Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 14 March 2022
Sec. Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognition, Foraging, and Energetics in Extant and Extinct Primates View all 10 articles

What Are the “Costs and Benefits” of Meat-Eating in Human Evolution? The Challenging Contribution of Behavioral Ecology to Archeology

  • 1UMR 7194 Histoire Naturelle de l’Homme Préhistorique, CNRS, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Université Perpignan Via Domitia, Institut de Paléontologie Humaine, Paris, France
  • 2UMR 7194 Histoire Naturelle de l’Homme Préhistorique, CNRS, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Université Perpignan Via Domitia, Musée de l’Homme, Paris, France

Despite the omnivorous diet of most human populations, meat foraging gradually increased during the Paleolithic, in parallel with the development of hunting capacities. There is evidence of regular meat consumption by extinct hominins from 2 Ma onward, with the first occurrence prior to 3 Ma in Eastern Africa. The number of sites with cut-marked animal remains and stone tools increased after 2 Ma. In addition, toolkits became increasingly complex, and various, facilitating carcass defleshing and marrow recovery, the removal of quarters of meat to avoid carnivore competition, and allowing the emergence of cooperative (i.e., social) hunting of large herbivores. How can we assess the energy costs and benefits of meat and fat acquisition and consumption for hunter-gatherers in the past, and is it possible to accurately evaluate them? Answering this question would provide a better understanding of extinct hominin land use, food resource management, foraging strategies, and cognitive abilities related to meat and fat acquisition, processing, and consumption. According to the Optimal Foraging Theory (OFT), resources may be chosen primarily on the basis of their efficiency rank in term of calories. But, could other factors, and not only calorific return, prevail in the choice of prey, such as the acquisition of non-food products, like pelts, bone tools or ornaments, or symbolic or traditional uses? Our main goal here is to question the direct application of behavioral ecology data to archeology. For this purpose, we focus on the issue of animal meat and fat consumption in human evolution. We propose a short review of available data from energetics and ethnographic records, and provide examples of several various-sized extant animals, such as elephants, reindeer, or lagomorphs, which were some of the most common preys of Paleolithic hominins.

Introduction

Energetics (e.g., food accessibility and availability, energy intake and balance…) comprise critical information for enhancing our understanding of human evolution (Aiello and Wheeler, 1995; Leonard and Robertson, 1997; Aiello and Wells, 2002; Snodgrass et al., 2009; Isler and Van Schaik, 2014; Burini and Leonard, 2018). A shift to a more energetical-rich food, such as meat, contributes to meeting elevated daily energy requirements (Pontzer, 2017; Pontzer and Wood, 2021). Meat consumption behavior and hunting are also observed in non-human primates. For example, bonobos focus primarily on small herbivores. Chimpanzees occasionally hunt monkeys using tools (Pruetz et al., 2015), but process carcasses without any use of tools. Meat represents less than 0.01–0.19 kg/day in their diet and only small proportions of their feeding time (Watts, 2020). During human evolution, meat consumption seems to increase and to become less sporadic than in extant non-human primates. As a famous primatologist, Washburn, expressed: “The taste for meat is one of the main characteristics distinguishing man from the apes, and this habit changes the whole way of life. It is therefore important to date the beginning of hunting in order to interpret the origin of human behavior” (Washburn, in Brain, 1981).

The purpose of this short literature review is to present the main steps of meat-eating in human evolution, and to discuss its cost and benefits in terms of biological and sociological aspects. In order to do so, we present different approaches for addressing the energetics of past hominins, crossing extant and archeological data. Finally, we discuss the relevance of paleoanthropological and archeological data in the context of meat consumption and assess the interests and limitations of applying behavioral ecology to archeology.

Meat-Eating in Human Evolution: Archeological Evidence

The first occurrences of meat consumption by early hominins are recorded in Africa around 3.4 Ma at Dikika, Ethiopia (McPherron et al., 2010; Thompson et al., 2015), although the identification of the butchery marks is still questioned (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2010, 2011; McPherron et al., 2011). Pliocene stone tools discovered at Lomekwi 3, Kenya, 3.3 Ma, support the hypothesis of stone tool making by Pliocene hominins more than 3 Ma ago, 500,000 years before the emergence of the Homo genus (Harmand et al., 2015). In addition, dental and cranial morphology, gut size or endurance running point to increased meat and fat consumption among Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene African hominins (Aiello and Wheeler, 1995; Teaford and Ungar, 2000; Teaford et al., 2002; Bramble and Lieberman, 2004; Ungar, 2004; Daegling and Grine, 2007; Ben-Dor et al., 2021), as do stable isotopes analyses (Balter et al., 2012; Martin et al., 2020). We observe an increased number of sites with cut-marked animal remains and stone tools after 2 Ma, in eastern Africa (Bunn, 1981; Potts and Shipman, 1981; De Heinzelin et al., 1999). For example, the Oldowan site of Kanjera (2 Ma), South Kenya, yielded numerous bone elements processed by hominins, comprising small as well as medium-sized bovids, highlighting meat consumption and also the first evidence of hunting small animals (Ferraro et al., 2013). In parallel to this early consumption of small animal tissues, some evidence shows the input of aquatic and plant-based resources. Meat from terrestrial mammals was not the only, nor the main source of protein. For example, in the Koobi Fora Formation, Kenya, some crocodile, fish and turtle remains bearing cut marks highlight the exploitation of water habitats by early Homo erectus (i.e., H. ergaster) (Braun et al., 2010; Archer et al., 2014). Besides, dental wear patterns and isotope ratios indicate wide diversity in the diet of Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene hominins, including plants and hard foods such as nuts, seeds and underground storage organs (Ungar and Sponheimer, 2011; Balter et al., 2012; Grine et al., 2012). Animal fats were also an important source of calories for past hunter-gatherers, and large herbivores in particular provide omega-3 fatty acids (Guil-Guerrero et al., 2014, 2015).

Toolkits may also have been essential to hominins for butchering carcasses, with cutting edges for processing soft tissue as well as percussion tools to extract marrow. With the onset of the Acheulean and the emergence of H. erectus sensu lato populations in Africa, between 1.9 and 1.7 Ma (Lepre et al., 2011; Hammond et al., 2021), we observe a dietary change with greater evidence of carnivorous and predatory behavior in faunal assemblages (Shipman and Walker, 1989; Echassoux, 2012; Pante, 2013; Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2014; Diez-Martín et al., 2015; Arriaza et al., 2017; Yravedra et al., 2020). However, although meat consumption occurred quite early in human evolution, common meat consumption, complex forms of cooperative hunting, entire butchery sequences and stone and carcass transport, which require advanced cognitive skills such as planning and more decision-making, seem to have developed later, after 1 Ma, throughout the African continent and the Levant in conjunction with early Middle Pleistocene environmental changes (Clark et al., 2006; Rabinovich et al., 2008; Forrest et al., 2018; Altamura et al., 2019; Daujeard et al., 2020).

Later, in Western Europe, the large quantities of herbivore remains found in Middle Paleolithic sites (c. 400–40 ka) (Gaudzinski and Roebroeks, 2000; Costamagno et al., 2006; Daujeard et al., 2012; Niven et al., 2012; Rendu et al., 2012), stable isotope ratios (Bocherens et al., 2005; Wißing et al., 2016; Jaouen et al., 2019) and tooth wear data (Estalrrich et al., 2017), highlight the high intake of animal proteins in the Neanderthal diet. Neanderthals could rely for up to 80% on animal protein and 20% on plant proteins (Naito et al., 2016), making them the most emblematic carnivorous and competitive big game hunters among extinct hominins (Karavanić et al., 2000). Despite this mostly carnivorous diet, more recently, dental calculus residues show that many Neanderthal groups consumed plants for food, medicinal or gustatory purposes (mushrooms, pine nuts, leguminous plants, etc.) over a widespread area ranging from the Mediterranean Basin to Central Europe (Henry et al., 2011; Hardy et al., 2012; Krief et al., 2015; Weyrich et al., 2017; Power et al., 2018). Moreover, in their recent article, Hardy et al. (2022) emphasize the importance of plants in nutrition for all hominin species, including Neanderthals, through indirect evidence, such as physiological requirements.

