Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 09 August 2022
Sec. Population, Community, and Ecosystem Dynamics
This article is part of the Research Topic New Perspectives and Emerging Directions in Predator–Prey Functional Response Research: Hommage to C.S. Holling (1930–2019) View all 18 articles

Food web functional responses

  • Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

This article reviews the nature of functional responses that have commonly been used to represent feeding relationships in the ecological literature. It compares these with the range of functional response forms that are likely to characterize species in natural communities. The latter set of responses involves many more variables. The article reviews the history of functional response models, and examines previous work that has allowed the functional response of a predator to a single type of prey to depend on additional variables beyond the abundance of that prey type. While a number of more complex functional responses have been discussed over the years, many variables affecting feeding rates are still typically omitted from models of food webs. The influences on functional responses from trophic levels above that of the predator or below that of the prey are particularly likely to be ignored, although models and data have suggested that they can have large effects on the functional response. The influences of adaptive behavior and of the time-scale of response measurement are also too often ignored. Some of the known and unknown consequences of these omissions are discussed.

      -An adequate study of any complex phenomenon ultimately requires an integrated analysis of major systems and not just fragments of them. –C. S. Holling (1965, p. 1).

Introduction; early functional response theory

The above quotation is the opening sentence in one of the foundational articles about functional responses. The functional response of a predator species to a particular prey species is a quantitative description of the amount eaten by an average predator individual over a relatively short time period in terms of the variables affecting that amount. The one variable that always affects that amount is the abundance of the focal prey. Holling (1959) introduced his original set of three functional response shapes as potential relationships between the abundance of a single prey species and the amount eaten by a single average predator individual from a particular predator population. This work inspired many empirical studies that examined the amount eaten of a single prey type by isolated predator individuals under conditions that were identical except for having a range of different prey abundances. The results of most, although not all, such experiments could be characterized as linear, concave or sigmoid; i.e., Holling's type I, II, and III responses. These were all strictly increasing relationships. The reviews by Jeschke et al. (2002, 2004) showed that all three types occurred, as did dome-shaped responses, which decreased with prey abundance above some threshold value. This last response (type IV) had also been predicted and observed by Holling (1965, 1966). Holling and Buckingham (1976) showed that decreasing segments in a functional response could also occur as the result of adaptive behavior in a detailed simulation model of mantids consuming flies. For unknown reasons, the possibility of this fourth type had been dropped from most functional response articles and from textbooks by the late 1970s.

Jeschke and Tollrian (2005) proposed what they labeled a “new type” of functional response, which they called “roller coaster shaped”. This response included a decreasing segment at moderately high prey densities, followed by an increasing segment at very high densities. The shape was a result of adding what they termed a “confusion effect”. Confusion caused the attack success rate to decrease with increasing prey density, and this was sufficient to overcome the positive effect of increased encounter rate over an intermediate range of densities. Such non-monotonic responses had been illustrated before (Abrams, 1989, Figure 1, p. 97). In that study, the predator's foraging strategy was determined by an optimal “foraging effort”, which increased both the intake rate and the foraging cost. This model assumed that fitness was a difference between the benefit function (food intake) and the cost function (disease, injury or other adverse effects from consumption). In models having different concave functions for the intake and cost relationships, such non-monotonic responses were common. Increasing relationships with more complex shapes had earlier been predicted based on optimal foraging (Abrams, 1982). It was clear that a simple three- or four-way classification was insufficient.

Holling's type II response is the most commonly observed form of single-prey functional response (Jeschke et al., 2002; Uiterwaal et al., 2022). It was originally defined as one that had a negative second derivative. However, it is now often equated with Holling's “disk equation” formula, which is identical to the Michaelis-Menten function. Holling's derivation was based on the idea that food required “handling” after capture, and that no new captures were possible during the handling period. Negative second derivatives can be produced by other mechanisms, including behavioral adjustment of foraging time or effort (Abrams, 1982). These other mechanisms, and cases where “handling” time can be adjusted adaptively (Abrams, 1982; Anholt et al., 1987) usually produce shapes different from the disk equation. However, for systems in which predator individuals differ from each other in their exact responses, the disk equation often produces a reasonable fit to the mean response. The approximate fit and the mathematical simplicity of the disk equation formula, make it by far the most common representation of type II responses. There has been at least one measured response that was flat over the range of prey densities that was characteristic of the natural environment (Abrams et al., 1990); this presumably would have been classified as type II if sufficiently (but unrealistically) low prey densities had been explored.

Holling's work represented a major advance over the linear (“mass action”) responses incorporated into the Lotka-Volterra models and early food web models (May, 1973; Pimm, 1982). Nevertheless, the set of responses proposed by Holling are unlikely to represent more than a small fraction of the predator-prey interactions in nature. They are not sufficient because the ultimate goal of describing predation quantitatively is to understand the dynamics of predators and prey that are found in natural ecological systems. These almost always have multiple predator and prey species, and also have additional trophic levels. The abundances of lower levels usually affect prey foraging, and thus prey availability to the predator. The abundances of higher-level predators will often alter the foraging activity by the focal predator species; if so, they will obviously affect the focal predator's consumption rates. The abundances of species on both higher and lower adjacent trophic levels should therefore enter into a functional response that is designed to understand the dynamics of any predator and prey species pair in a natural system (Abrams, 2010b). (Note that the term “predator” will be used here in a broad sense that includes all consumers, while “prey” will usually be used to include both plants and non-living resources.)

The abundances of prey/resources occupying other trophic levels are not the only “other” abundances that influence the consumption rates of one prey type by one predator type. Alternative prey/food types that can be utilized by the predator are almost certain to affect the consumption rate of a focal prey type. This is inevitable in the multi-species versions of Holling's non-linear responses because they all imply some form of handling or processing, which temporarily prevents foraging. When predator density affects the functional responses in a system with one predator species (or type), it is quite likely that the abundance of a second predator species would also have some effect on the intake rate of the first predator. In fact, many other species in the food web containing a particular predator and particular prey have the potential to affect the consumption of that prey by that predator. Different ages, sizes, and/or genotypes within any of the species in the food web may also alter the functional response of a given predator type to a given prey type (DeLong et al., 2021; Uiterwaal et al., 2022).

Because it ignores both other trophic levels and alternative prey, the simplified framework for studying functional responses that arose from Holling's early work is now largely at odds with the philosophy he expressed in the opening quotation from his early article. The present article will examine some potential effects of other species on the functional response of a particular predator to a particular prey. It will also document that the contemporary literature using or dealing with functional responses has continued to ignore many of these effects.

Current usage of the term “functional response” frequently assumes that the response is a characteristic of a given pair of predator and prey species. This assumption underlies the many attempts to determine the “best” expression for describing such a response (Jeschke et al., 2002, 2004; DeLong, 2021; Novak and Stouffer, 2021b). However, several recent experiments have shown that functional response shapes in the simplest case of an isolated predator individual consuming different abundances of a single prey type can be greatly altered by the spatial and physical properties of the experimental environment (Uiterwaal and DeLong, 2018; Juliano et al., 2022). Adaptive behavior and other types of phenotypic plasticity are also likely to produce different functional response shapes in different environments (Werner and Peacor, 2003).

