Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 April 2013
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Injury as a Neurodegenerative Disorder View all 10 articles

Traumatic brain injury and post-acute decline: what role does environmental enrichment play? A scoping review


Diana Frasca,* Diana Frasca1,2*Jennifer Tomaszczyk Jennifer Tomaszczyk2Bradford J. McFadyen, Bradford J. McFadyen3,4Robin E. Green, Robin E. Green1,2
  • 1Graduate Department of Rehabilitation Science, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Cognitive Neurorehabilitation Sciences Laboratory, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Département de Réadaptation, Université Laval, Québec City, QC, Canada
  • 4Centre Interdisciplinaire Recherche en Réadaptation et Intégration Sociale, Québec City, QC, Canada

Objectives: While a growing number of studies provide evidence of neural and cognitive decline in traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors during the post-acute stages of injury, there is limited research as of yet on environmental factors that may influence this decline. The purposes of this paper, therefore, are to (1) examine evidence that environmental enrichment (EE) can influence long-term outcome following TBI, and (2) examine the nature of post-acute environments, whether they vary in degree of EE, and what impact these variations have on outcomes.

Methods: We conducted a scoping review to identify studies on EE in animals and humans, and post-discharge experiences that relate to barriers to recovery.

Results: One hundred and twenty-three articles that met inclusion criteria demonstrated the benefits of EE on brain and behavior in healthy and brain-injured animals and humans. Nineteen papers on post-discharge experiences revealed that variables such as insurance coverage, financial, and social support, home therapy, and transition from hospital to home, can have an impact on clinical outcomes.

Conclusion: There is evidence to suggest that lack of EE, whether from lack of resources or limited ability to engage in such environments, may play a role in post-acute cognitive and neural decline. Maximizing EE in the post-acute stages of TBI may improve long-term outcomes for the individual, their family and society.

Introduction

Moderate to severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a ubiquitous injury: studies suggest an annual incidence of upwards of 20–60 per 100,000 (Narayan et al., 2002; Bruns and Hauser, 2003; Cassidy et al., 2004; C.I.H.I., 2006). Many of these injuries are sustained in young adulthood (C.I.H.I., 2006; Faul et al., 2010) and result in significant impairment to cognitive, motor, and emotional functioning. Predominant and persisting deficits to executive functioning, attention, memory, and speed of processing compromise psychosocial functioning and quality of life (Sander et al., 2001; Hawthorne et al., 2009; Resch et al., 2009). Because these deficits prevent many TBI survivors from returning to pre-injury levels of activity and participation (Dikmen et al., 1983, 1995; Lezak, 2004; Christensen et al., 2008), successful community integration is now recognized as a primary goal of rehabilitation for persons with brain injury (Sander et al., 2010).

The consequences of brain injury are particularly concerning given the high incidence of TBI. Murray and Lopez (1997) predicted that by 2020, TBI will be the third leading cause of disability in the world. Considering that males aged 15–24 years have the highest incidence of TBI (Pickett et al., 2004; C.I.H.I., 2006; Faul et al., 2010), this can mean decades of disability and lost productivity. Not surprisingly, the annual burden of acute care and rehabilitation in North America is estimated to be in the billions of dollars (SMARTRISK, 2006; Faul et al., 2010).

A theoretically intriguing and clinically important question that is emerging from the literature is whether an impediment to recovery and a contributing factor to failed community integration after moderate to severe TBI is cognitive and brain deterioration in the post-acute stages after brain injury. TBI recovery studies typically show an asymptotic pattern of recovery, with rapid improvement within the first weeks and months of injury, followed by a slower rate of improvement and then a plateau with limited measureable recovery thereafter (Basso, 1989; Heinemann et al., 1995; Blatter et al., 1997; Holbrook et al., 1999; Christodoulou et al., 2001; Farne et al., 2004). However, not only do many fail to return to pre-injury levels of function when they reach that plateau (Christensen et al., 2008), but there is growing evidence that a subset of TBI survivors show cognitive deterioration.

A number of studies that have examined post-acute cognitive changes in TBI survivors have demonstrated that across domains of functioning, a combination of maintenance, further recovery and frank declines are observed (Ruff et al., 1991; Millis et al., 2001; Sander et al., 2001; Himanen et al., 2006; Salmond et al., 2006; Till et al., 2008). For example, Till et al. (2008) showed that nearly 30% of their sample of moderate-severe TBI patients showed clinically significant decline (as measured by the reliable change index) in two or more domains of cognitive functioning.

In examining changes of the brain during the post-acute stages of TBI, imaging studies have also shown evidence of deterioration, including decreased cerebral blood flow (Kim et al., 2010), declines in whole brain volume (Blatter et al., 1997; Mackenzie et al., 2002; Trivedi et al., 2007; Ng et al., 2008; Sidaros et al., 2009; Hudak et al., 2011), atrophy of discrete gray, and white matter structures including the hippocampus and corpus callosum (Wilson et al., 1988; Bigler et al., 1997; Levine et al., 2008; Ng et al., 2008; Sidaros et al., 2008), lesion expansion (Ng et al., 2008); and reduced white matter integrity as measured by diffusion tensor imaging (Bendlin et al., 2008; Greenberg et al., 2008; Sidaros et al., 2008; Warner et al., 2010; Farbota et al., 2012).

Correlations between brain and behavioral decline have also been observed. In one study, 24 TBI survivors underwent MRI scans at 8 weeks and 12 months post-injury. The authors not only demonstrated increased atrophy during this time period, but also negatively correlated outcomes on the Glasgow Outcome Scale (Sidaros et al., 2009). In other studies, Hudak et al. (2011) found that decreases in brain volume correlated with depressive symptoms in the post-acute phase, and Farbota et al. (2012) demonstrated that diffusion tensor imaging findings (fractional anisotropy values) and neuropsychological task performance were positively correlated.

Given mounting evidence revealing post-acute decline, we suggest that it is important at this stage of research to begin to consider what factors may hold the potential to influence, and in particular, offset this decline.

One factor that may play a role is “environmental enrichment” (EE). As we will discuss further in the next section, EE broadly refers to enhanced stimulation, associated with (1) environments that provide access to cognitive as well as physical and social stimulation, and (2) conditions that encourage maximal participation. An extensive body of literature shows positive correlations between EE and cognitive and neuronal status. (Scarmeas and Stern, 2003; Will et al., 2004; Simpson and Kelly, 2011). There have also been some findings to suggest that EE influences post-acute decline. In Till et al.'s (2008) study of post-injury cognitive decline, the authors found a relationship between hours of therapy at 5 months post-injury and degree of cognitive decline from 12 to 24 months. They concluded that lack of access to complex and enriched environments, due in part to limited access to resources, may play a critical a role in decline. In another study, Miller and Green (in press) found that greater hippocampal volume loss in the chronic stages of TBI (12–24 months post-injury) was negatively correlated with degree of cognitive stimulation reported at 5 months post-injury.

These few but important findings raise the question whether post-acute decline in these survivors was influenced by the extent of enrichment, or lack thereof, in the environments to which they were discharged following the early and intensive months of therapy. A reduction in the level of enrichment in the later stages of recovery from TBI might occur when the number and hours of therapies are reduced or when patients move from in-patient neurorehabilitation back to the home environment. Additionally, TBI survivors may return to environments that are indeed complex and enriched, but without the expertise of therapists actively providing supports and adaptations to the environments, patients may be unable to engage due to cognitive, emotional and/or physical impairments that render the environments overwhelming or inaccessible.

The purposes of this paper, therefore, are to (1) examine evidence that EE can influence long-term outcome following TBI, and (2) examine the nature of post-acute environments, whether they vary in degree of EE, and what impact these variations have on outcomes. To accomplish these aims, we will undertake a scoping review summarizing literature related to EE and post-acute environments.

Scoping Review Methods

This paper addresses two of the reasons for undertaking a scoping review identified by Arksey and O'Malley (2005): to summarize and disseminate research findings and to identify research gaps in the existing literature. The scoping review typically unfolds in five steps: (1) identify the research question; (2) identify all pertinent studies; (3) select the studies for detailed analysis; (4) chart the data according to key concepts; and (5) collate and summarize the findings of the selected studies (Arksey and O'Malley, 2005; Rumrill et al., 2010):

Identify the Research Question

The research questions addressed in this paper are whether (1) EE can influence long-term recovery, and (2) post-discharge environments vary in degree of EE, and whether such variations influence outcomes.

Identify All Pertinent Studies

The literature review aimed to identify a comprehensive set of articles detailing the effects of EE in animals and humans, and the post-discharge experiences related to recovery and regaining independence in the post-acute stages after TBI. Articles that addressed these topics were obtained through use of a traditional keyword-driven electronic search guided by the following terms: TBI; recovery; EE; environmental complexity; active lifestyle; stimulation; neuroplasticity; cognitive reserve; intervention; multi-disciplinary; multi-contextual; post-rehabilitation; transition home; barriers; community integration; re-engagement. Peer-reviewed journals were searched using the PubMed and Cochrane Collaboration research databases as well as the Google Scholar search engine, for articles published between 1987 and 2012. Additionally, hand searches were conducted of references from key articles to follow up on seminal work and promising literature that might not have been captured by the databases used.

Select Studies for Detailed Analysis

To be considered for inclusion in the review, articles had to meet the following criteria: (1) describe EE (or components of EE) in animals or adult humans or (2) describe the post-discharge experience, and more specifically, the transition from hospital to home in adult brain-injury survivors; and (3) be available in English. No methodological limitations were applied to screen for levels of evidence.

Chart Data According to Key Concepts

Articles that met the inclusion criteria were reviewed in detail and categorized based on population examined, methodology, and study objectives to discover commonalities and provide connections between the sets of literatures reviewed.

Collate and Summarize Findings

The results are presented to correspond with the objectives of this paper. A numerical summary of the articles included is followed by a summary of the literature.

Results

Database searches identified 2053 articles. Of these, 142 were included in the review based on the selection criteria listed above. Table 1 provides a brief summary of the articles that were included. Tables 2 and 3 provide a more detailed summary of the papers that specifically addressed EE in brain-injured animals and humans, as well as EE and post-discharge experiences, respectively. Tables 2 and 3 detail the populations, methods, objectives, and main findings of the articles that were included.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Numerical summary of articles reviewed.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Detailed summary of articles included in “Brain-injured animals and EE” and “Brain-injured humans and EE” scoping review.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Detailed summary of articles included in “Post-discharge experiences and EE” scoping review.

Environmental Enrichment has Beneficial Effects on Brain and Behavior in Animals and Humans

Defining Environmental Enrichment

A number of definitions have been proposed for EE, and it is often defined in relative terms. In animal studies, EEs (e.g., cages with running wheels, novel toys, several animals) are typically contrasted with standard or impoverished environments (e.g., cages with a single animal, and only the basic necessities for living). In these studies, researchers have stressed the importance of having cognitive, social and physical stimulation for environments to be considered enriched and a key property is the maintenance of novelty, for example through regularly changing toys and food (Diamond, 2001; Simpson and Kelly, 2011).

