Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. For. Glob. Change, 21 February 2024
Sec. People and Forests

Forests serve vulnerable groups in times of crises: improved mental health of older adults by individual forest walking during the COVID-19 pandemic

Kiki Ekiawan LamatunggaKiki Ekiawan Lamatungga1Magdalna PichlerovMagdaléna Pichlerová2Júlia Halamov&#x;Júlia Halamová3Martin KanovskýMartin Kanovský4Dhanalakshmi TamatamDhanalakshmi Tamatam1Daniela JeovDaniela Ježová5Viliam Pichler
Viliam Pichler1*
  • 1Department of Natural Environment, Faculty of Forestry, Technical University in Zvolen, Zvolen, Slovakia
  • 2Department of Landscape Planning and Design, Faculty of Ecology and Environmental Science, Technical University in Zvolen, Zvolen, Slovakia
  • 3Faculty of Social and Economic Sciences, Institute of Applied Psychology, Comenius University in Bratislava, Bratislava, Slovakia
  • 4Faculty of Social and Economic Sciences, Institute of Social Anthropology, Comenius University in Bratislava, Bratislava, Slovakia
  • 5Institute of Experimental Endocrinology, Biomedical Research Center, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia

Understanding the effects of environment on the mental health of older adults is crucial in an aging society. Previous research concerning restorative benefits of forests almost entirely omitted older adults as the primary target group and typically involved group forest visits, which were largely restricted during the COVID-19 pandemic. Here we investigated the effects of individual walks on the mental health of adults aged 60 years or older (N = 54). A randomized parallel intervention study was conducted with one group walking in forests and the other in built-up city centres. Each participant completed eight individual 40-min walks during 1 month. Significant improvements in cognitive flexibility and heart rate variability (HRV) as an autonomic nervous system functioning indicator were established in the forest-walking group. The relationship between HRV and environment was modulated by ambient temperature. The study shows that access to forests during crises can support mental health of older adults as a vulnerable demographic.

1 Introduction

Exposure to nature and forests offers manifold restorative benefits for human mental health, including stress reduction, improved cognitive function, alleviation of depression, enhanced mood, and positive emotions (Hartig et al., 1996; Karjalainen et al., 2010; Jarosz, 2022; Pichlerová et al., 2023). Both health policymaking and forest management planning are expected to reflect the expanding knowledge about forest benefits for human health as one of the most important services provided by ecosystems (Vining and Tyler, 1999; Levy et al., 2012). To achieve this, more information on woodland experience and the exposure frequency and duration is needed to maximize benefits from time spent in forests and the management of forests according to people’s specific needs (Milcu et al., 2013; Doimo et al., 2020). For example, the demand for additional ways of coping with health problems, e.g., nature-based health interventions in this study, is also driven by the issues with access to healthcare and the rising costs of medical treatments of older adults (De Beurs et al., 1999; Rowan et al., 2013). Currently representing 10% of the global population, this demographic is projected to increase to 16% over the next 30 years (United Nations, 2022). This trend creates complex predicaments due to the high prevalence of mental disorders in older adults (World Health Organization, 2017). While older people can withstand stress better than younger people owing to, e.g., life experience and quality of relationships (Vahia et al., 2020), the declines in cognitive functioning begin as early as the mid-40s (Hughes et al., 2018). The importance of investigating the effects of forests on mental health was highlighted by the COVID-19 pandemic that saw an overall increase in the number of nature and forest visits, except in older adults who constituted the most disadvantaged population segment due to restrictive measures (Pichlerová et al., 2021).

The theoretical framework encompassing forest ecosystem services (FES), public health, healthy aging, and environmental psychology links the beneficial forest features shaped by nature or forest management with attention restoration (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989) and stress reduction (Ulrich, 1983; Ulrich et al., 1991). The relationship may be enhanced by relational restoration through supportive exchanges between people (Hartig, 2021), especially in older adults who often suffer from social isolation (Donovan and Blazer, 2020). Because nature’s effects on health and well-being result from multiple additive nature–health pathways (Kuo, 2015), it is crucial that studies provide descriptions of forest environments to pinpoint and promote the beneficial forest features through practical forest land management (Yamada, 2006; Meneguzzo and Zabini, 2021; Clark et al., 2023). Moreover, the relationship between restoration and the exposure to nature and forests can be conditioned or modulated by various factors inherent to natural environments, including bad weather or potentially dangerous wildlife that may trigger anxiety or stress (Ulrich, 1983; Grassini et al., 2016). An individual’s level of nature exposure can be assessed by the time spent in direct contact with nature and the amount of residential greenspace (Keniger et al., 2013), commonly assessed by the normalized difference vegetation index (NDVI), tree canopy coverage, and proximity to forests and parks.

Typical outcome measures for mental health characteristics used in nature intervention studies rely on instruments validated in many settings, such as Trail Making Test (Arbuthnott and Frank, 2000), combined with measures of autonomic nervous system function, for example heart rate (HR) and HR variability (HRV) (Kobayashi et al., 2018). TMT has two parts: TMT-A that accounts for rote memory, and TMT-B as an executive functioning index (Ciolek and Lee, 2020). It is scored by the time needed to draw a line connecting consecutive numbers (TMT-A), and numbers and letters in an alternating progressive sequence (TMT-B) (McMorris, 2016). The difference between TMT-B and TMT-A (TMT-B – A) is considered cognitive flexibility measure (Corrigan and Hinkeldey, 1987). HRV has been widely used as an indicator of autonomic nervous system functioning, especially stress (D’Angelo et al., 2023). It can be expressed by several parasympathetic tone indexes, e.g., the normalized root mean square of the successive difference of normal inter-beat intervals (RMSSD) (Daffre et al., 2022). RMSSD has been identified as a reliable metric of vagal activity (Cosmo et al., 2022) that tends to decrease with increased stress level and vice versa (Pietilä et al., 2018; Skov et al., 2022). HRV may be affected through medicaments used for hypertension treatment, especially β-blockers (Schroeder et al., 2003). Consumer wrist-worn fitness tracking devices with photoplethysmography (PPG) sensors have been increasingly used to measure HRV in outdoor environment research (Evenson and Spade, 2020; Natarajan et al., 2020). Binary classification of HRV changes (e.g., HRV increase detected versus not) is sometimes used in ecological momentary assessment research (Schwerdtfeger and Rominger, 2021).