For Pleistocene Anatomically Modern Humans (AMH), interpretations also point toward a high quantity of meat in their diet, predominantly from large game (Prat et al., 2011; Drucker et al., 2017; Ben-Dor et al., 2021). Although small game was consumed since the Early Paleolithic (Fernández-Jalvo et al., 1999; Braun et al., 2010) and the Middle Paleolithic (Hardy and Moncel, 2011; Cochard et al., 2012; Hardy et al., 2013; Blasco et al., 2016; Morin et al., 2019; Pelletier et al., 2019; Guillaud et al., 2021), we note an increase in small prey and fish at the end of the Upper Paleolithic in Europe and the Levant (Stiner et al., 2000; Costamagno and Laroulandie, 2004; Rufà et al., 2018). Concerning carbohydrates intake in the AMH diet, alongside the more numerous direct evidence, recent studies on the oral microbiome highlighted a high degree of similarity between modern humans and Neanderthals, with the specific acquisition of starch digestion (Fellows Yates et al., 2021).

Finally, whether prey species were passively or actively scavenged, trapped or hunted, the intensification of animal meat and fat in the diet of extinct hominins exposes them to competition with various other predators and scavengers, increasing interactions: direct attacks, passive confrontations, dispersals, commensalism, etc., and mutual contributions: providers of carrion, models for hunting strategies, etc. (Marean, 1989; Shipman and Walker, 1989; Turner, 1992; Rodríguez et al., 2012). Whatever the types of strategies used and the amount consumed, the consumption of meat and fat may have had significant consequences on human evolution in terms of biology, societies and also in terms of energetic costs and benefits.

Contribution of Ecological Models and Extant Data to the Study of Past Hominin Diets

Ecological Models

In order to address subsistence practices and the energetics of past hominins, a multi-proxy approach is necessary, taking into account various factors: hominin capacities (biological, social and cultural features), paleoenvironmental constraints (biomass, prey availability, seasons, topography, other competitors, etc.), archeological and geochemical data (faunal remains, isotopes, etc.) and actualistic data and ecological models, used for example in human behavioral ecology (HBE) (Smith, 1992). HBE is a neo-Darwinian approach in which changes in behavior may result from natural selection (adaptation to the environment), rather than from cultural motivations. HBE emerged in the mid-1970s with the application of optimal foraging models to hunter-gatherer decisions relating to resource selection and land use (Winterhalder and Smith, 2000; Codding and Bird, 2015).

In the 1980s, a theory developed by ecologists, called The Optimal Foraging Theory (OFT) (Stephens and Krebs, 1986; Pyke and Stephens, 2019), was applied to anthropology and archeology to specifically explain dietary patterns in hunter-gatherer societies (Smith, 1979; Winterhalder, 1981; Smith et al., 1983). OFT provides predictions and valuable models for grasping the relations between the environment and foragers in the past. What should be eaten? Where should food be sought and how large a group is needed to catch and exploit the food? According to the OFT, resources may be chosen primarily on the basis of their efficiency rank in term of calories. The most current of these foraging models is diet breadth, also called prey-choice model (PCM) or optimal diet (Winterhalder and Smith, 2000; Bird and O’Connell, 2006). According to this model, high-ranked resources are pursued whenever encountered, and lower-ranked resources are included in the diet depending on encounter rates with higher ranked resources. Thus, prey choice takes into account various parameters such as the main goal, which corresponds to the maximization of foraging efficiency, currency, which is usually a measure of energy costs and benefits; constraints, which are all the limiting factors, such as the time spent foraging, processing or digesting capacities; and the alternative options for making a decision, which incorporate the possible set of food resources, daily needs and other ways of spending time (childcare for example).

According to these models, energy is one of the main parameters influencing subsistence strategies in past hunter-gatherer societies, it is important to calculate the return rate of the animal. The Energy Cost-benefit or Post-Encounter Return Rate (PERR) or Efficiency rank is defined as the ratio of the Energetic benefit of the animal (expressed in kcal) to the Energetic cost related to searching and handling time (expressed in person-hours). Handling time corresponds to the time spent pursuing and dispatching prey, carcass processing and transport and consumption (chewing and digesting time). The latter depend on several parameters, such as the type of prey (size, weight, age, aggressiveness, encounter rate, etc.), consumed body parts, season, topography, distance, group size, other competitors, etc.

Extant Data on Animal Nutrients and Human Biology

The energy intake and nutrients supplied by terrestrial animals correspond to meat, fat, yellow, and red marrow, brain, tongue, viscera, etc. Meat and marrow nutrients provide proteins (amino acids), fats (saturated and unsaturated fat), vitamins (A, B1, B2, B3, B6, B12, and C) and minerals (Iron, Calcium, Zinc). Fat is the most energy-dense macronutrient (Ben-Dor et al., 2021). For example, lean beef meat provides between 100 and 200 kcal/100 g, but beef fat contains about twice as many calories (900 kcal/100 g) as protein or carbohydrates (400 kcal/100 g). Indeed, although meat-eating is of high calorific value, it induces high energy expenditure for the elimination of nitrogenous waste products. As a result, a diet rich in lean meat requires very high daily food rations to cover body energy requirements. In addition, excessive protein consumption leads to liver and kidney disorders. Excessive animal protein intake may lead to protein toxicity which can be dangerous for pregnant women and new-borns (Fiorenza et al., 2015). These deficiencies and health risks can be overcome by increasing the fat intake in the diet. Fat has a high energy value, and enables gluconeogenesis, a metabolic process occurring primarily in the liver and kidneys that produces carbohydrates from protein and fat (Speth and Spielmann, 1983; Speth, 2010). These advantages therefore go a long way to explaining why Paleolithic human groups sought to harvest fat wherever it was available, including fat contained in the marrow and cancellous tissue of very large and large mammals long bones (Costamagno and Rigaud, 2013; Smith et al., 2015; Ben-Dor et al., 2016; Blasco et al., 2019a; Boschian et al., 2019; Morin, 2020; Dodat et al., 2021). The preparation of broths based on fat contained in bone tissue observed among the Nunamiut by Binford (1978) suggests that such processes may have existed in Paleolithic societies (Speth, 2015). Moreover, in order to avoid excessive meat protein, in addition to animal fat, other alternative food resources, such as plants, might have been incorporated in the diet (Henry et al., 2011, 2014; Fiorenza et al., 2015).

Since the 1970s, the plethora of ethnographic studies of hunter-gatherer societies have provided abundant data on human biology and diet, and fostered the development of ethnoarchaeological research through the 1980s and 1990s (Binford, 1978; Keene, 1985; O’Connell et al., 1988a,1990; O’Connell and Marshall, 1989; Bartram, 1993; Bunn, 1993, 1994; Kelly, 1995; Bartram and Marean, 1999). We know for example that we have a daily energy expenditure of 2,000–3,000 kCal per day, which is related more to body size, age, and sex than to cultural differences (Pontzer et al., 2012). Relative to other great apes, hunter-gatherers spend more daily energy for food-finding, but with a greatest return rate, making possible the sharing with others, and thus the possibility for some to carry out tasks other than foraging (Kraft et al., 2021). The similarity in daily energy expenditure among hunter-gatherers and sedentary Westerners suggests that the effect of marked lifestyle differences is minor and that differences between populations result primarily in energy intake, rather than expenditure. In the same way, variation in the diet is mainly related to temperatures and primary production, for example, hunting territories are generally larger in northern latitudes (Kelly, 1995). Regarding the proportion of meat, animal fats or plants in the human diet, we have to be careful with the data from present-day hunter-gatherers. Environmental conditions, technical, and physiological adaptations must prevent us from making direct analogies with Pleistocene hominins, and even more so with the earliest ones (Ben-Dor and Barkai, 2020; Ben-Dor et al., 2021).