Kramer et al. (2004) have suggested that while numerous positive changes in cognitive functioning, neuroanatomy, and neurochemistry have been demonstrated as a result of exposure to EE in animals, we must examine the degree to which these findings translate to humans, and that an operational definition of EE applicable to humans needs to be established. Whereas novel toys and food, running wheels, and housing several animals together maps on well to cognitive, physical, and social stimulation in animals, identification of such concrete mappings in humans has proven more difficult. Subject factors, such as motivation and mental effort play a large role in reaping the benefits of EE. Thus, personality and earlier life experiences may influence engagement with the environment, such that what is a stimulating and engaging environment for one person may not be for another (Johansson, 2003). Thus, for humans, the definition of EE is more complex, addressing both the nature of the environment and factors that influence engagement with it.

These ideas of Kramer et al. (2004) are consistent with earlier work by Schooler (1987), who defined the related concept of environmental complexity as being determined by stimulus and demand characteristics. He theorized that greater diversity of stimuli in one's environment could lead to more options/plans of action to consider and decisions to make. When cognitive efforts are reinforced and rewarded, people are motivated to continue engaging in complex environments, which in turn enhances cognitive functioning. Thus, enhancing cognitive functioning further promotes participation in complex environments, illustrating a dynamic facet of EE and its benefits. Enrichment, therefore, reflects environmental complexity (e.g., opportunity to participate in different sports, clubs or social networks, and to engage in intellectually demanding activities), one's inclination to participate in the environment, and the frequency of participation.

Given the limitations in defining, manipulating and controlling EE in human studies, animal studies have arguably provided the most compelling evidence for the causal effects of EE.

Benefits of Environmental Enrichment

Environmental enrichment in healthy animals

In 1947, Hebb examined if rats exposed to EE would improve behaviorally on problem-solving tasks, compared to a control group (Hebb, 1947). EE rats were free to roam his home while control rats were housed in standard laboratory cages. Hebb found performance was superior in the EE group. Since then, more systematically controlled studies have had similar findings.

Exposure to EE has been associated with increases in cognitive functioning, specifically improvements in response selectivity, learning ability, spatial and problem solving skills, memory, and processing speed (Mohammed et al., 1990; Rosenzweig and Bennett, 1996; Nilsson et al., 1999; Van Praag et al., 2000; Kobayashi et al., 2002; Milgram, 2003; Valero et al., 2011; Leger et al., 2012; Speisman et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2013). Reductions in boredom (Meagher and Mason, 2012) and frustration (Latham and Mason, 2010) have been demonstrated as well. The benefits of EE are also observable at cellular and molecular levels. There is evidence of increased neurogenesis, synaptogenesis and dendritic spine density in parts of the brain associated with memory and learning (i.e., hippocampus, dentate gyrus and cerebellar Purkinje cells) in response to EE (Kolb and Whishaw, 1998; Johansson, 2000, 2002; Van Praag et al., 2000; Churchill et al., 2002; Kempermann et al., 2002; Valero et al., 2011; Eckert and Abraham, 2012; Jung and Herms, 2012; Leger et al., 2012; Speisman et al., 2012; Fares et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2013). Increases in brain weight and cortical thickness have also been demonstrated, as have increases in the amount of nerve growth factor, brain derived neurotropic factor, myelination, acetylcholinesterase activity, neurotransmitters, glial proliferation, blood vessels (number and size), and protein synthesis (Rosenzweig, 1966; West and Greenough, 1972; Bennett et al., 1974; Mohammed et al., 1990; Rosenzweig and Bennett, 1996; Kolb and Whishaw, 1998; Van Praag et al., 2000; Diamond, 2001; Churchill et al., 2002; Pietropaolo et al., 2004; Will et al., 2004; Bennett et al., 2006; Hoffman et al., 2008; Gabriel et al., 2009a,b; Lores-Arnaiz et al., 2010; Qiu et al., 2011, 2012; Williamson et al., 2012; Fares et al., 2013; Vazquez-Sanroman et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2013).

In examining properties of EE that produce beneficial effects, factors such as intensity and duration emerge as critical for reaping the most benefits. Bennett et al. (2006) examined the effects of long-term continuous EE on memory performance in aged mice. Mice received a 10-week treatment of either 5 min of daily handling housed in impoverished environments (i.e., clear plastic shoe boxes), 3 h of daily exposure to a basic EE (i.e., small bin with fresh bedding, a running wheel, a plastic rodent dwelling, a plastic tube for climbing, a few toys), or continuous complex EE (i.e., larger bin to allow for more mice to be housed together, and for larger and more complex objects). Continuously enriched older mice performed significantly better than other groups of aged mice in spatial memory tasks, and were indistinguishable from younger control mice in their performance.

Amaral et al. (2008) examined durations of EE in mice and influences on behavioral changes (open field habituation and locomotion). They compared mice exposed to 1, 4, and 8 weeks of EE. While mice exposed to 4 and 8 weeks showed behavioral effects, the 1-week group did not. Furthermore, mice in the 4-week exposure group demonstrated effects lasting 2 months post-EE, but only mice in the 8-week EE group demonstrated effects lasting up to 6 months. The authors concluded that (1) a minimum EE period is necessary to induce beneficial behavioral effects, (2) the effect of EE can persist at least partially for many months after its cessation, and (3) the degree of persistence is greater in animals exposed to longer durations of EE. Relevant to the discharge environment of humans and the importance of ongoing therapy/stimulation, these findings showed that the benefits of EE are lost when animals are removed from enriched environments.

Environmental enrichment in healthy humans

Scarmeas and Stern (2003) undertook a review of the relationship between lifestyle and cognitive reserve, which they defined as the degree to which the brain can create and use networks or cognitive paradigms that are more efficient or flexible, and thus less susceptible to disruption. They asked whether exercising the brain had the same healthy effects as exercising the body. They found that greater participation in intellectual and social leisure activities was associated with less cognitive decline in healthy older adults and a lower incidence of dementia. These important “use it or lose it” findings support the need for EE in the sub-acute and chronic stages of TBI to avert decline. The authors proposed that leisure activities and an active lifestyle might buffer against cognitive decline by: (1) increasing synaptic density, which results in more efficient cognitive functioning of unaffected neurons; (2) more efficient use of the same brain networks; and (3) more efficient use of alternative brain networks. They also found that although it is a very active area of research, our understanding of the specific active ingredients that buffer against cognitive decline has not been fully elucidated.

In addition to studies examining how mentally active lifestyles are associated with cognitive benefits (Gribbin et al., 1980; Pushkar et al., 1997; Wang et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2002, 2003; Crowe et al., 2003; Verghese et al., 2003; Newson and Kemps, 2005; Fujiwara et al., 2009), studies have also illustrated the benefits of maintaining physically active (Clarkson-Smith and Hartley, 1989; Schuit et al., 2001; Pignatti et al., 2002; Voss et al., 2011; Xu et al., 2011) and socially active lifestyles (Bassuk et al., 1999; Fratiglioni et al., 2000; Mahoney et al., 2000; Seeman et al., 2001). However, they are correlational in design and leave open the possibility that higher functioning people seek out the continuous exposure.

A number of further studies have shown the benefits of EE for healthy older adults (Huppert, 1991; Christensen and Mackinnon, 1993; Davidson and Bar-Yam, 2006; Sampson et al., 2009; Marioni et al., 2012a,b; Paillard-Borg et al., 2012). Winocur and Moscovitch (1990) compared cognitive functioning in community-dwelling and institutionalized older adults. After controlling for variables such as IQ, age, and health, they found that community-dwelling older adults had higher levels of cognitive functioning. They also identified a subgroup of high-functioning institutionalized older adults who performed similarly to the community-dwelling group. For this subgroup, they postulated that cognitive functioning may have been influenced by adjustment to institutional life, such that more social activity was correlated with better cognitive functioning, as was found in a prior study (Winocur et al., 1987). Winocur and Moscovitch suggested that lower cognitive functioning observed in the institutionalized group might be a result of an under-stimulating environment. This interpretation is also consistent with the position of Schooler and Mulatu (2001), who suggested that even if people have higher levels of education, it is the continued practice in complex activities that maintains their cognitive abilities—speaking directly to the thesis of this paper.

A number of studies have examined the benefits of continuous exposure (Blackerby, 1990; Spivack et al., 1992; Willer et al., 1999; De Weerdt et al., 2000; Shiel et al., 2001; Zhu et al., 2001; Cifu et al., 2003; De Wit et al., 2005), and interventions that incorporate many elements of EE, such as cognitive, social, and physical stimulation (Hayslip et al., 1995; Neely and Backman, 1995; Fasotti et al., 2000; Powell et al., 2002; Vance and Johns, 2002; Gunther et al., 2003; Rath et al., 2003; Noice and Noice, 2004; Van De Winckel et al., 2004; Green et al., 2006; Stine-Morrow et al., 2007; Spikman et al., 2009). These types of interventions can be contrasted with interventions involving manipulations over a discrete period of time that are designed to enhance a specific cognitive domain or skill. The former have broadly led to more generalizable gains, both cognitively and physically.

Brain-injured animals and environmental enrichment

Brain-injured animals show clear-cut benefits from exposure to EE. Studies comparing recovery in brain-injured animals reared in EEs to those reared in standard environments have demonstrated increased neurogenesis, upregulation of neurotrophic factors, increased neuronal survival, increased afferent innervation, as well as reductions in spontaneous apoptosis and infarct size (Van Praag et al., 2000; Dobrossy and Dunnett, 2001, 2004; Passineau et al., 2001; Gobbo and O'Mara, 2004; Will et al., 2004; Pereira et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2008; Sozda et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2010; Monaco et al., 2013; Shin et al., 2013). Superior performance on tasks of learning and memory, and spatial reference has also been found (Hamm et al., 1996; Johansson and Ohlsson, 1996; Passineau et al., 2001; Gobbo and O'Mara, 2004; Will et al., 2004; Lippert-Gruener et al., 2007; Pereira et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2008; Sozda et al., 2010; Monaco et al., 2013). Hamm et al. (1996) compared cognitive functioning in brain-injured and sham-brain-injured rats housed in EEs to those housed in standard environments. Rats housed in EEs were in social groups and had access to a variety of foods, novel toys, scented objects, and a running wheel. At 15 days post-injury, brain-injured rats exposed to EE performed significantly better than brain-injured rats in standard environments. Interestingly, they also found that brain-injured rats recovering in EE performed just as well as non-brain injured animals.

In an older but seminal study by Johansson and Ohlsson (1996), brain-injured rats recovering in comprehensive EE (i.e., cognitive, social, and physical enrichment) were compared to those recovering in either enriched social environments or enriched exercise environments. Rats in the EE group were placed in one cage with several other rats, and novel toys and objects that promoted exercise, whereas rats in the social-interaction group were caged with other rats without objects or toys, and rats in the exercise group were individually caged with access to a running wheel. The social-interaction group improved more than the exercise group; however, the EE group improved more than either group. More recently, Hoffman et al. (2008) demonstrated the importance of timing and duration of EE, comparing brain-injured rats exposed to continuous EE to those in early and temporary EE (immediately post-injury lasting 1 week), late EE (commencing 1 week post-injury) and no EE. The continuous EE group not only had significantly less hippocampal cell loss, but also performed better on motor and cognitive tests. Interestingly, they found an advantage of continuous and early EE for motor tasks, but an advantage of continuous and delayed EE for cognitive tasks. The benefits of increased EE-exposure in brain-injured rats have since been replicated (De Witt et al., 2011; Matter et al., 2010), and, moreover, shown to last for up to 6 months (Cheng et al., 2012).