Despite previous research on the health benefits of forest walking and viewing (Lanki et al., 2017; Stigsdotter et al., 2017; de Brito et al., 2020), there is a need for more studies in this area (Piva et al., 2022). A recent scoping review of the research on forest features and mental health or wellbeing (Clark et al., 2023) did not identify studies with a specific focus on elderly people as a vulnerable population segment. In addition to that, numerous interventions were implemented in groups, thus implying social interaction. The presented study aimed to address the knowledge gap through evaluating the effects of individual, one-month intervention on the autonomic nervous system and cognitive functions specifically in older adults (60+). Our goal was to determine and compare the restoration effects of eight one-hour walking and viewing visits to forest and urban environments, evenly distributed during 1 month with a frequency of two walks per week. The working hypotheses were: (1) Both forest and urban interventions reduce stress and improve cognitive function. (2) Forest walks reduce stress and improve cognitive flexibility more than urban walks. (3) Intervention effect size on cognitive function is greater than on stress reduction. (4) Interrelationship between stress reduction and environment is modulated by ambient temperature.

2 Materials and methods

The study was performed from the 1st to the 31st of October 2021, during a period without heatwaves or sub-zero daily temperatures. The selected time period was between two COVID-19 pandemic waves when the pandemic-related measures were eased, but social interaction had to be kept at a minimum.

2.1 Study participants

The study was conducted on a sample consisting of 54 older adults (60 years of age and older) living in the district of Zvolen, a town located in one of the urbanized and industrialized Slovak regions, with a population of 43 thousand citizens. The participants were recruited in collaboration with the municipal social and older adult welfare authorities from June to August 2021. To be enrolled in the study, participants had to meet the inclusion criteria: (1) age of 60+, (2) vaccinated from COVID-19, (3) being able to walk independently, and (4) willing to participate in a one-month intervention. The exclusion criteria were: (5) staying in institutional care, (6) prescribed antidepressant drugs. Seventy-four older adults took part on the orientation day, and 54 of them met the criteria. Study participants were randomly assigned into the forest (intervention) and urban (active control) groups with a ratio of 1:1 through the web-based research randomizer.1 Most of the participants characteristics (age, BMI, marital status, education, tobacco/smoking status, number of chronic illnesses, occupation, household income) were relatively similar in both groups. There were 5 and 9 participants either on β-blockers or on undeclared medication for hypertension treatment in the forest and urban groups, respectively. The remaining subjects declared either non-β-blocker medications or no prescriptions for hypertension. Among environmental features (tree canopy coverage, distance to parks and forests), the residential areas of the urban group members were characterized by higher NDVI (p < 0.01). The detailed characteristics of 54 participants (randomly assigned to the forest and urban groups) and their residential environments are shown in Supplementary Table S1; Supplementary Figure S1. An administrator with professional experience in communication was appointed to maintain telephone contact with the participants and provide organizational instructions. Eight field assistants provided guidance to the participants in the intervention localities and collected their HRV data.

2.2 Study design and procedure

The study was based on a randomized, parallel intervention design (Figure 1). Before starting the intervention, all subjects signed the informed consent form. Blinding from the specific research questions was conducted for participants and the research assistants. Four to 2 days prior to the intervention, the baseline data were collected and measurements were taken. The participants filled out the baseline questionnaire and afterward, research assistants administered TMT to each individual. Finally, the participants were provided with and instructed to use Garmin Venu Sq, consumer wrist-worn fitness tracking devices using photoplethysmography sensors, for 24 h a day during the whole intervention period.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Design and research timeline of the randomized parallel intervention.

Participants were asked to complete two interventions per week, i.e., a total of eight visits to intervention sites during 4 weeks. The intervention times were randomly assigned to each participant. The intervention schedule was provided to each participant at the beginning of each week via telephone call. One day before each intervention, the concerned participant was reminded by phone to be ready for individual transport to one of the research sites. After being picked up from their home address by a dedicated driver and arriving individually at the assigned intervention site, each participant was met by a site assistant who repeated the walking instructions. The use of cell phones, eating, and casual conversation were not allowed. Drinking water was permitted. The assistants navigated the participants by walking approximately 10 m in front of them without interaction during the intervention lasting approximately 40 min, of which 25–30 min were spent walking at a pace of about 3 km per hour, and around 10 min viewing the environment from a portable chair or bench. The assistants initiated HRV measurement every pre-post walk and recorded the values or any observations regarding participants’ feelings, e.g., anxiety. At the end, the participants were driven back to their homes. Each day, 15 to 18 participants were individually mobilized to the sites from 9:00 a.m. until 4:00 p.m. The wearable devices were collected after 2 weeks for data download and cleaning. The intervention was thereby divided into two 14-day sub-periods that facilitated further statistical analyses. The TMT test was administered again at the end of the whole intervention period (endline).

2.3 Intervention sites

There were three urban and three forest intervention localities, and each participant completed a minimum of two interventions in each of them according to their assignment to the urban or forest group. Multiple locations were selected to avoid boredom and fatigue. At the end, the aggregated means of the separate effects of the forest and urban environments were used. The localities were selected based on topographic data and field reconnaissance to ensure comfortable walkability on relatively even surfaces (trails, pavement) with a max. 3° inclination to avoid physical exertion and acceptable driving times from participants’ addresses, similar for both urban and forest localities and groups (5–25 min). The study was conducted in the Central Slovakia. For urban sites, two major towns (Banská Bystrica, Zvolen) were selected. Two main walking spots were established in Zvolen and one in Banská Bystrica (Figure 2). The forest sites featured species and structural diversity: Mláčik (48.664°N, 19.026°E) – a non-managed, mixed fir-beech-spruce old-growth forest; Stráže (48.579° N, 19.095°E) – a beech-oak-hornbeam forest subject to shelterwood management; and Sliač (48.611°N, 19.160°E) – a mixed pine-hornbeam-spruce forest surrounding a spa complex (Figure 3). The walking pathways in each site were around 1 km in length. The average air temperatures for October 2021 were obtained from the main weather station of the Slovak Hydrometeorological Institute, Bratislava, in Sliač, and several nearby stations operated by the National Forestry Centre, Zvolen, and the Technical University in Zvolen. The air temperatures at the five intervention localities were 12.1°C (Sliač), 11.0°C (Stráže), 9.5°C (Mláčik), 12.0°C (Banská Bystrica), and 12.5°C (Zvolen). The average minimum temperature between 9:00 a.m. and 4:00 p.m. in Sliač declined from 9.2°C during the first period (walks 1–4) to 6.4°C during the second period (walks 5–8) of the intervention.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Walking routes in the urban sites: (A) Zvolen; (B) Banská Bystrica.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The interiors of the three forest intervention localities: (A) Mláčik, a fir-beech-spruce old-growth forest; (B) Stráže, a beech-hornbeam forest; (C) Sliač, a mixed pine-hornbeam-spruce forest. The pictures were taken in October 2021, when the experiment was conducted.