Eating Raw or Cooked Meat: What Is the Energetic Impact?

The control of fire and its use for cooking marks another critical shift in the diet. Indeed, despite the apparent importance of meat in human evolution, some studies (Carmody and Wrangham, 2009) showed that a raw food diet provides insufficient energy for the maintenance of body weight, suggesting that food processing and cooking are very important. Indeed, when starch or proteins are cooked, they are more tender, more digest, mastication is facilitated (reduction of chewing time), the cost of digestion is reduced and energy extraction per unit mass increases (Wrangham et al., 1999; Carmody and Wrangham, 2009; Carmody et al., 2011; Hardy et al., 2015). Furthermore, cooking kills food pathogens. However, iron and certain vitamins, such as for example vitamins B12 and C (which primates are unable to synthesize), decrease with high temperatures and cooking time. Cooking can be quite costly in terms of energetic expenditure in terms of fuel collection, the time needed to make and maintain fire, and requires some cognitive skills, such as for example for fuel collection (choice of wood, bones, stones etc.), and fireplace location (Henry et al., 2018; Magargal, 2022). Cooking or fire use was initially very occasional, and could have occurred with H. erectus in Africa c. 1.8 Ma ago. This hypothesis is based on indirect evidence of hot springs (Sistiaga et al., 2020), thermally altered lithics and bone fragments at 1.5 Ma in Kenya (Hlubik et al., 2017), microstratigraphic evidence at 1 Ma in South Africa (Berna et al., 2012), and the decrease in the size of the digestive system (see below “Expensive Tissue Hypothesis”). Therefore, this indirect evidence predates the first evidence of the control of fire in the Near East at 790 ka (Alperson et al., 2004; Alperson-Afil, 2008) and the oldest evidence of the habitual use of fire attested around 300–400 ka in Europe, Africa and the Levant, which suggests that this behavior is relatively recent (Roebroeks and Villa, 2011; Shahack-Gross et al., 2014; Barkai et al., 2017; Richter et al., 2017; MacDonald et al., 2021).

Aiello and Wheeler (1995) put forward the “Expensive Tissue Hypothesis” to explain how primates can have relatively large brains without a high basal metabolic rate. For those authors, the increased energetic demands of a larger brain are compensated by the reduction in the mass-specific metabolic rates of other tissues, such as the gastro-intestinal tract. Their analyses in human and non-human primates infer coevolution between brain and gut sizes, which is dependent on energy intake and could be determined by dietary quality. They consider that hominins, especially after 2 Ma (e.g., early H. erectus sensu lato), had small guts, and would have required the use of fire and cooking to efficiently process difficult-to digest food, such as meat. However, some authors consider that this trade-off scenario seems to be insufficient (Navarrete et al., 2011; Isler and van Schaik, 2012), and they stress that brain size is not correlated with other energetic expensive organs or with digestive tract mass, and that encephalization and fat storage in primates are strategies to buffer against caloric shortfalls. Furthermore, Cornélio et al. (2016), revisiting correlations between brain size and cooking, showed that large primate encephalization occurred long before the control of fire and that hominins were likely to obtain enough calories from raw meat to afford brain size increase. Indeed, processing methods such as pounding (Zink et al., 2014; Zink and Lieberman, 2016), or eating rotten or putrefied meat, also result in an important increase in energy compared to unprocessed raw diets (meat or tubers). In addition, such methods preserve vitamins, favor pre-digestion and do not require fuel (Speth, 2017). Likewise, other studies propose that the increased need for more energetical-rich nutrients due to reduced gut size might also have been compensated by animal fat (Ben-Dor et al., 2011), or even by semi-digested chyme, i.e., gastrophagy (Buck et al., 2016).

Meat-Eating and the Origin of Social Cooperation and Food Sharing

Meat involves a relatively high level of cooperation. Compared to plants, meat is difficult to obtain, and very demanding in both time and energy. This becomes particularly critical when the subsistence strategy focuses on big game, where cooperation is often needed to acquire resources and fend off predators. In such cases, distribution needs to be efficient within the group and all the participants have to receive a payoff (Alvard, 2011). Food sharing, and in particular meat sharing, is thought to be central to the evolution of hominin behavior and to hunter-gatherer economics where food transfer practices vary greatly, depending on resource availability in the environment, or/and social organization (Hawkes et al., 1991; Winterhalder, 1997; Lee and Daly, 1999; Enloe, 2003). Food sharing reduces the risk of caloric shortfall, especially when it concerns large animals, which are high-risk food as they are difficult to obtain but also high-reward food. Hunting is considered to be one of the most difficult foraging activities. Direct encounters with big game and successful capture require substantial skills and knowledge sharing which develop at least 10 years after attaining adult body size (Gurven et al., 2006). In most hunter-gatherer populations, the meat of big animals is widely shared with the opportunity to get it back later. The cost of sharing with the whole group is quite low, as the total amount of meat cannot be eaten at one time by the hunter’s family. On the contrary, the benefits of the reciprocity are quite high, as sharing avoids meat spoilage, long-term food storage and reduces the risk of nutrients shortfalls, especially for large prey such as elephants (Hawkes et al., 1991; Enloe, 2003; Gurven and Jaeggi, 2015; Barkai, 2019). Hadza populations consider carcasses of large sized animals as public goods. There are few nutritional advantages to being an efficient hunter, and the benefits to the generous sharers might only be their hunter reputation (Kaplan et al., 1985; Stibbard-Hawkes et al., 2020).

Hominins could have used different acquisition modes over time to obtain meat from small or large animals: trapping, passive or confrontational scavenging, ambush or pursuit hunting using short or long distance weapons, hand capture of small-sized prey, etc., which favored the development of social cooperation and food sharing (Domínguez-Rodrigo, 2002; Stiner et al., 2009; Gaudzinski-Windheuser et al., 2018; Conard et al., 2020). Primary distribution occurred at the kill site, as the carcass was partitioned for transport. Secondary distribution, which is redistribution to family, took place at the campsite or the consumption area. These two stages can be observed in the archeological record thanks to the spatial distribution and butchering patterns of the carcasses. However, the origins of food sharing and its definition and purpose are still debated in evolutionary anthropology and Paleolithic archeology (Isaac, 1978; Kaplan et al., 1985; Blurton Jones, 1987; Hawkes, 1991; Winterhalder, 1996, 2001). It is reasonable to expect that Pleistocene hominins lived in social groups, but food-sharing patterns must have differed a lot through time and depended on the size of game (Stiner et al., 2009). Willems and Van Schaik (Willems and van Schaik, 2017) suggest that at the time of the emergence of the Homo genus, the social organization of H. ergaster revolved around reduced fission-fusion dynamics and very large multi-male groups, which could have helped to defend the group against predators. Through time, when hominins moved toward higher latitudes (further north), increasingly complex multilevel fission-fusion social systems could have helped hominins to adjust foraging patterns, constrained by large group size, low population densities and increased foraging demands (Grove et al., 2012). Furthermore, the most common anthropological scenario also suggests a link between the transition toward eating a high-quality diet, such as meat, for example, and cooperation among hominins (including care of the young, protection from predators, group hunting, and food storage) (O’Connell et al., 1988b,2002; Hawkes et al., 2001; Bramble and Lieberman, 2004; Pontzer, 2012). Intergenerational cooperation brings increased energetic benefits, with the coevolution of increases in the duration of lactation, gestation, longevity, neonate and adult body mass and daily energy expenditure, but a decrease in sexual dimorphism (Smith et al., 2012). Indeed, according to Smith et al. (2012) evidence from carnivores indicates that both the pace of reproduction and the emergence of cooperative defense against predators can respond in a flexible fashion to variations in the availability and acquisition of energy-rich foods. Thus, for those authors, it is possible that similar flexibility influenced shifts in reproductive investment and reproduction rates among hominins.

