Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 29 April 2022
Sec. Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology
This article is part of the Research Topic What Sensory Ecology Might Learn From Landscape Ecology? View all 13 articles

Aeroscapes and the Sensory Ecology of Olfaction in a Tropical Dry Forest

\r\nAllegra DePasquale*Allegra DePasquale1*Jeremy D. HoganJeremy D. Hogan1Christopher Guadamuz ArayaChristopher Guadamuz Araya2Nathaniel J. DominyNathaniel J. Dominy3Amanda D. Melin,*\r\nAmanda D. Melin1,4*
  • 1Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 2Área de Conservación Guanacaste, La Cruz, Costa Rica
  • 3Department of Anthropology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
  • 4Department of Medical Genetics, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada

Aeroscapes—dynamic patterns of air speed and direction—form a critical component of landscape ecology by shaping numerous animal behaviors, including movement, foraging, and social and/or reproductive interactions. Aeroecology is particularly critical for sensory ecology: air is the medium through which many sensory signals and cues propagate, inherently linking sensory perception to variables such as air speed and turbulence. Yet, aeroscapes are seldom explicitly considered in studies of sensory ecology and evolution. A key first step towards this goal is to describe the aeroscapes of habitats. Here, we quantify the variation in air movement in two successional stages (early and late) of a tropical dry forest in Costa Rica. We recorded air speeds every 10 seconds at five different heights simultaneously. Average air speeds and turbulence increased with height above the ground, generally peaked midday, and were higher overall at the early successional forest site. These patterns of lower air speed and turbulence at ground level and overnight have important implications for olfactory foraging niches, as chemotaxis is most reliable when air movement is low and steady. We discuss our results in the context of possible selective pressures and observed variation in the foraging ecology, behaviors, and associated morphologies of resident vertebrates, with a focus on mammals. However, these data also have relevance to researchers studying socioecology, invertebrate biology, plant evolution, community ecology and more. Further investigation into how animals use different forest types, canopy heights and partition activities across different times of day will further inform our understanding of how landscape and sensory ecology are interrelated. Finally, we emphasize the timeliness of monitoring aeroecology as global wind patterns shift with climate change and human disturbance alters forest structure, which may have important downstream consequences for biological conservation.

Introduction

Air is a dynamic and ever-changing medium, and aeroscapes (defined as patterns in air speed and direction; Vogel, 1996) are an integral part of terrestrial ecosystems. Efforts to integrate aeroscapes into the study of organismal behavior and ecology (collectively termed aeroecology; Kunz et al., 2008; Diehl, 2013) have revealed that animals react adaptively to variables such as air speed and turbulence (Frick et al., 2013; Diehl et al., 2017). These same factors are expected to have pronounced effects on the propagation and uptake of sensory information (Finelli et al., 2000; Muller-Schwarze, 2006). For example, greater air speeds will disperse the odors of plants and animals farther, but the resulting turbulence is likely to disrupt the spatial distribution of odor plumes, challenging the ability of organisms to navigate toward the odor source (i.e., anemotaxis; Murlis, 1997; Conover, 2007; Bingman and Moore, 2017). This tradeoff in signal propagation and efficacy is well-studied in insects, which optimize their flight paths in response to air speed while tracking odor plumes (Aluja et al., 1993; Cardé and Willis, 2008; Hennessy et al., 2020). At the same time, many mammals possess relatively complex olfactory systems, and they, too, are sensitive to variations in the aeroscape (Moulton, 1967; Svensson et al., 2014). For example, air speed is known to affect the olfactory orientation and behavior of carnivorans—red foxes, striped skunks, raccoons, polar bears, domestic dogs (Ruzicka and Conover, 2011, 2012; Togunov et al., 2017; Jinn et al., 2020)—as well as primates, such as ring-tailed lemurs (Cunningham et al., 2021). Still, there has been little effort to explore the spatiotemporal factors that govern a given aeroscape, or the effects of this variability on the aero-sensory ecology of mammals, especially in forest ecosystems.

Forests are complex habitats in the vertical and horizontal planes (Ennos, 1997), and this level of heterogeneity is reflected in the form of highly variable aeroscapes (Baynton, 1969; Heydel et al., 2014). For example, the understory is essentially sheltered from the winds affecting the upper canopy, which can create striking vertical disparities in air speeds (Aoki et al., 1978; McCay, 2003). Scant air movement in the understory is expected to favor efficient anemotaxis, but the magnitude of vertical variation in an aeroscape can be offset temporally—e.g., at night, when air is cooler and moving at diminished speed—or spatially as a function of standing forest biomass (Murlis et al., 2000; McCay, 2003). The essential limitation is that these factors are rarely measured simultaneously or folded into our understanding of mammalian aero-sensory ecology and evolution.

To contribute toward building this literature, we studied spatiotemporal variation in the aeroscape of a lowland tropical dry forest in Costa Rica. We recorded variation in air speed and turbulence as a function of: (1) vertical position and (2) diel periodicity. We measured each of these variables at two sites, chosen to reflect two habitat types—early and late successional forest. Our goal is to better understand how aeroscapes vary within a forest, and how this variation might mediate the distribution of odorant molecules through the aeroscape. This information is essential for understanding the selective pressures that have shaped the olfactory anatomy and behaviors of resident animals, as well as plant reproductive strategies.

Materials and Methods

Study Site

We collected data in Sector Santa Rosa of the Área de Conservación Guanacaste, northwestern Costa Rica. The site is a tropical dry forest with two distinct seasons: a dry season from December through May and a wet season from June through November (Campos, 2018; Janzen and Hallwachs, 2020; Melin et al., 2020). Sector Santa Rosa forests are primarily secondary, stemming from restoration and reforestation efforts that began in the 1970s (Janzen and Hallwachs, 2020), and forest composition and structure varies between early and late successional stages. Canopy height ranges from 6 to 15 m depending on successional stage; the canopy is typically taller in areas of later succession (Kalacska et al., 2004; Powers et al., 2009).