2.4 Measured variables

The main outcome variables in the presented study were cognitive flexibility and HRV.

2.4.1 Cognitive function

The Trail Making Test (parts TMT-A and TMT-B) was administered to assess cognitive performance of older adults before and after the intervention. We used the validated Slovak TMT version that was earlier used to establish normative data for the adult Slovak population (Málišová et al., 2021). The cutoff time of 300 s was applied to discontinue the test administration (Bowie and Harvey, 2006; National Institute of Health, 2013) and the respective cases were not used in further analysis. The TMT-B – A values were calculated as differences between TMT-B and TMT-A scores, both at baseline and endline. Similar to numerous other cognitive function tests, the scores are affected by the repeated administration up to 1 year after the initial exposure (Basso et al., 1999).

2.4.2 Heart rate variability

The HRV was measured through Garmin Venu Sq, a consumer wrist-worn tracking device equipped with a PPG sensor (Garmin Elevate; Garmin, 2020), during 3 min windows before and after each of the eight interventions. It was measured at rest 5 min after the arrival at the sites and 5 min after the slow walking. The HRV (RMSSD) was extracted through Test HRV, v. 1.3.1, a custom Garmin Connect IQ application (Retty, 2022). The post-to pre-walk HRV difference (∆ HRV) was analyzed as an interval variable and a nominal variable in the binarized form (∆ HRV > 0, ∆ HRV ≤ 0). HRV values from walks during which a participant reported strong anxiety or acute stress due to biophobia were not included in the analysis. The main analysis of HRV change scores only included respondents who declared either non-β-blocker medications or no prescriptions for hypertension.

2.5 Statistical analyses

A total of 54 participants (27 in each group) entered the two-treatment parallel design study, which included individual leisure walking and viewing in forest and urban environments. Baseline characteristics were assessed using the chi-square test for categorical variables and the Student’s t-test for continuous variables. The Shapiro–Wilk test (p < 0.01) was applied to both the TMT and HRV datasets. Overall, the normal distribution assumption was not met in 2 out of 6 HRV data groups. Possible outliers in the normally distributed data were identified through the Dixon’s test at the significance level of p < 0.01. In the non-normally distributed data, outliers were flagged through the Tukey’s fences procedure, as values lying below and above the first and third quartiles, respectively, by a distance of 3 times the interquartile range or more. When detected, the most distant outlier was removed from each concerned group. One outlier (1.9% of the sample size) identified in the urban group was removed from the TMT analysis. Before the HRV analysis of the first intervention period, one outlier (1.2% of the sample) was removed from the forest group. In the dataset for the entire period, one outlier representing 0.6% of the respective sample was also excluded from the forest group sample. This approach was taken because our experiment was conducted in real-world forests characterised by potential, uncontrolled for events with unknown probability. Furthermore, a potential intervention routine disturbance, arising from technical issues like a misunderstood time or place for participant pick-up, could significantly impact momentary HRV data.

Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to evaluate interaction effects of time as the within-subject factor (baseline to endline) and environment as the between-subjects factor (forest vs. urban) on TMT-A, TMT-B, and TMT-B – A scores. The homogeneity of variance assumption in the repeated measures ANOVA was met according to the Levene’s test (p > 0.10). With only two levels for each factor, the sphericity test was not applicable. In our specific case with two factors and two levels, the repeated measures ANOVA approach can also be looked at as the comparison of the difference or change scores between forest and urban groups (Winer, 1971; Izakova et al., 2020). When an interaction effect on TMT scores emerged as significant, we examined the simple effects of each independent variable (baseline to endline, forest versus urban) on the dependent variable (TMT) at individual levels of the other independent variable. The F-tests with Bonferroni-adjusted p-values were utilized to compare baseline to endline scores within each environment and to evaluate forest and urban scores separately at baseline and endline. Otherwise, only main effects were reported.

The post-and pre-walk HRV change (∆ HRV) scores in the forest and the urban groups were compared through the independent t-test during the first, the second, and the entire periods. Given that t-test was found to be sufficiently robust against deviations from non-normal distribution on samples >100 or even smaller (Lumley et al., 2002), it was also applied on the non-normally distributed samples (85 ≤ N ≤ 168). The simple effects were determined when a respective change score was found significant. Specifically, differences between pre-and post-walk HRV values in the two environments, as well as the differences between the environments prior to and after walking, were separately appraised through paired and independent t-tests. Their results were screened through the Bonferroni–Holm correction (Cramer et al., 2016). Moderation analysis relying on the Johnson-Neyman technique (Lin, 2020) was employed to establish whether air temperature modulated the relationship between ∆ HRV and the environment, as well as the modulation region of significance. The assessment of binarized ∆ HRV changes (∆ HRV > 0 vs. ∆ HRV ≤ 0) expressed as proportions of positive ∆ HRV cases relative to all measurements (N) in a given cell was conducted using chi-square contingency table.

The respective effect sizes were expressed as partial eta-squared (η2 p ) in the repeated measures ANOVA, Cohen’s d for t-tests, and phi (φ) for the chi-square contingency test. The results were considered statistically significant when p < 0.05. All analyses were performed with SPSS statistical software (version 28; IBM SPSS Statistics, Armonk, NY, United States), except the Dixon’s test implementation in ControlFreak (Contchart Software, Sechelt, Canada).

3 Results

The results encompass the evaluation of cognitive function in older adults, focusing on three components: rote memory (TMT-A), executive functioning index (TMT-B), and cognitive flexibility (TMT-B – A). Additionally, the analysis of autonomic nervous system functioning is presented in terms of HRV change (∆ HRV) scores, binarized ∆ HRV, and the modulation of the relationship between ∆ HRV and environment by ambient temperature.

