Skip to main content

SPECIALTY GRAND CHALLENGE article

Front. Ethol., 13 December 2022
Sec. Social Behavior and Communication

Social behavior and communication grand challenges – Frontiers in Ethology

  • Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States


The ubiquity of social behavior and communication

All animals – indeed, all living things – communicate (e.g., Diggle et al., 2008; Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011; Schaefer and Ruxton, 2011; Babikova et al., 2013). Communication is perhaps the key body-to-body or cell-to-cell interaction that makes fundamental biological processes possible. These processes include everything from biochemical changes on the surface of one cell that alter the metabolic activity of a nearby cell to the production of a mating signal by one individual that alters the likelihood of reproductive activity of another individual. Communication is therefore largely about changes induced in an individual by a perceived action or structure of another individual. It is thus the social behavior that lies at the foundation of all other social behavior and includes individuals producing visual displays that deter the pursuit of a predator, vocalizations that change the movement patterns of others in their midst, volatile chemical compounds that attract potential mates, and vibrational waves that pause the signaling behavior of nearby conspecifics.

Individuals of many species engage regularly if not constantly in social behavior – individuals in social species spend large parts of their lives in social groups, in close proximity and often engaging in interactions with conspecifics. Even in species in which individuals spend most of their lives solitary (e.g., orb weaving spiders), individuals still have to mate, and occasionally have to interact with others in ways that affect social proximity (e.g., territoriality: Leyhausen, 1965; Graw et al., 2019). Furthermore, we are learning more about the importance to individuals of living in social groups composed of multiple species (Dhondt, 2012; Goodale et al., 2017).

Living in social groups brings benefits such as an enhanced ability to detect and respond to predators and food resources (Wilson, 1975; Krause and Ruxton, 2002). These benefits of group living are often thought to accrue to prey species, but predators clearly benefit from sociality as well (Beauchamp, 2014). Although we know a great deal about benefits of social living for both conspecific-only and mixed-species groups, we know less about the costs of such social living, and so more work on this question is needed (Dhondt, 2012; Goodale et al., 2017). As a rule, social living will evolve when the benefits outweigh the costs. The benefits of living in social groups often critically depend on the signals and cues of others (Hauser, 1997; Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 1998; D'Ettorre and Hughes, 2008; Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011).

Signals and cues

Individuals can gain considerable information about other individuals simply by assessing their non-signaling behavior – their cues. For example, small prey species typically respond with strong anti-predatory behavior to bird-hunting avian predators flying overhead, but may not display as much anti-predatory behavior if those same avian predators are actively courting one another. Likewise, by watching a pet dog sleep, we can predict with great certainty what its immediate subsequent behavior will be based upon these cues – more sleep. Most of the focus of research in communication has been on signals, however. Unlike cues, signals have evolved for the particular communicative function they serve. As a few examples consider the anti-predatory alarm signals in3vervet monkeys, Chlorocebus pygerythrus, mate attraction signals in stalk-eyed flies, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni, and food detection signals in ravens, Corvus corax (for a key reference describing cues and signals, see Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003). Although the distinction between a cue and a signal is important, sometimes distinguishing cues from signals is not a trivial matter (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011).

When we study the signals that animals use, we notice in many cases that the signals and signaling are quite complex. Dating back at least to Darwin (1872), fundamental questions in the study of communication relate to signal complexity. Why is there such incredible diversity in signal complexity across different species, or perhaps even across different populations of the same species? What factors select for the complexity of signals or of signal use? One of the hypotheses to explain signal complexity in animals is the idea that increased social complexity (larger groups, greater diversity of relationships within groups) drives a need for greater signaling complexity (Freeberg et al., 2012). More complex groups often demand increased social cognition in group members; therefore, in such groups we should expect strong selection for enhanced communicative systems that provide individuals diverse behavioral ways to assess and manage the behavior of others (Dunbar, 2003; Freeberg et al., 2012). This argument is relevant for all signaling modalities, and not just vocal and visual (e.g., delBarco-Trillo et al., 2012; Gebhardt et al., 2012). The relationships among social complexity, social cognition, and communication – and the neural bases underlying these – are currently a major focus of inquiry (Lucas et al., 2018; Freeberg et al., 2019; Roberts and Roberts, 2020: Roberts et al., 2022).

