et al. (2004). Visual event detection during simulated driving: identifying the neural correlates with functional neuroimaging. Transport. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 7, 271–286.
Grealy, M. A., Johnson, D. A., and Rushton, S. K. (1999). Improving cognitive function after brain injury: the use of exercise and virtual reality. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80, 661–667.
Hsieh, L., Young, R. A., Bowyer, S. M., Moran, J. E., Genik, R. J. II., Green, C. C., et al. (2009). Conversation effects on neural mechanisms underlying reaction time to visual events while viewing a driving scene: fMRI analysis and asynchrony model. Brain Res. 1251, 162–175.
Jacobson, P. D., and Gostin, L. O. (2010). Reducing distracted driving: regulation and education to avert traffic and fatalities. JAMA 303, 1419–1420.
Jancke, L., Brunner, B., and Esslen, M. (2008). Brain activation during fast driving in a driving simulator: the role of the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroreport 19, 1127–1130.
Johnson, J. A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2006). Neural substrates for dividing and focusing attention between simultaneous auditory and visual events. Neuroimage 31, 1673–1681.
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Emery, L., Zajac, H., and Thulborn, K. R. (2001). Interdependence of nonoverlapping cortical systems in dual cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 14, 417–426.
Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., and Cynkar, J. (2008). A decrease in brain activation associated with driving when listening to someone speak. Brain Res. 1205, 70–80.
Kan, K., Schweizer, T., Tam, F., and Graham, S. (2013). Methodology for functional MRI of simulated driving. Med. Phys. 40:012301. doi: 10.1118/1.4769107
Liu, T., Saito, H., and Oi, M. (2012). Distinctive activation patterns under intrinsically versus extrinsically driven cognitive loads in prefrontal cortex: a near-infrared spectroscopy study using a driving video game. Neurosci. Lett. 506, 220–224.
Lundqvist, A., Gerdle, B., and Ronnberg, J. (2000). Neuropsychological aspects of driving after a stroke—in the simulator and on the road. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, 135–150.
MacDonald, N., and Hébert, P. C. (2010). Driving retirement program for seniors: long overdue. CMAJ 182:645. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.100273
Maples, W. C., DeRosier, W., Hoenes, R., Bendure, R., and Moore, S. (2008). The effects of cell phone use on peripheral vision. Optometry 79, 36–42.
Marshall, C. M., Spasoff, R., Nair, R., and van Walraven, C. (2002). Restricted driver licensing for medical impairments: does it work? CMAJ 167, 747–751.
Mathias, J. L., and Lucas, L. K. (2009). Cognitive predictors of unsafe driving in older drivers: a meta-analysis. Int. Psychogeriatr. 21, 637–653.
McEvoy, S. P., Stevenson, M. R., McCartt, A. T., Woodward, M., Haworth, C., Palamara, P., et al. (2005). Role of mobile phones in motor vehicle crashes resulting in hospital attendance: a case-crossover study. Br. Med. J. 331:428. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38537.397512.55
Michon, J. A. (1989). Explanatory pitfalls and rule-based driver models. Accid. Anal. Prev. 21, 341–353.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2009). Traffic Safety Facts. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Transportation; 2011. DOT HS 811 402. Accessed March 27, 2012. Available online at: http://www.thenewspaper.com/rlc/docs/2011/us-tsf09.pdf
Newman, S. D., Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2007). Volitional control of attention and brain activation in dual-task performance. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 109–117.
Ott, B. R., Heindel, W. C., Whelihan, W. M., Caron, M. D., Piatt, A. L., and Noto, R. B. (2000). A SPECT imaging study of driving impairment in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 11, 153–160.
Paus, T., Petrides, M., Evans, A. C., and Meyer, E. (1993). Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 453–469.
Peres, M., van de Moortele, P. F., Pierard, C., Lehericy, S., LeBihan, D., and Guezennec, C. Y. (2000). fMRI of mental strategy in a simulated aviation performance task. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 71, 1218–1231.
Petit, L., Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Haxby, J. V. (1998). Sustained activity in the medial wall during working memory delays. J. Neurosci. 18, 9429–9437.
Posner, M. I., Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., and Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240, 1627–1631.