Another problem with current usage of the term “functional response” is the implicit idea that functional responses are independent of the time frame over which they are measured. The use of functional responses in the ordinary differential equation models that characterize most theory in community ecology is implicitly independent of time; the response at one point in time only depends on current abundances. However, a change in the rate of predation (or even just nutrient uptake) following some change in food/prey abundance is often not instantaneous. The change in abundance must often be detected. Consumption itself requires time. Consumption also often leads to growth, which causes a change in future foraging and consumption behaviors. Organisms have finite lifespans and it is rare for optimal traits to be independent of the point in that lifespan and the individual's condition at that point (de Roos and Persson, 2013). A functional response that is linear when measured over a short time span will usually become non-linear when measured over a longer time span (Abrams, 1991b); non-linear responses will change shape when measured over different time-spans. The issue of the duration of the measurement is particularly important when reproduction only occurs after a significant period of resource intake. Clearly, if a differential equation model is being used to describe population dynamics, the interval should not be overly long. However, there is no single “best” interval, and different intervals may be optimal for answering different questions about system dynamics. This may require that responses be measured over a number of time intervals.

The four major omissions from much of the functional response literature identified above are: (1) lack of alternative prey/foods; (2) lack of effects from abundances of species occupying other trophic levels, and frequent lack of predator abundance effects on its own response; (3) lack of consideration of adaptive processes affecting functional response shape, and the fact that these differ depending on the environment and other members of the biological community; (4) lack of attention to the effects of the time interval being described on the shape of a functional response. All of these issues share an underlying need to consider the wide diversity of food webs and of processes affecting the form of functional responses. These will be considered in more detail in subsequent sections. My own work and that of others (e.g., Kratina et al., 2007; DeLong, 2021) has raised this need for multispecies responses, but much of the recent literature has concentrated on the two-species case, as mentioned above. DeLong's (2021) book on functional responses, while calling for more work on multispecies responses, notably does not refer to most of the existing theoretical literature on such responses.

Two-trophic level functional responses that incorporate two or more variables

Including non-living resources as a “trophic level” and plants as consumers means that functional responses are required to describe all of the consumer-resource interactions in a biological community. In most real-world systems, the response of a consumer to any single resource will incorporate two or more population sizes. The two simplest cases of functional responses requiring two population sizes are those involving two prey/resource types and those in which the number of predators/consumers affects the short-term intake rate of an individual predator/consumer to a single prey/resource. Both of these cases have a relatively long history of inclusion in some models, even though they are only considered in the minority of the literature of empirical functional response measurements.

Choice between two or more prey/resources

One of the earliest modifications of Holling's framework was to consider the impact of having more than a single prey type (although this was implicit in the logic behind the type III response). Studies in the 1970s employed an extension of the type II response in which “handling” any of the resource types made it impossible to capture any of the other resource types (Murdoch and Oaten, 1975; Oaten and Murdoch, 1975). Under this response, handling any resource prevented capture of either that or any other resource. Thus, the “passive” saturation effect was based on the combined effects of “handling” all resources. This was an advance over single-species responses, but it did not consider adaptive behavior.

This multi-species type II model formed the basis of early theory on optimal diet choice (Schoener, 1971; Pyke et al., 1977), which predicted a sudden dropping of lower quality foods from the diet when the abundance of higher quality food(s) exceeded a threshold abundance. However, the actual functional form of predation rate functions (i.e., functional responses) implied by optimal diet theory failed to be incorporated into models in population or community ecology until much later (Fryxell and Lundberg, 1994; Ma et al., 2003). These were theoretical works, and there has been little subsequent experimental exploration of functional responses in two or more prey systems involving diet choice based on handling/processing time. As a result, major reviews of empirical works have concentrated on studies of functional responses in single resource systems: this includes Jeschke et al. (2002, 2004), DeLong (2021), and Novak and Stouffer (2021b). The last of these (DeLong, 2021) has a chapter (Chapter 5) on “multispecies functional responses” and one (Chapter 7) on “optimal foraging”. However, neither chapter presents a mathematical expression for a response incorporating adaptive diet choice.

“Switching” (Murdoch, 1969) is the only other type of adaptive diet choice that has been included in a significant number of community models. This describes the increase in relative consumption rate of a single resource with increases in its relative abundance. This is adaptive when resources are nutritionally substitutable and there is a tradeoff in relative consumption rates. Matsuda (1985) used a simple instantaneous switching function in an analysis of the evolution of switching behavior. One potential problem with this approach is the lack of a time lag required to estimate abundances and shift preferences, a feature that is capable of changing dynamics significantly (Abrams and Matsuda, 2004). Even if there is no time lag, the likely inability to estimate the relative abundances of two rare resources can cause significant differences in the dynamics of systems that do not come to a stable equilibrium (Abrams and Matsuda, 2003). The small number of empirical studies of switching has prevented adoption of a “standard approach” to modeling this phenomenon.

Switching in multiple-prey systems depends on the nature of the tradeoffs involved; i.e., the similarity of different prey in terms of their spatial location and required capture techniques (Matsuda and Namba, 1991). The behavioral dynamics approach of Abrams and Matsuda (2004) can be applied to multiple resources. van Leeuwen et al. (2013) proposed a framework for modeling switching in multiple-prey systems, but there are likely to be a variety of different representations depending on the nature of the similarities and differences between prey in the foraging strategy required to capture them.

Most models of consumption in multi-prey (food) systems have assumed nutritional substitutability of the foods. This is true of the work described above, and it leads to increased relative consumption of foods when their relative abundance increases. The presence of nutritional interactions between food types in conjunction with adaptive foraging has a variety of potential effects on functional responses to each food (Abrams, 1987). Several mechanisms produce the opposite of switching. One of these operates in the case of two nutritionally essential resources that involve a consumer foraging/intake tradeoff. Abrams (1987) showed that increasing the abundance of one essential resource favors an increase in the relative foraging effort directed at another essential resource, as this would then be the one limiting population growth. A second mechanism involves resources of different qualities under time- and capacity-limited foraging (Abrams, 1990a,b); here, increasing the abundance of a low quality resource favors increasing foraging time or effort on the higher quality resource(s). Hossie et al. (2021) have recently reported a case in which the relative amounts of palatable and unpalatable prey determine the level of predator-dependence in the functional response. Increasing palatable prey also increased predation on unpalatable prey in their system, the opposite of what was predicted by Abrams (1990a,b). However, it is consistent with the increased foraging time predicted with a tradeoff between foraging (for either resource) and predation risk.