Social behavior and communication: Where we’ve come from and where we might go

As with so many things in biology, one of the earliest major treatments of social behavior and communication was carried out by Darwin in his foundational The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (Darwin, 1872). A century after that book was first published, three founders of the field of ethology won the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in part for their work on social behavior and communication – Niko Tinbergen, Karl von Frisch, and Konrad Lorenz (Burkhardt, 2005). In the last several decades, researchers in social behavior and communication have continued to make major advances. There was a sustained consideration of the value (or damage) of the term “information” in the study of communication (Shannon and Weaver, 1949; Burghardt, 1970; Smith, 1977; Dawkins and Krebs, 1978), and the information debate reared again recently (Owings and Morton, 1998; Hailman, 2008; Owren et al., 2010; Seyfarth et al., 2010; Fischer, 2013; Wiley, 2013a). For some the ‘communication as information’ vs ‘communication as manipulation’ debate is largely one of semantics, for others communication involves both information and manipulation, and for others the debate raises some fundamental ideas about how communicative interactions actually influence behavior and shape interactions and relationships among individuals. Game theory models helped advance our understanding of signaling both in cooperative/pro-social and in competitive/contest situations as, unlike in basic optimality models, frequency dependence and what signaling behavior other individuals are using are important (Maynard Smith, 1982; Bergstrom and Lachmann, 1998; Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003; Hurd and Enquist, 2005).

Over the last few decades, it has become clear that the standard dyadic way of thinking about communication is too limiting. The sender ➔ signal/cue ➔ receiver relationship almost always occurs within a complicated social milieu of both conspecifics and heterospecifics and of both cooperators and competitors – individuals commonly communicate within complex communication networks (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1999; McGregor, 2005). Recent advances in social network approaches are now putting us in a position to understand better how networks of signaling relate to networks of other social behavior (such as affiliations or agonistic interactions: Snijders and Naguib, 2017), and this should help us better understand how social interactions build into the social relationships that create the structure of groups (Hinde, 1976; Croft et al., 2008; Wey et al., 2008; Barrett et al., 2012; Farine et al., 2015). Over the last few decades it also became clear that our unimodal way of observing and experimenting with animal signaling greatly oversimplified the ways in which individuals actually communicate with one another in natural settings. For example, decades of study revealed the developmental and functional implications of variation in songs of songbirds using analyses of the recorded acoustic signals. But the songs of many songbird species are often produced in conjunction with visual displays that can range from relatively simple changes in body posture to relatively ornate changes in feather and wing placement and movements. These visual displays that are produced with songs are important signals to receivers, and it is increasingly clear that communication in animal species is regularly multi-modal, and this has important implications for our proximate and ultimate understanding of animal communication (Partan and Marler, 1999; Cooper and Goller, 2004; Partan and Marler, 2005; Higham and Hebets, 2013; Partan, 2013; Peckre et al., 2019). Furthermore, multi-modal signaling is seen in a wide range of taxa (e.g., Narins et al., 2005; de Luna et al., 2010).

Outside of the realms of studies of mate choice related to signal variation and studies of predator behavior related to prey cue variation, most of the focus of research on animal communication had traditionally been on the sender and the signal it produces. Increased work on the receiver side of the equation is needed for a wider range of signaling modalities, a wider range of communicative contexts, and a wider range of taxa. What kinds of proximate and ultimate factors influence signal perception by receivers (King et al., 2003; Schmidt and Romer, 2011; Sheehan et al., 2014; Henry et al., 2016)? In any channel of communication, noise distorts or diminishes the chances of successful communication – the ability of a receiver to pull a signal or cue out of the background becomes more difficult the more noise exists in the system (Wiley, 2017). Given the prevalence of noise in most every channel of communication – and the increasing presence of human noise in non-human animal communication – more research on signal and cue perception in the contexts of acoustic, chemical, visual noise is needed (Grafe et al., 2012; Naguib, 2013; Wiley, 2013b; Wiley, 2017). Going back to the sender, furthermore, we know that signals are frequently produced in streams of signals. How important is the particular sequence of signals to receivers? We have developed sophisticated analytical approaches to analyzing signal sequences (Kershenbaum et al., 2016), but now need to devote more effort to understanding how variation in sequences maps on to variation in sender and receiver behavior. We are also beginning to understand how variation between signal presentations can be meaningful to receivers, but much more work is needed.