These studies provide evidence that EE help animals to recover functions to levels that make them indistinguishable from healthy controls, and that continual EE is necessary to maintain both neural and cognitive improvements. The benefits of EE are substantial, but not permanent, thus highlighting the importance of ongoing stimulation. These findings are particularly relevant to the post-discharge environment, and the factors may contribute to decline.

Brain-injured humans and environmental enrichment

EE interventions for brain-injured humans can be grouped into two broad types: those that provide increased hours and duration of therapy (without prescription of specific content of therapies) and those that address discrete impairments and focus on improvement of a specific skill. Note that in clinical practice, the latter might be referred to as “training” rather than EE. However, we include them here as they entail increased cognitive stimulation.

While EE intervention designs have not been as comprehensive in human TBI studies, the effects of some of the classical properties of EE, such as novelty, intensity, and duration, have been examined. However, much like in healthy individuals, interventions for TBI survivors aimed at training specific skills have often resulted in poor generalization to everyday performance (Ruff et al., 1991; Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005; Sohlberg et al., 2000; Park and Ingles, 2001; Boman et al., 2004; Kennedy et al., 2008). In contrast to skills training, Cicerone et al. (2000) have suggested that cognitive rehabilitation should not focus exclusively on remediation of impairments, but should reduce disability and help restore social role functioning. In an earlier framework, Toglia (1991) proposed a “multi-context” treatment approach to enhance generalizability that contained hallmarks of what later became EE: namely, varied stimulation and environments that are meaningful to the person that thereby enhance engagement. Interventions that have incorporated such components have demonstrated better generalizability, as well as improvements in community integration (Fasotti et al., 2000; Powell et al., 2002; Vance and Johns, 2002; Rath et al., 2003; Van De Winckel et al., 2004; Toglia et al., 2010; Leon-Carrion et al., 2012).

The second broad category of interventions that are more intensive have also resulted in significant functional gains (Willer et al., 1999; Cicerone et al., 2004). Cicerone et al. (2004) compared the effectiveness of an intensive cognitive rehabilitation program to standard neurorehabilitation for persons with TBI. Participants in the intensive program demonstrated greater improvement in cognitive function (composite score based on attention, speed of processing, memory and executive function test scores) as well as greater improvement in community integration. Increasing hours of therapy has also led to greater functional and cognitive gains and faster recoveries (Blackerby, 1990; Spivack et al., 1992; De Weerdt et al., 2000; Shiel et al., 2001; Zhu et al., 2001; Slade et al., 2002; Cifu et al., 2003; De Wit et al., 2005; Zhu et al., 2007). Turner-Stokes et al.'s (2005) review of multi-disciplinary rehabilitation for brain injury recommended that “intensive intervention appears to lead to earlier gains.” Therefore, they concluded that “Patients discharged from in-patient rehabilitation should have access to out-patient or community-based services appropriate to their needs.” Cernich et al. (2010) provided a similar recommendation in their review of cognitive rehabilitation following TBI. Recently, Hayden et al. (2013) provided support for this recommendation, demonstrating functional gains for TBI survivors receiving rehabilitation services in the post-acute phase after brain injury.

Absence or Reduction of Environmental Enrichment

If exposure to EE is beneficial at both behavioral and neural levels, it logically follows that the absence or reduction of EE would lead to poorer behavioral and neural health. Experience-dependent plasticity refers to changes in the brain that result from behavioral experiences. While stimulation can enhance or maintain neural pathways, the absence of stimulation can depress or weaken them with associated loss of a function that was previously acquired or mastered (Rubinov et al., 2009; Warraich and Kleim, 2010). Overall, there appears to be a growing consensus that maintaining and increasing neuroplasticity of the brain depends on continual and intensive cognitive, physical, and social stimulation (Kleim and Jones, 2008). TBI survivors transitioning from a rehabilitation setting to a home setting are therefore vulnerable to reversal of those gains made in the early and intensive therapeutic setting.

This assertion is supported by the flipside of EE experiments: animals that are exposed to less stimulating or impoverished environments do not fare as well. As described above, mice with less EE exposure have lower performances on spatial memory tasks, as compared to continuously enriched mice (Bennett et al., 2006), and rats that received less EE exposure maintain benefits for a shorter period of time (Amaral et al., 2008). Similarly, brain injured rats recovering in impoverished environments show poorer recovery of cognitive and motor functioning (Hamm et al., 1996; Hoffman et al., 2008; Matter et al., 2010; De Witt et al., 2011) and do not maintain benefits gained from short-term EE (Matter et al., 2010; De Witt et al., 2011; Cheng et al., 2012).

Furthermore, there is evidence that effects of environmental conditions are reversible (Bernstein, 1972; Winocur, 1998). Winocur (1998) demonstrated that when he transferred rats from impoverished to enriched environments, they improved significantly, whereas rats transferred from enriched and standard environments to impoverished ones declined. Similarly, more sedentary lifestyles have been linked to disease and disability, and overall poorer health (Huppert, 1991; Pushkar et al., 1997; Mackinnor et al., 2003; Salthouse, 2006; Shors et al., 2012).

Environmental Enrichment, Post-Acute Decline and Sub-Acute Atrophy

As therapies diminish in frequency over time (e.g., when patients move from in-patient neurorehabilitation to the home or a chronic-care facility), the amount of EE may also lessen. In the context of chronic TBI, Turner and Green (2008) examined the principles of negative neuroplasticity (maladaptive morphological changes to the brain in response to environmental factors), as described by Mahncke et al. (2006a,b), in the context of normal aging. These are: (1) reduced schedules of activity, (2) noisy processing in peripheral and central sensory systems, (3) alterations in neuromodulary control, and (4) negative learning. They suggest that a similar downward spiral of negative neuroplastic change may undermine long-term outcome in TBI due to similar environmental changes (as well as physiological changes) that result in withdrawal from communities and social networks, resulting in reduced stimulation. Such changes are withdrawal from the workforce (or school), physical losses that preclude travel, and perceptual and cognitive decrements that compromise communication.

As previously noted, in the case where neural pathways are under-stimulated, it is possible to lose the function that was acquired or mastered (Rubinov et al., 2009; Warraich and Kleim, 2010). Findings of increased neural representation after training (Cirillo et al., 2011; Cardinali et al., 2012) and different brain activation when comparing exposure vs. deprivation of stimulation (Klinge et al., 2012) provide evidence for experience-dependent neuroplasticity. This raises the question whether under-stimulation of neural pathways, due to disuse, may be responsible for post-acute neural degeneration seen in TBI survivors. If so, EE exposure may play a role in minimizing, or averting, this problem.

With respect to neural degeneration, mechanisms of apoptosis have been modeled in animals to understand the cascade of events that takes place after TBI (Raghupathi et al., 2000; Keane et al., 2001; Raghupathi, 2004). Coulson et al. (2004) provided support for their hypothesis that synaptic input may be the key to regulating neuronal survival and death pathways following neurotrauma. Synaptic activity regulates expression levels of neurotrophins and facilitates growth factor signaling. There is evidence that EE exposure enhances nerve growth factor and brain-derived neurotropic factor in animals (Clarkson-Smith and Hartley, 1989; Blackerby, 1990; Bassuk et al., 1999; De Weerdt et al., 2000; Mahoney et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2002; Cifu et al., 2003; Johansson, 2003; Rath et al., 2003; Spikman et al., 2009). As well, animal studies have demonstrated that EE can enhance neurogenesis and attenuate apoptosis in the injured brain (Van Praag et al., 2000; Dobrossy and Dunnett, 2001, 2004; Passineau et al., 2001; Gobbo and O'Mara, 2004; Will et al., 2004; Pereira et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2008). Lastly, neurons deprived of synaptic input can be rescued from death by an increased supply of growth factors (Coulson et al., 2004). Taken together, these findings offer the possibility that EE can help to prevent neuronal death.

The relation between changes in brain structure and neuropsychological performance has also been recently explored (Farbota et al., 2012), providing further evidence that as we see declines at a neural levels, we also see parallel declines in cognitive functioning. Recent work from our laboratory has demonstrated that in TBI survivors, more engagement in EE, defined as participation in activities involving cognitive, physical, and social demands, is correlated with less hippocampal volume loss in the post-acute stages (Miller and Green, in press). While these findings are correlational, they provide preliminary evidence of the positive impact that an enriched environment can have in TBI survivors.

The research reviewed within this section of the scoping review supports that EE improves outcomes and that a dearth of EE results in poorer outcomes. Studies demonstrate continued and repeated EE exposure (Blackerby, 1990; Spivack et al., 1992; De Weerdt et al., 2000; Schooler and Mulatu, 2001; Shiel et al., 2001; Zhu et al., 2001, 2007; Cifu et al., 2003; De Wit et al., 2005; Amaral et al., 2008), which provides intensive cognitive, social, and physical stimulation is necessary to induce beneficial effects (Willer et al., 1999; Cicerone et al., 2004; Bennett et al., 2006). Furthermore, if this exposure is reduced or removed, the benefits will not be maintained and may diminish (Bernstein, 1972; Winocur, 1998; Cheng et al., 2012). Synaptic input may be necessary to maintain and strengthen neural pathways and connections, particularly those at risk after brain injury (Coulson et al., 2004; Turner and Green, 2008). Moreover, this research supports our contention that for the subset of people with moderate-severe TBI that show post-acute cognitive decline and neural deterioration in the post-acute phase, there may be some environmental variables that contribute to their negative outcomes.

Post-Discharge Experiences and Environmental Enrichment

In the first section, we discussed findings revealing that the brain atrophies and that cognitive abilities decline in the post-acute stages of brain injury. In the second section, we discussed findings that demonstrate that EE can alter the brain (both healthy and damaged), and perhaps most importantly that it has the potential to minimize atrophy after brain injury. Moreover, a mechanism of disuse-mediated atrophy has been proposed (i.e., synaptic input and neuronal survival) (Coulson et al., 2004).

A critical question, then, is to what extent do post-discharge factors reflect EE? This is important because it is possible that the post-discharge environment may be un-enriched, and this would in theory exacerbate/lead to post-acute cognitive and neural declines. Thus, it is of value to explore the factors that influence the environments in which people with brain injuries return with respect to level of enrichment, so that factors can be adjusted to provide optimal levels of EE. In the current section, we will discuss how post-discharge variables are conceptually related to EE and we will also discuss findings that show how these examples of EE (or lack thereof) correspond to human TBI outcomes.

What is Environmental Enrichment in the Post-Discharge Environment?

Continued participation in environments that are challenging, yet at levels that allow people to participate and remain motivated to do so, is the crux of EE. In the section “Environmental Enrichment in Healthy Humans” we summarized findings demonstrating that people who (1) attend more social events, (2) are more physically active, and (3) engage in activities with continuous novelty (e.g., learning a new language, playing bridge) are mentally healthier. While demographic variables (i.e., age, pre-morbid intelligence, level of education) influence outcome after brain injury (Ruff et al., 1991; Green et al., 2008), environmental variables (e.g., access to insurance coverage) also play a vital role in recovery, as will be discussed. We contend that these variables map onto EE in that they provide cognitive, social, and physical stimulation through access to therapy, community resources, fostering return to meaningful occupations, and encouraging engagement in their environments.