Data Collection

We used cup anemometers (WL-11; Scarlet Tech, Taipei, Taiwan) to collect air speed data from May to June 2021. The instruments have a sensitivity range of 0.6–50 m/s, a resolution of 0.1 m/s, and an accuracy of ±2%, per manufacturer specifications. We built and erected two scaffold towers: one in a late successional forest (10.838617, −85.614283; 1,086 m a.s.l.), and the other in an early successional forest (10.839383, −85.616383; 906 m a.s.l.). To each tower, we affixed five anemometers at heights of 0.5, 3.5, 5.5, 7.5, and 10 m (Figure 1). The devices were set to data-logging mode, recording average and maximum air speed in 10-s intervals.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scaffold system used to fix five anemometers per tower at heights of 0.5, 3.5, 5.5, 7.5, and 10 m. The anemometers were moved between our early successional forest site and our late successional forest site at 2 week intervals.

Data Analysis

We obtained the mean, median, and standard deviation of air speed for: (1) each sampling height and (2) time of day. We calculated air turbulence as standard deviation of air speed/mean air speed (McCay, 2003). The sensitivity threshold (0.6 m/s) of our anemometer risks a systematic bias against low air speeds, especially in the understory. Accordingly, we imputed values below this limit by using survival analyses, a method developed for the health sciences but adopted for analyzing environmental data with detection limits (Helsel, 2004). Using the survival (Therneau, 2021) and NADA (Lee, 2020) packages in R (v. 4.1.0., R Core Team, 2021), we constructed Kaplan–Meier estimates for data recorded at each site independently for each height and time of day, considering air speeds ≤0.6 m/s to be left-censored.

To detect significant differences in air speeds and turbulence as a function of vertical height and time of day, we conducted Cox proportional hazard modeling via the survival package in R. As these hazard models are designed for right-censored data, we transformed our data by using the “flipping” method of Helsel (2004), which subtracts all observed values from a constant to convert left-censored data to right-censored. Since the same column of wind is recorded simultaneously at all 5 observed heights for a given site, there is a high degree of collinearity between the height and time of day predictor variables, therefore we modeled each separately and present two models. To address our first aim, we modeled air speed as a function of vertical position, based on height from the ground (“Height”) in two different habitat types. To address our second aim, we modeled air speed as a function of diel periodicity (“Time of Day”) in two different habitat types. For both models, the outcome variable was the Kaplan–Meier estimate of average wind speed, and an interaction term of study site and height, or study site and time of day, was included as a predictor variable. For the Height model, the continuous variable distance from the ground (in meters) was used, while for the Time of Day model, because of the circular nature of temporal data, we employed a sinusoidal model with separate cos and sin terms (Simmons, 1990; Cazelles et al., 2008). Code for all analyses can be found at https://github.com/allegradepasquale/wind_speed_project.git.

Results

Vertical Variation

Air speeds increased as a positive function of vertical height, and mean air speeds differed between the study sites, with greater mean speeds recorded in the early successional forest (Figure 2 and Table 1). The interaction term of study site and height was also significant (Table 1). Variation in air turbulence followed a similar pattern: mean air turbulence scores varied as a positive function of vertical height and were greater in the early successional forest (Figure 2). Site differences in our measure of air turbulence were pronounced near ground level (0.5 m), but values tended to converge with increasing vertical height (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Average air speeds (±standard deviation) and turbulence score at different heights from the ground for our early successional and late successional forest sites, generated from Kaplan–Meier estimates.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of cox proportional hazard models for mean air speed by vertical position and time of day (i.e., diel cycle).

Temporal (Diel) Variation

Air speed varied significantly with time of day: winds were stronger from late morning to early afternoon and were lowest overnight (Figures 3, 4). As with our analyses of height, the main effect of study site, as well as the interaction between study site and time of day, were also significant (Figure 4 and Table 1). Turbulence scores were highest at midday and lowest overnight.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Maximum air speeds in meters per second at our early and late successional sites over the diel cycle. Black dots represent turbulence scores.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean air speeds at two forest sites as a function vertical height and diel periodicity. The x-axis begins at 00:00 h (midnight) and ends at 23:00 h (11:00 pm).

Discussion

Air movement is a dynamic aspect of landscape ecology that shapes the way animals experience the world. In this study, we quantified air movement in a tropical dry forest as a function of vertical position and diel periodicity in two forest types. We found that air speed and turbulence increased with height, peaked midday, and were lower in the late successional forest. Taken together, our findings suggest spatiotemporal predictability in the aeroscape of a tropical forest and motivate a discussion of how animals adapt to and exploit these patterns.

Vertical Variation in Aeroscapes

We detected a steep increase in air speeds as a function of vertical height, a result that replicates findings from other forest ecosystems (Baynton, 1969; Oliver and Mayhead, 1974; Aoki et al., 1978; Kruijt et al., 2000; McCay, 2003). The prevailing explanation for this pattern is that air movement is impeded near the ground by understory vegetation, tree trunks, and surface topography; however, the implications of such a gradient for olfactory ecology are underexplored. Fruits, for example, emit odors (generally lightweight, ephemeral volatile and semivolatile organic compounds) that are easily dispersed by air movement (Rodríguez et al., 2013; Nevo et al., 2018, 2020), although excessive air speeds can over-disperse odor compounds and disrupt anemotaxis (Svensson et al., 2014). Still, it follows that the vertical position of fruit will determine the probability and efficiency of long-distance odor detection and foraging by seed-dispersing mutualists (Santana et al., 2021). Anemotaxis toward food resources is well-studied among invertebrates (Zjacic and Scholz, 2022) and increasingly so among mammals. For example, experiments with bats have elicited klinotaxis in response to fruit odors (Thies et al., 1998; Korine and Kalko, 2005; Leiser-Miller et al., 2020; Brokaw et al., 2021; Brokaw and Smotherman, 2021), and a field experiment by Fleming et al. (1977) found that bats will deviate from their flyways by as much as 50 m to acquire fruits mounted to 1.5-m poles set up moments before sunset. Further, experiments with coatis and ring-tailed lemurs have shown that they can detect fruit from distances up to 20 m (Hirsch, 2010; Cunningham et al., 2021). These findings suggest that aero-sensory ecology via stimulus response can complement and extend the critical importance of spatial memory to the localization of foods (Janson, 1998; Janmaat et al., 2014; Dahmani et al., 2018). Looking forward, it stands to reason that our understanding of cognitive ecology will only be strengthened as we incorporate and integrate the systematic study of aeroscapes and other sensory landscapes into research frameworks.