Individual behavior can be heavily influenced by variation in social context – contextual variables like the density of individuals and the composition of different personalities or behavioral types of individuals within groups or populations. These influences can be both proximate factors impacting individuals over their lifetimes, but can also be powerful selective pressures shaping behavior patterns in populations over generations (e.g., lizards: Cote et al., 2008; birds: Roth et al., 2021; fish: Almany and Webster, 2004). More work is needed that integrates assessments of social contextual factors like density and variation in social roles with assessments of individual behavior to increase our understanding of behavioral variation across populations and across generations (Wright et al., 2019).

Social grouping and social behavior are often constrained by environmental factors, including predation pressure and physical habitat structure (Orpwood et al., 2008; Griesser and Nystrand, 2009; Cenni et al., 2010; Bettridge and Dunbar, 2012; Goodale et al., 2014; Gentry et al., 2019). Increased sociality is important for anti-predatory benefits and may help individuals deal better with habitat disturbance. Although increased mixed-species group sociality is also thought to be important for anti-predatory benefits, the stability and composition of mixed-species groups often breaks down with habitat disturbance (Lee et al., 2005; Mokross et al., 2014; Mammides et al., 2018). Why habitat disturbance might impact social behavior and communication of single species groups differently from mixed-species groups is an important question to try to answer given the ubiquity of anthropogenic disturbance in non-human animal systems – urbanization and hard reflective surfaces, light and noise pollution, climate change, etc.

Beyond the question of how mixed-species groups respond to habitat disturbance, our understanding of the proximate and ultimate factors driving mixed-species grouping is still in its infancy (Goodale et al., 2017; Goodale et al., 2020). What factors cause an individual of one species to join with individuals of another species, what factors keep it in the group for minutes, hours, or days, and what factors cause it to leave the group (Sridhar et al., 2009)? We do know that individuals regularly eavesdrop – they perceive and attend to the signals of other species (e.g., Templeton and Greene, 2007; Goodale and Kotagama, 2008) and such heterospecific communication can influence the structure of mixed-species groups (Goodale et al., 2010). We also know that mixed-species group size and composition can impact the ways in which individuals communicate (Coppinger et al., 2020). The dynamics and determinants of signaling and mixed-species group composition will prove to be an important area of future research.

Researchers in a wide range of fields have long been interested in the similarities and differences between non-human animal communication on the one hand and human language on the other hand (Cheney and Seyfarth, 2010; Collier et al., 2014). For non-human animal communication systems beyond bird song, how (in)flexible are signaling systems with regard to variation in social experience during development? Recent evidence suggests more plasticity in vocal development than had been appreciated (Lemasson et al., 2011; Koda et al., 2013). We are also beginning to understand better the significance of syntax-like structure in communicative systems outside of human language (Bohn et al., 2009; Ouattara et al., 2009; Collier et al., 2014; Suzuki et al., 2016; Griesser et al., 2018). Most of this work has been carried out in systems of vocal communication, but recent research is pointing to the importance of visual communication with regard to the ‘communication ➔ language’ transition (Roberts et al., 2019; Roberts and Roberts, 2019; Damjanovic et al., 2022). Circling back to earlier discussion, how important is the complexity of the social milieu to the complexity of signals and signal sequences, and how might social complexity explain the evolutionary transition to language (Dunbar, 1993; Dunbar, 2003; Freeberg et al., 2012)?