Post-Discharge Environmental Factors that Influence Mental and Physical Stimulation

A number of factors influence the degree of cognitive, social, and physical stimulation TBI survivors experience either directly (e.g., presence of community resources such as a support groups or fitness centers) or indirectly (e.g., by influencing access to therapy). These include insurance, financial support, social support, and community resources.

Insurance

As recommended by Turner-Stokes et al. (2005), after discharge from in-patient rehabilitation, TBI survivors should have access to out-patient or community-based services appropriate to their needs to facilitate the recovery process. Till et al. (2008) demonstrated that post-acute cognitive decline was negatively correlated with hours of therapy, which was associated with insurance coverage. It has been widely demonstrated that individuals who have insurance coverage receive more access to therapy after discharge than those who do not (Pressman, 2007; Heffernan et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2012; Lundqvist and Samuelsson, 2012). Those that are insured have better access to post-acute medical care, which includes physical, occupational, and cognitive therapies, as well as home health and nursing needs, modification of living environment, vocational training, and job retraining (Shafi et al., 2007). In line with these findings, Shafi et al. (2007) found that ethnic minorities were less likely to be insured and more likely to have moderate to severe disability at follow-up.

Financial support

Diminished financial resources may reduce opportunities for accessing EEs. For example, Sander et al. (2009) reported that after controlling for age, education, injury severity, and race/ethnicity, income made a significant contribution to the variance in social integration, and in a more recent study, Sander et al. (2011) reported that TBI survivors perceived financial issues (e.g., home ownership, insufficient funds), as contributing to decreased participation in the community. Similar to Shafi et al. (2007) discussed previously, Staudenmayer et al. (2007) examined ethnic disparities in long-term functional outcomes after TBI. They concluded that less social and financial resources were likely implicated as an explanation. Additionally, in a study examining perceived needs after brain injury, many TBI survivors reported still requiring help managing money 1 year post-injury (Corrigan et al., 2004), which may compound the challenges of diminished finances after TBI. Increased financial burden can mean less access to expensive resources (e.g., therapy, participating in social activities, transportation).

Social support

Logically, a supportive social network influences participation in therapy (Sander et al., 2009; Turner et al., 2009b; Keightley et al., 2011), for example by providing transportation, accompaniment to therapies, supervision for recommended regimens for which safety is of concern (e.g., a gym program). Turner et al. (2007) found that those who had more supportive and closer networks at home had better transitions from the hospital setting to the home environment, as this was related to more access to social activities and transportation outside the home.

Post-Discharge Factors that Influence Engagement

Without private insurance, and with less social and financial resources, TBI survivors may be less able to engage in stimulating activities to facilitate recovery. However, as we will discuss, there are also factors that are purported to influence the level of engagement in the post-discharge environment, such as the amount of family support and patient education provided, as well as the structure and routine present in their home environment.

Family support

In Freeman's (1997) review of community-based rehabilitation for TBI survivors, he suggested that the level of care in the home setting, with a strong support network, can play a major role in successful rehabilitation. The family environment may provide a wide variety of activities that are inclusive, stimulating, and meaningful to the individual. Importantly, these are all properties found to be critical for experience-dependent plasticity (Kleim and Jones, 2008) and generalization of relearnt skills (Toglia, 1991). In a case study by McCormack and Liddiard (2009), a TBI survivor receiving community rehabilitation was followed for 3 years. The authors concluded that the supportive and effective care system in his family facilitated his recovery following severe TBI. His progress was attributed to active familial involvement that fostered goal setting and carry-over between sessions. Furthermore, learning took place in his home environment, increasing his ability to generalize skills. The authors concluded that these findings “add further weight … to the thesis that, with the right support, there is no place like home.”

Post-discharge information and education

Several studies have indicated that the most often-reported barrier is adequate preparation prior to discharge (Rotondi et al., 2007; Sander et al., 2009; Keightley et al., 2011; Sander et al., 2011; Turner et al., 2011b). Many TBI survivors and their caregivers report that they were not given enough information regarding brain injuries (e.g., behavioral sequelae), how to access community resources (e.g., rehabilitation, emotional support, respite services), or how to access or implement home therapies (Rotondi et al., 2007; Sander et al., 2009, 2011; Keightley et al., 2011; Turner et al., 2011b). Corrigan et al. (2004) reported that TBI survivors who initially reported requiring help participating in recreation still did not have their perceived needs (viewed as a measure of quality of life) met 1 year post-injury. Likewise, Sander et al. (2011) reported that for TBI survivors, feeling more integrated into the community was related to greater participation in their environments. Without receiving appropriate assistance and information/educational resources to facilitate the post-discharge process, successful community integration will continue to be a challenge.

Routine and schedules

Many studies document that TBI survivors report feeling ill-prepared for the transition from hospital to home (Rusconi and Turner-Stokes, 2003). Several qualitative studies have explored the transition home, and the barriers or difficulties that TBI survivors experience (Rittman et al., 2004; Turner et al., 2007, 2009b, 2011a). Rittman et al. (2004) found that, post-discharge, commonly reported problems were increased idle time, boredom, and little-to-no engagement in meaningful activities. Survivors often reported, “… finding something new every day that they couldn't do …” What appears to influence the above is routine, or lack thereof. When routines were not re-established, survivors and caregivers experienced more chaos and disruption in the transition home. Turner et al. (2011a) examined perspectives concerning recovery and adjustment during the transition phase from hospital to home, and found that the process of adjusting emotionally to life at home posed significant challenges for many brain-injury survivors during this phase.

Turner et al.'s (2007) qualitative study of brain injury survivors' experiences with the transition from hospital to home made similar observations in terms of difficulty re-engaging in meaningful activities and occupations. However, those who were able to create a routine or structure once at home had better transitions. Essentially, this facilitated participation in their environments by providing organization in their daily schedules, which had been previously often provided for them in the rehabilitation setting.

Turner et al. (2009b) extended their work by examining re-engagement in meaningful occupations during the transition from hospital to home in brain-injury survivors. Many survivors reported that recovery sped up at home, and caregivers frequently attributed this to key elements such as creating routines and schedules, which enabled participation in meaningful activities. However, for those that did not have such experiences, not being able to participate in desired occupations led to stress and frustration. Turner et al. (2009b) found that “for some participants, the impact of this change in environment, coupled with the reality of not being able to engage in their desired occupations, led to a relatively unproductive lifestyle in which the main activity of their day involved little more than watching television or playing computer games.” In Hoogerdijk et al.'s (2011) study of identity after TBI, the authors suggested that the adaptation process is a necessary struggle to gain new identity and it is facilitated by engagement in familiar occupations in familiar environments. In line with this finding, Nalder et al. (2012a,b) reported that TBI survivors that were able to reengage in meaningful occupations had greater perceived success of the transition from hospital to home. Reengagement in meaningful occupations was often experienced by individuals with higher levels of global functioning and psychosocial integration.

Summary of Post-Discharge Experiences and Environmental Enrichmment

The research presented in this section of the scoping review provides evidence that there are variables in the post-discharge environment that influence the amount stimulation and other variables that influence the level of engagement in EE. Largely, evidence suggests that those with better access and resources that map onto EE (i.e., insurance coverage, financial and social support) and those that are in environments that encourage participation (i.e., strong familial support, access to educational resources, more structure) have better functional outcomes.

Discussion

The most direct (preliminary) evidence of the benefits of EE in the post-acute stages of recovery comes from work by Miller and Green (in press), where more engagement in enriched environments, defined as participation in activities involving cognitive, physical, and social demands, was correlated with less hippocampal volume loss. Further direct evidence of comes from Till et al.'s (2008) finding of a relationship between hours of therapy at 5 months post-injury and degree of cognitive decline, which the authors speculated was ay be due in part to lack of access to complex and enriched environments.

As we have discussed, a large corpus of studies have demonstrated the benefits of EE in healthy and brain-injured animals. There is evidence of neuroplastic change after EE exposure, with beneficial changes to neuroanatomy and neurochemistry (Kramer et al., 2004). These studies further support Coulson et al.'s (2004) hypothesis that synaptic input is critical to offset atrophy, in that increased input regulates neuronal survival, and prevents or attenuates apoptosis. This is of particular relevance to the neural decline observed in the post-acute stages after TBI. Turner and Green's (2008) re-formulation of the work of Mahncke et al. (2006a,b) in healthy older adults—that negative neuroplasticity (i.e., reduced activity schedules) may undermine long-term outcome in TBI—is also in accord with the hypothesis that EE may indeed play an important role in recovery.

In humans, engaging in cognitively, socially, and physically stimulating activities is related to better cognitive functioning in younger and older adults. Studies have also demonstrated that therapies for TBI survivors delivering continuous and intensified interventions result in better recovery at all levels of analysis, as well as better functional gains. These therapies have critical elements of EE, such as novelty, intensity, and prolonged periods of engagement in meaningful activities.

According to Schooler's (1987) model of environmental complexity, when cognitive efforts are reinforced and rewarded, people are motivated to continue engaging in complex environments, which in turn enhances cognitive functioning. However, TBI survivors may show reduced participation in complex environments, due to cognitive impairment, lack of psychological support/facilitation, or lack of resources, and thereby fail to obtain the emotional and neuromodulatory rewards for engaging. These conditions create a downward spiral of negative neuroplasticity (Turner and Green, 2008). Animal literature examining EE has provided critical evidence for the benefits of maintaining high levels of enrichment and stimulation post-discharge (Hamm et al., 1996; Bennett et al., 2006; Amaral et al., 2008; Hoffman et al., 2008). Findings that support the “Use it or Lose it” theory also suggest that continued engagement in mentally effortful activities is necessary to maintain cognitive functioning (Huppert, 1991; Pushkar et al., 1997; Mackinnor et al., 2003; Salthouse, 2006; Shors et al., 2012), and that an individual's environment (e.g., stressful or under-stimulating) can influence level of activity (Winocur and Moscovitch, 1990; Winocur, 1998). While TBI survivors may benefit from capacity-enhancing and intensified therapies provided in hospital, animal models have demonstrated that the effects of this enrichment will only continue to persist with longer exposure periods (Amaral et al., 2008). Furthermore, transitioning from a more stimulating/complex environment to a lesser one may result in a loss of benefits (Winocur, 1998).

The aim of this paper was to examine the question whether EE in the sub-acute and chronic stages of injury can influence progressive neural and cognitive decline. The aim of the first section was to establish the role of EE in long-term outcomes, and in particular, in offsetting decline; the aim of the second section was to illustrate that in the post-acute stages of injury, a number of factors influence the degree of EE. We have argued that post-discharge environments map onto conventional variables considered to create EE. Access to rehabilitative, social, and community resources, as well as strong support networks, provides essential cognitive, social, and physical stimulation. Moreover, these factors have shown to influence outcomes.

While there is strong evidence to suggest that TBI may be a progressive injury, more research needs to be conducted to further elucidate the role of EE in influencing this decline. It is important to note that the exact active ingredients of current interventions that incorporate multi-context environments and higher intensity therapies are still unclear, and how they compare to therapies that incorporate all elements of EE is still unknown. Furthermore, studies are needed that examine whether there is a correlation between post-acute brain changes and declines in function, as well as the type of environments that TBI survivors return to post-discharge. Turner et al. (2008) recommended that research is needed to explore the transition home after brain injury in order to “(1) Develop a comprehensive theoretical framework of the transition phase; and (2) facilitate both the validation of current intervention strategies and the development of innovative/tailored intervention approaches.”