3.1 Cognitive function

This study primarily aimed to examine the interaction effect of time and environment on the cognitive function, i.e., to establish whether the baseline to endline TMT change scores differ between the two environments, i.e., treatment (forest walking) and active control (urban walking). The results of the two-way repeated measures ANOVA of the effects of time (within subject: baseline to endline), environment (between subjects: forest versus urban), and their interaction (time*environment) on the TMT-A, TMT-B, and TMT-B – A scores are presented in Table 1; Figure 4. Time (baseline to endline) had a large effect on all TMT scores, environment showed a strong influence on TMT-A, and only TMT-B – A was affected by the interaction of the two factors with a medium effect size (η2 p  = 0.09).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA of the effects of time (within subject: baseline to endline), environment (between subjects: forests versus urban), and the interaction of the two factors (time ⨯ environment) on the TMT-A, TMT-B, and TMT-B – A scores.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Baseline to endline average scores for TMT-A; (B) Baseline to endline average scores for TMT-B; (C) Baseline to endline average scores for TM-B – A. The error bars indicate the 95% CI of the marginal means.

3.1.1 The main effects of time and environment

The significant main effects of time on TMT-A and TMT-B are shown in Table 2. The baseline to endline decrease of TMT-A and TMT-B values was to a large extent attributable to the repeated test administration after 4 weeks, and environment had no effect on the corresponding change scores. However, the average TMT A values (i.e., irrespective of baseline and endline) in the forest group were significantly higher than those in the urban group (Table 2; Figure 4A).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The F-tests of the main effects of time and environment on TMT scores.

3.1.2 The simple effects of time and environment

While the main effect of time on TMT-B – A score was found significant, F1,46 = 5.70, p = 0.021, η2 p  = 0.11, it was qualified by a significant interaction between time and environment, F1,46 = 4.30, p = 0.044, η2 p  = 0.09 (Table 1). The Bonferroni-adjusted comparisons in Table 3 indicated that forest walking group participants had their TMT-B – A score 19.05 ms lower at endline than at baseline (p = 0.004). Concurrently, there was no significant difference between the baseline and endline TMT-B – A scores in the urban group members. Taken together, the repeated measures ANOVA followed by the analysis of the simple effects on TMT-B – A documented that forest walking intervention had a significant positive effect on the cognitive flexibility.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The F-tests of the simple effects of the within group and between groups factors on TMT-B – A within each level of the other effects.

3.2 Heart rate variability

The HRV analysis examined whether environment had an effect on the pre-to post walk HRV changes related to the autonomic nervous system functioning. Subsequently, Johnson-Neyman technique was applied to explore the modulation of the relationship between ∆ HRV and environment by ambiental temperature. Finally, binarized HRV changes were used to establish whether the intervention affected the majority of participants.

3.2.1 HRV change scores

Independent t-tests showed that a difference between pre-to post-walk HRV change (∆ HRV) scores existed only during the first half of the intervention, i.e., during the first 2 weeks (Table 4). Specifically, forest (treatment) group exhibited a higher pre-to post-walk HRV change score compared to the urban (active control) group (p = 0.047), with a small to medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.32). The subsequent analysis of the simple effects involving the Bonferroni–Holm correction (Table 5) showed a significant pre-to post-walk HRV increase in the forest group (p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.37), and, at the same time, a higher average pre-walk HRV in the urban group (p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.50), resulting in the positive ∆ HRV difference score in the forest group compared to the urban group. There was no significant difference between pre-to post-walk HRV change scores for the two environments measured in participants on β-blockers or with unspecified medication for hypertension treatment (p > 0.60).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Independent t-test of the pre-to post-walk HRV change (∆ HRV) scores between the forest and urban groups.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Paired and independent t-tests of the simple effects of the within subject (pre-to post-walk) and between subjects (forest versus urban) factors on the heart rate variability (HRV) within each level of the other effect during the first period.

3.2.2 Modulation by air temperature

The breakdown of the relationship between ∆ HRV and environment in the second sub-period (Table 4) suggests a potential modulation through an ambient factor, specifically temperature, which decreased by 2.8°C between the first and second periods. The modulating effect of temperature, interacting with the environment, was identified using the Johnson-Neyman technique [R2 = 0.05, b = 2.56, CI (0.35, 4.77), t = 2.33, tcrit = 2.03, p = 0.024]. This indicates that, after adjusting for the interaction, the average ∆ HRV in the forest group was 2.56 ms higher than in the urban group. The interaction plot in Figure 5A illustrates that as temperature decreased, ∆ HRV decreased in the forest settings and increased in the urban settings. However, statistical significance for this modulation was only observed within the Johnson-Neyman region of significance, approximately below 9.9°C (p = 0.05), as depicted in Figure 5B. Notably, the partial overlap between the interaction’s region of significance and the decline in average 9:00 a.m. to 4:00 p.m. air temperature—from 9.2°C in the first period to 6.4°C in the second period—contributed to the loss of ∆ HRV score differences between the two environments during the second sub-period.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Influence of the interaction between environment and temperature on HRV (RMSSD) change; (B) Johnson-Neyman plot for the effect of environment on HRV (RMSSD) change modulated by temperature within the region of significance.

3.2.3 Binarized ∆ HRV changes

The full chi-square contingency table (Table 6) and the proportions of binarized ∆ HRV (∆ HRV > 0 vs. N) according to environment (Table 7) mirror the results from the analysis of ∆ HRV as an interval variable (Table 4). The chi-square test of the difference in proportions, with time as the layering variable, showed a directional relationship between the binarized ∆ HRV and environment, as well as a medium effect size during the first sub-period, χ2 (1) = 6.22, p = 0.013, φ = 0.20. In other words, the number of cases with increased HRV relative to N tended to be higher in the forest group than in the urban group. Importantly, these results indicate that the intervention and modulation through air temperature had an effect on the majority of participants in the two groups.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Chi-square contingency table with the numbers of cases of increased and decreased HRV (∆ HRV > 0, ∆ HRV ≤ 0, respectively) in the forest and urban groups during the intervention sub-periods (walks 1–4 and 5–8) and for the whole intervention period (walks 1–8).

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. The chi-square test of the proportions of cases with positive pre-to post-walk heart rate variability change (∆ HRV > 0) between the forest and the urban groups for the first period (walks 1–4), the second period (walks 5–8), and for the entire intervention period (walks 1–8).