The “Social Behavior and Communication” specialty section seeks to address these, and many other, questions in this field. We are at a particularly interesting and exciting time in the field as technological and statistical advances have made the study of signal use and social behavioral metrics incredibly sensitive and sophisticated. There are enormously important questions remaining to be addressed in animal social behavior and communication, some of which are raised here, and this specialty section is an ideal home for articles that describe work aimed at addressing those questions. We welcome both experimental and descriptive/correlational studies and approaches at all levels of analysis – mechanistic, ontogenetic, functional, and phylogenetic. Furthermore, studies that integrate more than one of these levels are particularly needed to help push our understanding of social behavior and communication forward.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Almany G. R., Webster M. S. (2004). Odd species out as predators reduce diversity of coral-reef fishes. Ecology 85 (11), 2933–2937. doi: 10.1890/03-3150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babikova Z., Gilbert L., Bruce T. J.A., Birkett M., Caulfield J. C., Woodcock C., et al. (2013). Underground signals carried through common mycelial networks warn neighbouring plants of aphid attack. Ecol. Lett. 16 (7), 835–843. doi: 10.1111/ele.12115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett L., Henzi S. P., Lusseau D. (2012). Taking sociality seriously: the structure of multi-dimensional social networks as a source of information for individuals. Philos. Transact: Biol. Sci. 367 (1599), 2108–2118. doi: 10.2307/23250488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchamp G. (2014). Social predation: How group living benefits predators and prey (London: Academic Press / Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Bergstrom C. T., Lachmann M. (1998). Signaling among relatives. III. talk is cheap. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. United States America 95 (9), 5100–5105. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettridge C. M., Dunbar R. I. M. (2012). Predation as a determinant of minimum group size in baboons. Folia Primatolog 83 (3-6), 332–352. doi: 10.1159/000339808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohn K. M., Schmidt-French B., Schwartz C., Smotherman M., and Pollak G. D. (2009). Versatility and stereotypy of free-tailed bat songs. PLoS One 4 (8), e6746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradbury J. W., Vehrencamp S. L. (1998). Principles of animal communication. 1st Ed (Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer).

Google Scholar

Bradbury J. W., Vehrencamp S. L. (2011). Principles of animal communication. 2nd Ed (Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer).

Google Scholar

Burghardt G. M. (1970). “Defining 'communication',” in Communicaiton by chemical signals. Eds. Johnston J. W., Moulton D. G., Turk A. (New York: Appleton Century-Crofts), 5–18.

Google Scholar

Burkhardt R. W. Jr. (2005). Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology (Chicago: University of Chicago Press).

Google Scholar

Cenni F., Parisi G., Gherardi F. (2010). Effects of habitat complexity on the aggressive behaviour of the American lobster (Homarus americanus) in captivity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 122 (1), 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheney D. L., Seyfarth R. M. (1999). Recognition of other individuals' social relationships by female baboons. Anim. Behav. 58, 67–75. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheney D. L., Seyfarth R. M. (2010). Primate Communication and Human Language: Continuities and Discontinuities. In: Kappeler P., Silk J. Eds. Tracing the Origins of Human Universals (Berlin Heidelberg: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier K., Bickel B., van Schaik C. P., Manser M. B., Townsend S. W. (2014). Language evolution: syntax before phonology? Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 281 (1788), 1–7.

Google Scholar

Cooper B. G., Goller F. (2004). Multimodal signals: Enhancement and constraint of song motor patterns by visual display. Science 303 (5657), 544–546. doi: 10.1126/science.1091099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppinger B. A., Kania S. A., Lucas J. R., Sieving K. E., Freeberg T. M. (2020). Experimental manipulation of mixed-species flocks reveals heterospecific audience effects on calling. Anim. Behav. 167, 193–207. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cote J., Dreiss A., Clobert J. (2008). Social personality trait and fitness. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 275 (1653), 2851–2858. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croft D. P., James R., Krause J. (2008). Exploring animal social networks (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).

Google Scholar

D'Ettorre P., Hughes D. P. (2008). Sociobiology of communication: An interdisciplinary perspective (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Damjanovic L., Roberts S. G. B., Roberts A. I. (2022). Language as a tool for social bonding: Evidence from wild chimpanzee gestural, vocal and bimodal signals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 377 (1860). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals (London: John Murray).

Google Scholar

Dawkins R., Krebs J. R. (1978). “Animal signals: Information or manipulation?,” in Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Eds. Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. (Oxford: Blackwell), 282–309.