It is known that some predators may have difficulty distinguishing some non-prey items from their prey. Kratina et al. (2007) showed that increased abundance of a non-prey species reduced the consumption rate of prey by a predatory flatworm. This type of effect may be common, but there appear to have been very few experimental studies designed to examine this possibility. It is also not known how frequently these non-prey items occupy the same trophic level as the prey whose risk of predation is reduced. Another possibility left out of formal functional response models of two- or more-prey systems is direct interactions between prey species that change the vulnerability/availability of one or more of them to the predator.

The factors that produce decreasing sections of a predator's response to a single prey can have similar effects in systems having two or more prey. Two of the major factors producing such a response are related to potential negative effects of prey consumption on the predator. These include risk of injury during the capture process and the potential presence of parasites and/or toxic substances in the prey (Abrams, 1989). These features can have a number of effects in multiple-prey systems, depending on the nature and consequences of interactions between the negative effects. These consequences have yet to be explored in detail in models or empirical systems.

A final feature of functional responses to two or more foods/resources is the presence of between-individual variation in response parameters. Nearly 40 years ago, Chesson (1984) used models in which two types of predators within a single species each had standard multi-species type II responses with different parameter values. He pointed out that the presence of such variation could cause either positive or negative switching when functional responses are measured at the level of the entire consumer/predator population. Berkhout and Morozov (2022) examined the individual and population-wide responses of a snail species in which individuals are specialized, but the population contains many different specialists. The population-wide functional response in this system clearly varies with the composition of the predator population, but the relative consumption rates of different prey types is relatively insensitive to their relative abundances.

The set of multiple-resource models reviewed above have largely been limited to the case of just two resources. Adaptive diet choice involving three or more prey/resources is potentially more complicated because of the much greater variety of tradeoff relationships that may occur. If, for example, tradeoffs arise from different spatial distributions of the prey, the degree of spatial overlap of all of the possible pairs of species and their movement rates should affect the shape of the multi-species responses. In cases with several prey that have different spatial distributions, it is possible for increased abundance of a particular prey with a spatial distribution similar to that of the focal prey to increase the predation rate on that focal prey. Empirical studies of functional responses with three or more prey are uncommon; Smout et al. (2010) and Beardsell et al. (2022) are exceptions to this generalization. However, Smout et al. (2010) do not incorporate any aspects of diet choice or optimal foraging.

Predator-dependent single-prey models

A second exception to single-variable functional responses is the class of models of a single prey (food) type that include an effect of the predator's abundance on its own functional response, an effect that is usually assumed to be negative. Beddington (1975) and DeAngelis et al. (1975) independently extended the type II response by adding a term proportional to predator density to the denominator of the function. This could reflect interference of predator individuals with each other's foraging. It could also arise from the prey exhibiting behaviors that reduce their own activity levels when predators are abundant, thereby reducing both food intake and predation risk. Both of these early models [and the related model of Crowley and Martin (1989)] assumed a simple homogeneous environment. Even without active habitat selection by prey, spatially heterogeneous environments produce results that could be interpreted as predator dependence if the functional response were measured at the level of the metacommunity as a whole (Abrams and Wilson, 2004).

When they are considered, the effects of predator density on the predator's functional response are usually assumed to be negative (Arditi and Ginzburg, 2012; DeLong, 2021; Ginzburg and Damuth, 2022). However, positive effects may occur when predators hunt cooperatively (Cosner et al., 1999), or simply when prey escape behavior in response to one predator individual makes the prey more easily detected by other predator individuals. We do not have any reviews estimating how often such positive effects occur, or what range of functional forms is needed to describe them. Both positive and negative effects may occur in the same predator-prey pair. Two different mechanisms producing negative effects are predator individuals attacking other predators (or other direct interference effects) and predators causing prey to change their behavior (e.g., remain in refugia), reducing their availability to other predator individuals. These two mechanisms require quite different functional representations.

Predator-prey models that have considered adaptive predator-avoidance by prey have all implied predator-dependence of the predator's functional response to the prey. In these cases, the functional form of the predator-dependence depends on the nature and functional form of the prey's tradeoff between predator avoidance and some other fitness-enhancing activity. As a result, different models have not suggested a single functional response formula. Those that have expressed predator-avoidance effects in terms of the general shape of the tradeoff (e.g., Abrams, 1991a,b; Abrams and Vos, 2003) are particularly likely to be ignored in the functional response literature because they do not propose a specific functional form. In these cases, the functional response shapes have been expressed in terms of the derivatives of the component functions of the expression for predator per capita growth rate. These functions are seldom studied. In many animals, social or reproductive behaviors vary with abundance, and can reduce the time available for foraging; this provides additional potential influences of predator abundance on their own functional responses.

A large literature on predator-dependent functional responses has been devoted to discussing the extreme simplification of predator effects in which the functional response is represented as an increasing function of the ratio of prey abundance to predator abundance (Arditi and Ginzburg, 1989, 2012; Ginzburg and Damuth, 2022). This obviously restricts the formula to cases of negative predator density effects on the predator's functional response. Ginzburg and Damuth (2022) have recently argued for an expanded ratio dependence in which the denominator (predator abundance) can be raised to a positive power other than 1. Various reviews have shown significant differences between the results of empirical measurements of predator effects and the original ratio-dependent models (Skalski and Gilliam, 2001; Novak and Stouffer, 2021b). An early critique of ratio dependence pointed out that there were many causes of predator-dependence, virtually none of which led to ratio dependence (Abrams, 1994). Some of these mechanisms led to more extreme predator dependence, and most of the mechanisms required functional forms different from either pure or expanded ratio dependence. The simple 1-predator-1-prey version of either the original or expanded ratio-dependent response predicts that a rare predator population can increase on arbitrarily low prey abundance, provided the predator abundance is sufficiently low. This property, which can produce unusual outcomes, has never been demonstrated experimentally, and is impossible with reasonable constraints on predator movement rates and energy stores. In spite of continuing defenses of ratio dependence (e.g., Tyutyunov and Titova, 2021; Ginzburg and Damuth, 2022), the problem of extending this form to incorporate multispecies effects is another one of its major limitations (Abrams, 1997, 2015). The issue here is that different mechanisms producing strong predator dependence require qualitatively different modifications when two or more prey are considered. Barraquand (2014) provides other arguments against general use of ratio-dependent response.

Ecological systems and functional responses are both extremely diverse. No predator species is characterized by an exactly ratio dependent response; this was one of many points of agreement in Abrams and Ginzburg (2000). There are some experimental studies where generalized ratio dependence with a predator exponent of 1 fits the data better than other exponents (Novak and Stouffer, 2021b), but there are likely many models that are more consistent with the underlying mechanisms of predator dependence that would provide a better fit in these cases. The goal of ecological theory is to understand natural systems; how they change with environmental shifts and how they evolve over time. Predator dependence can arise from many causes, two of the main ones being anti-predator behavior by prey and antagonism between predators. Abrams and Ginzburg (2000, p. 339) list three additional mechanisms and point out the existence of more. All of these mechanisms lead to different models. Making a model without consideration of which mechanism applies makes it impossible to predict responses to environmental change or the nature of evolutionary change in predatory or anti-predatory traits. Knowing the basic mechanism for predator-dependence of the response is needed to address these questions. For example, does defending against one type of predator increase or decrease vulnerability to the second predator type? Ecological theory requires a diversity of more complex models, rather than simpler ones, if it is to describe natural rather than simplified laboratory systems. The simplest versions of such models should have functional response forms that are tailored to the known or suspected mechanism of predator- (and other species-) dependence.