As reported by caregivers and TBI survivors, creating a routine and structure around daily activities facilitated engagement. Furthermore, activities that are meaningful and at an appropriate level (e.g., tailored to the individuals specific impairments) to facilitate participation are critical. Researchers that have examined the needs of TBI survivors and their caregivers in the post-acute phase have recommended increased education provided by health care institutions would be beneficial to ease the transition from hospital to home (Turner et al., 2007, 2009a, 2011b; Arango-Lasprilla et al., 2011). Based on the results of our scoping review, we suggest that more resources and guidelines on how to create structure and daily routines, as well as information regarding self-administered therapies and activities for the individual and their caregiver, would be beneficial. Further rehabilitation support during the post-acute stages of recovery, such as home assessments or on-going therapy maintenance, would also be optimal. Gan et al. (2010) examined the support needs after brain injury and made several recommendations for support services. They suggested that family systems-based services should be accessible, life-long, individualized and flexible, as well as efficient, and that support system-based services should include proper diagnoses, incorporate a multi-component approach, provide brain-injury-specific services that are responsive to needs with proactive follow-ups, and importantly, provide continuous education. The benefits of social peer-mentoring programs as an intervention to enhance improvements in social integration for TBI survivors has also shown promise in recent studies and is currently being further investigated (Struchen et al., 2011; Hanks et al., 2012).

TBI survivors often fail to return to pre-injury levels of cognitive function, as well as employment and income, and these factors have been shown to be related with life satisfaction, perceived quality of life, depression and social isolation (Christensen et al., 2008; Eriksson et al., 2009; Hawthorne et al., 2009; Resch et al., 2009; Shigaki et al., 2009; Strandberg, 2009; Tsaousides et al., 2009). Assuming that post-acute atrophy contributes to the failure to return to pre-injury levels of cognitive function and thus successful community integration, then the clinical consequences are notable.

While the elements that comprise EE may be different from one person to another, it is evident that engagement in such environments is beneficial at both the cognitive and neural level, and thus it is plausible that EE can offset post-acute deterioration. Providing information and support to ease the transition home, information about the benefits of an enriched environment and guidelines on how to successfully participate in enriched settings, may be key elements in improving recovery. Most importantly, it may be a critical factor to facilitate successful community integration.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Amaral, O. B., Vargas, R. S., Hansel, G., Izquierdo, I., and Souza, D. O. (2008). Duration of environmental enrichment influences the magnitude and persistence of its behavioral effects on mice. Physiol. Behav. 93, 388–394.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Nicholls, E., Villasenor Cabrera, T., Drew, A., Jimenez-Maldonado, M., and Martinez-Cortes, M. L. (2011). Health-related quality of life in caregivers of individuals with traumatic brain injury from Guadalajara, Mexico. J. Rehabil. Med. 43, 983–986.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arksey, H., and O'Malley, L. (2005). Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 8, 19–32.

Basso, A. (1989). “Spontaneous recovery and language rehabilitation,” in Cognitive Approaches in Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, eds X. Seron and G. Deloche (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 17–37.

Bassuk, S., Glass, T., and Berkman, L. (1999). Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Ann. Intern. Med. 131, 165–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bendlin, B. B., Ries, M. L., Lazar, M., Alexander, A. L., Dempsey, R. J., Rowley, H. A., et al. (2008). Longitudinal changes in patients with traumatic brain injury assessed with diffusion-tensor and volumetric imaging. Neuroimage 42, 503–514.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bennett, E. L., Rosenzweig, M. R., Diamond, M. C., Morimoto, H., and Hebert, M. (1974). Effects of successive environments on brain measures. Physiol. Behav. 12, 621–631.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bennett, J., McRae, P., Levy, L., and Frick, K. (2006). Long-term continuous, but not daily, environmental enrichment reduces spatial memory decline in aged male mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 85, 139–152.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bernstein, L. (1972). The reversibility of learning deficits in early environmentally restricted rats as a function of amount of experience in later life. J. Psychosom. Res. 16, 71–73.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bigler, E. D., Blatter, D. D., Anderson, C. V., Johnson, S. C., Gale, S. D., Hopkins, R. O., et al. (1997). Hippocampal volume in normal aging and traumatic brain injury. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 18, 11–23.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Blackerby, W. F. (1990). Intensity of rehabilitation and length of stay. Brain Inj. 4, 167–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Blatter, D. D., Bigler, E. D., Gale, S. D., Johnson, S. C., Anderson, C. V., Burnett, B. M., et al. (1997). MR-based brain and cerebrospinal fluid measurement after traumatic brain injury: correlation with neuropsychological outcome. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 18, 1–10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Boman, I. L., Lindstedt, M., Hemmingsson, H., and Bartfai, A. (2004). Cognitive training in home environment. Brain Inj. 18, 985–995.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bruns, J. Jr., and Hauser, W. A. (2003). The epidemiology of traumatic brain injury: a review. Epilepsia 44(Suppl. 10), 2–10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cardinali, L., Jacobs, S., Brozzoli, C., Frassinetti, F., Roy, A. C., and Farne, A. (2012). Grab an object with a tool and change your body: tool-use-dependent changes of body representation for action. Exp. Brain Res. 218, 259–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cassidy, J. D., Carroll, L. J., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., Von Holst, H., Holm, L., et al. (2004). Incidence, risk factors and prevention of mild traumatic brain injury: results of the WHO collaborating centre task force on mild traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Med. 43(Suppl), 28–60.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cernich, A. N., Kurtz, S. M., Mordecai, K. L., and Ryan, P. B. (2010). Cognitive rehabilitation in traumatic brain injury. Curr. Treat. Options Neurol. 12, 412–423.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, A. Y., Zagorski, B., Parsons, D., Van der Laan, R., Chan, V., and Colantonio, A. (2012). Factors associated with discharge destination from acute care after acquired brain injury in Ontario, Canada. BMC Neurol. 12:16. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-12-16

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cheng, J. P., Shaw, K. E., Monaco, C. M., Hoffman, A. N., Sozda, C. N., Olsen, A. S., et al. (2012). A relatively brief exposure to environmental enrichment after experimental traumatic brain injury confers long-term cognitive benefits. J. Neurotrauma 29, 2684–2688.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen, B. K., Colella, B., Inness, E., Hebert, D., Monette, G., Bayley, M., et al. (2008). Recovery of cognitive function after traumatic brain injury: a multilevel modeling analysis of Canadian outcomes. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, S3–S15.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen, H., and Mackinnon, A. (1993). The association between mental, social and physical activity and cognitive performance in young and old subjects. Age Ageing 22, 175–183.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christodoulou, C., Deluca, J., Ricker, J. H., Madigan, N. K., Bly, B. M., Lange, G., et al. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment after traumatic brain injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 71, 161–168.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Churchill, J. D., Galvez, R., Colcombe, S., Swain, R. A., Kramer, A. F., and Greenough, W. T. (2002). Exercise, experience and the aging brain. Neurobiol. Aging 23, 941–955.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cicerone, K., Mott, T., Azulay, J., and Friel, J. (2004). Community integration and satisfaction with functioning after intensive cognitive rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 943–950.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Kalmar, K., Langenbahn, D. M., Malec, J. F., Bergquist, T. F., et al. (2000). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 81, 1596–1615.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn, D. M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., et al. (2005). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 86, 1681–19692.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cifu, D. X., Kreutzer, J. S., Kolakowsky-Hayner, S. A., Marwitz, J. H., and Englander, J. (2003). The relationship between therapy intensity and rehabilitative outcomes after traumatic brain injury: a multicenter analysis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84, 1441–1448.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

C.I.H.I. (2006). Head injuries in Canada: a decade of change (1994-1995 to 2003-2004). National Trauma Regist. Anal. Brief. Available online at: http://secure.cihi.ca/cihiweb/dispPage.jsp?cw_page=bl_ntr_aug2006_e. [Accessed February 10, 2010].

Cirillo, J., Todd, G., and Semmler, J. G. (2011). Corticomotor excitability and plasticity following complex visuomotor training in young and old adults. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1847–1856.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clarkson-Smith, L., and Hartley, A. (1989). Relationships between physical exercise and cognitive abilities in older adults. Psychol. Aging 2, 183–189.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Corrigan, J. D., Whiteneck, G., and Mellick, D. (2004). Perceived needs following traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 19, 205–216.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Coulson, E. J., Reid, K., Shipham, K. M., Morley, S., Kilpatrick, T. J., and Bartlett, P. F. (2004). The role of neurotransmission and the Chopper domain in p75 neurotrophin receptor death signaling. Prog. Brain Res. 146, 41–62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crowe, M., Andel, R., Pedersen, N., Johansson, B., and Gatz, M. (2003). Does participation in leisure activities lead to a reduced risk of Alzheimer's disease? A prospective study of Swedish twins. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 58B, 249–255.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davidson, A., and Bar-Yam, Y. (2006). “Environmental complexity: information for human-environment well-being,” in Unifying Themes in Complex Systems, eds A. Bar-Yam and Y. Minai (New England; Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 157–168.

De Weerdt, W., Selz, B., Nuyens, G., Staes, F., Swinnen, D., Van De Winckel, A., et al. (2000). Time use of stroke patients in an intensive rehabilitation unit: a comparison between a Belgian and a Swiss setting. Disabil. Rehabil. 22, 181–186.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

De Wit, L., Putman, K., Dejaeger, E., Baert, I., Berman, P., Bogaerts, K., et al. (2005). Use of time by stroke patients: a comparison of four european rehabilitation centers. Stroke 36, 1977–1983.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Witt, B. W., Ehrenberg, K. M., McAloon, R. L., Panos, A. H., Shaw, K. E., Raghavan, P. V., et al. (2011). Abbreviated environmental enrichment enhances neurobehavioral recovery comparably to continuous exposure after traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 343–350.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diamond, M. (2001). Response of the brain to enrichment. An. Acad. Bras. Cienc. 73, 211–220.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dikmen, S., Reitan, R. M., and Temkin, N. R. (1983). Neuropsychological recovery in head injury. Arch. Neurol. 40, 333–338.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dikmen, S. S., Ross, B. L., Machamer, J. E., and Temkin, N. R. (1995). One year psychosocial outcome in head injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1, 67–77.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dobrossy, M. D., and Dunnett, S. B. (2001). The influence of environment and experience on neural grafts. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 871–879.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobrossy, M. D., and Dunnett, S. B. (2004). Environmental enrichment affects striatal graft morphology and functional recovery. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 159–168.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eckert, M. J., and Abraham, W. C. (2012). “Effects of environmental enrichment exposure on synaptic transmission and plasticity in the hippocampus,” in Current Topics in Behavorial Neurosciences, eds M. A. Geyer, B. A. Ellenbroek, and C. A. Marsden (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 1–23.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eriksson, G., Kottorp, A., Borg, J., and Tham, K. (2009). Relationship between occupational gaps in everyday life, depressive mood and life satisfaction after acquired brain injury. J. Rehabil. Med. 41, 187–194.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farbota, K. D., Bendlin, B. B., Alexander, A. L., Rowley, H. A., Dempsey, R. J., and Johnson, S. C. (2012). Longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates in traumatic brain injury patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:160. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00160

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fares, R. P., Belmeguenai, A., Sanchez, P. E., Kouchi, H. Y., Bodennec, J., Morales, A., et al. (2013). Standardized environmental enrichment supports enhanced brain plasticity in healthy rats and prevents cognitive impairment in epileptic rats. PLoS ONE 8:e53888. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053888

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farne, A., Buxbaum, L. J., Ferraro, M., Frassinetti, F., Whyte, J., Veramonti, T., et al. (2004). Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disorders in acute right brain-damaged patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 75, 1401–1410.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fasotti, L., Kovacs, F., Eling, P., and Brouwer, W. (2000). Time pressure management as a compensatory strategy training after closed head injury. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 10, 47–65.