Another adaptive reason why Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene hominins had to amplify forms of cooperation regarding immature individuals is related to human life history, with juveniles remaining dependent for a longer time on mothers than other species. In the course of human evolution, this was made possible through the support of group members other than the mother, i.e., “food transfer between alloparents and offspring: cooperative breeders” (e.g., Burkart et al., 2009; Hrdy, 2009; Strassmann and Kurapati, 2010; Kramer et al., 2015). Cooperative breeding, which affects parental care (shorter birth intervals, juvenile dependence), is presumed to appear in the Lower Pleistocene with early H. erectus (O’Connell et al., 1999; Hrdy, 2009; van Schaik and Burkart, 2010). Indeed, Aiello and Key (2002), suggest that H. erectus females were able to cope with increased energetic costs per offspring (daily energy requirements during gestation and lactation) by shortening interbirth intervals and cooperating with others in feeding dependent children. Moreover, Wrangham and Conklin-Brittain (2003) have also suggested that cooking, which makes raw food soft enough for young individuals to chew, may have facilitated shorter inter-birth intervals.

To conclude, animal acquisition and consumption represent an undeniable advantage in terms of time and energy benefits, by providing considerable quantities of proteins, fat and other nutrients. However, it may also sometimes constitute a high-cost food source, and if considering only meat, it can be even unhealthy in excessive amounts. Hunter-gatherers should not exceed a maximum quantity of protein intake (plant and animal) per day, and so they must also obtain a large part of non-protein food in their environment (fat and carbohydrates) (Speth, 1989). As a matter of fact, big-game hunting is not always advantageous for feeding a human group. The risk of failing, the time to pursuit, the type and size of the prey, their physiological condition throughout the year, sometimes with a scarcity of fat, and many other parameters, all challenge the prominence of hunting large mammals in the early human diet (Speth, 2010).

The Case of three Types of Prey Size: At the Crossroads Between Extant and Archeological Data

We can hypothesize on the prey acquisition and carcass transport strategies developed by past hunter-gatherer societies using extant and energetic data. These strategies depend on several parameters in terms of energy costs and benefits: (1) the number of hunters; (2) the type of prey (size, weight, age, aggressiveness, encounter rate, etc.); (3) the nutritive value of chosen carcass elements; (4) the season; (5) the topography and the distance from the kill site to the camp; (6) the distribution within the group and repayment; and (7) the presence of other competitors; etc. Concerning the latter parameter, early experimental and actualistic research in African national parks yield a better idea of carcass availability in the environment, the degree of competition and resource partitioning strategies for the different predators and scavengers (Brain, 1981; Blumenschine, 1986; Brantingham, 1998; Domínguez-Rodrigo, 1999). Besides, as far as the weight of the animal is concerned, we know that for the Hadza in Tanzania, 80% of the anatomical elements of skeleton are abandoned at the kill site for large or very large adult animals, compared to only 30% for those of medium size, in relation with the cost/benefit rate of each carcass portion (O’Connell et al., 1990; Lupo, 2001). The age of the animal and season of death may also be relevant in the transport strategies. Adult marrow is richer than that of juveniles, and in the same way, marrow represents the last fat resource during the winter, which explains the interest for bones such as metapodials, which are poor in meat but rich in yellow marrow (Speth and Spielmann, 1983; Speth, 1987). Here, we will take the example of three animals of different sizes (elephants, reindeer, and lagomorphs) exploited by past and extant hunter-gatherers, and examine their specificities in term of energy and capture difficulty.

An adult elephant provides a huge amount of meat; almost two tons. Despite its weight, this colossal animal is not always sought by past and present hunter-gatherers. According to Lupo and Schmitt, whose data come from ethnographic and historic sources (Lupo and Schmitt, 2016): “Applications of the Prey Choice Model to ethnographic populations show that prey size does not always predict profitability.” First of all, hunting failure has to be considered, considering many variables, including the prey itself. According to the authors, the bigger the prey, the more likely hunters are to miss it. Larger-sized preys are less abundant in the landscape, and hunters spend more time tracking them. The success rate for elephants is only 20% for Bisa hunters in Zambia, which drastically reduces the PERR. Secondly, the time spent pursuing the animal can be up to 38 h for an elephant. Lupo and Schmitt highlight the strong and positive correlation between the size of the prey and pursuit time. Lastly, large game requires extensive processing and transport efforts involving many individuals, with 86 h of butchering time for an elephant.

In comparison, for the Paleolithic, the role of megafauna in the hominin diet is often questioned. The association of megafauna with archeological sites is sometimes problematic and the full range of possibilities should be discussed. One of the critical factors for many of the megafauna sites is that some may be the results of scavenging or bone collecting for fuel, tools and building materials (Gaudzinski et al., 2005; Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2014; Lupo and Schmitt, 2016). Besides, zooarchaeological evidence of megafauna exploitation in Early and Middle Paleolithic assemblages is scarce, which rather supports the opportunistic role of megafauna during these ancient periods (Demay et al., 2012, 2016a,b; Smith, 2015). Nevertheless, the role of proboscideans in human evolution should not be minimized. In this regard it is important to notice that butchery and fracturing marks are particularly difficult to observe on pachyderm bones, as it was previously pointed out (Crader, 1983; Haynes and Krasinski, 2021). In the same way, a large quantity of food from Proboscideans in the Paleolithic has been highlighted by high ratios of nitrogen in humans (Wißing et al., 2016; Drucker et al., 2017). Yet, these data should be treated with caution as other parameters can induce high nitrogen levels, such as a diet based on aquatic resources, breastfeeding, putrefied meat or episodes of caloric shortfalls (Speth, 2017). Although evidence for elephant hunting is mostly provided for the Upper Paleolithic times (Svoboda et al., 2005; Agam and Barkai, 2018; Demay et al., 2021), for earlier periods there is the case of the Pleistocene elephant single-carcass sites, such as the Acheulean sites of Nadung’a 4, Kenya (Delagnes et al., 2006), or that of Barranc de la Boella, Spain (Mosquera et al., 2015). There are also the example of the Middle Pleistocene sites in the Latium in Italy with many Elephant remains (Anzidei et al., 2012; Boschian and Saccà, 2015). They have given rise to various hypotheses. Are they related to specific human behavior, to an exceptional preservation context or to a higher number of pachyderms in the environment?

Concerning reindeer, the two most famous ethnographic examples of reindeer husbandry and hunting in Northern latitudes are the Nunamiut (Alaska) and the Evenki (Russia), among others (Winterhalder, 1983; Costamagno and David, 2009). The Nunamiut are large bands of a hundred individuals. They are 80% dependent on reindeer hunts and can kill more than 200 reindeer for a year through the mass killing of migratory tundra gregarious reindeers (Binford, 1978, 1981). Evenki are small family groups of 5–10 individuals. Among the Evenki, Abe observed the kills of four woodland reindeer for 21 reindeer hunts for a year, which represents a very low success rate (19%) compared to those of the Nunamiut (43%) (Abe, 2005). Tracking a reindeer on foot can last more than 11 h, including kill site butchery. The PERR of a reindeer is thus 80,000 kcal/11 h, and lower once the success rate is considered.