Many animals use olfactory signals to communicate with one another for reproduction, dominance, and territory defense, as well as to discriminate conspecifics from heterospecifics. In mammals, these occur mainly through the deposition of scent marks (Johnson, 1973; Irwin et al., 2004; Kollikowski et al., 2019). Our results suggest that olfactory signals may remain more localized when deposited nearer to the ground than higher in the canopy. It is noteworthy, then, that olfactory communication appears to be particularly prevalent in terrestrial mammals, which have olfactory receptor gene repertoires that have undergone three times as much gene duplication than those of volant, arboreal, and aquatic mammals (Hughes et al., 2018). At the same time, low air speeds will limit long-distance dispersal of odors. It is possible that species and habitat-specific optima for olfactory signal height exist and may vary across taxa. For example, Ethiopian wolves, gray wolves, and pine martens use raised-leg urination, a behavior thought to reinforce territorial boundaries by dispersal of urinary odor plumes. Urine deposition above, rather than on, the ground may improve scent dispersal (Peters and Mech, 1975; Macdonald, 1980; Pulliainen, 1981; Alberts, 1992; Sillero-Zubiri and Macdonald, 1998). Turning to another mammalian radiation, many primate species that have evolved glands dedicated for scent deposition, including ring-tailed lemurs, mandrills, drills, and sifakas, are largely terrestrial (Delbarco-Trillo et al., 2011; Drea, 2015; Vaglio et al., 2016). Systematic study of olfactory signaling and receiving behaviors, along with investigation of co-occurring anatomical and genetic variation, in taxa occupying different vertical niches will allow this hypothesis to be tested. In general, the intersection of sensory and aeroecology holds untapped potential for better understanding the wide variation in animal sensory traits.

Temporal (Diel) Variation in Aeroscapes

We detected pronounced variation across the 24-h day, with peak and nadir air movements at midday and overnight, respectively. This pattern is almost certainly due to ambient temperature flux because warmer air moves faster and is less stable (Pleijel et al., 1996; McCay, 2003; Monteith and Unsworth, 2013; but c.f. Baynton, 1968, 1969; Rapp and Silman, 2012 for other patterns). Following similar arguments to those concerning vertical variation, nocturnal aeroscapes may be preferential for animals that detect and localize scents. Many nocturnal animals rely more on olfaction than vision, but the reasons are usually couched in the language of constraint—there is scant light at night, so vision is limited (Barton et al., 1995; Balkenius et al., 2006; Borges, 2018; Niimura et al., 2018). Underappreciated, however, is the idea that olfaction is more effective at night due to the relative stillness of air, especially in the understory, where air speeds and turbulence tend to be lowest (Murlis, 1997; Muller-Schwarze, 2006). Lack of wind produces a high signal-to-noise ratio for a given odor plume, resulting in a stronger olfactory signal. This nocturnal environment may thus select for olfactory-driven ecologies and social interactions, perhaps even in the absence of selection driven by the loss of visual ability (Drea et al., 2019).

Spatiotemporal Interactions

Vertical and diel variation interact to create diverse aeroscapes, ranging from the windiest and most turbulent environment of the upper canopy at midday, to the comparative stillness of the understory at night. Such interactions create opportunities for convergence. We found that aeroscapes in the daytime understory are comparable to those in the canopy at night, which raises the possibility of convergent olfactory signals and sensitivities in the animals that occupy these distinct niches (Barton, 2006; Valenta et al., 2013; Brokaw and Smotherman, 2020; Nevo and Ayasse, 2020). Interestingly, the olfactory ecology of nocturnal terrestrial frugivores may depend in part on wind-mediated fruit falls during daytime, suggesting vertical day-night integration of aeroecologies (Augspurger and Franson, 1987; Borah and Beckman, 2021). The upshot is that daytime canopy conditions are suboptimal for anemotaxis in the service of frugivory and seed dispersal, which may explain why so-called “bird-fruits” in the upper canopy emit little scent (Gautier-Hion et al., 1985; Howe, 1986; Lomáscolo et al., 2010; Valenta et al., 2018; Valenta and Nevo, 2020).

Some angiosperm plants, including Ficus and Nicotiana, exhibit diurnal rhythms in fruit and flower chemistry (Raguso et al., 2003; Borges et al., 2011; Burdon et al., 2015; Ripperger et al., 2019; Balducci et al., 2020) timed to maximize their availability and attractiveness for pollinators and seed dispersers. These diurnal rhythms may have evolved in response to diurnal patterns in air movement, such that compounds produced during the day may be heavier and more robust against turbulent conditions than those produced at night, which may be lighter and more easily propagated under calmer conditions (Alberts, 1992; Muller-Schwarze, 2006). Flowers, which are generally more delicate and ephemeral than fruits, are often produced early in the morning, which may reflect a compromise between protection from wind-damage and availability to vision-mediated pollinators such as bees and birds (Herrera, 1990; Bloch et al., 2017). Setting aside the aeroecology of frugivory and pollination, aeroscapes are also essential to another aspect of plant reproductive biology: wind dispersal (Kennedy, 1978; Friedman and Barrett, 2008). Flowering and the eventual abscission of wind-dispersed seeds is greatest at midday, when air movement is highest, which suggests some level of aerosensation (Bohrer et al., 2008; Wright et al., 2008; Caplat et al., 2012). Such hypotheses invite future testing.

Intraforest Variation

We recorded higher air speeds and greater turbulence in the early successional habitat compared to the later one, possibly reflecting the lower levels of standing biomass. This pattern is expected for habitats with greater levels of anthropogenic disturbance that have caused vegetative loss (Raynor, 1971; Muller-Schwarze, 2006; Klein et al., 2021). Such results have potentially important implications. For example, increased air movement may affect the relative colonization of wind-dispersed versus animal-dispersed plants in disturbed areas (Cadenasso and Pickett, 2001; Cubiña and Aide, 2006; Nathan et al., 2008), altering community dynamics, habitat suitability to frugivores, and reforestation efforts (Janzen, 1988; Vieira and Scariot, 2006; de la Peña-Domene et al., 2018; Camargo et al., 2020). Greater air movement and turbulence are also expected to negatively affect animals that rely on odor plumes (Shukla et al., 1990; Zhang et al., 1996; Gandu et al., 2004). Consequences include impediments to animal foraging and communication, as discussed above, as well as effects on predator-prey networks.