Faul, M., Xu, L., Wald, M. M., and Coronado, V. G. (2010). Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States: Emergency Department Visits, Hospitalizations, and Deaths. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control.

Fratiglioni, L., Wang, H., Ericsson, K., Maytan, M., and Winblad, B. (2000). Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 355, 1315–1319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freeman, E. A. (1997). Community-based rehabilitation of the person with a severe brain injury. Brain Inj. 11, 143–153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fujiwara, Y., Chaves, P. H., Yoshida, H., Amano, H., Fukaya, T., Watanabe, N., et al. (2009). Intellectual activity and likelihood of subsequently improving or maintaining instrumental activities of daily living functioning in community-dwelling older Japanese: a longitudinal study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 547–555.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gabriel, A., Paoletti, G., Della Seta, D., Panelli, R., Marcus, M., Farabollini, F., et al. (2009a). Enriched environment and the recovery from inflammatory pain: social versus physical aspects and their interaction. Behav. Brain Res. 208, 90–95.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gabriel, A. F., Marcus, M. A., Honig, W. M., Helgers, N., and Joosten, E. A. (2009b). Environmental housing affects the duration of mechanical allodynia and the spinal astroglial activation in a rat model of chronic inflammatory pain. Brain Res. 1276, 83–90.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gan, C., Gargaro, J., Brandys, C., Gerber, G., and Boschen, K. (2010). Family caregivers' support needs after brain injury: a synthesis of perspectives from caregivers, programs, and researchers. NeuroRehabilitation 27, 5–18.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gobbo, O. L., and O'Mara, S. M. (2004). Impact of enriched-environment housing on brain-derived neurotrophic factor and on cognitive performance after a transient global ischemia. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 231–241.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Green, R. E., Colella, B., Christensen, B., Johns, K., Frasca, D., Bayley, M., et al. (2008). Examining moderators of cognitive recovery trajectories after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, S16–S24.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Green, R. E., Melo, B., Christensen, B., Ngo, L., and Skene, C. (2006). Evidence of transient enhancement to cognitive functioning in healthy young adults through environmental enrichment: implications for rehabilitation after brain injury. Brain Cogn. 60, 201–203.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Greenberg, G., Mikulis, D. J., Ng, K., Desouza, D., and Green, R. E. (2008). Use of diffusion tensor imaging to examine subacute white matter injury progression in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, S45–S50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gribbin, K., Schaie, K., and Parham, I. (1980). Complexity of life style and maintenance of intellectual abilities. J. Soc. Issues 36, 47–61.

Gunther, V. K., Schafer, P., Holzner, B. J., and Kemmler, G. W. (2003). Long-term improvements in cognitive performance through computer-assisted cognitive training: a pilot study in a residential home for older people. Aging Ment. Health 7, 200–206.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamm, R., Temple, M., O'Dell, D., Pike, B., and Lyeth, B. (1996). Exposure to environmental complexity promotes recovery of cognitive function after traumtic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 13, 41–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hanks, R. A., Rapport, L. J., Wertheimer, J., and Koviak, C. (2012). Randomized controlled trial of peer mentoring for individuals with traumatic brain injury and their significant others. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 1297–1304.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hawthorne, G., Gruen, R. L., and Kaye, A. H. (2009). Traumatic brain injury and long-term quality of life: findings from an Australian study. J. Neurotrauma 26, 1623–1633.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayden, M. E., Plenger, P., Bison, K., Kowalske, K., Masel, B., and Qualls, D. (2013). Treatment effect versus pretreatment recovery in persons with traumatic brain injury: a study regarding the effectiveness of postacute rehabilitation. PM R. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2012.12.005. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayslip, B., Maloy, R. M., and Kohl, R. (1995). Long-term efficacy of fluid ability interventions with older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 50, P141–P149.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hebb, D. O. (1947). The effects of early experience on problem solving in maturity. Am. Psychol. 2, 306–307.

Heffernan, D. S., Vera, R. M., Monaghan, S. F., Thakkar, R. K., Kozloff, M. S., Connolly, M. D., et al. (2011). Impact of socioethnic factors on outcomes following traumatic brain injury. J. Trauma 70, 527–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heinemann, A. W., Hamilton, B., Linacre, J. M., Wright, B. D., and Granger, C. (1995). Functional status and therapeutic intensity during inpatient rehabilitation. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 74, 315–326.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Himanen, L., Portin, R., Isoniemi, H., Helenius, H., Kurki, T., and Tenovuo, O. (2006). Longitudinal cognitive changes in traumatic brain injury: a 30-year follow-up study. Neurology 66, 187–192.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoffman, A., Malena, R., Westergoma, B., Luthra, P., Chenga, J., Aslama, H., et al. (2008). Environmental enrichment-mediated functional improvement after experimental traumatic brain injury is contingent on task-specific neurobehavioral experience. Neurosci. Lett. 431, 226–230.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holbrook, T. L., Anderson, J. P., Sieber, W. J., Browner, D., and Hoyt, D. B. (1999). Outcome after major trauma: 12-month and 18-month follow-up results from the Trauma Recovery Project. J. Trauma 46, 765–771. discussion: 771–763.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hoogerdijk, B., Runge, U., and Haugboelle, J. (2011). The adaptation process after traumatic brain injury an individual and ongoing occupational struggle to gain a new identity. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 18, 122–132.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hudak, A., Warner, M., Marquez De La Plata, C., Moore, C., Harper, C., and Diaz-Arrastia, R. (2011). Brain morphometry changes and depressive symptoms after traumatic brain injury. Psychiatry Res. 191, 160–165.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huppert, F. (1991). Age-Related Changes in Memory: Learning and Remembering New Information. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Johansson, B. B. (2000). Brain plasticity and stroke rehabilitation: the Willis lecture. Stroke 31, 223–230.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johansson, B. B. (2002). “Environmental effects on recovery after stroke,” in Cerebrovascular Disease, 22nd Princeton Conference, ed P. H. Chan (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 328–336.

Johansson, B. B. (2003). Environmental influence on recovery after brain lesions - experimental and clinical data. J. Rehabil. Med. 35, 11–16.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Johansson, B. B., and Ohlsson, A. L. (1996). Environment, social interaction, and physical activity as determinants of functional outcome after cerebral infarction in the rat. Exp. Neurol. 139, 322–327.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jung, C. K., and Herms, J. (2012). Structural dynamics of dendritic spines are influenced by an environmental enrichment: an in vivo imaging study. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs317. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keane, R. W., Kraydieh, S., Lotocki, G., Alonso, O. F., Aldana, P., and Dietrich, W. D. (2001). Apoptotic and antiapoptotic mechanisms after traumatic brain injury. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 21, 1189–1198.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keightley, M., Kendall, V., Jang, S. H., Parker, C., Agnihotri, S., Colantonio, A., et al. (2011). From health care to home community: an Aboriginal community-based ABI transition strategy. Brain Inj. 25, 142–152.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kempermann, M., Gast, G., and Gage, F. (2002). Neuroplasticity in old age: sustained fivefold induction of hippocampal neurogensis by long-term environmental enrichment. Ann. Neurol. 52, 135–143.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kennedy, M., Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M., Sohlberg, M., Yorkston, K., et al. (2008). Intervention for executive functions after traumatic brain injury: a systematic review, meta-analysis and clinical recommendations. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18, 257–299.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J., Whyte, J., Patel, S., Avants, B., Europa, E., Wang, J., et al. (2010). Resting cerebral blood flow alterations in chronic traumatic brain injury: an arterial spin labeling perfusion FMRI study. J. Neurotrauma 27, 1399–1411.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kleim, J. A., and Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 51, S225–S239.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klinge, C., Roder, B., and Buchel, C. (2012). Does training or deprivation modulate amygdala activation? Neuroimage 59, 1765–1771.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kobayashi, S., Ohashi, Y., and Ando, S. (2002). Effects of enriched environments with different durations and starting times on learning capacity during aging in rats assessed by a refined procedure of the Hebb-Williams maze task. J. Neurosci. Res. 70, 340–346.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kolb, B., and Whishaw, I. (1998). Brain plasticity and behaviour. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 43–64.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., and Greenough, W. T. (2004). Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 59, M940–M957.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Latham, N., and Mason, G. (2010). Frustration and perseveration in stereotypic captive animals: is a taste of enrichment worse than none at all? Behav. Brain Res. 211, 96–104.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leger, M., Quiedeville, A., Paizanis, E., Natkunarajah, S., Freret, T., Boulouard, M., et al. (2012). Environmental enrichment enhances episodic-like memory in association with a modified neuronal activation profile in adult mice. PLoS ONE 7:e48043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leon-Carrion, J., Dominguez-Morales, M. R., Barroso Y Martin, J. M., and Leon-Dominguez, U. (2012). Recovery of cognitive function during comprehensive rehabilitation after severe traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Med. 44, 505–511.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levine, B., Kovacevic, N., Nica, E. I., Cheung, G., Gao, F., Schwartz, M. L., et al. (2008). The Toronto traumatic brain injury study: injury severity and quantified MRI. Neurology 70, 771–778.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lezak, M. (2004). “The neuropsycholgical examination: procedures,” in Neuropsychological Assessment, ed M. Lezak (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 110–143.

Lippert-Gruener, M., Maegele, M., Garbe, J., and Angelov, D. N. (2007). Late effects of enriched environment (EE) plus multimodal early onset stimulation (MEOS) after traumatic brain injury in rats: ongoing improvement of neuromotor function despite sustained volume of the CNS lesion. Exp. Neurol. 203, 82–94.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lores-Arnaiz, S., Lores Arnaiz, M. R., Czerniczyniec, A., Cuello, M., and Bustamante, J. (2010). Mitochondrial function and nitric oxide production in hippocampus and cerebral cortex of rats exposed to enriched environment. Brain Res. 1319, 44–53.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundqvist, A., and Samuelsson, K. (2012). Return to work after acquired brain injury: a patient perspective. Brain Inj. 26, 13–14.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mackenzie, J. D., Siddiqi, F., Babb, J. S., Bagley, L. J., Mannon, L. J., Sinson, G. P., et al. (2002). Brain atrophy in mild or moderate traumatic brain injury: a longitudinal quantitative analysis. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23, 1509–1515.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mackinnor, A., Christensen, H., Hofer, S., Korten, A., and Jorm, A. (2003). Use it and still lose it? The association between activity and cognitive performance established using latent growth techniques in a community sample. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 10, 215–229.