To illustrate the exploitation of reindeer by past societies, we will take the example of a Middle Paleolithic site from south-eastern France. The Abri du Maras yielded Neanderthal occupations dated to around 50–40 ka for layer 4.1 (Moncel et al., 2021). Like many other archeological records with monospecific assemblages from the Middle Paleolithic of Western Europe, a great number of reindeers were present in the layer (88% of the NISP, representing 16 individuals), which also includes horse, bison, giant deer, red deer, ibex, and lagomorphs. Zooarchaeological analyses evidence a catastrophic age profile and autumnal kills, highlighting mass reindeer predation during migration events (Daujeard et al., 2019). At this camp site, we have in situ evidence of the reindeer exploitation of all the edible resources (marrow, meat, bone grease, etc.). The scarcity of the spongious part of the skeleton, axial parts (heads, vertebrae, and ribs), as well as girdles (pelvis and scapula), long bone epiphyses and short articular bones (carpals, tarsals, and phalanges), underlines either their abandon at the kill site and/or specific processing methods at the camp for grease bone extraction and/or for fuel. Only a few portions of the skeleton are represented in relation to their utility index (expressed in meat, grease, and marrow quantity). Anyway, although the choice of some portions is correlated to the Food Utility Index, bone grease as well as non-food products (like animal pelts) may also have mattered in the choice of past hunter-gatherers.

We have taken here the example of this faunal assemblage to illustrate Prey Choice Model and Carcass transport strategies. For that purpose, we assessed the energy intake from the faunal spectrum, and classified species according to their weight in meat. The total amount obtained is 3.5 tons of available meat if we count the whole carcasses of slaughtered animals, representing 4 M kcal, which can feed about 20 hunter-gatherers for two and a half months. In terms of Optimal Foraging Theory, some assumptions can be made:

- The selective diet would highlight a rich environment with a short encounter time;

- Reindeer probably had the highest encounter rate, with great autumnal migration events along the Rhone Valley;

- They were also safer and more easily transportable than Bison, while 16 reindeer are equal to two bison in term of meat weight;

- The few processed lagomorphs were probably the result of opportunistic encounters.

Finally, although this type of modeling opens up assumptions in terms of subsistence strategies, little can be said in the end as many occupations have certainly succeeded and mixed in this assemblage, as it was taken here as a whole.

Concerning lagomorphs, the encounter rate is much higher than for elephants (80% success) but they are much less productive. Indeed, the rabbit is small, only provides around 2.5 kg of meat and is in addition very poor in lipids, which can lead to protein toxicity called “rabbit starvation” (Cochard, 2004; Rufa Bonache, 2017). For lagomorphs, similarly, exploitation is scarce during the Early and Middle Paleolithic, with only a few archeological examples (Fernández-Jalvo et al., 1999; Blasco and Fernández Peris, 2012; Cochard et al., 2012; Hardy et al., 2013; Morin et al., 2019).

In spite of the low energy efficiency of lagomorphs when they are not slaughtered en masse, like other small preys, at the end of the Upper Paleolithic and during the Mesolithic, small prey hunting intensified. This could be explained by various hypotheses: cultural changes; environmental pressure with too many lagomorphs in a valuable territory; decrease of mobility and longer human occupations; or demographic pressure, which would have led to the broad-spectrum revolution. The demographic pulses of the UP and Epi-Paleolithic would have then forced the human diet to exploit fast-growing preys (Stiner et al., 2000).

Discussion and Conclusion: The Limits of Applying Behavioral Ecology to Archeology

Above, we saw how relevant energetic and ethnographic studies can be to human evolution. Here, we would like to address some of the more problematic points related to these approaches.

The Problem of the Scarcity of Archeological Evidence and Time Scales

To start with, it should be noted that data from extinct populations only yield isolated points in the paleoanthropological and archeological record. In addition, the range of error for the chronological framework could be around 50,000–100,000 years for Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene periods, due to the dating methods used, and it is thus difficult to precisely link two different subsistence proxies. In addition, information on the diet consumed and the quality of the diet cannot be strictly compared between hominins and extant human and non-human primates. For hominins, dental microwear analyses provide data on the diet consumed a few days before the death of the individual. Isotopic analyses based on dental remains give some information of the diet consumed in early life. Furthermore, as data are cumulative, we only have an overall picture of the diet and it is impossible to gauge the diet consumed on a daily basis. Moreover, it is difficult to discern the ratio of meat in the diet of past hominins, and especially for the Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene periods, although some new isotopic analyses using barium, calcium or zinc isotopes seem quite relevant for this purpose (Balter et al., 2012; Jaouen et al., 2016; Martin et al., 2020; Dodat et al., 2021).

Most of the time, Paleolithic sites are composed of palimpsests of multiple human occupations, often mixed with natural and carnivore deposits. Such mixed accumulations really challenge the reconstruction of ecological models, based on the balance between hominins’ daily energetic needs, and the energetic intake provided by animals accumulated over very long time periods. Moreover, we have seen that little is known of the past hominin diet, especially during the Early Paleolithic. The diet of Late Pliocene and Early Pleistocene hominins was probably more diverse and less carnivorous than thought. Indeed, evidence of butchery is scarce and mostly based on a very small number of remains with some questionable cut marks (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2010; McPherron et al., 2010, 2021; Sahle et al., 2017; Toth, 2017; Domínguez-Rodrigo and Baquedano, 2018).

The Problem of Applying Anatomical and Energetic Data From Extant Primates to Ancient Hominins

A higher-quality diet (and for example meat consumption) is often linked to an increase in brain size and a decrease in posterior tooth size (Aiello and Wheeler, 1995; Aiello and Wells, 2002; Snodgrass et al., 2009; Ungar, 2012). Improvements in dietary quality probably played an important role in brain expansion during the course of human evolution, but cannot alone explain why hominin brains grew (Leonard et al., 2011). Therefore, it is important to note that the measurement of the whole brain could be a poor proxy. Indeed, some more specific brain regions are more precisely related to feeding ecology, in particular to dietary quality, than the whole brain (DeCasien and Higham, 2019; Louail et al., 2019).

The reduction in molar size in H. erectus, Neanderthals and modern humans can be explained by a shorter chewing time owing to non-thermal food processing or cooking methods rather than by the rate of craniodental and body size evolution (Organ et al., 2011). This assumption is based on molar size (M2) and body mass. Firstly, we must be cautious about estimating body mass. Indeed, its inference in hominins takes into account the length of lower limb bones based on data from non-human primates. However, it is very difficult to associate isolated lower limb bones with skulls (from which species are determined), especially when different species are in the same spatio-chronological setting, as for example in the Turkana Basin during the Early Pleistocene (Paranthropus boisei, H. ergaster, H. habilis, and H. rudolfensis). Furthermore, the scenario is often only based on a few individuals, and does not consider the range variation of past populations. Secondly, the use of M2 size alone does not consider molar proportion variation across the dental arch, hominin species with a small overall dental size, and disproportionally highlights M2s and M3s. Furthermore, it has been shown that brain enlargement and dental reduction were decoupled and evolved at different rates (Gómez-Robles et al., 2017).

Moreover, the morphology of the masticatory system, in particular dentition, does not represent the diet, but reflects what individuals were able to eat rather than what they ate. This is Liem’s paradox (Liem, 1980), of which Paranthropus is a good illustration, with its apparent ambiguity between anatomy and diet.