Google Scholar

delBarco-Trillo J., Sacha C. R., Dubay G. R., Drea C. M. (2012). Eulemur, me lemur: the evolution of scent-signal complexity in a primate clade. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 367 (1597), 1909–1922. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Luna A. G., Hodl W., Amezquita A. (2010). Colour, size and movement as visual subcomponents in multimodal communication by the frog allobates femoralis. Anim. Behav. 79 (3), 739–745. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhondt A. A. (2012). Interspecific competition in birds (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Diggle S. P., West S. A., Gardner A., Griffin A. S. (2008). “Communication in bacteria,” in Sociobiology of communication: An interdisciplinary perspective. Eds. D'Ettorre P., Hughes D. P. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), p 11–p 31.

Google Scholar

Dunbar R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behav. Brain Sci. 16 (04), 681–694. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00032325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annu. Rev. Anthropol 32, 163–181. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farine D. R., Aplin L. M., Sheldon B. C., Hoppitt W. (2015). Interspecific social networks promote information transmission in wild songbirds. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 282 (1803), 1–9. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer J. (2013). “Information, inference and meaning in primate vocal behaviour,” in Animal communication theory: Information and influence. Ed. Stegmann U. E. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 297–317.

Google Scholar

Freeberg T. M., Dunbar R. I. M., Ord T. J. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity introduction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 367 (1597), 1785–1801. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeberg T. M., Gentry K. E., Sieving K. E., Lucas J. R. (2019). On understanding the nature and evolution of social cognition: A need for the study of communication. Anim. Behav. 155, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebhardt K., Bohme M., von der Emde G. (2012). Electrocommunication behaviour during social interactions in two species of pulse-type weakly electric fishes (Mormyridae). J. Fish Biol. 81 (7), 2235–2254. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03448.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentry K. E., Roche D. P., Mugel S. G., Lancaster N. D., Sieving K. E., Freeberg T. M., et al. (2019). Flocking propensity by satellites, but not core members of mixed-species flocks, increases when individuals experience energetic deficits in a poor-quality foraging habitat. PLoS One 14 (1), 1–29. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale E., Beauchamp G., Ruxton G. D. (2017). Mixed-species groups of animals: Behavior, community structure, and conservation (London: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Goodale E., Beauchamp G., Magrath R. D., Nieh J. C., Ruxton G. D., et al. (2010). Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25 (6), 354–361. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale E., Kotagama S. W., Raman T. R.S., Sidhu S., Goodale U., Parker S., et al. (2014). The response of birds and mixed-species bird flocks to human-modified landscapes in Sri Lanka and southern India. For. Ecol. Manage. 329, 384–392. doi: 10.1016/j.foreco.2013.08.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale E., Sridhar H., Sieving K. E., Bangal P., Colorado G. J., Farine D. R., et al. (2020). Mixed company: A framework for understanding the composition and organization of mixed-species animal groups. Biol. Rev. 95 (4), 889–910. doi: 10.1111/brv.12591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale E., Kotagama S. W. (2008). Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Behav. Ecol. 19 (4), 887–894. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafe T. U., Preininger D., Sztatecsny M., Kasah R., Dehling J. M., Proksch S., et al. (2012). Multimodal communication in a noisy environment: A case study of the bornean rock frog staurois parvus. PLoS One 7 (5). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graw B., Kranstauber B., Manser M. B. (2019). Social organization of a solitary carnivore: spatial behaviour, interactions and relatedness in the slender mongoose. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6 (5), 182160. doi: 10.1098/rsos.182160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griesser M., Nystrand M. (2009). Vigilance and predation of a forest-living bird species depend on large-scale habitat structure. Behav. Ecol. 20 (4), 709–715. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griesser M., Wheatcroft D., Suzuki T. N. (2018). From bird calls to human language: exploring the evolutionary drivers of compositional syntax. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 21, 6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hailman J. P. (2008). Coding and redundancy: Man-made and animal-evolved signals (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).