Mahncke, H., Connor, B., Appelman, J., Ahsanuddin, O., Hardy, J., Wood, R., et al. (2006a). Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: a randomized, controlled study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 12523–12528.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mahncke, H. W., Bronstone, A., and Merzenich, M. M. (2006b). Brain plasticity and functional losses in the aged: scientific bases for a novel intervention. Prog. Brain Res. 157, 81–109.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mahoney, J., Eisner, J., Havighurst, T., Gray, S., and Palta, M. (2000). Problems of older adults living alone after hospitalization. J. Gen. Int. Med. 15, 611–619.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marioni, R. E., Valenzuela, M. J., Van Den Hout, A., Brayne, C., and Matthews, F. E. (2012a). Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS ONE 7:e50940. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050940

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marioni, R. E., Van Den Hout, A., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C., and Matthews, F. E. (2012b). Active cognitive lifestyle associates with cognitive recovery and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. J. Alzheimers Dis. 28, 223–230.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matter, A. M., Folweiler, K. A., Curatolo, L. M., and Kline, A. E. (2010). Temporal effects of environmental enrichment-mediated functional improvement after experimental traumatic brain injury in rats. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 558–564.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McCormack, E., and Liddiard, H. (2009). Home or away? Community rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury: a case report. Physiother. Res. Int. 14, 66–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meagher, R. K., and Mason, G. J. (2012). Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS ONE 7:e49180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milgram, N. (2003). Cognitive experience and its effect on age-dependent cognitive decline in beagle dogs. Neurochem. Res. 2, 1677–1682.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Miller, L., and Green, R. E. (in press). Can environmental enrichment serve as a protective intervention for neurodegeneration in traumatic brain injury? Front. Hum. Neurosci.

Millis, S. R., Rosenthal, M., Novack, T. A., Sherer, M., Nick, T. G., Kreutzer, J. S., et al. (2001). Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 16, 343–355.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mohammed, A. K., Winblad, B., Ebendal, T., and Larkfors, L. (1990). Environmental influence on behaviour and nerve growth factor in the brain. Brain Res. 528, 62–72.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Monaco, C. M., Mattiola, V. V., Folweiler, K. A., Tay, J. K., Yelleswarapu, N. K., Curatolo, L. M., et al. (2013). Environmental enrichment promotes robust functional and histological benefits in female rats after controlled cortical impact injury. Exp. Neurol. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.01.007. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murray, C. J., and Lopez, A. D. (1997). Alternative projections of mortality and disability by cause 1990-2020: Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet 349, 1498–1504.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nalder, E., Fleming, J., Cornwell, P., Foster, M., and Haines, T. (2012a). Factors associated with the occurrence of sentinel events during transition from hospital to home for individuals with traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Med. 44, 837–844.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nalder, E., Fleming, J., Foster, M., Cornwell, P., Shields, C., and Khan, A. (2012b). Identifying factors associated with perceived success in the transition from hospital to home after brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 27, 143–153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Narayan, R. K., Michel, M. E., Ansell, B., Baethmann, A., Biegon, A., Bracken, M. B., et al. (2002). Clinical trials in head injury. J. Neurotrauma 19, 503–557.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neely, A. S., and Backman, L. (1995). Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: generalizability across tasks and individuals. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 50, P134–P140.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Newson, R., and Kemps, E. (2005). General lifestyle activities as a predictor of current cognition and cognitive change in older adults: a cross-sectional and longitudinal examination. J. Gerontol. 60B, P113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ng, K., Mikulis, D. J., Glazer, J., Kabani, N., Till, C., Greenberg, G., et al. (2008). Magnetic resonance imaging evidence of progression of subacute brain atrophy in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, S35–S44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nilsson, M., Perfilieva, E., Johansson, U., Orwar, O., and Eriksson, P. S. (1999). Enriched environment increases neurogenesis in the adult rat dentate gyrus and improves spatial memory. J. Neurobiol. 39, 569–578.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noice, H., and Noice, T. (2004). A short term intervention to enhance cognitive and affective functioning in older adults. J. Aging Health 16, 562–585.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paillard-Borg, S., Fratiglioni, L., Xu, W., Winblad, B., and Wang, H. X. (2012). An active lifestyle postpones dementia onset by more than one year in very old adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 31, 835–842.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Park, N. W., and Ingles, J. L. (2001). Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after an acquired brain injury: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 15, 199–210.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Passineau, M., Green, E., and Dalton, D. (2001). Therapeutic effects of environmental enrichment on cognitive function and tissue integrity following severe traumatic brain injury in rats. Exp. Neurol. 168, 373–384.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pereira, L. O., Arteni, N. S., Petersen, R. C., Da Rocha, A. P., Achaval, M., and Netto, C. A. (2007). Effects of daily environmental enrichment on memory deficits and brain injury following neonatal hypoxia-ischemia in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 101–108.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pickett, W., Simpson, K., and Brison, R. J. (2004). Rates and external causes of blunt head trauma in Ontario: analysis and review of Ontario Trauma Registry datasets. Chronic Dis. Can. 25, 32–41.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pietropaolo, S., Branchi, I., Cirulli, F., Chiarotti, F., Aloe, L., and Alleva, E. (2004). Long-term effects of the periadolescent environment on exploratory activity and aggressive behaviour in mice: social versus physical enrichment. Physiol. Behav. 81, 443–453.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pignatti, F., Rozzini, R., and Trabucchi, M. (2002). Physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch. Intern. Med. 162, P361.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Powell, J., Heslin, J., and Greenwood, R. (2002). Community based rehabilitation after severe traumatic brain injury: a randomised controlled trial. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 72, 193–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pressman, H. T. (2007). Traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: case management and insurance-related issues. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. N. Am. 18, 165–174. viii.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pushkar, D., Arbuckle, T., Conway, M., Chaikelson, J., and Maag, U. (1997). Everyday activity parameters and competence in older adults. Psychol. Aging 12, 600–609.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Qiu, X., Huang, C. X., Lu, W., Yang, S., Li, C., Shi, X. Y., et al. (2012). Effects of a 4 month enriched environment on the hippocampus and the myelinated fibers in the hippocampus of middle-aged rats. Brain Res. 1465, 26–33.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Qiu, X., Li, C., Jiang, R., Chen, L., Huang, C., Yang, S., et al. (2011). The effects of short-term enriched environment on capillaries of the middle-aged rat cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 505, 186–190.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raghupathi, R. (2004). Cell death mechanisms following traumatic brain injury. Brain Pathol. 14, 215–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Raghupathi, R., Graham, D. I., and McIntosh, T. K. (2000). Apoptosis after traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 17, 927–938.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rath, J., Simon, D., Langenbahn, D., Sherr, R., and Diller, L. (2003). Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: a randomised outcome study. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 13, 461–488.

Resch, J. A., Villarreal, V., Johnson, C. L., Elliott, T. R., Kwok, O. M., Berry, J. W., et al. (2009). Trajectories of life satisfaction in the first 5 years following traumatic brain injury. Rehabil. Psychol. 54, 51–59.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rittman, M., Faircloth, C., Boylstein, C., Gubrium, J. F., Williams, C., Van Puymbroeck, M., et al. (2004). The experience of time in the transition from hospital to home following stroke. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 41, 259–268.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rosenzweig, M. (1966). Environmental complexity, cerebral change, and behavior. Am. Psychol. 21, 321–332.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rosenzweig, M., and Bennett, E. (1996). Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 78, 57–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rotondi, A. J., Sinkule, J., Balzer, K., Harris, J., and Moldovan, R. (2007). A qualitative needs assessment of persons who have experienced traumatic brain injury and their primary family caregivers. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 22, 14–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rubinov, M., McIntosh, A. R., Valenzuela, M. J., and Breakspear, M. (2009). Simulation of neuronal death and network recovery in a computational model of distributed cortical activity. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 210–217.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruff, R., Barth, J., Kreutzer, J., Levin, H., and Foulkes, M. (1991). Verbal learning deficits following severe head injury: heterogeneity in recovery over 1 year. J. Neurosurg. 75, S50–S58.

Rumrill, P. D., Fitzgerald, S. M., and Merchant, W. R. (2010). Using scoping literature reviews as a means of understanding and interpreting existing literature. Work 35, 399–404.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rusconi, S., and Turner-Stokes, L. (2003). An evaluation of aftercare following discharge from a specialist in-patient rehabilitation service. Disabil. Rehabil. 25, 1281–1288.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salmond, C. H., Menon, D. K., Chatfield, D. A., Pickard, J. D., and Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Changes over time in cognitive and structural profiles of head injury survivors. Neuropsychologia 44, 1995–1998.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salthouse, T. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 68–87.

Sampson, E. L., Bulpitt, C. J., and Fletcher, A. E. (2009). Survival of community-dwelling older people: the effect of cognitive impairment and social engagement. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 57, 985–991.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sander, A. M., Clark, A., and Pappadis, M. R. (2010). What is community integration anyway?: defining meaning following traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 25, 121–127.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sander, A. M., Pappadis, M. R., Clark, A. N., and Struchen, M. A. (2011). Perceptions of community integration in an ethnically diverse sample. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 26, 158–169.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sander, A. M., Pappadis, M. R., Davis, L. C., Clark, A. N., Evans, G., Struchen, M. A., et al. (2009). Relationship of race/ethnicity and income to community integration following traumatic brain injury: investigation in a non-rehabilitation trauma sample. NeuroRehabilitation 24, 15–27.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sander, A. M., Roebuck, T. M., Struchen, M. A., Sherer, M., and High, W. M. (2001). Long-term maintenance of gains obtained in postacute rehabilitation by persons with traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 16, 356–373.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Scarmeas, N., and Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 625–633.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schooler, C. (1987). “Psychological effects of complex environments during the life span: a review and theory,” in Cognitive Functioning and Social Structure Over the Life Course, ed C. S. K. W. Schaie (Norwood, NJ: Ablex), 24–49.

Schooler, C., and Mulatu, M. (2001). The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychol. Aging 16, 466–482.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schuit, A., Feskens, E., Launer, L., and Kromhout, D. (2001). Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of apolipoprotein e4 allele. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 33, 772–777.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Seeman, T., Lusignolo, T., Albert, M., and Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychol. 20, 243–255.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Shafi, S., Marquez De La Plata, C., Diaz-Arrastia, R., Shipman, K., Carlile, M., Frankel, H., et al. (2007). Racial disparities in long-term functional outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Trauma 63, 1263–1268. discussion: 1268–1270.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shiel, A., Burn, J. P., Henry, D., Clark, J., Wilson, B. A., Burnett, M. E., et al. (2001). The effects of increased rehabilitation therapy after brain injury: results of a prospective controlled trial. Clin. Rehabil. 15, 501–514.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shigaki, C. L., Johnstone, B., and Schopp, L. H. (2009). Financial and vocational outcomes 2 years after traumatic brain injury. Disabil. Rehabil. 31, 484–489.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shin, S. S., Bales, J. W., Yan, H. Q., Kline, A. E., Wagner, A. K., Lyons-Weiler, J., et al. (2013). The effect of environmental enrichment on substantia nigra gene expression after traumatic brain injury in rats. J. Neurotrauma 30, 259–270.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shors, T. J., Anderson, M. L., Curlik, D. M. 2nd., and Nokia, M. S. (2012). Use it or lose it: how neurogenesis keeps the brain fit for learning. Behav. Brain Res. 227, 450–458.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sidaros, A., Engberg, A. W., Sidaros, K., Liptrot, M. G., Herning, M., Petersen, P., et al. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury and relation to clinical outcome: a longitudinal study. Brain 131, 559–572.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sidaros, A., Skimminge, A., Liptrot, M. G., Sidaros, K., Engberg, A. W., Herning, M., et al. (2009). Long-term global and regional brain volume changes following severe traumatic brain injury: a longitudinal study with clinical correlates. Neuroimage 44, 1–8.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simpson, J., and Kelly, J. P. (2011). The impact of environmental enrichment in laboratory rats–behavioural and neurochemical aspects. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 246–264.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slade, A., Tennant, A., and Chamberlain, M. A. (2002). A randomised controlled trial to determine the effect of intensity of therapy upon length of stay in a neurological rehabilitation setting. J. Rehabil. Med. 34, 260–266.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

SMARTRISK. (2006). The Economic Burden of Injury. Toronto, ON: SMARTRISK.