With regard to energetics, some researchers have proposed models to investigate hominin energy requirements, based on activity patterns, body mass and life history data from extant primates, with inferences based on hominin body mass. However, as mentioned in the previous paragraph, extreme caution should be taken with this proxy. For example, it has been estimated that the daily energy expenditure (DEE) of a H. erectus female was 2,086 kilocalories per day, 2,269 during gestation and 2,487 during lactation (Aiello and Key, 2002), and that she had to chew raw meat for 5.7–6.2 h/day to satisfy her energetic needs (Wrangham and Conklin-Brittain, 2003). However, according to Simmen et al. (2021) DEE increased with the augmentation of the resting metabolic rate, but life-history variables (maximum lifespan, gestation and lactation duration, interbirth interval, litter mass, age of first reproduction) would not be correlated with DEE after controlling for body mass and phylogeny. In the same way, Pontzer et al. (2012) stressed that the DEE seems to be more related to body size, age and sex than to cultural differences. The similarity of the DEE in hunter-gatherers and sedentary Westerners suggests that even our strong differences in lifestyle only produce a minor effect, and differences between populations result mainly from energy intake rather than expenditure (Pontzer et al., 2012). It should also be noted that the total DEE is measured outside a laboratory using the doubly labeled water method on extant primates in free-living conditions (Pontzer, 2017; Simmen et al., 2021), which of course, cannot be applied to extinct hominins. Therefore, most estimations of daily energy cost should be used with caution.

The Silent Part of Culture and Symbolism

While human behavioral ecology models are useful to predict which resource is edible and suitable or not in the diet, when a potential resource is not consumed, it may be a question of tradition, for example, totems or taboos, or of non-foraging activities, such as childcare or reproduction. Indeed, we have to keep in mind that other goals, and not only calorific return may prevail in diet choices, such as the acquisition of fat, vitamins, minerals or non-food products like pelts, bone tools or ornaments. For example, to date, a dozen European Middle Paleolithic sites have yielded evidence of the use of large raptor phalanges as personal ornaments or for symbolic purposes by Neanderthals (Morin and Laroulandie, 2012; Laroulandie et al., 2016; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2019). Others showed evidence of intentional feather removal, proffering another type of explanation for the Neanderthal exploitation of raptors (Peresani et al., 2011). Thus, we may ask what really governed forager’s subsistence strategies in the past. Was it acquisition circumstances and the energetic return rate, as assumed by ecological approaches, or cultural and symbolic representations of animals, which is very difficult to assess for past societies. In reality, both probably played their part (Blasco et al., 2019b; Barkai, 2020). The role of culture and symbolism in food choices should of course not be overlooked. Far from being restricted to so-called “complex” societies, the eminently cultural nature of food is indeed a feature common to all human societies (Lévi-Strauss, 1964). Thus, choosing the appropriate food does indeed depend to a large extent on the available techniques and resources, but also on cultural and symbolic parameters. For example, in the case of the elephant, considerations other than the energetic return rate can motivate big game hunting, such as maintaining social position (Lupo and Schmitt, 2016). The development of ethnoarchaeological approaches through the 1970s and 1980s provided insights into these socio-cultural, but also technological aspects (Leroi-Gourhan and Brézillon, 1972; Yellen, 1977; Binford, 1978, 1980; Gallay, 1980). But this type of approach calls for caution. Indeed, when using current data from ethnology, we have to overcome the inherent biases of ethnoarchaeology, i.e., a too strict and direct analogism and the determinism that this may imply (Lévi-Strauss, 1952; Cleuziou, 1984).

In any case, for the Paleolithic, the social and symbolic dimension of food is unfortunately particularly difficult to approach (see Costamagno and Daujeard, 2021). Nevertheless, some recent studies show that the marks left by marrow extraction can highlight the existence of traditional butchery practices specific to certain Middle and Late Paleolithic groups of hunter-gatherers (Blasco et al., 2013; Masset et al., 2016; Vettese et al., 2017). It has been also been suggested for example that the intense disarticulation and fragmentation of phalanges and short bones in the European Middle Magdalenian, may reflect ritualized butchery practices, in connection with the relationships maintained between Paleolithic people and their game (Birouste, 2020). These works extend slightly beyond the strictly economic character to which the diet of our Paleolithic ancestors is generally reduced in zooarcheological studies.

Finally, we must be aware that in applying HBE to Archeology, it is necessary to contextualize data according to palimpsest biases, using geoarchaeology, refitting, seasonal data, spatial analyses, etc., in order to obtain the best time resolution. We also have to keep in mind that a whole section is missing from our understanding of animal acquisition choices among past societies, i.e., cultural and symbolic representations, which explain a large part of the relationships we have with nature, if not all (Descola, 2005).

Author Contributions

CD and SP wrote the body of the manuscript. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We would like to thank the ANR HOMTECH (ANR-17-CE27-0005) for the partial funding of this publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

Part of this review is taken from a lecture given by one of the author (CD) in the framework of an international symposium on hominin energetics which took place in June 2018 at the Collège de France in Paris (dir. Jean-Jacques Hublin). SP would also like to thank Cécile Garcia and Sébastien Bouret for discussion concerning meat sharing and cooking on an earlier version of the manuscript. We also acknowledge the comments and suggestions of two reviewers that helped to improve the clarity and quality of our manuscript, and the editor of this article, Florent Rivals. Finally, we would like to thank Louise Byrne for the English editing.

References

Abe, Y. (2005). Hunting and Butchery Patterns of the Evenki in Northern Transbaikalia. New York, NY: Stony Brook University.

Google Scholar

Agam, A., and Barkai, R. (2018). Elephant and mammoth hunting during the Paleolithic: a review of the relevant archaeological, ethnographic and ethno-historical records. Quaternary 1:3. doi: 10.3390/quat1010003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiello, L. C., and Key, C. (2002). Energetic consequences of being a Homo erectus female. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 14, 551–565. doi: 10.1002/AJHB.10069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiello, L. C., and Wells, J. C. K. (2002). Energetics and the evolution of the genus Homo. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 31, 323–338. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.31.040402.085403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiello, L. C., and Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 199–221. doi: 10.1086/204350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alperson, N., Kislev, M. E., Simchoni, O., Melamed, Y., Ben-Nun, A., and Werker, E. (2004). Evidence of hominin control of fire at Gesher Benot Ya‘aqov, Israel naama goren-inbar. Science 304, 725–727. doi: 10.1126/science.1095443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alperson-Afil, N. (2008). Continual fire-making by hominins at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Quat. Sci. Rev. 27, 1733–1739. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2008.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altamura, F., Gaudzinski-windheuser, S., Melis, R. T., and Mussi, M. (2019). Reassessing hominin skills at an early Middle pleistocene hippo butchery site: Gombore II-2 (Melka Kunture, upper Awash valley, Ethiopia). J. Paleolit. Archaeol. 3, 1–32. doi: 10.1007/s41982-019-00046-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvard, M. (2011). Genetic and cultural kinship among the lamaleran whale hunters. Hum. Nat. 22, 89–107. doi: 10.1007/s12110-011-9104-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anzidei, A. P., Bulgarelli, G. M., Catalano, P., Cerilli, E., Gallotti, R., Lemorini, C., et al. (2012). Ongoing research at the late Middle pleistocene site of La Polledrara di Cecanibbio (central Italy), with emphasis on human-elephant relationships. Quat. Int. 255, 171–187. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, W., Braun, D. R., Harris, J. W. K., McCoy, J. T., and Richmond, B. G. (2014). Early Pleistocene aquatic resource use in the Turkana basin. J. Hum. Evol. 77, 74–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arriaza, M. C., Yravedra, J., Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Mate-González, M. Á, García Vargas, E., Palomeque-González, J. F., et al. (2017). On applications of micro-photogrammetry and geometric morphometrics to studies of tooth mark morphology: the modern olduvai carnivore site (Tanzania). Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 488, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.palaeo.2017.01.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balter, V., Braga, J., Télouk, P., and Thackeray, J. F. (2012). Evidence for dietary change but not landscape use in South African early hominins. Nature 489, 558–560. doi: 10.1038/nature11349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkai, R. (2019). “An elephant to share: rethinking the origins of meat and fat sharing in Palaeolithic societies,” in Towards a Broader View of Hunter Gatherer Sharing, eds N. Lavi and D. E. Friesem (Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs Series), 153–167.