3.3 External influences

The study was implemented smoothly according to the protocol. Only two participants dropped out during the intervention period and two external disturbances were observed during the entire 4-week intervention. There was moderate but persistent rain in both forest and urban locations for two consecutive days. During the second week, one participant reported a sighting of a brown bear (Ursus arctos) approximately 1 km from one of the forest intervention sites while being transported home after forest walk. The sighting was confirmed by the driver on duty. Potential influence of the uncontrolled-for incident on HRV is briefly explored in the Discussion (section 4.2 Heart rate variability).

4 Discussion

The obtained results allow for revisiting the working hypotheses: (1) The first hypothesis that both forest and urban interventions reduce stress and improve cognitive function was contradicted by the lack of association between improved cognitive function and the urban walking. (2) In contrast, forest walks were associated not only with sizeable improvement in cognitive flexibility, but also with greater HRV gain scores in the forest walking group (treatment) than those in the urban walking group (active control) in the first intervention period (p = 0.047). These findings lend support to the second hypothesis postulating that forest walks reduce stress and improve cognitive function more than urban walks. (3) The results partially align with the third hypothesis suggesting that the intervention effect size on the cognitive function is greater than on HRV change. While it was true for the forest intervention, whose positive effect on the cognitive function and HRV change, relative to the urban intervention, was medium (η2 p  = 0.09) and small to medium (Cohen’s d = 0.32), respectively, the urban intervention failed to produce significant effect on the cognitive function. (4) Finally, the fourth hypothesis asserting the role of ambient temperature as the modulator of the relationship between HRV change and environment was upheld by the established modulation of that relationship by air temperature below approximately 10°C (p = 024). Overall, all four working hypotheses were productive as their pursuit revealed the necessity to examine different intervention environments and to consider specific intervention goals, target groups, and potential modulators.

4.1 Cognitive function

The obtained results revealed a significant improvement in the executive function and cognitive flexibility (TMT-B – A) among participants who engaged in frequent individual forest walks, in comparison to those who walked in urban environments. The medium-size effect on the cognitive flexibility manifested in the significantly greater baseline to endline TMT-B – A difference score in the forest group compared to the urban group. These findings suggest that the forest intervention considerably enhanced the ability to switch between different mental sets, tasks, or strategies that constitute cognitive flexibility, as defined by Diamond (2013) and Miyake and Friedman (2012). Recent systematic reviews documented partial evidence for improved cognitive flexibility after exposure to natural environments (Ohly et al., 2016; Stevenson et al., 2018) coming almost entirely from studies with university students and general population but not with older adults. As an exception, Setti et al. (2017) and Cassarino et al. (2019) found absence of cognitive restoration in older adults exposed to either natural or urban scenes on pictures. Our study, which exclusively focused on older adults, showed that the markedly improved cognitive flexibility was associated with forest walking in the presence of elements characterised by high fractal dimension, such as old trees (Seidel et al., 2019) in all three forest intervention sites. Specifically, higher fractal complexity of nature scenes supports higher perceptual fluency (Franěk et al., 2019) and in turn, higher perceptual speed contributes to better cognitive flexibility (Cepeda et al., 2001). Relatedly, Davis et al. (2023) established that navigational and switching abilities demonstrated by highly mobile Bolivian first nation forager-farmers remained without significant decline at mid-life through older age (40–70 years old), owing to their exposure to complex forest environments.

4.2 Heart rate variability

The significantly larger pre-to post-walk HRV difference score in the forest group compared to the urban group, also when represented in terms of the proportion of cases with ∆ HRV > 0 relative to the total number of cases, was observed in the forest group compared to the urban group during the first 2 weeks of the intervention (walks 1–4), and when adjusted for the interaction between environment and ambient temperature. A loss of ∆ HRV differences between the forest and urban environments was detected in the second sub-period, resulting from the interaction between environment and air temperature and its effect on ∆ HRV, revealed by the Johnson-Neyman procedure. As the average temperature dropped from 9.2°C during the first period to 6.4°C during the second intervention period, ∆ HRV decreased under the forest canopy but increased in open urban settings HRV (RMSSD) values can be expected to decline in non-thermally comfortable environments (Nkurikiyeyezu et al., 2017). Concurrently, it is possible that an adaptive change occurred after approximately four forest walks as a positive intervention outcome in the forest group members. The lack of significant difference between HRV change scores of the forest and urban subgroups, which comprise participants on β-blockers or with undeclared medication for hypertension treatment, may be partly caused by the tendency of β-blockers to increase total background HRV variability (Zhang et al., 2013).

The individual effect size of forest walking on ∆ HRV was small to medium, but even minor individual-level effects can translate to large population health effects (Matthay et al., 2021). Some other studies detected no HRV differences between intervention outcomes from green areas or forest environments and those from built-up or industrial urban areas (Brown et al., 2014; Stigsdotter et al., 2017). However, they involved university students, office workers, and shorter (approx. 20 min) exposures to distinct environments. Martinez et al. (2022), who, in agreement with the present study, also reported a significant but small relationship between perceived stress and HRV in IT workers, suggested that the association strength diminishes in real-life settings in the absence of specific and isolated stressors. This type of situation prevailed by design in our experiment before the bear sighting as an uncontrolled-for event. We can only speculate that the incident could have an impact on ∆ HRV and the proportion of cases with positive HRV change via anxiety and the demand on sustained attention in the forest group members who eventually learnt about it by word of mouth. The demand on sustained attention was shown to have an overriding effect on HRV decrement (Luque-Casado et al., 2016).

4.3 Interdisciplinary novelty

By linking the FES framework with the attention restoration and stress reduction theories, the presented study filled the existing knowledge gap by providing evidence for a meaningful effect of the exposure to forests on the cognitive function and stress level in older adults. Thus far, positive effects of forest walks on some cognitive outcomes and the state of anxiety were observed in studies involving younger people and students (Shin et al., 2011; de Brito et al., 2020). In addition, connecting the biophysical foundation of FES with stress reduction theory elucidated the role of air temperature as the modulator of the relationship between HRV and environment. Our results indicate that the association between perceived stress reduction and forest settings with lower temperatures and higher relative humidity postulated by Park et al. (2011) may dissolve under forest canopy when air temperature drops below 10°C. Overall, the obtained results highlight the need for differentiated FES management and planning according to specific target group (such as older adults in this study), the likelihood of obtaining desired intervention effects on specific mental health characteristics, and the public health priorities, e.g., societally and individually affordable healthy aging.