Other trophic levels and additional predators; the missing components of functional response formulas

The main theme of this article is the excessive focus on 1-predator-1-prey interactions in studying trophic functions (i.e., functional responses). The functional response literature has very little consideration of species/resources occupying trophic levels above and below that of the focal predator-prey pair. It has long been known that one or both of these may influence the predator's functional response in a focal predator-prey pair by affecting the predator's foraging behavior (Sih, 1980; Abrams, 1982, 1984; Lima and Dill, 1990). The evidence for such effects has increased steadily over the years (Lima, 1998; Werner and Peacor, 2003; Preisser et al., 2005; Creel and Christianson, 2008; Peckarsky et al., 2008; Loeuille, 2010; Allen et al., 2022). Nevertheless, even relatively comprehensive reviews of functional responses have largely ignored the effects of abundances at trophic levels above or below the predator-prey pair (e.g., DeLong, 2021; Novak and Stouffer, 2021a,b; Stouffer and Novak, 2021; Uiterwaal et al., 2022). Having adaptive behavior of the two intermediate species in a four-species food chain in theory allows effects of species that are three levels removed from the focal species (Abrams, 1992a). This scenario also can produce reversal in the sign of the immediate effect of increased density of one species on its predator or its prey (Abrams, 1992a).

Even without considering additional trophic levels, the presence of a second predator species or type on the same trophic level as the first one will usually alter a prey's functional response to its own resource(s) as well as altering the functional response of the first predator. The existence and nature of these effects again depends on the nature of the prey's correlation between food/resource intakes and exposure to the different predator types. There are many possibilities here (Matsuda et al., 1994, 1996), so they do not lead to a single functional response formula. As a result, the analyses of such systems have usually addressed other issues, such as the stability of the dynamics produced by the adaptive foraging, and have not called attention to the implications for functional response theory.

Adaptive foraging under predation risk is not the only way that other trophic levels can influence a functional response. Holt and Barfield (2013) pointed out that, in many systems with plant, herbivore, and carnivore trophic levels, the abundance of the plant influences the ability of the carnivore to detect herbivores. This often involves a decreased ability of the herbivore to detect the predator in the presence of abundant vegetation. In other cases, the plant may produce chemicals that attract predators when it is itself under attack by the herbivores. Both of these mechanisms make the predator's functional response to herbivores an increasing function of plant abundance. Holt and Barfield's (2013) used simple models to show that such interactions can destabilize simple food chains or lead to alternative equilibria.

Given the long history of work documenting the effects of predators on prey activity and feeding, it seems strange that there has not been more effort devoted to exploring the effect of predators on the prey species' functional response(s) to their own foods. One possibility for this continuing neglect is the assumption that the effects of higher-level predators are independent of the effects of prey density on the mid-level predator's functional responses to its foods. In this case, higher-level predators would reduce the lower-level predator's food intake by the same proportion, regardless of food abundance. However, this is not what models of adaptive behavior suggest (Abrams, 1984, 1992a, 1995; Liu et al., 2014). The model discussed in the following paragraphs is another, even simpler example of the impacts of adaptive foraging under predation risk on the prey's functional response.

If the abundance of higher-level predators led to a relatively uniform decrease in foraging by their prey, this would not change the shape of the prey's functional response to resources. However, even the simplest possible model of adaptive prey foraging under predation risk (i.e., one with linear functional and numerical responses for a fixed prey behavior) predicts that different predator abundances will alter the shape of the prey's functional response to its resource, when prey foraging implies increased predation risk. Assume that the prey and predator species have underlying linear functional responses in the absence of adaptive prey behavior. The prey's intake of food is assumed to increase linearly with foraging time (t), while predation risk increases faster than linearly. The latter is inevitable if some times of day entail more foraging risk than others. The abundance of predator, prey and resource are denoted P, N, and R. The prey's functional response to its resource is given by cRt, where c is a capture rate per unit resource abundance while a prey individual is foraging, and t is the fraction of available time spent foraging. Intake may include some resource capture independent of active foraging (at a rate, c0R) and intake while foraging (c1tR). There may be other sources of increased mortality with foraging and these may also increase non-linearly (here assumed to increase quadratically, d1t + d2t2). The per capita capture rate by a predator individual increases with the prey foraging time, with a per capita attack rate given by, s0 + s1t +s2t.2 A simple expression for the per capita growth rate of the prey is thus,

b(c0+c1t)R-d0-(d1t+d2t2)-(s0+s1t+s2t2)P    (1)

Solving for the foraging time t that maximizes fitness yields,

t0=bc1R-d1-s1P2(d2+s2P)    (2)

I assume this optimum is reached very rapidly relative to population changes. I also assume that P is not large enough to make the optimum zero, although this is a potential aspect of the functional response. Substituting this optimal foraging time into the expressions for the prey's functional response and the predator's functional response yields the following expression for the prey's functional response to its resource:

(c0+c1(bc1R-s1P)2(d2+s2P))R    (3)

Note that this is no longer linear in R, and that it is a decreasing function of P. Substituting the optimal time into the expression for the predator's functional response, (s0 + s1t +s2t2)N, yields,

N[s0+(bc1Rd1s1P)(bc1s2R+s1s2P+2d2s1d1s2)4(d2+s2P)2]    (4)

This expression is still linear in N, but it is affected non-linearly by both R and P. Abrams (1984) presents another simple example in which increases in resource abundance can reduce prey foraging, changing the sign of the effect of resource abundance on the top-predator's functional response. Neither that model nor the one presented above is meant to be a realistic model of any particular system. However, the dependence of functional responses on the abundances of multiple trophic levels is a general feature of situations with a tradeoff between food intake and predation risk (Abrams, 1992a, 1995; Abrams and Vos, 2003).

Theory such as this suggests that behavioral responses to foraging reward and risk lead to effects of species on non-adjacent trophic levels on each other's functional responses. The obvious question is why these higher and low levels have not appeared in the functional responses of most community and food web models. The most likely explanation is the lack of experiments that have measured responses in more than a single pair of species. On a practical level, predators usually have greater space requirements than their prey. As a result, sufficient replication of treatments with a range of different, but realistic predator abundances is not consistent with the space and time limitations of most laboratory environments. This is discussed further in the final section Reshaping functional response theory. Another practical issue is the likely dependence of anti-predator responses to the duration of the functional response measurement, an issue discussed in the next section.