Sohlberg, M. M., McLaughlin, K. A., Pavese, A., Heidrich, A., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Evaluation of attention process training and brain injury education in persons with acquired brain injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 656–676.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sozda, C. N., Hoffman, A. N., Olsen, A. S., Cheng, J. P., Zafonte, R. D., and Kline, A. E. (2010). Empirical comparison of typical and atypical environmental enrichment paradigms on functional and histological outcome after experimental traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 27, 1047–1057.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Speisman, R. B., Kumar, A., Rani, A., Pastoriza, J. M., Severance, J. E., Foster, T. C., et al. (2012). Environmental enrichment restores neurogenesis and rapid acquisition in aged rats. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 263–274.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spikman, J. M., Boelen, D. H., Lamberts, K. F., Brouwer, W. H., and Fasotti, L. (2009). Effects of a multifaceted treatment program for executive dysfunction after acquired brain injury on indications of executive functioning in daily life. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 118–129.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spivack, G., Spettell, C. M., Ellis, D. W., and Ross, S. E. (1992). Effects of intensity of treatment and length of stay on rehabilitation outcomes. Brain Inj. 6, 419–434.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Staudenmayer, K. L., Diaz-Arrastia, R., De Oliveira, A., Gentilello, L. M., and Shafi, S. (2007). Ethnic disparities in long-term functional outcomes after traumatic brain injury. J. Trauma 63, 1364–1369.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stine-Morrow, E., Parisi, J., Morrow, D., Greene, J., and Park, D. (2007). An engagement model of cognitive optimization through adulthood. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 62B, 62–69.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Strandberg, T. (2009). Adults with acquired traumatic brain injury: experiences of a changeover process and consequences in everyday life. Soc. Work Health Care 48, 276–297.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Struchen, M. A., Davis, L. C., Bogaards, J. A., Hudler-Hull, T., Clark, A. N., Mazzei, D. M., et al. (2011). Making connections after brain injury: development and evaluation of a social peer-mentoring program for persons with traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 26, 4–19.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sun, H., Zhang, J., Zhang, L., Liu, H., Zhu, H., and Yang, Y. (2010). Environmental enrichment influences BDNF and NR1 levels in the hippocampus and restores cognitive impairment in chronic cerebral hypoperfused rats. Curr. Neurovasc. Res. 7, 268–280.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Till, C., Colella, B., Verwegen, J., and Green, R. E. (2008). Postrecovery cognitive decline in adults with traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, S25–S34.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Toglia, J., Johnston, M. V., Goverover, Y., and Dain, B. (2010). A multicontext approach to promoting transfer of strategy use and self regulation after brain injury: an exploratory study. Brain Inj. 24, 664–677.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Toglia, J. P. (1991). Generalization of treatment: a multicontext approach to cognitive perceptual impairment in adults with brain injury. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 45, 505–516.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Trivedi, M. A., Ward, M. A., Hess, T. M., Gale, S. D., Dempsey, R. J., Rowley, H. A., et al. (2007). Longitudinal changes in global brain volume between 79 and 409 days after traumatic brain injury: relationship with duration of coma. J. Neurotrauma 24, 766–771.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tsaousides, T., Warshowsky, A., Ashman, T. A., Cantor, J. B., Spielman, L., and Gordon, W. A. (2009). The relationship between employment-related self-efficacy and quality of life following traumatic brain injury. Rehabil. Psychol. 54, 299–305.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B., Fleming, J., Cornwell, P., Worrall, L., Ownsworth, T., Haines, T., et al. (2007). A qualitative study of the transition from hospital to home for individuals with acquired brain injury and their family caregivers. Brain Inj. 21, 1119–1130.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B., Fleming, J., Cornwell, P., Haines, T., and Ownsworth, T. (2009a). Profiling early outcomes during the transition from hospital to home after brain injury. Brain Inj. 23, 51–60.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B., Ownsworth, T., Cornwell, P., and Fleming, J. (2009b). Reengagement in meaningful occupations during the transition from hospital to home for people with acquired brain injury and their family caregivers. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 63, 609–620.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B., Fleming, J., Ownsworth, T., and Cornwell, P. (2011a). Perceptions of recovery during the early transition phase from hospital to home following acquired brain injury: a journey of discovery. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 64–91.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B. J., Fleming, J., Ownsworth, T., and Cornwell, P. (2011b). Perceived service and support needs during transition from hospital to home following acquired brain injury. Disabil. Rehabil. 33, 818–829.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, B. J., Fleming, J. M., Ownsworth, T. L., and Cornwell, P. L. (2008). The transition from hospital to home for individuals with acquired brain injury: a literature review and research recommendations. Disabil. Rehabil. 30, 1153–1176.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turner, G. R., and Green, E. (2008). Cognitive remediation in aging and ABI: a question of negative plasticity? Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18, 372–384.

Turner-Stokes, L., Disler, P. B., Nair, A., and Wade, D. T. (2005). Multi-disciplinary rehabilitation for acquired brain injury in adults of working age. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 3:CD004170. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004170.pub2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Valero, J., Espana, J., Parra-Damas, A., Martin, E., Rodriguez-Alvarez, J., and Saura, C. A. (2011). Short-term environmental enrichment rescues adult neurogenesis and memory deficits in APP(Sw, Ind) transgenic mice. PLoS ONE 6:e16832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016832

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van De Winckel, A., Feys, H., De Weerdt, W., and Dom, R. (2004). Cognitive and behavioural effects of music-based exercises in patients with dementia. Clin. Rehabil. 18, 253–260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., and Gage, F. (2000). Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Neuroscience 1, 191–198.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vance, D., and Johns, R. (2002). Montessori improved cognitive domains in adults with Alzheimer's Disease. Phys. Occup. Ther. Geriatr. 20, 19–36.

Vazquez-Sanroman, D., Sanchis-Segura, C., Toledo, R., Hernandez, M. E., Manzo, J., and Miquel, M. (2013). The effects of enriched environment on BDNF expression in the mouse cerebellum depending on the length of exposure. Behav. Brain Res. 243C, 118–128.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Verghese, J., Lipton, R., Katz, M., Hall, C., Derby, C., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N. Engl. J. Med. 348, 2508–2616.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voss, M. W., Nagamatsu, L. S., Liu-Ambrose, T., and Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J. Appl. Physiol. 111, 1505–1513.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, H., Karp, A., Wonblad, B., and Fratiglioni, L. (2002). Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. Am. J. Epidemiol. 155, 1081–1087.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Warner, M. A., Marquez De La Plata, C., Spence, J., Wang, J. Y., Harper, C., Moore, C., et al. (2010). Assessing spatial relationships between axonal integrity, regional brain volumes, and neuropsychological outcomes after traumatic axonal injury. J. Neurotrauma 27, 2121–2130.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Warraich, Z., and Kleim, J. A. (2010). Neural plasticity: the biological substrate for neurorehabilitation. PM R 2, S208–S219.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

West, R. W., and Greenough, W. T. (1972). Effect of environmental complexity on cortical synapses of rats: preliminary results. Behav. Biol. 7, 279–284.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Will, B., Galani, R., Kelche, C., and Rosenzweig, M. R. (2004). Recovery from brain injury in animals: relative efficacy of environmental enrichment, physical exercise or formal training (1990-2002). Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 167–182.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Willer, B., Button, J., and Rempel, R. (1999). Residential and home-based postacute rehabilitation of individuals with traumatic brain injury: a case control study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80, 399–406.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Williamson, L. L., Chao, A., and Bilbo, S. D. (2012). Environmental enrichment alters glial antigen expression and neuroimmune function in the adult rat hippocampus. Brain Behav. Immun. 26, 500–510.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, J. T., Wiedmann, K. D., Hadley, D. M., Condon, B., Teasdale, G., and Brooks, D. N. (1988). Early and late magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological outcome after head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 51, 391–396.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, R., Barnes, L., and Bennett, D. (2003). Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 634–642.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, R. S., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., et al. (2002). Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 287, 742–748.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winocur, G., and Moscovitch, M. (1990). A comparison of cognitive function in community-dwelling and institutionalized old people of normal intelligence. Can. J. Psychol. 44, 435–444.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M., and Freedman, J. (1987). An investigation of cognitive function in relation to psychosocial variables in institutionalized old people. Can. J. Psychol. 41, 257–269.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Winocur, G. (1998). Environmental influences on cognitive decline in aged rats. Neurobiol. Aging 19, 589–597.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Xu, L., Jiang, C. Q., Lam, T. H., Zhang, W. S., Thomas, G. N., and Cheng, K. K. (2011). Dose-response relation between physical activity and cognitive function: guangzhou biobank cohort study. Ann. Epidemiol. 21, 857–863.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, S., Li, C., Qiu, X., Zhang, L., Lu, W., Chen, L., et al. (2013). Effects of an enriched environment on myelin sheaths in the white matter of rats during normal aging: a stereological study. Neuroscience 234C, 13–21.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhu, X. L., Poon, W. S., Chan, C. H., and Chan, S. H. (2001). Does intensive rehabilitation improve the functional outcome of patients with traumatic brain injury? Interim result of a randomized controlled trial. Br. J. Neurosurg. 15, 464–473.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zhu, X. L., Poon, W. S., Chan, C. C., and Chan, S. S. (2007). Does intensive rehabilitation improve the functional outcome of patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI)? A randomized controlled trial. Brain Inj. 21, 681–690.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: traumatic brain injury, environmental enrichment, post-acute decline, adult, moderate to severe, post-discharge, transition home

Citation: Frasca D, Tomaszczyk J, McFadyen BJ and Green RE (2013) Traumatic brain injury and post-acute decline: what role does environmental enrichment play? A scoping review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:31. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00031

Received: 01 March 2012; Accepted: 25 January 2013;
Published online: 17 April 2013.

Edited by:

Hauke R. Heekeren, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Barbara B. Bendlin, Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, USA
Lisa A. Brenner, University of Colorado Denver, USA

Copyright © 2013 Frasca, Tomaszczyk, McFadyen and Green. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Diana Frasca, Cognitive Neurorehabilitation Sciences Laboratory, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, 550 University Ave, Room 11-207, Toronto, ON M5G 2A2, Canada. e-mail: diane.frasca@uhn.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.