Sensory detection of predators often involves scent, and many species are particularly attuned to the odors of their relevant predators (Weldon, 1990; Kats and Dill, 1998; Apfelbach et al., 2005). Rats are particularly sensitive to 2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline, a component of red fox anal gland secretions (Laska et al., 2005) and mice, rats, and stoats, for example, have been shown to avoid carnivore and apex predator odors (Ferrero et al., 2011; Garvey et al., 2016). Indeed, higher wind speeds have been found to impede predator detection by mule deer (Bowyer et al., 2001) and other mammal species (Cherry and Barton, 2017). The sensory impact of air movement can be further compounded by spillover to the other senses: wind creates acoustic and visual noise, which may reduce detection of stimuli by other senses, further impeding predator detection (Hayes and Huntly, 2005; Carr and Lima, 2010; Francis et al., 2012). Overall, future work could usefully address the diverse ways that anthropogenically modified aeroscapes affect the aero-sensory ecology and habitat use of resident flora and fauna (Damschen et al., 2008). As these influences could ultimately affect species distributions (Bowyer and Kie, 2009; Breitbach et al., 2012), we urge the incorporation of aeroscapes into existing conservation and evolutionary frameworks (e.g., assessment of “edge effects” and “landscapes of fear”) and general inclusion into future studies of anthropogenic disturbance and deforestation/reforestation dynamics (Laundre et al., 2010).

Limitations and Future Directions

Our results offer insight into the variability of air movement within a heterogeneous landscape, although some caution must be noted. First, due to equipment limitations, we were unable to sample early and late successional forest sites simultaneously, and instead sampled them sequentially. This serial approach raises the possibility that temporal differences in climate, not habitat heterogeneity, were driving the difference that we observed between sampling locations. Further, due to the logistical challenges of erecting scaffolds in a protected habitat, we were unable to sample replicates of early and late successional forest conditions. The extent to which our two study locations exemplify such habitat conditions is therefore uncertain, and could usefully be explored in greater detail in future studies sampling multiple locations per forest type. Lastly, the detection limits of our anemometers prohibited direct measurement of the slowest air speeds. While prohibitively expensive for our study design, solid-state anemometers have the advantage of greater measurement range. Future studies may also benefit from incorporating variation in wind direction, as this also has important implications for olfactory-based orientation (Kennedy, 1978; Togunov et al., 2017; Jinn et al., 2020).

Our study contributes to the emerging field of aeroecology by quantifying variation in the aeroscape of a lowland tropical dry forest and drawing attention to the implications for sensory signal propagation, plant reproduction, and mammalian sensory evolution. This topic is timely as shifting air movement patterns due to anthropogenic climate change may disrupt aeroecological interactions, with the potential far-reaching effects on animal behavior, population dynamics, and species distributions (Usbeck et al., 2010; McInnes et al., 2011; Young et al., 2011; Lewis et al., 2015). Understanding the impacts of aeroscapes on sensory ecology and evolution will be key for predicting how animals will respond to changing environments.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author Contributions

AD contributed to the study design, led data collection and analysis, and wrote the manuscript. JH co-led data analysis alongside AD and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. CG contributed to the data collection, study design, and writing of the manuscript. ND contributed to the study design, data analysis, and writing of the manuscript. AM contributed to the study design, data collection and analysis, and writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the NSERC(RGPIN-2017-03782) and Canada Research Chairs Program (950-231257) to AM, National Geographic Early Career Research Grant (EC-59267R-19) to AD, and University of Calgary, Faculty of Arts (JH).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank Roger Blanco, Maria Marta Chavarria, and the staff of the Área de Conservación Guanacaste for making this work possible. We also thank Saúl Cheves Hernandez and Danny Montiel for help with the construction of our scaffolds for this project.

References

Alberts, A. C. (1992). Constraints on the design of chemical communication systems in terrestrial vertebrates. Am. Nat. 139, S62–S89.

Google Scholar

Aluja, M., Prokopy, R. J., Buonaccorsi, J. P., and Cardé, R. T. (1993). Wind tunnel assays of olfactory responses of female Rhagoletis pomonella flies to apple volatiles: effect of wind speed and odor release rate. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 68, 99–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1993.tb01693.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoki, M., Yabuki, K., and Koyama, H. (1978). Micrometeorology of pasoh forest. Malayan Nat. J. 30, 149–159. doi: 10.1007/978-4-431-67008-7_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apfelbach, R., Blanchard, C. D., Blanchard, R. J., Hayes, R. A., and McGregor, I. S. (2005). The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: a review of field and laboratory studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1123–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Augspurger, C. K., and Franson, S. E. (1987). Wind dispersal of artifical fruits varying in mass, area, and morphology. Ecology 68, 27–42. doi: 10.2307/1938802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balducci, M. G., Van der Niet, T., and Johnson, S. D. (2020). Diel scent and nectar rhythms of an African orchid in relation to bimodal activity patterns of hawkmoth pollinators. Ann. Bot. 126, 1155–1164. doi: 10.1093/aob/mcaa132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balkenius, A., Rosén, W., and Kelber, A. (2006). The relative importance of olfaction and vision in a diurnal and a nocturnal hawkmoth. J. Comp. Physiol. A 192, 431–437. doi: 10.1007/s00359-005-0081-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, R. A. (2006). Olfactory evolution and behavioral ecology in primates. Am. J. Primatol. 68, 545–558. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, R. A., Purvis, A., and Harvey, P. H. (1995). Evolutionary radiation of visual and olfactory brain systems in primates, bats and insectivores. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London. Series B: Biol. Sci. 348, 381–392. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1995.0076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baynton, H. W. (1968). The ecology of an elfin forest in Puerto Rico, 2. the microclimate of pico del oeste. J. the Arnold Arboretum 49, 419–430. doi: 10.5962/p.185747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baynton, H. W. (1969). The ecology of an elfin forest in Puerto Rico, 3. Hilltop and forest influences on the microclimate of Pico del Oeste. J. Arnold Arboretum 50, 80–92.