The non-instantaneous nature of functional responses

Barraquand (2014, p. 3) ends the abstract of his article on functional responses with this statement: “The loose connection of ODE models with empirical data and spatial/temporal scaling up of empirical measurements constitute more serious challenges to our understanding of trophic interactions and their consequences on ecosystem functioning.” Even in the absence of adaptive behavioral change based on the abundances of other species, functional responses are often imperfectly represented by ordinary differential equations (ODEs) based solely on current population densities. Modeling of anti-predator behavior has long incorporated dynamics for learned anti-predator behaviors (Dill, 1973). This study used a simulation model, and most subsequent work has been analytical. Incorporating dynamic behaviors in the context of the ODE models that are typically used in population/community ecology requires that behaviors themselves be represented by specific dynamical equations, as in Abrams (2000, 2010a) and Abrams et al. (2007). The presence of behavioral dynamics raises the possibility that those traits do not reach a stable equilibrium, particularly in multi-species systems (Abrams et al., 2007). This would mean that the functional response in those cases may not even be approximated by an expression that only involves current abundances. Individual-based simulation models could incorporate more detailed schemes for the dynamics of behavior, such as the “Sarsa” model of learning (Sutton and Barto, 2018; McNamara and Leimar, 2020). Nevertheless, in some simple types of predation based on rapid changes in satiation or hunger, functional responses based solely on current abundances are likely to be a good approximation.

Many of the influences on consumption rates discussed in this article are not instantaneous, and can have large stochastic components. In addition, the full effect is likely to be delayed. Holling's suggested learning mechanism in his type III functional response implies that, if a given prey density drops to a value lower than it had previously, the initial consumption rate will be greater than its quasi-equilibrium value for that lower prey density. On the other hand, if the prey density is increased very rapidly, the capture rate will initially be lower than its quasi-equilibrium, because the predator has had relatively little recent practice capturing this prey. These lags were not part of Holling's original formulas, which implied instantaneous change in predator consumption with prey density. It is also possible that rapid behavioral dynamics produces population cycles; this is the case when an effectively type II response is produced from a linear one by behavioral or rapid evolutionary change, as in Abrams (1992b).

In spite of these possibilities, there are likely to be many cases in which the behavioral change is rapid enough to assume a quasi-equilibrium with respect to current abundances. However, it is desirable to consider the possibility of significant lags, particularly when the abundances influencing the benefits or costs of foraging cannot be quickly and accurately estimated by the consumer.

The next section will propose some steps that could be taken to change the direction of current theory regarding trophic functions so that it is more in keeping with the opening quotation from Holling (1965). An alternative representation of functional responses is to break them down into dynamic components that define the foraging and anti-predator strategies of all the species involved. Even when this approach is not adopted, the “species involved” should include many others in addition to the focal predator and prey. Such a model may also need dynamic variables describing different nutrient stores in the predator or nutrient contents in the different prey/food types. There have been other recent calls for breaking trophic interactions into components for modeling (e.g., Wootton et al., 2021). However most of these (including Wootton et al., 2021) do not consider adaptively changing components of species other than the focal predator-prey pair.

Reshaping functional response theory

At the current time we have very few if any models that have predicted the dynamics of ecological systems in the field over many generations. It will obviously be quite a while before such models are both developed and tested. In the meantime, theory can contribute to this ultimate goal by identifying the types of effects and processes that might be important in determining the form of such models. These processes will need to include a more flexible concept of functional responses than the current focus on pairwise interactions influenced only by immediate predator and prey densities. As suggested in the previous section, this may require that the parameters of current functional responses, such as attack rates or handling times, be expressed as dynamic variables. Below is a list of six more specific changes or additions to current functional response theory.

1. It is necessary to have a better understanding of the relationship between functional and numerical responses, as the latter determines how the former affects an organism's fitness.

Our understanding of numerical responses is much more limited than our understanding of functional responses. It is commonly assumed in differential equation models of interacting species that mortality is independent of food consumption, and that reproduction is a simple increasing function of food consumption rate (or consumption rate of the limiting nutrient). However, mortality is in fact affected by food consumption in many systems (Montagnes et al., 2019), and there are few studies in animals quantifying how intake rates of different resources affect the different components of fitness.

Most ecologists would agree that the vast majority of consumers require more than a single resource, and that there are limits to consumption rates. Even animals that are highly specialized on a single food are likely to have other requirements (water sources for terrestrial animals; areas with cover from predation; nesting sites), and it is common for such “other factors” to be limited in abundance. Many of these categories constitute “essential” or nearly essential resources, and “consuming” some of these categories interferes with consuming other categories. However, an additional resource from a different nutritional category is generally expected to have a different effect on a consumer's functional response to a particular resource than is a second resource from the same nutritional category. If a simplified representation is being used, some of the “different category” resources may be approximated by a direct negative effect of predator abundance on its per capita growth rate.

2. The taxonomic and ecological range of functional response studies should be broadened.

The limited range of species used in functional response studies is illustrated by the comprehensive database by Uiterwaal et al. (2022). Functional response theory should not be restricted to “predators” in the narrow sense of animals that eat other animals. Holling did not require that the food items be living entities, as was clear from his famous experiment with blindfolded humans searching for paper disks on a table top. However, the consumption of nutrients by plants or fungi as well as non-living material by detritivores have been largely ignored in the functional response literature. Tilman's (1982) book on resource competition (based on plants) did not mention functional responses by name. The models used in that book assumed that the uptake rate of a single resource by a consumer was proportional to the consumer per capita population growth plus the consumer per capita death or loss rate. In practice this was equivalent to assuming a type II functional response to the single limiting resource, a linear numerical response to that limiting resource, with a mortality rate independent of nutrient intake. Theory involving plants and other consumers of non-living resources has used a very narrow range of functional forms for functional responses, usually following Tilman's (1982) effective assumption of a single-resource type II functional response to whichever resource type was limiting, and consumption of the other resources at a rate just sufficient to make them co-limiting. Tilman (1982) and later authors (e.g., Huisman and Weissing, 2001) followed León and Tumpson (1975), who first proposed this set of assumptions, but did not provide empirical support. The reduced uptake rates of other resources are assumed to have no effect on the uptake rate of the limiting resource. This assumption also appears not to have been tested in most systems.

In both plants and animals, there has been very little systematic exploration of the shapes of the relationships between other (non-resource) species abundances and foraging/uptake by a focal consumer species on a focal resource. Holt and Barfield's (2013) treatment of plant effects on predator-herbivore encounter rates also calls for more study of such cases. However, effects of species that are two-or-more trophic levels removed from the focal species have seldom if ever been examined with experiments having the same multiple-density treatments as in traditional functional response studies.