Google Scholar

Barkai, R. (2020). Lower paleolithic bone handaxes and chopsticks: tools and symbols? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 30892–30893. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2016482117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkai, R., Rosell, J., Blasco, R., and Gopher, A. (2017). Fire for a reason: barbecue at Middle Pleistocene Qesem cave, Israel. Curr. Anthropol. 58, S314–S328. doi: 10.1086/691211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartram, L. E. (1993). An Ethnoarchaeological Analysis of Kua San (Botswana) Bone Food Refuses. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin.

Google Scholar

Bartram, L. E., and Marean, C. W. (1999). Explaining the “ Klasies Pattern “: kua ethnoarchaeology, the die kelders middle stone age archaeofauna, long bone. J. Archaeol. Sci. 26, 9–29. doi: 10.1006/jasc.1998.0291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Dor, M., and Barkai, R. (2020). The importance of large prey animals during the Pleistocene and the implications of their extinction on the use of dietary ethnographic analogies. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 59:101192. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2020.101192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Dor, M., Gopher, A., and Barkai, R. (2016). Neandertals’ large lower thorax may represent adaptation to high protein diet. Am. J. Biol. Anthropol. 160, 367–378. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Dor, M., Gopher, A., Hershkovitz, I., and Barkai, R. (2011). Man the fat hunter: the demise of Homo erectus and the emergence of a new hominin lineage in the Middle Pleistocene (ca. 400 kyr) Levant. PLoS One 6:e28689. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Dor, M., Sirtoli, R., and Barkai, R. (2021). The evolution of the human trophic level during the Pleistocene. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 175, 27–56. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.24247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berna, F., Goldberg, P., Horwitz, L. K., Brink, J., Holt, S., Bamford, M., et al. (2012). Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of wonderwerk cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, E1215–E1220. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117620109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binford, L. R. (1978). Nunamiut Ethnoarchaeology. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Binford, L. R. (1980). Le rôle de l’ethnoarchéologie dans la recherche archéologique. Les Nouv. Archéol. 4, 31–33. doi: 10.3917/lgh.050.0031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binford, L. R. (1981). Bones: Ancient Men and Modern Myths. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Bird, D. W., and O’Connell, J. F. (2006). Behavioral ecology and archaeology. J. Archaeol. Res. 14, 143–188. doi: 10.1007/s10814-006-9003-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birouste, C. (2020). Espèces animales et individus au Magdalénien moyen. Anthropozoologica 55, 233–246. doi: 10.5252/anthropozoologica2020v55a16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco, R., and Fernández Peris, J. (2012). A uniquely broad spectrum diet during the Middle Pleistocene at bolomor cave (Valencia, Spain). Quat. Int. 252, 16–31. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Arilla, M., Margalida, A., Villalba, D., Gopher, A., et al. (2019a). Bone marrow storage and delayed consumption at Middle Pleistocene qesem cave, Israel (420 to 200 ka). Sci. Adv. 5:eaav9822. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav9822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Sánchez-Marco, A., Gopher, A., and Barkai, R. (2019b). Feathers and food: human-bird interactions at Middle Pleistocene qesem cave, Israel. J. Hum. Evol. 136:102653. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.102653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Lozano, S., Pastó, I., Riba, D., et al. (2013). Learning by heart: cultural patterns in the faunal processing sequence during the Middle Pleistocene. PLoS One 8:e55863. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Rufà, A., Sánchez Marco, A., and Finlayson, C. (2016). Pigeons and choughs, a usual resource for the Neanderthals in Gibraltar. Quat. Int. 421, 62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.10.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumenschine, R. J. (1986). Early Hominid Scavenging Opportunities: Implications of Carcass Availability in the Serengeti and Ngorongoro Ecosystems, Vol. 283-284. Oxford: British Archaeological Reports. doi: 10.30861/9780860543657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blurton Jones, N. G. (1987). Tolerated theft, suggestions about the ecology and evolution of sharing, hoarding and scrounging. Soc. Sci. Inf. 26, 31–54. doi: 10.1177/053901887026001002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocherens, H., Drucker, D. G., Billiou, D., Patou-Mathis, M., and Vandermeersch, B. (2005). Isotopic evidence for diet and subsistence pattern of the Saint-Césaire I Neanderthal: review and use of a multi-source mixing model. J. Hum. Evol. 49, 71–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boschian, G., Caramella, D., Saccà, D., and Barkai, R. (2019). Are there marrow cavities in Pleistocene elephant limb bones, and was marrow available to early humans? New CT scan results from the site of Castel di Guido (Italy). Quat. Sci. Rev. 215, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2019.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boschian, G., and Saccà, D. (2015). In the elephant, everything is good: carcass use and re-use at Castel di Guido (Italy). Quat. Int. 361, 288–296. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2014.04.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brain, C. K. (1981). The Hunters or the Hunted? An Introduction to African Cave Taphonomy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Bramble, D. M., and Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature 432, 345–352. doi: 10.1038/nature03052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brantingham, P. J. (1998). Hominid-carnivore coevolution and invasion of the predatory guild. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 17, 327–353. doi: 10.1006/jaar.1998.0326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, D. R., Harris, J. W. K., Levin, N. E., McCoy, J. T., Herries, A. I. R., Bamford, M. K., et al. (2010). Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 10002–10007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002181107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buck, L. T., Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B. M., and Stringer, C. B. (2016). Tropical forager gastrophagy and its implications for extinct hominin diets. J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 5, 672–679. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2015.09.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunn, H. T. (1981). Archaeological evidence for meat-eating by plio-Pleistocene hominids from Koobi Fora and Olduvai gorge. Nature 291, 574–577. doi: 10.1038/291574a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunn, H. T. (1993). “Bone assemblages at base camps: a further consideration of carcass transport and bone destruction by the Hadza,” in From Bones to Behavior: Ethnoarchaeological and Experimental Contributions to the Interpretation of Faunal Remains, ed. J. Hudson (Carbondale: Southern Illinois University), 156–168.

Google Scholar

Bunn, H. T. (1994). Early Pleistocene hominid foraging strategies along the ancestral omo river at Koobi Fora, Kenya. J. Hum. Evol. 27, 247–266. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1994.1045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burini, R. C., and Leonard, W. R. (2018). The evolutionary roles of nutrition selection and dietary quality in the human brain size and encephalization. Nutrire 43:19. doi: 10.1186/s41110-018-0078-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., and Van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evol. Anthropol. 18, 175–186. doi: 10.1002/EVAN.20222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, R. N., Weintraub, G. S., and Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Energetic consequences of thermal and nonthermal food processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 19199–19203. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112128108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, R. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2009). The energetic significance of cooking. J. Hum. Evol. 57, 379–391. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, P. U., Archer, D., Pollard, D., Blum, J. D., Rial, J. A., Brovkin, V., et al. (2006). The Middle Pleistocene transition: characteristics, mechanisms, and implications for long-term changes in atmospheric pCO2. Quat. Sci. Rev. 25, 3150–3184. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2006.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleuziou, S. (1984). Des objets, des archéologues et des sauvages: vers une archéologie anthropologique? Raison Présente 69, 77–101. doi: 10.3406/raipr.1984.2351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochard, D. (2004). Les Léporidés Dans la Subsistance Paléolithique du Sud de la France. Bordeaux: Université de Bordeaux.