Google Scholar

Bingman, V. P., and Moore, P. (2017). “Properties of the atmosphere in assisting and hindering animal navigation,” in Aeroecology, eds P. Chilson, W. Frick, J. Kelly, and F. Liechti (Cham: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-68576-2_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloch, G., Bar-Shai, N., Cytter, Y., and Green, R. (2017). Time is honey: circadian clocks of bees and flowers and how their interactions may influence ecological communities. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 372:20160256. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohrer, G., Katul, G. G., Nathan, R., Walko, R. L., and Avissar, R. (2008). Effects of canopy heterogeneity, seed abscission and inertia on wind-driven dispersal kernels of tree seeds. J. Ecol. 96, 569–580. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2745.2008.01368.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borah, B., and Beckman, N. G. (2021). Studying seed dispersal through the lens of movement ecology. Oikos 2022, 1–13. doi: 10.1111/oik.08310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borges, R. M. (2018). Dark matters: challenges of nocturnal communication between plants and animals in delivery of pollination services. Yale J. Biol. Med. 91, 33–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Borges, R. M., Ranganathan, Y., Krishnan, A., Ghara, M., and Pramanik, G. (2011). When should fig fruit produce volatiles? pattern in a ripening process. Acta Oecologica 37, 611–618. doi: 10.1016/j.actao.2011.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowyer, R. T., McCullough, D. R., and Belovsky, G. E. (2001). Causes and consequences of sociality in mule deer. Alces 37, 371–402.

Google Scholar

Bowyer, R., and Kie, J. (2009). Thermal landscapes and resource selection by black-tailed deer: implications for large herbivores. California Fish Game 95, 128–139.

Google Scholar

Breitbach, N., Tillmann, S., Schleuning, M., Grünewald, C., Laube, I., Steffan-Dewenter, I., et al. (2012). Influence of habitat complexity and landscape configuration on pollination and seed-dispersal interactions of wild cherry trees. Oecologia 168, 425–437. doi: 10.1007/s00442-011-2090-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brokaw, A. F., and Smotherman, M. (2020). Role of ecology in shaping external nasal morphology in bats and implications for olfactory tracking. PLoS One 15:e0226689. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brokaw, A. F., and Smotherman, M. (2021). Olfactory tracking strategies in a neotropical fruit bat. J. Exp. Biol. 224:jeb231829. doi: 10.1242/jeb.231829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brokaw, A. F., Davis, E., Page, R. A., and Smotherman, M. (2021). Flying bats use serial sampling to locate odour sources. Biol. Lett. 17:20210430. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2021.0430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burdon, R. C. F., Raguso, R. A., Kessler, A., and Parachnowitsch, A. L. (2015). Spatiotemporal floral scent variation of penstemon digitalis. J. Chem. Ecol. 41, 641–650. doi: 10.1007/s10886-015-0599-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cadenasso, M. L., and Pickett, S. T. A. (2001). Effect of edge structure on the flux of species into forest interiors. Conservation Biol. 15, 91–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2001.99309.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camargo, P. H. S. A., Pizo, M. A., Brancalion, P. H. S., and Carlo, T. A. (2020). Fruit traits of pioneer trees structure seed dispersal across distances on tropical deforested landscapes: implications for restoration. J. Appl. Ecol. 57, 2329–2339. doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.13697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, F. A. (2018). “A synthesis of long-term environmental change in Santa Rosa, Costa Rica,” in Primate Life Histories, Sex Roles, and Adaptability: Essays in Honour of Linda M. Fedigan, eds U. Kalbitzer and K. M. Jack (Berlin: Springer), 331–358. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-98285-4_16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplat, P., Nathan, R., and Buckley, Y. M. (2012). Seed terminal velocity, wind turbulence, and demography drive the spread of an invasive tree in an analytical model. Ecology 93, 368–377. doi: 10.1890/11-0820.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardé, R. T., and Willis, M. A. (2008). Navigational strategies used by insects to find distant, wind-borne sources of odor. J. Chem. Ecol. 34, 854–866. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9484-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, J. M., and Lima, S. L. (2010). High wind speeds decrease the responsiveness of birds to potentially threatening moving stimuli. Animal Behav. 80, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cazelles, B., Chavez, M., Berteaux, D., Ménard, F., Vik, J. O., Jenouvrier, S., et al. (2008). Wavelet analysis of ecological time series. Oecologia 156, 287–304. doi: 10.1007/s00442-008-0993-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherry, M. J., and Barton, B. T. (2017). Effects of wind on predator-prey interactions. Food Webs 13, 92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.fooweb.2017.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conover, M. R. (2007). Predator-Prey Dynamics: The Role of Olfaction. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Cubiña, A., and Aide, T. M. (2006). The effect of distance from forest edge on seed rain and soil seed bank in a tropical pasture1. Biotropica 33, 260–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2001.tb00177.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, E. P., Edmonds, D., Stalter, L., and Janal, M. N. (2021). Ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) use olfaction to locate distant fruit. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 175, 300–307. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.24255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahmani, L., Patel, R. M., Yang, Y., Chakravarty, M. M., Fellows, L. K., and Bohbot, V. D. (2018). An intrinsic association between olfactory identification and spatial memory in humans. Nat. Commun. 9:4162. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06569-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damschen, E. I., Brudvig, L. A., Haddad, N. M., Levey, D. J., Orrock, J. L., and Tewksbury, J. J. (2008). The movement ecology and dynamics of plant communities in fragmented landscapes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 105, 19078–19083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0802037105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Peña-Domene, M., Martínez-Garza, C., Ayestarán-Hernández, L. M., and Howe, H. F. (2018). Plant attributes that drive dispersal and establishment limitation in tropical agricultural landscapes. Forests 9:620. doi: 10.3390/f9100620