Google Scholar

Hauser M. D. (1997). The evolution of communication (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Henry K. S., Gall M. D., Velez A., Lucas J. R.. (2016). “Avian auditory processing at four different scales: Variation among species, seasons, sexes, and individuals,” in Psychological mechanisms in animal communication. Eds. Bee M. A., Miller C. T. (Cham: Springer International Publishing Ag), 17–55.

Google Scholar

Higham J. P., Hebets E. A. (2013). An introduction to multimodal communication. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 67 (9), 1381–1388. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1590-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinde R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11 (1), 1–17. doi: 10.2307/2800384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurd P. L., Enquist M. (2005). A strategic taxonomy of biological communication. Anim. Behav. 70 (5), 1155–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kershenbaum A., Blumstein D. T., Roch M. A., Akcay C., Backus G., Bee M. A., et al. (2016). Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus. Biol. Rev. 91 (1), 13–52. doi: 10.1111/brv.12160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King A. P., West M. J., White D. J. (2003). Female cowbird song perception: Evidence for plasticity of preference. Ethology 109 (11), 865–877. doi: 10.1046/j.0179-1613.2003.00924.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koda H., Lemasson A., Oyakawa C., Rizaldi Pamungkas J., Masataka N., et al. (2013). Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One 8 (8), 1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause J., Ruxton G. D. (2002). Living in groups (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Lee T. M., Soh M. C.K., Sodhi N., Koh L. P., Lim S. L.H.. (2005). Effects of habitat disturbance on mixed species bird flocks in a tropical sub-montane rainforest. Biol. Conserv. 122 (2), 193–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2004.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemasson A., Ouattara K., Petit. E. J., Zuberbuhler K. (2011). Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evolutio Biol. 11 (362), 1–7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-11-362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyhausen P. (1965). The communal organization of solitary mammals. Symp. Zool Soc. London 14, 249–262.

Google Scholar

Lucas J. R., Gentry K. E., Sieving K. E., Freeberg T. M. (2018). Communication as a fundamental part of Machiavellian intelligence. J. Comp. Psychol. 132 (4), 442–454. doi: 10.1037/com0000138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammides C., Chen J., Goodale U. M., Kotagama S. W., Goodale E. (2018). Measurement of species associations in mixed-species bird flocks across environmental and human disturbance gradients. Ecosphere 9 (7), 1–14. doi: 10.1002/ecs2.2324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maynard Smith J. (1982). Evolution and the theory of games (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Maynard Smith J., Harper D. (2003). Animal signals (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

McGregor P. K. (2005). Animal communication networks (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Mokross K., Ryder T. B., Cortes M. C., Wolfe J. D., Stouffer P. C. (2014). Decay of interspecific avian flock networks along a disturbance gradient in Amazonia. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 281 (1776), 1–10. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2599

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naguib M. (2013). Living in a noisy world: Indirect effects of noise on animal communication. Behaviour 150 (9-10), 1069–1084. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narins P. M., Grabul D. S., Soma K. K., Gaucher P., Hold W. (2005). Cross-modal integration in a dart-poison frog. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. United States America 102 (7), 2425–2429. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406407102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orpwood J. E., Magurran A. E., Armstrong J. D., Griffiths S. W. (2008). Minnows and the selfish herd: Effects of predation risk on shoaling behaviour are dependent on habitat complexity. Anim. Behav. 76, 143–152. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouattara K., Lemasson A., Zuberbuhler K. (2009). Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. United States America 106 (51), 22026–22031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908118106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owings D. H., Morton E. S. (1998). Animal vocal communication: A new approach (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Owren M. J., Rendall D., Ryan M. J. (2010). Redefining animal signaling: Influence versus information in communication. Biol. Philos 25 (5), 755–780. doi: 10.1007/s10539-010-9224-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partan S. R. (2013). Ten unanswered questions in multimodal communication. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 67 (9), 1523–1539. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1565-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partan S., Marler P. (1999). Behavior - communication goes multimodal. Science 283 (5406), 1272–1273. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5406.1272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partan S. R., Marler P. (2005). Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am. Nat. 166 (2), 231–245. doi: 10.1086/431246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peckre L., Kappeler P. M., Fichtel C. (2019). Clarifying and expanding the social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 73 (1). doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2605-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts S. G. B., Dunbar R. I. M., Roberts A. I. (2022). Communicative roots of complex sociality and cognition: Neuropsychological mechanisms underpinning the processing of social information. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 2022, 377 (1860), 1–4. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0295.