4.4 Study limitations

The nature of the forest intervention meant that the study was exposed to external factors that could not be controlled for, such as weather or the possible presence of potentially dangerous wildlife. In the Slovak context, between 2000 and 2015, there were 54 cases of brown bear attacks on humans (Bombieri et al., 2019). Next, the use of PPG sensors embedded in commercial wearable devices entailed lesser accuracy and precision relative to dedicated but less comfortable and more intrusive wearable devices relying on chest strap HR readings. Easy-to-use features are especially important in outdoor settings (Byrom et al., 2018; Gradl et al., 2019).

4.5 Implication for further research, forest management, and public health policy

Further research should focus on analysing patterns of HRV change throughout interventions to capture the potential influence of other modulators and conditioning factors. Also, the optimum intervention length required for the restoration of the cognitive and the autonomous nervous system functions warrants further investigation. Recently, White et al. (2019) postulated that spending at least 120 min per week in contact with nature is associated with well-being and good health. The longer minimum time in the latter study may reflect a broader definition of the natural environment, including city parks and farmland. Importantly, the presented study suggests that forest conservation and management should promote the availability of elements enhancing complexity and fractal dimension of forest scenes, mainly the presence of old trees, to achieve positive effect on the cognitive function as a health-related forest ecosystems service for older adults of the paramount importance.

5 Conclusion

The presented study showed that walking and viewing in forest settings offering structurally rich elements can significantly improve the cognitive function and, to a lesser degree, elicit higher heart rate variability as a parasympathetic activity marker in older adults. The lack of association between urban walking and improved cognitive function and comparatively smaller effect on HRV change suggest that forest walking should be preferred to urban walking by older adults, when possible. The implications of this study for public health and the management of forests and their ecosystem services underscore that policies promoting access to forests, particularly during pandemics or analogous events, can support cognitive function and mental health of older adults as a vulnerable group. However, public health and forest management experts need to develop and implement scalable frameworks integrating selection, designing, and servicing suitable forest restorative areas, with a particular emphasis on older adults’ needs, mainly safety and accessibility.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Independent Ethical Committee of the Banska Bystrica Self-Governing Region (BBSK) for Biomedical Research, registration No. 37828100. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MP: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. JH: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. MK: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. DT: Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. DJ: Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. VP: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the project RISE-WELL – Critical solutions for elderly well-being (Grant agreement no. 860173) awarded by the European Commission through the MSCA-ITN-EID – European Industrial Doctorate funding scheme (H2020-MSCA-ITN-2019), Scientific Grant Agency of the Ministry of Education, Science, Research, and Sport of the Slovak Republic (Grant numbers 1/0810/21 and 1/0644/23).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/ffgc.2024.1287266/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

Arbuthnott, K., and Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Basso, M. R., Bornstein, R. A., and Lang, J. M. (1999). Practice effects on commonly used measures of executive function across twelve months. Clin. Neuropsychol. 13, 283–292. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.3.283.1743

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bombieri, G., Naves, J., Penteriani, V., Selva, N., Fernández-Gil, A., López-Bao, J. V., et al. (2019). Brown bear attacks on humans: a worldwide perspective. Sci. Rep. 9:8573. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44341-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowie, C. R., and Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of the trail making test. Nat. Protoc. 1, 2277–2281. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, D. K., Barton, J. L., Pretty, J., and Gladwell, V. F. (2014). Walks4Work: assessing the role of the natural environment in a workplace physical activity intervention. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 40, 390–399. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3421

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrom, B., Watson, C., Doll, H., Coons, S. J., Eremenco, S., Ballinger, R., et al. (2018). Selection of and evidentiary considerations for wearable devices and their measurements for use in regulatory decision making: recommendations from the ePRO consortium. Value Health 21, 631–639. doi: 10.1016/j.jval.2017.09.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassarino, M., Tuohy, I. C., and Setti, A. (2019). Sometimes nature doesn’t work: absence of attention restoration in older adults exposed to environmental scenes. Exp. Aging Res. 45, 372–385. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1627497

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., and Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Dev. Psychol. 37, 715–730. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciolek, C. H., and Lee, S. Y. (2020). “Cognitive issues in the older adult” in Guccione’s geriatric physical therapy. eds. D. Avers and R. A. Wong (St. Louis: Mosby), 425–452.

Google Scholar

Clark, H., Vanclay, J., and Brymer, E. (2023). Forest features and mental health and wellbeing: a scoping review. J. Environ. Psychol. 89:102040. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, J. D., and Hinkeldey, N. S. (1987). Relationship between parts a and B of trail making test. J. Clin. Psychol. 43, 402–409. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198707)43:4<402::AID-JCLP2270430411>3.0.CO;2-E

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosmo, C., Seligowski, A. V., Aiken, E. M., van’t Wout-Frank, M., and Philip, N. S. (2022). Heart rate variability features as predictors of intermittent theta-burst stimulation response in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuromodulation Technol. Neural Interface 25, 588–595. doi: 10.1111/ner.13529

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, A. O., van Ravenzwaaij, D., Matzke, D., Steingroever, H., Wetzels, R., Grasman, R. P., et al. (2016). Hidden multiplicity in exploratory multiway ANOVA: prevalence and remedies. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 640–647. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0913-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, J., Ritchie, S. D., Oddson, B., Gagnon, D. D., Mrozewski, T., Little, J., et al. (2023). Using heart rate variability methods for health-related outcomes in outdoor contexts: a scoping review of empirical studies. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:1330. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20021330

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daffre, C., Oliver, K. I., Nazareno, J. R. S., Mader, T., Seo, J., Dominguez, J. P., et al. (2022). Rapid eye movement sleep parasympathetic activity predicts wake hyperarousal symptoms following a traumatic event. J. Sleep Res. 32:e13685. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13685

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, H. E., Gurven, M., and Cashdan, E. (2023). Navigational experience and the preservation of spatial abilities into old age among a tropical forager-farmer population. Top. Cogn. Sci. 15, 187–212. doi: 10.1111/tops.12602

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Beurs, E., Beekman, A. T. F., Van Balkom, A. J. L. M., Deeg, D. J. H., and Van Tilburg, W. (1999). Consequences of anxiety in older persons: its effect on disability, well-being and use of health services. Psychol. Med. 29, 583–593. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008351