Ranney, T. A. (1994). Models of driving behavior: a review of their evolution. Accid. Anal. Prev. 26, 733–750.
Rapoport, M. J., Herrmann, N., Molnar, F. J., Man-Son-Hing, M., Marshall, S. C., Shulman, K., et al. (2007). Sharing the responsibility for assessing the risk of the driver with dementia. CMAJ 177, 599–601.
Redelmeier, D. A., and Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. N. Engl. J. Med. 336, 453–458.
Rees, G., Frith, C. D., and Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science 278, 1616–1619.
Richardson, E. D., and Marottoli, R. A. (2003). Visual attention and driving behavior among community-living older persons. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 58, 832–836.
Rizzo, M. (2011). Impaired driving from medical conditions: a 70-year-old man trying to decide if he should continue driving. JAMA 305, 1018–1026.
Sagaspe, P., Taillard, J., Akerstedt, T., Bayon, V., Espié, S., Chaumet, G., et al. (2008). Extended driving impairs nocturnal driving performances. PLoS ONE 3:e3493. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003493
Sakai, H., Takahara, M., Honjo, N. F., Doi, S., Sadato, N., and Uchiyama, Y. (2012). Regional frontal gray matter volume associated with executive function capacity as a risk factor for vehicle crashes in normal aging adults. PLoS ONE 7:e45920. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045920
Schubert, T., and Szameitat, A. J. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: am fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 733–746.
Seraglia, B., Gamberini, L., Priftis, K., Scatturin, P., Martinelli, M., and Cutini, S. (2011). An exploratory fNIRS study with immersive virtual reality: a new method for technical implementation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:176. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00176
Spiers, H. J., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Neural substrates of driving behaviour. Neuroimage 36, 245–255.
Staplin, L., Gish, K. W., and Wagner, E. K. (2003). MaryPODS revisted: Updated crash analysis and implications for screening program implementation. J. Safety Res. 34, 389–397.
Strayer, D. L. (2007). Presentation at Cell Phones and Driver Distraction. Washington, DC: Traffic Safety Coalition.
Strayer, D. L., Drew, F. A., and Johnston, W. A. (2003). Cell-phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 9, 23–32.
Strayer, D. L., and Johnston, W. A. (2001). Cell phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 9, 23–32.
Talairach, P., and Tournoux, J. (1988). A Stereotactic Coplanar Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart: Thieme.
Uchiyama, Y., Ebe, K., Kozato, A., Okada, T., and Sadato, N. (2003). The neural substrates of driving at a safe distance: a functional MRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 352, 199–202.
Vogt, B. A., Hof, P. R., and Vogt, L. J. (2004). “Cingulate Gyrus,” in The Human Nervous System, 2nd Edn, eds G. Paxinos and J. K. Mai (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science), 915–949.
Walter, H., Vetter, S. C., Grothe, J., Wunderlich, A. P., Hahn, S., and Spitzer, M. (2001). The neural correlates of driving. Neuroreport 12, 1763–1767.
Whelihan, W. M., DiCarlo, M. A., and Paul, R. H. (2005). The relationship of neuropsychological functioning to driving competence in older persons with early cognitive decline. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 217–228.
Appendix
General Linear Model: Repeated Measure ANOVA
Table A2. Post-hoc tests suggest that speed in the distracted left-turn-traffic condition was significantly slower than both the left and the right turn speed, and the left turn speed was significantly slower than the right turn speed.
Keywords: driving, driving distractions, neural correlates of driving, driving complexity, fMRI
Citation: Schweizer TA, Kan K, Hung Y, Tam F, Naglie G and Graham SJ (2013) Brain activity during driving with distraction: an immersive fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:53. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00053
Received: 05 November 2012; Accepted: 08 February 2013;
Published online: 28 February 2013.
Edited by:
Daniel S. Margulies, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, GermanyReviewed by:
Kimmo Alho, University of Helsinki, FinlandLutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Copyright © 2013 Schweizer, Kan, Hung, Tam, Naglie and Graham. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
*Correspondence: Tom A. Schweizer, Keenan Research Centre of the Li Ka Shing Knowledge Institute, St. Michael's Hospital, 30 Bond Street, Toronto, ON M5B 1W8, Canada. e-mail: schweizerT@smh.ca