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delbarco-Trillo, J., Burkert, B. A., Goodwin, T. E., and Drea, C. M. (2011). Night and day: the comparative study of strepsirrhine primates reveals socioecological and phylogenetic patterns in olfactory signals. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 82–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, R. H. (2013). The airspace is habitat. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 377–379. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, R. H., Peterson, A. C., Bolus, R. T., and Johnson, D. H. (2017). “Extending the habitat concept to the airspace,” in Aeroecology, eds P. B. Chilson, W. F. Frick, J. F. Kelly, and F. Liechti (Berlin: Springer), 47–69. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-68576-2_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drea, C. M. (2015). D’scent of man: a comparative survey of primate chemosignaling in relation to sex. Horm. Behav. 68, 117–133. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drea, C. M., Goodwin, T. E., and Delbarco-Trillo, J. (2019). P-Mail: the information highway of nocturnal, but not diurnal or cathemeral, strepsirrhines. Folia Primatol. 90, 422–438. doi: 10.1159/000495076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennos, A. R. (1997). Wind as an ecological factor. Trends Ecol. Evol. 12, 108–111. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(96)10066-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrero, D. M., Lemon, J. K., Fluegge, D., Pashkovski, S. L., Korzan, W. J., Datta, S. R., et al. (2011). Detection and avoidance of a carnivore odor by prey. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 108, 11235–11240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103317108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finelli, C. M., Pentcheff, N. D., Zimmer, R. K., and Wethey, D. S. (2000). Physical constraints on ecological processes: a field test of odor-mediated foraging. Ecology 81, 784–797. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[0784:pcoepa]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, T. H., Heithaus, E. R., and Sawyer, W. B. (1977). An experimental analysis of the food location behavior of frugivorous bats. Ecology 58, 619–627. doi: 10.2307/1939011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, C. D., Kleist, N. J., Ortega, C. P., and Cruz, A. (2012). Noise pollution alters ecological services: enhanced pollination and disrupted seed dispersal. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 279, 2727–2735. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.0230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, W. F., Chilson, P. B., Fuller, N. W., Bridge, E. S., and Kunz, T. H. (2013). “Aeroecology,” in Bat Evolution, Ecology, and Conservation, eds R. A. Adams and S. C. Pedersen (Berlin: Springer), 149–167. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-7397-8_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, J., and Barrett, S. C. H. (2008). A phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of wind pollination in the angiosperms. Int. J. Plant Sci. 169, 49–58. doi: 10.1086/523365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandu, A. W., Cohen, J. C. P., and de Souza, J. R. S. (2004). Simulation of deforestation in eastern Amazonia using a high-resolution model. Theoretical Appl. Climatol. 78, 123–135. doi: 10.1007/s00704-004-0048-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garvey, P. M., Glen, A. S., and Pech, R. P. (2016). Dominant predator odor triggers caution and eavesdropping behavior in a mammalian mesopredator. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 481–492. doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2063-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautier-Hion, A., Duplantier, J.-M., Quris, R., Feer, F., Sourd, C., Decoux, J.-P., et al. (1985). Fruit characters as a basis of fruit choice and seed dispersal in a tropical forest vertebrate community. Oecologia 65, 324–337. doi: 10.1007/BF00378906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. R., and Huntly, N. J. (2005). Effects of wind on the behavior and call transmission of pikas (Ochotona princeps). J. Mammal. 86, 974–981. doi: 10.1644/1545-1542(2005)86[974:eowotb]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helsel, D. R. (2004). Nondetects and Data Analysis: Statistics for Censored Environmental Data. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience.

Google Scholar

Hennessy, G., Harris, C., Eaton, C., Wright, P., Jackson, E., Goulson, D., et al. (2020). Gone with the wind: effects of wind on honey bee visit rate and foraging behavior. Animal Behav. 161, 23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.12.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrera, C. M. (1990). Daily patterns of pollinator activity, differential pollinating effectiveness, and floral resource availability, in a summer-flowering mediterranean shrub. Oikos 58, 277–288. doi: 10.2307/3545218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heydel, F., Cunze, S., Bernhardt-Römermann, M., and Tackenberg, O. (2014). Long-distance seed dispersal by wind: disentangling the effects of species traits, vegetation types, vertical turbulence and wind speed. Ecol. Res. 29, 641–651. doi: 10.1007/s11284-014-1142-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, B. T. (2010). Tradeoff between travel speed and olfactory food detection in ring-tailed coatis (Nasua nasua). Ethology 116, 671–679. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01783.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, H. F. (1986). Seed dispersal by fruit-eating birds and mammals. Seed Dispersal 123:189.

Google Scholar

Hughes, G. M., Boston, E. S. M., Finarelli, J. A., Murphy, W. J., Higgins, D. G., and Teeling, E. C. (2018). The birth and death of olfactory receptor gene families in mammalian niche adaptation. Mol. Biol. Evol. 35, 1390–1406. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msy028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, M. T., Samonds, K. E., Raharison, J.-L., and Wright, P. C. (2004). Lemur latrines: observations of latrine behavior in wild primates and possible ecological significance. J. Mammal. 85, 420–427. doi: 10.1644/1383937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janmaat, K. R., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 111, 16343–16348. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1407524111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janson, C. H. (1998). Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behav. 55, 1229–1243. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1997.0688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen, D. H. (1988). Management of habitat fragments in a tropical dry forest: growth. Annals Missouri Botanical Garden 75, 105–116. doi: 10.2307/2399468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen, D. H., and Hallwachs, W. (2020). Área de Conservación Guanacaste, northwestern Costa Rica: converting a tropical national park to conservation via biodevelopment. Biotropica 52, 1017–1029. doi: 10.1111/btp.12755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jinn, J., Connor, E. G., and Jacobs, L. F. (2020). How ambient environment influences olfactory orientation in search and rescue dogs. Chem. Senses 45, 625–634. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjaa060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, R. P. (1973). Scent marking in mammals. Animal Behav. 21, 521–535. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80012-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalacska, M., Sanchez-Azofeifa, G. A., Calvo-Alvarado, J. C., Quesada, M., Rivard, B., and Janzen, D. H. (2004). Species composition, similarity and diversity in three successional stages of a seasonally dry tropical forest. Forest Ecol. Manag. 200, 227–247. doi: 10.1016/j.foreco.2004.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kats, L. B., and Dill, L. M. (1998). The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Écoscience 5, 361–394. doi: 10.1080/11956860.1998.11682468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, J. S. (1978). The concepts of olfactory ‘arrestment’ and ‘attraction.’. Physiol. Entomol. 3, 91–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1978.tb00138.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, M., Garvelmann, J., and Förster, K. (2021). Revisiting forest effects on winter air temperature and wind speed—new open data and transfer functions. Atmosphere 12:710. doi: 10.3390/atmos12060710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kollikowski, A., Zimmermann, E., and Radespiel, U. (2019). First experimental evidence for olfactory species discrimination in two nocturnal primate species (Microcebus lehilahytsara and M. murinus). Sci. Rep. 9:20386. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56893-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korine, C., and Kalko, E. K. V. (2005). Fruit detection and discrimination by small fruit-eating bats (Phyllostomidae): echolocation call design and olfaction. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 12–23. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0003-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruijt, B., Malhi, Y., Lloyd, J., Norbre, A. D., Miranda, A. C., Pereira, M. G. P., et al. (2000). Turbulence statistics above and within two Amazon rain forest canopies. Boundary-Layer Meteorol. 94, 297–331. doi: 10.1023/A:1002401829007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunz, T. H., Gauthreaux, S. A. Jr., Hristov, N. I., Horn, J. W., Jones, G., Kalko, E. K. V., et al. (2008). Aeroecology: probing and modeling the aerosphere. Int. Comp. Biol. 48, 1–11. doi: 10.1093/icb/icn037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laska, M., Fendt, M., Wieser, A., Endres, T., Hernandez Salazar, L. T., and Apfelbach, R. (2005). Detecting danger—or just another odorant? olfactory sensitivity for the fox odor component 2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline in four species of mammals. Physiol. Behav. 84, 211–215. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laundre, J., Hernández, L., and Ripple, W. (2010). The landscape of fear: ecological implications of being afraid. Open Ecol. J. 3, 1–7. doi: 10.2174/1874213001003030001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, L. (2020). Nondetects and Data Analysis for Environmental Data. R Package Version 1.6-1.1. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=NADA (accessed September 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Leiser-Miller, L. B., Kaliszewska, Z. A., Lauterbur, M. E., Mann, B., Riffell, J. A., and Santana, S. E. (2020). A fruitful endeavor: scent cues and echolocation behavior used by Carollia castanea to find fruit. Int. Organismal Biol. 2:obaa007. doi: 10.1093/iob/obaa007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, S., Phillips, R. A., Burthe, S. J., Wanless, S., and Daunt, F. (2015). Contrasting responses of male and female foraging effort to year-round wind conditions. J. Animal Ecol. 84, 1490–1496. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomáscolo, S. B., Levey, D. J., Kimball, R. T., Bolker, B. M., and Alborn, H. T. (2010). Dispersers shape fruit diversity in Ficus (Moraceae). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 107, 14668–14672. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008773107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macdonald, D. W. (1980). Patterns of scent marking with urine and faeces amongst carnivore communities. Symposia Zool. Soc. London 45, 107–139.