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Brito, J. N., Pope, Z. C., Mitchell, N. R., Schneider, I. E., Larson, J. M., Horton, T. H., et al. (2020). The effect of green walking on heart rate variability: a pilot crossover study. Environ. Res. 185:109408. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2020.109408

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doimo, I., Masiero, M., and Gatto, P. (2020). Forest and wellbeing: bridging medical and forest research for effective forest-based initiatives. Forests 11:791. doi: 10.3390/f11080791

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, N. J., and Blazer, D. (2020). Social isolation, loneliness in older people pose health risk. National Institute of aging. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 1233–1244. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evenson, K. R., and Spade, C. L. (2020). Review of validity and reliability of Garmin activity trackers. J. Meas. Phys. Behav. 3, 170–185. doi: 10.1123/jmpb.2019-0035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Franěk, M., Petružálek, J., and Šefara, D. (2019). Eye movements in viewing urban images and natural images in diverse vegetation periods. Urban For. Urban Green. 46:126477. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126477

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garmin. (2020). Garmin Venu Sq, Available at: https://www.garmin.com/it-IT/p/707174

Google Scholar

Gradl, S., Wirth, M., Richer, R., Rohleder, N., and Eskofier, B. M.. (2019). An overview of the feasibility of permanent, real-time, unobtrusive stress measurement with current wearables. Proceedings of the 13th EAI international conference on pervasive computing Technologies for Healthcare, 360–365.

Google Scholar

Grassini, S., Holm, S., Railo, H., and Koivisto, M. (2016). Who is afraid of the invisible snake? Subjective visual awareness modulates posterior brain activity for evolutionarily threatening stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 121, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T. (2021). “Restoration in nature: beyond the conventional narrative” in Nature and psychology: Biological, cognitive, developmental, and social pathways to well-being. eds. A. R. Schutte, J. C. Torquati, and J. R. Stevens (Cham: Springer), 89–151.

Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Böök, A., Garvill, J., Olsson, T., and Gärling, T. (1996). Environmental influences on psychological restoration. Scand. J. Psychol. 37, 378–393. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1996.tb00670.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, M., Agrigoroaei, S., Jeon, M., Bruzzese, M., and Lachman, M. (2018). Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: findings from the midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 805–820. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000425

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Izakova, L., Hlavacova, N., Segeda, V., Kapsdorfer, D., Morovicsova, E., Jezova, D., et al. (2020). Salivary aldosterone, cortisol, and their morning to evening slopes in patients with depressive disorder and healthy subjects: acute episode and follow-up 6 months after reaching remission. Neuroendocrinology 110, 1001–1009. doi: 10.1159/000505921

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarosz, E. (2022). Direct exposure to green and blue spaces is associated with greater mental wellbeing in older adults. J. Aging Environ. 37, 460–477. doi: 10.1080/26892618.2022.2109792

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: a psychological perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

Google Scholar

Karjalainen, E., Sarjala, T., and Raitio, H. (2010). Promoting human health through forests: overview and major challenges. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 15, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s12199-008-0069-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keniger, L. E., Gaston, K. J., Irvine, K. N., and Fuller, R. A. (2013). What are the benefits of interacting with nature? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10, 913–935. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10030913

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobayashi, H., Song, C., Ikei, H., Park, B.-J., Lee, J., Kagawa, T., et al. (2018). Forest walking affects autonomic nervous activity: a population-based study. Front. Public Health 6:278. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00278

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, M. (2015). How might contact with nature promote human health? Promising mechanisms and a possible central pathway. Front. Psychol. 6:1093. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanki, T., Siponen, T., Ojala, A., Korpela, K., Pennanen, A., Tiittanen, P., et al. (2017). Acute effects of visits to urban green environments on cardiovascular physiology in women: a field experiment. Environ. Res. 159, 176–185. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.07.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, K., Daily, G., and Myers, S. S.. (2012). Human health as an ecosystem service: a conceptual framework. In Integrating ecology and poverty reduction: ecological dimensions, eds J. C. Ingram, F. DeClerck, and C. R. Riodel, 231–251. New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

Google Scholar

Lin, H. (2020). Probing two-way moderation effects: a review of software to easily plot Johnson-Neyman figures. Struct. Equ. Model. 27, 494–502. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2020.1732826

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque-Casado, A., Perales, J. C., Cárdenas, D., and Sanabria, D. (2016). Heart rate variability and cognitive processing: the autonomic response to task demands. Biol. Psychol. 113, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lumley, T., Diehr, P., Emerson, S., and Chen, L. (2002). The importance of the normality assumption in large public health data sets. Annu Rev Public Health. 23, 151–169. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140546

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Málišová, E., Dančík, D., Heretik, A., Abrahámová, M., Krakovská, S., Brandoburová, P., et al. (2021). Slovak version of the trail making test: normative data. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 29, 1476–1483. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2021.1890596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez, G. J., Grover, T., Mattingly, S. M., Mark, G., D’Mello, S., Aledavood, T., et al. (2022). Alignment between heart rate variability from fitness trackers and perceived stress: perspectives from a large-scale in situ longitudinal study of information workers. JMIR Hum. Factors 9:e33754. doi: 10.2196/33754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthay, E. C., Hagan, E., Gottlieb, L. M., Tan, M. L., Vlahov, D., Adler, N., et al. (2021). Powering population health research: considerations for plausible and actionable effect sizes. SSM Popul. Health 14:100789. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100789

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McMorris, T. (2016). “History of research into the acute exercise-cognition interaction: a cognitive psychology approach” in Exercise-cognition interaction: Neuroscience perspectives. ed. T. McMorris (New York: Elsevier Academic Press), 1–28.

Google Scholar

Meneguzzo, F., and Zabini, F. (2021). “Forest ecosystem Services for Human Health” in (eds). F. Meneguzzo and F. Zabini Agri-food and Forestry Sectors for Sustainable Development. Sustainable Development Goals Series (Cham: Springer), 33–44.

Google Scholar

Milcu, I. A., Hanspach, J., Abson, D., and Fischer, J. (2013). Cultural ecosystem services: a literature review and prospects for future research. Ecol. Soc. 18:44. doi: 10.5751/ES-05790-180344

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and Organization of Individual Differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Natarajan, A., Pantelopoulos, A., Emir-Farinas, H., and Natarajan, P. (2020). Heart rate variability with photoplethysmography in 8 million individuals: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Digit. Health 2, e650–e657. doi: 10.1016/S2589-7500(20)30246-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nkurikiyeyezu, K. N., Suzuki, Y., Tobe, Y., Lopez, G. F., and Itao, K.. (2017). Heart rate variability as an indicator of thermal comfort state. Proceedings of the 56th annual conference of the Society of Instrument and Control Engineers of Japan (SICE), 1510–1512. Kanazawa, Japan.