Google Scholar

Roberts A. I., Murray L., Roberts S. G. B. (2019). Complex sociality of wild chimpanzees can emerge from laterality of manual gestures. Hum. Nature-an Interdiscip. Biosocial Perspective 30 (3), 299–325. doi: 10.1007/s12110-019-09347-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts S. G. B., Roberts A. I. (2019). Visual attention, indicative gestures, and calls accompanying gestural communication are associated with sociality in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). J. Comp. Psychol. 133 (1), 56–71. doi: 10.1037/com0000128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts A. I., Roberts S. G. B. (2020). Communicative roots of complex sociality and cognition. Biol. Rev. 95 (1), 51–73. doi: 10.1111/brv.12553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth A. M., Dingemanse N. J., Nakagawa S., McDonald G. C., Løvlie H., Robledo-Ruiz D. A., et al. (2021). Sexual selection and personality: Individual and group-level effects on mating behaviour in red junglefowl. J. Anim. Ecol. 90 (5), 1288–1306. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.13454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer H. M., Ruxton G. D. (2011). Plant-animal communication (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Schmidt A. K. D., Romer H. (2011). Solutions to the cocktail party problem in insects: Selective filters, spatial release from masking and gain control in tropical crickets. PLoS One 6 (12). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyfarth R. M., Cheney D. L., Bergman T., Fischer J., Zuberbuhler K., Hammerschmidt K. (2010). The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Anim. Behav. 80 (1), 3–8. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shannon C. E., Weaver W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication (Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press).

Google Scholar

Sheehan M. J., Jinn J., Tibbetts E. A. (2014). Coevolution of visual signals and eye morphology in polistes paper wasps. Biol. Lett. 10 (4), 1–4. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating: an ethological approach (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).

Google Scholar

Snijders L., Naguib M. (2017). “Communication in animal social networks: A missing link?,” in Advances in the study of behavior (San Diego, CA, US: Elsevier Academic Press), 297–359.

Google Scholar

Sridhar H., Beauchamp G., Shanker K. (2009). Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? a large-scale synthesis. Anim. Behav. 78 (2), 337–347. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki T. N., Wheatcroft D., Griesser M. (2016). Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nat. Commun. 7. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10986

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Templeton C. N., Greene E. (2007). Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104 (13), 5479–5482. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605183104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wey T., Blumstein D. T., Shen W., Jordan F.. (2008). Social network analysis of animal behaviour: A promising tool for the study of sociality. Anim. Behav. 75, 333–344. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.06.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiley R. H. (2013a). “Communication as a transfer of information: measurement, mechanism and meaning,” in Animal communication theory: Information and influence. Ed. Stegmann U. E., 113–131.

Google Scholar

Wiley R. H. (2013b). A receiver-signaler equilibrium in the evolution of communication in noise. Behaviour 150 (9-10), 957–993. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiley R. H. (2017). How noise determines the evolution of communication. Anim. Behav. 124, 307–313. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press).

Google Scholar

Wright J., Bolstad G. H., Araya-Ajoy Y. G., Dingemanse N. J. (2019). Life-history evolution under fluctuating density-dependent selection and the adaptive alignment of pace-of-life syndromes. Biol. Rev. 94 (1), 230–247. doi: 10.1111/brv.12451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: communication, cues, information, multimodal signaling, networks, noise, signals, social behavior

Citation: Freeberg TM (2022) Social behavior and communication grand challenges – Frontiers in Ethology. Front. Ethol. 1:1066186. doi: 10.3389/fetho.2022.1066186

Received: 10 October 2022; Accepted: 31 October 2022;
Published: 13 December 2022.

Edited and Reviewed by:

Gordon M. Burghardt, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States

Copyright © 2022 Freeberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Todd M. Freeberg, tfreeber@utk.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.