Google Scholar

McCay, M. G. (2003). Winds under the rain forest canopy: the aerodynamic environment of gliding tree frogs. Biotropica 35, 94–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2003.tb00266.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McInnes, K. L., Erwin, T. A., and Bathols, J. M. (2011). Global climate model projected changes in 10 m wind speed and direction due to anthropogenic climate change. Atmospheric Sci. Lett. 12, 325–333. doi: 10.1002/asl.341

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melin, A. D., Hogan, J. D., Campos, F. A., Wikberg, E., King-Bailey, G., Webb, S., et al. (2020). Primate life history, social dynamics, ecology, and conservation: contributions from long-term research in Área de Conservación Guanacaste, Costa Rica. Biotropica 52, 1041–1064. doi: 10.1111/btp.12867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monteith, J. L., and Unsworth, M. H. (2013). Principles of Environmental Physics: Plants, Animals, and the Atmosphere, 4th Edn. Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Moulton, D. G. (1967). Olfaction in mammals. Am. Zool. 7, 421–429. doi: 10.1093/icb/7.3.421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller-Schwarze, D. (2006). Chemical Ecology of Vertebrates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Murlis, J. (1997). “Odor plumes and the signal they provide,” in Insect Pheromone Research: New Directions, eds R. T. Cardé and A. K. Minks (Berlin: Springer), 221–231. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-6371-6_21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murlis, J., Willis, M. A., and Cardé, R. T. (2000). Spatial and temporal structures of pheromone plumes in fields and forests. Physiol. Entomol. 25, 211–222. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3032.2000.00176.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nathan, R., Schurr, F. M., Spiegel, O., Steinitz, O., Trakhtenbrot, A., and Tsoar, A. (2008). Mechanisms of long-distance seed dispersal. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 638–647. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevo, O., and Ayasse, M. (2020). “Fruit scent: biochemistry, ecological function, and evolution,” in Co-Evolution of Secondary Metabolites, eds J.-M. Mérillon and K. G. Ramawat (Berlin: Springer International Publishing), 403–425. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-96397-6_33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevo, O., Razafimandimby, D., Jeffrey, J. A. J., Schulz, S., and Ayasse, M. (2018). Fruit scent as an evolved signal to primate seed dispersal. Sci. Adv. 4:eaat4871. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aat4871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevo, O., Valenta, K., Kleiner, A., Razafimandimby, D., Jeffrey, J. A. J., Chapman, C. A., et al. (2020). The evolution of fruit scent: phylogenetic and developmental constraints. BMC Evol. Biol. 20:138. doi: 10.1186/s12862-020-01708-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niimura, Y., Matsui, A., and Touhara, K. (2018). Acceleration of olfactory receptor gene loss in primate evolution: possible link to anatomical change in sensory systems and dietary transition. Mol. Biol. Evol. 35, 1437–1450. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msy042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, H. R., and Mayhead, G. J. (1974). Wind measurements in a pine forest during a destructive gale. Forestry: Int. J. Forest Res. 47, 185–194. doi: 10.1093/forestry/47.2.185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, R. P., and Mech, L. D. (1975). Scent-marking in wolves. Am. Sci. 63, 628–637.

Google Scholar

Pleijel, H., Wallin, G., Karlsson, P. E., and Skärby, L. (1996). Ozone gradients in a spruce forest stand in relation to wind speed and time of the day. Atmospheric Environ. 30, 4077–4084. doi: 10.1016/1352-2310(96)00141-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. S., Becknell, J. M., Irving, J., and Pèrez-Aviles, D. (2009). Diversity and structure of regenerating tropical dry forests in Costa Rica: geographic patterns and environmental drivers. Forest Ecol. Manag. 258, 959–970. doi: 10.1016/j.foreco.2008.10.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulliainen, E. (1981). Scent-marking in the pine marten (Maries martes) in Finnish Forest Lapland in winter. Zeitschrift Säugetierkunde 47, 91–99.

Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Raguso, R. A., Levin, R. A., Foose, S. E., Holmberg, M. W., and McDade, L. A. (2003). Fragrance chemistry, nocturnal rhythms and pollination “syndromes” in Nicotiana. Phytochemistry 63, 265–284. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9422(03)00113-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, J., and Silman, M. (2012). Diurnal, seasonal, and altitudinal trends in microclimate across a tropical montane cloud forest. Climate Res. 55, 17–32. doi: 10.3354/cr01127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raynor, G. S. (1971). Wind and temperature structure in a coniferous forest and a contiguous field. Forest Sci. 17, 351–363.

Google Scholar

Ripperger, S. P., Rehse, S., Wacker, S., Kalko, E. K. V., Schulz, S., Rodriguez-Herrera, B., et al. (2019). Nocturnal scent in a ‘bird-fig’: a cue to attract bats as additional dispersers? PLoS One 14:e0220461. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, A., Alquézar, B., and Peña, L. (2013). Fruit aromas in mature fleshy fruits as signals of readiness for predation and seed dispersal. New Phytol. 197, 36–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2012.04382.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruzicka, R. E., and Conover, M. R. (2011). Influence of wind and humidity on foraging behavior of olfactory mesopredators. Canadian Field-Naturalist 125, 132–139. doi: 10.22621/cfn.v125i2.1196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruzicka, R. E., and Conover, M. R. (2012). Does weather or site characteristics influence the ability of scavengers to locate food? Ethology 118, 187–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01997.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santana, S. E., Kaliszewska, Z. A., Leiser-Miller, L. B., Lauterbur, M. E., Arbour, J. H., Dávalos, L. M., et al. (2021). Fruit odorants mediate co-specialization in a multispecies plant-animal mutualism. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 288:20210312. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2021.0312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shukla, J., Nobre, C., and Sellers, P. (1990). Amazon deforestation and climate change. Science 247, 1322–1325. doi: 10.1126/science.247.4948.1322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sillero-Zubiri, C., and Macdonald, D. W. (1998). Scent-marking and territorial behavior of ethiopian wolves Canis simensis. J. Zool. 245, 351–361. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1998.tb00110.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, L. F. (1990). Time-series decomposition using the sinusoidal model. Int. J. Forecast. 6, 485–495. doi: 10.1016/0169-2070(90)90025-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, G. P., Strandh, M., and Löfstedt, C. (2014). “Movements in the olfactory landscape,” in Animal Movements Across Scales, eds L.-A. Hansson and S. Åkesson (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Therneau, T. (2021). A Package for Survival Analysis in R. R Package Version 3.2-13. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=survival (accessed September 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Thies, W., Kalko, E. K. V., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1998). The roles of echolocation and olfaction in two neotropical fruit-eating bats, Carollia perspicillata and C. castanea, feeding on Piper. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42, 397–409. doi: 10.1007/s002650050454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Togunov, R. R., Derocher, A. E., and Lunn, N. J. (2017). Windscapes and olfactory foraging in a large carnivore. Sci. Rep. 7:46332. doi: 10.1038/srep46332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Usbeck, T., Wohlgemuth, T., Pfister, C., Volz, R., Beniston, M., and Dobbertin, M. (2010). Wind speed measurements and forest damage in Canton Zurich (Central Europe) from 1891 to winter 2007. Int. J. Climatol. 30, 347–358. doi: 10.1002/joc.1895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaglio, S., Minicozzi, P., Romoli, R., Boscaro, F., Pieraccini, G., Moneti, G., et al. (2016). Sternal gland scent-marking signals sex, age, rank, and group identity in captive mandrills. Chem. Senses 41, 177–186. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenta, K., and Nevo, O. (2020). The dispersal syndrome hypothesis: how animals shaped fruit traits, and how they did not. Funct. Ecol. 34, 1158–1169. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.13564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenta, K., Burke, R. J., Styler, S. A., Jackson, D. A., Melin, A. D., and Lehman, S. M. (2013). Colour and odor drive fruit selection and seed dispersal by mouse lemurs. Sci. Rep. 3:2424. doi: 10.1038/srep02424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenta, K., Kalbitzer, U., Razafimandimby, D., Omeja, P., Ayasse, M., Chapman, C. A., et al. (2018). The evolution of fruit colour: phylogeny, abiotic factors and the role of mutualists. Sci. Rep. 8:14302. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32604-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vieira, D. L. M., and Scariot, A. (2006). Principles of natural regeneration of tropical dry forests for restoration. Restoration Ecol. 14, 11–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-100X.2006.00100.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, S. (1996). Life in Moving Fluids: The Physical Biology of Flow. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Weldon, P. J. (1990). “Responses by vertebrates to chemicals from predators,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates V, eds D. W. MacDonald, D. Müller- Schwarze, and S. E. Natynczuk (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Wright, S. J., Trakhtenbrot, A., Bohrer, G., Detto, M., Katul, G. G., Horvitz, N., et al. (2008). Understanding strategies for seed dispersal by wind under contrasting atmospheric conditions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 105, 19084–19089. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0802697105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, I., Zieger, S., and Babanin, A. (2011). Global trends in wind speed and wave height. Science 332, 451–455. doi: 10.1126/science.1197219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Henderson-Sellers, A., and McGuffie, K. (1996). Impacts of tropical deforestation. Part II: the role of large-scale dynamics. J. Climate 9, 2498–2521. doi: 10.1175/1520-0442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zjacic, N., and Scholz, M. (2022). The role of food odor in invertebrate foraging. Genes Brain Behav. 21:e12793. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aeroecology, olfactory ecology, tropical dry forest (bosque seco tropical), air speed, sensory landscape, sensory evolution

Citation: DePasquale A, Hogan JD, Guadamuz Araya C, Dominy NJ and Melin AD (2022) Aeroscapes and the Sensory Ecology of Olfaction in a Tropical Dry Forest. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10:849281. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.849281

Received: 05 January 2022; Accepted: 28 March 2022;
Published: 29 April 2022.

Edited by:

Felipe M. Gawryszewski, University of Brasilia, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Ben T. Hirsch, James Cook University, Australia
Raúl Alberto Laumann, Embrapa Genetic Resources and Biotechnology, Brazil

Copyright © 2022 DePasquale, Hogan, Guadamuz Araya, Dominy and Melin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Allegra DePasquale, YWxsZWdyYS5kZXBhc3F1YWxlQHVjYWxnYXJ5LmNh; Amanda D. Melin, YW1hbmRhLm1lbGluQHVjYWxnYXJ5LmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.