3. Functional response experiments with different durations should be carried out and compared.

Experimental duration is likely to have a larger impact on the functional response when the abundance being varied is that of a higher-level predator, rather than a prey species. The initial detection of a predator by a prey individual will typically not allow accurate estimation of predation risk. In such a case, the optimal strategy is usually for the prey individual to reduce its activity until a better estimate or risk can be obtained. Even without more information, the prey will eventually exhaust its energy (nutrient) reserves, and this will tend to increase the advantage of foraging, even if the risk is still unknown. This process will usually occur on a more rapid time scale than major changes in species abundances. Variation in the food available to the prey individual experiencing risk should also contribute to time-dependent responses in their own behavior. Because periods of high food abundance are not likely to persist indefinitely, an organism should initially have higher consumption rates than would be optimal if that food abundance were known to persist for a long time in the future. The consequence of these behavioral considerations is that experiments with a fixed, very short time interval (the usual basis of functional response measurements) may be misleading when using a time scale that is more relevant to population dynamics. One study examining the impact of the measurement duration for functional response shape is Li et al. (2018).

4. The effects of additional species on different trophic levels should be explored both theoretically and experimentally.

Most of the literature on predator functional responses that considers multiple species only addresses additional prey species. The difficulty of carrying out a sufficient number of experimental treatments to determine the shape of multi-variable functional responses has deterred studies with two or more predator types or different/multiple prey. However, even comparing two different abundances of two variables would give some insight into the qualitative nature of the interaction between these variables in determining the consumption rate. Abrams and Schmitz (1999) showed that the combination of high and low quality foods in habitats involving different predation risks produced several novel features in a time- and capacity- limited forager's functional response. Many other scenarios combining diet choice and predation risk have yet to be explored.

5. Adaptively flexible functional responses in models with three or more trophic levels and two or more species per level should be investigated.

Paradoxically, models of food webs containing many species are more likely to use the simplest possible (type I) functional responses than are models with only a few (five or fewer) species (Al-Khudhairy and Rossberg, 2022). Early food web models (e.g., May, 1973; Pimm, 1982) almost always assumed linear functional responses. A system with two adaptively flexible species on each of three trophic levels is likely to exhibit qualitatively different functional response forms than any of those discussed above. A relatively full investigation of such a model should offset the tendency of models of larger foodwebs to assume linear functional responses. Recent empirical work has suggested that a variety of different interference effects occur between different mammalian top predators (Engebretsen et al., 2021). Gobin et al. (2022) discuss several modifications of type II responses in the context of models of a relatively well-studied boreal system in which five vertebrate species were represented. They analyze food web properties of best-fit models with different functional response forms, and conclude that type II responses produce relative poor fits to observed population and food web data. This was at least in part due to the inability of this functional response formula to represent known aspects of consumption, including predator interference, switching, caching, and surplus killing.

6. More in-situ studies of functional responses are required.

There is little doubt that the difficulty of carrying out multi-species functional response studies on an appropriate spatial scale has contributed to their scarcity. This is particularly true of potential studies involving the effects of higher-level predators. The space and labor required for monitoring multiple species in an experimental setup is often prohibitive, and it is often impossible even come close to replicating field conditions, which usually include spatial heterogeneity. In situ experiments are likely to provide the best chance of studying the types of multi-species responses discussed here. These include experimental use of predator cues, such as scent or sound (e.g., Allen et al., 2022), or implanted sensors that can detect predation events (Wilmers et al., 2015; Cruz-Font et al., 2019).

The functional response literature has grown significantly in the past decade. This suggests that the time may be ripe for a broadening of the set of assumptions made about those responses. I am not alone in calling for a broadened scope for functional response studies. For example, Kratina et al. (2009, p. 425) opened their article with the following two sentences “Realistic functional responses are required for accurate model predictions at the community level. However, controversy remains regarding which type of dependencies need to be included in functional response models.” I would disagree with the idea that there was or is much controversy; the majority of studies still ignore dependency of functional responses on species other than the focal prey without presenting any justification for doing so.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

The research was funded by a Discovery Grant to Peter Abrams from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC).

Acknowledgments

I thank Thomas Hossie and Dennis Murray for inviting this article, and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada for their financial support. Thomas Hossie and two reviewers provided comments that significantly improved and broadened this manuscript. I am particularly indebted to my Ph.D. co-advisor, C. S. Holling for laying the groundwork for my later interest in functional responses.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abrams, P. A. (1982). Functional responses of optimal foragers. Am. Nat. 120, 382–390. doi: 10.1086/283996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1984). Foraging time optimization and interactions in food webs. Am. Nat. 124, 80–96. doi: 10.1086/284253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1987). The functional responses of adaptive consumers of two resources. Theor. Popul. Biol. 32, 262–288. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(87)90050-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1989). Decreasing functional responses as a result of adaptive consumer behavior. Evol. Ecol. 3, 95–114. doi: 10.1007/BF02270915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1990a). Mixed responses to resource densities and their implications for character displacement. Evol. Ecol. 4, 93–102. doi: 10.1007/BF02270907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1990b). The effects of adaptive behavior on the type 2 functional response. Ecology 71, 8778885. doi: 10.2307/1937359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1991a). Indirect effects generated by optimal foraging. Oikos 62, 167–176. doi: 10.2307/3545262

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1991b). Life history and the relationship between food availability and foraging effort. Ecology 72, 1242–1252. doi: 10.2307/1941098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1992a). Predators that benefit prey and prey that harm predators: unusual effects of interacting foraging adaptations. Am. Nat. 140, 573–600. doi: 10.1086/285429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1992b). Adaptive foraging by predators as a cause of predator-prey cycles. Evol. Ecol. 6, 56–72. doi: 10.1007/BF02285334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1994). The fallacies of ‘ratio-dependent’ predation. Ecology 75, 1842–1850. doi: 10.2307/1939644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1995). Implications of dynamically variable traits for identifying, classifying, and measuring direct and indirect effects in ecological communities. Am. Nat. 146, 112–134. doi: 10.1086/285789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (1997). Anomalous predictions of ratio-dependent models of predation. Oikos 80, 163–171. doi: 10.2307/3546527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (2000). The impact of habitat selection on the heterogeneity of resources in varying environments. Ecology 81, 2902–2913. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[2902:TIOHSO]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (2010a). Quantitative descriptions of resource choice in ecological models. Pop. Ecol. 52, 47–58. doi: 10.1007/s10144-009-0175-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (2010b). Implications of flexible foraging for interspecific interactions: lessons from simple models. Funct. Ecol. 24, 7–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01621.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A. (2015). Why ratio dependence is (still) a bad model of predation. Biol. Revs. 90, 794–814. doi: 10.1111/brv.12134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., Cressman, R., and Krivan, V. (2007). The role of behavioral dynamics in determining the patch distributions of interacting species. Am. Nat. 169, 505–518. doi: 10.1086/511963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Ginzburg, L. R. (2000). The nature of predation; prey dependent, ratio dependent or neither? Trends Ecol. Evol. 15, 337–341. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(00)01908-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., Hill, C., and Elmgren, R. (1990). The functional response of the predatory polychaete, Harmothoe sarsi, to the amphipod, Pontoporeia affinis. Oikos 59, 261–269. doi: 10.2307/3545543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Matsuda, H. (2003). Population dynamical consequences of switching at low total prey densities. Pop. Ecol. 45, 175–185. doi: 10.1007/s10144-003-0159-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Matsuda, H. (2004). Consequences of behavioral dynamics for the population dynamics of predator-prey systems with switching. Pop. Ecol. 46, 13–25. doi: 10.1007/s10144-003-0168-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Schmitz, O. J. (1999). The effect of risk of mortality on the foraging behaviour of animals faced with time and digestive capacity constraints. Evol. Ecol. Res. 1, 285–301.

Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Vos, M. (2003). Adaptation, density dependence, and the abundances of trophic levels. Evol. Ecol. Res. 5, 1113–1132.

Google Scholar

Abrams, P. A., and Wilson, W. G. (2004). Coexistence of competitors in metacommunities due to spatial variation in resource growth rates; does R* predict the outcome of competition? Ecol. Letts. 7, 929–940. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00644.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Khudhairy, O. U. G., and Rossberg, A. G. (2022). Evolution of prudent predation in complex food webs. Ecol. Letts. 25, 1055–1074. doi: 10.1111/ele.13979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M. C., Clinchy, M., and Zanette, L. Y. (2022). Fear of predators in free-living wildlife reduces population growth over generations. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 119, e2112404119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2112404119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anholt, B. R., Ludwig, D., and Rasmussen, J. B. (1987). Optimal pursuit times: how long should predators pursue their prey? Theor. Popul. Biol. 31, 453–464. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(87)90015-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arditi, R., and Ginzburg, L. (1989). Coupling in predator-prey dynamics: ratio dependence. J. Theor. Biol. 139, 311–326. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(89)80211-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arditi, R., and Ginzburg, L. (2012). How Species Interact. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Barraquand, F. (2014). Functional responses and predator-prey models: a critique of ratio dependence. Theor. Ecol. 7, 3–20. doi: 10.1007/s12080-013-0201-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beardsell, A., Gravel, D., Clermont, J., Berteaux, D., Gauthier, G., and Bêty, J. (2022). A mechanistic model of functional response provides new insights into indirect interactions among arctic tundra prey. Ecology 2022, e3734. doi: 10.1002/ecy.3734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beddington, J. R. (1975). Mutual interference between parasites or predators and its effect on searching efficiency. J. Anim. Ecol. 44, 331–340. doi: 10.2307/3866

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkhout, B. W., and Morozov, A. (2022). Assassin snails (Anentome helena) as a biological model for exploring the effects of individual specialization within generalist predators. PLoS ONE 17, e0264996. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesson, P. L. (1984). Variable predators and switching behavior. Theor. Popul. Biol. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(84)90021-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosner, C., DeAngelis, D. L., Ault, J. S., and Olson, D. B. (1999). Effects of spatial grouping on the functional response of predators. Theor. Popul. Biol. 56, 65–75. doi: 10.1006/tpbi.1999.1414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creel, S., and Christianson, D. (2008). Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. TREE 23, 194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowley, P. H., and Martin, E. K. (1989). Functional responses and interference within and between year classes of a dragonfly population. J. N. Am. Benthol. Soc. 8, 211–221. doi: 10.2307/1467324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cruz-Font, L., Shuter, B. J., Blanchfield, P. J., Minns, C. K., and Rennie, M. D. (2019). Life at the top: lake ecotype influences the foraging pattern, metabolic costs and life history of an apex fish predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 88, 702–716. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Roos, A. M., and Persson, L. (2013). Population and Community Ecology of Ontogenetic Development. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

DeAngelis, D. L., Goldstein, R. A., and O'Neill, R. V. (1975). A model for trophic interaction. Ecology 56, 881–892. doi: 10.2307/1936298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLong, J. P. (2021). Predator Ecology: Evolutionary Ecology of the Functional Response. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

DeLong, J. P., Uiterwaal, S. F., and Dell, A. I. (2021). Trait-based variation in the foraging performance of individuals. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 649542. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.649542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dill, L. M. (1973). An avoidance learning submodel for a general predation model. Oecologia 13, 291–312. doi: 10.1007/BF01825523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engebretsen, K. N., Beckmann, J. P., Lackey, C. W., Andreasen, A., Schroeder, C., Jackson, P., et al. (2021). Recolonizing carnivores: is cougar predation behaviorally mediated by bears? Ecol. Evol. 11, 5331–5343. doi: 10.1002/ece3.7424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fryxell, J. M., and Lundberg, P. (1994). Diet choice and predator-prey dynamics. Evol. Ecol. 8, 407–421 doi: 10.1007/BF01238191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginzburg, L. R., and Damuth, J. (2022). The issue isn't which model of consumer interference is right, but which one is least wrong. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10, 860542. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.860542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobin, J., Hossie, T. J., Derbyshire, R. E., Sennega, S., Cambridge, T. W., Scholl, L., et al. (2022). Functional responses shape node and network level properties of a simplified boreal food web. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10, 898805. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.898805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holling, C. S. (1959). The components of predation as revealed by a study of small mammal predation of the European pine sawfly. Can. Entomol. 91, 293–320. doi: 10.4039/Ent91293-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holling, C. S. (1965). The functional response of predators to prey and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 455, 1–60. doi: 10.4039/entm9745fv

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holling, C. S. (1966). The functional response of invertebrate predators to prey density. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 48, 1–86. doi: 10.4039/entm9848fv

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holling, C. S., and Buckingham, S. (1976). A behavioral model of predator-prey functional responses. Behav. Sci. 21, 183–195. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830210305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt, R. D., and Barfield, M. (2013). Direct plant-predator interactions as determinants of food chain dynamics. J. Theor. Ecol. 339, 47–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.04.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hossie, T. J., Chan, K., and Murray, D. L. (2021). Increasing availability of palatable prey induces predator-dependence and increases predation on unpalatable prey. Sci. Repts. 11, 6763. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-86080-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huisman, J., and Weissing, F. J. (2001). Biological conditions for oscillations and chaos generated by multispecies competition. Ecology 82, 2682–2695. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2001)082[2682:BCFOAC]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeschke, J., and Tollrian, R. (2005). Effects of predator confusion on functional responses. Oikos 111, 547–555. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2005.14118.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeschke, J. M., Kopp, M., and Tollrian, R. (2002). Predator functional responses: discriminating between handling and digesting prey. Ecol. Monog. 72, 95–112. doi: 10.1890/0012-9615(2002)072[0095:PFRDBH]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeschke, J. M., Kopp, M., and Tollrian, R. (2004). Consumer-food systems: why type I functional responses are exclusive to filter feeders. Biol. Revs. 79, 337–349. doi: 10.1017/S1464793103006286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juliano, S. A., Goughnour, J. A., and Ower, G. D. (2022). Predation in many dimensions: spatial context is important for meaningful functional response experiments. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10, 845560. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.845560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kratina, P., Vos, M., and Anholt, B. R. (2007). Species diversity modulates predation. Ecology 88, 1917–1923. doi: 10.1890/06-1507.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kratina, P., Vos, M., Bateman, A., and Anholt, B. R. (2009). Functional responses modified by predator density. Oecologia 159, 425–433. doi: 10.1007/s00442-008-1225-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