Google Scholar

Ohly, H. P., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., et al. (2016). Attention restoration theory: a systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health, Part B Crit. Rev. 19, 305–343. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, B. J., Furuya, K., Kasetani, T., Takayama, N., Kagawa, T., and Miyazaki, Y. (2011). Relationship between psychological responses and physical environments in forest settings. Landsc. Urban Plan. 102, 24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.03.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichlerová, M., Önkal, D., Bartlett, A., Výboštok, J., and Pichler, V. (2021). Variability in forest visit numbers in different regions and population segments before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3469. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichlerová, M., Výbošťok, D., Önkal, K. E., Lamatungga, D., and Tamatam, V. P. (2023). Increased appreciation of forests and their restorative effects during the COVID-19 pandemic. Ambio 52, 647–664. doi: 10.1007/s13280-022-01816-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietilä, J., Helander, E., Korhonen, I., Myllymäki, T., Kujala, U., and Lindholm, H. (2018). Acute effect of alcohol intake on cardiovascular autonomic regulation during the first hours of sleep in a large real-world sample of Finnish employees: observational study. JMIR Mental Health 5:e23. doi: 10.2196/mental.9519

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piva, G., Caruso, L., Gómez, A. C., Calzolari, M., Visintin, E. P., Davoli, P., et al. (2022). Effects of forest walking on physical and mental health in elderly populations: a systematic review. Rev. Environ. Health. doi: 10.1515/reveh-2022-0093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Retty, T. (2022). The effects of nose-breathing-only training on physiological parameters related to running performance. Master thesis, North Bay, ON, Canada: Nipissing University.

Google Scholar

Rowan, K., McAlpine, D. D., and Blewett, L. A. (2013). Access and cost barriers to mental health care, by insurance status, 1999–2010. Health Aff. 32:172330. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2013.0133

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, E. B., Liao, D., Chambless, L. E., Prineas, R. J., Evans, G. W., and Heiss, G. (2003). Hypertension, blood pressure, and heart rate variability: the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. Hypertension 42, 1106–1111. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000100444.71069.73

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, A. R., and Rominger, C. (2021). Feelings from the heart: developing HRV decrease-trigger algorithms via multilevel hyperplane simulation to detect psychosocially meaningful episodes in everyday life. Psychophysiology 58:e13914. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidel, D., Ehbrecht, M., Annighöfer, P., and Ammer, C. (2019). From tree to stand-level structural complexity — Which properties make a forest stand complex? Agric. For. Meteorol. 278:107699. doi: 10.1016/j.agrformet.2019.107699

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Setti, A., Cassarino, M., and Tuohy, I. (2017). Exposure to green does not always restore attention in older adults. Innov. Aging 1:328. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igx0004.1207

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, W. S., Shin, C. S., Yeoun, P. S., and Kim, J. J. (2011). The influence of interaction with forest on cognitive function. Scand. J. For. Res. 26, 595–598. doi: 10.1080/02827581.2011.585996

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skov, R. A. C., Lawaetz, J., Konge, L., Westerlin, L., Aasvang, E. K., Meyhoff, C. S., et al. (2022). Simulation-based education of endovascular scrub nurses reduces stress and improves team performance. J. Surg. Res. 280, 209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jss.2022.07.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T., and Bentsen, P. (2018). Attention restoration theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B Crit. Rev. 21, 227–268. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2018.1505571

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stigsdotter, U. K., Corazon, S. S., Sidenius, U., Kristiansen, J., and Grahn, P. (2017). It is not all bad for the grey city – a crossover study on physiological and psychological restoration in a forest and an urban environment. Health Place 46, 145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.05.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S. (1983). “Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment” in Behavior and the natural environment. eds. I. Altman and J. F. Wohlwill (Boston: Springer), 85–125.

Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations. (2022). World population prospects 2022: Summary of results. UN DESA/POP/2022/TR/NO.3. Available at: https://www.un.org/development/desa/pd/sites/www.un.org.development.desa.pd/files/wpp2022_summary_of_results.pdf

Google Scholar

Vahia, I. V., Jeste, D. V., and Reynolds, C. F. (2020). Older adults and the mental health effects of COVID-19. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 324, 2253–2254. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.21753

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vining, J., and Tyler, E. (1999). Emotions and desired outcomes reflected in public responses to forest management plans. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 6, 21–34.

Google Scholar

White, M. P., Alcock, I., Grellier, J., Wheeler, B. W., Hartig, T., Warber, S. L., et al. (2019). Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing. Sci. Rep. 9:7730. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2017). Mental health of older adults, Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-of-older-adults

Google Scholar

Yamada, Y. (2006). Soundscape-based forest planning for recreational and therapeutic activities. Urban For. Urban Green. 5, 131–139. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2006.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., de Peuter, O. R., Kamphuisen, P. W., and Karemaker, J. M. (2013). Search for HRV-parameters that detect a sympathetic shift in heart failure patients on β-blocker treatment. Front. Physiol. 4:81. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: forest walking, older adults, cognitive function, heart rate variability, ambient temperature, environmental modulation

Citation: Lamatungga KE, Pichlerová M, Halamová J, Kanovský M, Tamatam D, Ježová D and Pichler V (2024) Forests serve vulnerable groups in times of crises: improved mental health of older adults by individual forest walking during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. For. Glob. Change. 7:1287266. doi: 10.3389/ffgc.2024.1287266

Received: 01 September 2023; Accepted: 05 February 2024;
Published: 21 February 2024.

Edited by:

Alejandro Lopez-Feldman, University of Gothenburg, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Lorenzo Caruso, University of Ferrara, Italy
Samantha Elizabeth Robinson, University of Arkansas, United States

Copyright © 2024 Lamatungga, Pichlerová, Halamová, Kanovský, Tamatam, Ježová and Pichler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Viliam Pichler, pichler@tuzvo.sk;vilopichler@gmail.com

ORCID: Júlia Halamová, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2655-2327

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.