León, J. A., and Tumpson, D. B. (1975). Competition between two species for two complementary or substitutable resources. J. Theor. Biol. 50, 185–201. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90032-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Rall, B. C., and Kalinkat, G. (2018). Experimental duration and predator satiation levels systematically affect functional response parameters. Oikos 127, 590–598. doi: 10.1111/oik.04479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, S. L. (1998). Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. BioScience. 48, 25–34. doi: 10.2307/1313225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, S. L., and Dill, L. M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 68, 619–640. doi: 10.1139/z90-092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, R., DeAngelis, D. L., and Bryant, J. P. (2014). Dynamics of herbivores and resources on a landscape with interspersed resources and refuges. Theor. Ecol. 7, 195–208. doi: 10.1007/s12080-013-0210-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeuille, N. (2010). Consequences of adaptive foraging in diverse communities. Funct. Ecol. 24, 18–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01617.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, B. O., Abrams, P. A., and Brassil, C. E. (2003). Dynamic vs. instantaneous models of diet choice. Am. Nat. 162, 668–684. doi: 10.1086/378783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H. (1985). Evolutionarily stable strategies for predator switching. J. Theor. Biol. 115, 351–366. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80197-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H., Hori, M., and Abrams, P. (1996). Effects of predator-specific defense on biodiversity and community complexity in two-trophic-level communities. Evol. Ecol. 10, 13–28. doi: 10.1007/BF01239343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H., Hori, M., and Abrams, P. A. (1994). Effects of predator-specific defense on community complexity. Evol. Ecol. 8, 628–638. doi: 10.1007/BF01237846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H., and Namba, T. (1991). Food web graph of a coevolutionarily stable community. Ecology 72, 267–276. doi: 10.2307/1938920

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, R. M. (1973). Stability and Complexity in Model Ecosystems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

McNamara, J. M., and Leimar, O. (2020). Game Theory in Biology. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

Google Scholar

Montagnes, D. J. S., Zhu, X., Gu, L., Sun, Y., Wang, J., Horner, R., et al. (2019). False exclusion: a case to embed predator performance in classical population models. Am. Nat. 194, 654–670. doi: 10.1086/705381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdoch, W. W. (1969). Switching in general predators: experiments on predator specificity and stability of prey populations. Ecol. Monogr. 39, 335–354. doi: 10.2307/1942352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murdoch, W. W., and Oaten, A. (1975). Predation and population stability. Adv. Ecol. Res. 9, 1–131. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2504(08)60288-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novak, M., and Stouffer, D. B. (2021a). Systematic bias in studies of consumer functional responses. Ecol. Lett. 24, 580–593. doi: 10.1111/ele.13660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novak, M., and Stouffer, D. B. (2021b). Geometric complexity and the information-theoretic comparison of functional response models. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 740362. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.740362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, A., and Murdoch, W. W. (1975). Switching, functional response and stability in predator-prey systems. Am. Nat. 109, 299–318. doi: 10.1086/282999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peckarsky, B. L., Abrams, P. A., Bolnick, D. I., Dill, L. M., Grabowski, J. H., Luttbeg, B., et al. (2008). Rewriting the textbooks: considering non-consumptive effects in classic studies of predator-prey interactions. Ecology 89, 2416–2425. doi: 10.1890/07-1131.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pimm, S. L. (1982). Food Webs. London: Chapman and Hall.

Google Scholar

Preisser, E. L., Bolnick, D. I., and Benard, M. F. (2005). Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86, 501–509. doi: 10.1890/04-0719

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R., and Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Q. Rev. Biol. 52, 137–154. doi: 10.1086/409852

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoener, T. W. (1971). Theory of feeding strategies. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 11, 369–404. doi: 10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih, A. (1980). Optimal behavior: can foragers balance two conflicting demands? Science 210, 1041–1043. doi: 10.1126/science.210.4473.1041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skalski, G. T., and Gilliam, J. W. (2001). Functional responses with predator interference: viable alternatives to the Holling Type II model. Ecology 82, 3083–3092. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2001)082[3083:FRWPIV]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smout, S., Asseburg, C., Matthiopoulos, J., Fernández, C., Redpath, S., Thirgood, S., et al. (2010). The functional response of a generalist predator. PLoS ONE. 5, e10761. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stouffer, D. B., and Novak, M. (2021). Hidden layers of density dependence in consumer feeding rates. Ecol. Lett. 24, 520–532. doi: 10.1111/ele.13670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, R. S., and Barto, A. G. (2018). Reinforcement Learning: an Introduction, 2nd Edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Tilman, D. (1982). Resource Competition and Community Structure. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Tyutyunov, Y. V., and Titova, L. I. (2021). Ratio-dependence in predator-prey systems as an edge and basic minimal model of predator interference. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 725041. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.725041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uiterwaal, S. F., and DeLong, J. P. (2018). Multiple factors, including arena size, shape the functional responses of ladybird beetles. J. Appl. Ecol. 55, 2429–2438. doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.13159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uiterwaal, S. F., Lagerstrom, I. T., Lyon, S. R., and DeLong, J. P. (2022). FoRAGE database: a compilation of functional responses for consumers and parasitoids. Ecology 103, e3706. doi: 10.1002/ecy.3706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Leeuwen, E., Brannstrom, A., Jansen, V. A. A., Dieckmann, U., and Rossberg, A. G. (2013). A generalized functional response for predators that switch between multiple prey species. J. Theor. Biol. 328, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, E. E., and Peacor, S. (2003). A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84, 1083–1100. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1083:AROTII]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilmers, C. C., Nickel, B., Bryce, C. N., Smith, J. A., Wheat, R. E., and Yovovich, V. (2015). The golden age of bio-logging: how animal-borne sensors are advancing the frontiers of ecology. Ecology 96, 1741–1753. doi: 10.1890/14-1401.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wootton, K. L., Curtsdotter, A., Roslin, T., Bommarco, R., and Jonsson, T. (2021). Towards a modular theory of trophic interactions. Funct. Ecol. 2021, 1–18. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.13954

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adaptive behavior, consumer-resource interaction, feeding, food web, functional response, predation, switching

Citation: Abrams PA (2022) Food web functional responses. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10:984384. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.984384

Received: 02 July 2022; Accepted: 22 July 2022;
Published: 09 August 2022.

Edited by:

Thomas John Hossie, Trent University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Donald DeAngelis, United States Geological Survey (USGS), United States
John Paul DeLong, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, United States

Copyright © 2022 Abrams. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter A. Abrams, cGV0ZXIuYWJyYW1zJiN4MDAwNDA7dXRvcm9udG8uY2E=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.