Google Scholar

Cochard, D., Brugal, J. P., Morin, E., and Meignen, L. (2012). Evidence of small fast game exploitation in the Middle paleolithic of Les Canalettes Aveyron, France. Quat. Int. 264, 32–51. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2012.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Codding, B. F., and Bird, D. W. (2015). Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological science. J. Archaeol. Sci. 56, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/J.JAS.2015.02.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conard, N. J., Serangeli, J., Bigga, G., and Rots, V. (2020). A 300,000-year-old throwing stick from Schöningen, northern Germany, documents the evolution of human hunting. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 4, 690–693. doi: 10.1038/s41559-020-1139-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornélio, A. M., de Bittencourt-Navarrete, R. E., de Bittencourt Brum, R., Queiroz, C. M., and Costa, M. R. (2016). Human brain expansion during evolution is independent of fire control and cooking. Front. Neurosci. 10:167. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2016.00167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costamagno, S., and Daujeard, C. (2021). “L’alimentation au Paléolithique et au Mésolithique,” in F. Quellier (dir.), Histoire de l’alimentation. De la préhistoire à nos jours, Belin Ed., 13–37.

Google Scholar

Costamagno, S., and David, F. (2009). Comparaison des pratiques bouchères et culinaires de différents groupes sibériens vivant de la renniculture. Archaeofauna 18, 9–25.

Google Scholar

Costamagno, S., and Laroulandie, V. (2004). “L’exploitation des petits vertébrés dans les pyrénées françaises du paléolithique au mésolithique: un inventaire taphonomique et archéozoologique,” in Petits Animaux et Sociétés Humaines. Du Complément Alimentaire Aux Ressources Utilitaires. Actes des XXIVe Rencontres Internationales D’archéologie et D’histoire, eds J.-P. Brugal and J. Desse (Antibes: APDCA), 369–382.

Google Scholar

Costamagno, S., Meignen, L., Beauval, C., Vandermeersch, B., and Maureille, B. (2006). Les Pradelles (marillac-le-franc, France): a mousterian reindeer hunting camp? J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 25, 466–484. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2006.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costamagno, S., and Rigaud, J.-P. (2013). “L’exploitation de la graisse au Paléolithique,” in 138ème Congrès National des Sociétés Historiques et Scientifiques, ed. C. Sandrine (Paris: Édition électronique du CTHS), 134–152.

Google Scholar

Crader, D. C. (1983). “Recent single-carcass bone scatters and the problem of ‘butchery’ sites in the archaeological record,” in Animals and Archaeology, 1. Hunters and their Prey, eds J. Clutton-Brock and C. Grigson (Oxford: BAR International Series), 107–141.

Google Scholar

Daegling, D. J., and Grine, F. E. (2007). “Mandibular biomechanics and the paleontological evidence for the evolution of human diet,” in The Evolution of Human Diet: The Known, the Unknown, and the Unknowable, ed. P. S. Ungar (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 77–105.

Google Scholar

Daujeard, C., Falguères, C., Shao, Q., Geraads, D., Hublin, J.-J., Lefèvre, D., et al. (2020). Earliest African evidence of carcass processing and consumption in cave at 700 ka, Casablanca, Morocco. Sci. Rep. 10:4761. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61580-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daujeard, C., Fernandes, P., Guadelli, J. L., Moncel, M. H., Santagata, C., and Raynal, J. P. (2012). Neanderthal subsistence strategies in Southeastern France between the plains of the rhone valley and the mid-mountains of the massif central (MIS 7 to MIS 3). Quat. Int. 252, 32–47. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.01.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daujeard, C., Vettese, D., Britton, K., Béarez, P., Boulbes, N., Crégut-Bonnoure, E., et al. (2019). Neanderthal selective hunting of reindeer? The case study of Abri du Maras (south-eastern France). Archaeol. Anthropol. Sci. 11, 985–1011. doi: 10.1007/s12520-017-0580-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Heinzelin, J., Clark, J. D., White, T., Hart, W., Renne, P., WoldeGabriel, G., et al. (1999). Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old bouri hominids. Science 284, 625–629. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5414.625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeCasien, A. R., and Higham, J. P. (2019). Primate mosaic brain evolution reflects selection on sensory and cognitive specialization. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 3, 1483–1493. doi: 10.1038/s41559-019-0969-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delagnes, A., Lenoble, A., Harmand, S., Brugal, J. P., Prat, S., Tiercelin, J. J., et al. (2006). Interpreting pachyderm single carcass sites in the African lower and early Middle Pleistocene record: a multidisciplinary approach to the site of Nadung’a 4 (Kenya). J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 25, 448–465. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2006.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demay, L., Belyaeva, V. I., Kulakovksa, L. V., Patou-Mathis, M., Péan, S., Stupak, D. V., et al. (2016a). New evidences about human activities during the first part of the upper pleniglacial in Ukraine from zooarchaeological studies. Quat. Int. 412, 16–36. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.12.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demay, L., Péan, S., Belyaeva, V. I., Vasil’ev, P. M., and Patou-Mathis, M. (2016b). Zooarchaeological study of an Upper Palaeolithic site with mammoth remains, pushkari I-excavation VII (Chernigov oblast, Ukraine). Quat. Int. 406, 183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demay, L., Péan, S., Germonpré, M., Obada, T., Haynes, G., Khlopachev, G. A., et al. (2021). “Upper Pleistocene hominins and wooly mammoths in the east European plain,” in Human-Elephant Interactions from Past to Present, eds G. Konidaris, R. Barkai, V. Tourloukis, and K. Harvati (Tübingen: Tübingen University Press). doi: 10.15496/publikation-55604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demay, L., Péan, S., and Patou-Mathis, M. (2012). Mammoths used as food and building resources by Neanderthals: zooarchaeological study applied to layer 4, Molodova I (Ukraine). Quat. Int. 276–277, 212–226. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.11.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Descola, P. (2005). Par-Delà Nature et Culture. Paris: Gallimard.

Google Scholar

Diez-Martín, F., Sánchez Yustos, P., Uribelarrea, D., Baquedano, E., Mark, D. F., Mabulla, A., et al. (2015). The origin of the acheulean: the 1.7 million-year-old site of FLK west, Olduvai Gorge (Tanzania). Sci. Rep. 5:17839. doi: 10.1038/srep17839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodat, P. J., Tacail, T., Albalat, E., Gómez-Olivencia, A., Couture-Veschambre, C., Holliday, T., et al. (2021). Isotopic calcium biogeochemistry of MIS 5 fossil vertebrate bones: application to the study of the dietary reconstruction of Regourdou 1 Neandertal fossil. J. Hum. Evol. 151:102925. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2020.102925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. (1999). Flesh availability and bone modifications in carcasses consumed by lions: palaeoecological relevance in hominid foraging patterns. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 149, 373–388. doi: 10.1016/S0031-0182(98)00213-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. (2002). Hunting and scavenging by the early hominids: the state of the debate. J. World Prehist. 16, 1–53. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63250-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., and Baquedano, E. (2018). Distinguishing butchery cut marks from crocodile bite marks through machine learning methods. Sci. Rep. 8:5786. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24071-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Bunn, H. T., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Baquedano, E., Uribelarrea, D., Pérez-González, A., et al. (2014). On meat eating and human evolution: a taphonomic analysis of BK4b (Upper Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania), and its bearing on hominin megafaunal consumption. Quat. Int. 322–323, 129–152. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2013.08.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Pickering, T. R., and Bunn, H. T. (2010). Configurational approach to identifying the earliest hominin butchers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 20929–20934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013711107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Pickering, T. R., and Bunn, H. T. (2011). Reply to McPherron et al.: doubting dikika is about data, not paradigms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108:E117. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104647108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drucker, D. G., Naito, Y. I., Péan, S., Prat, S., Crépin, L., Chikaraishi, Y., et al. (2017). Isotopic analyses suggest mammoth and plant in the diet of the oldest anatomically modern humans from far southeast Europe. Sci. Rep. 7:6833. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-07065-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Echassoux, A. (2012). Comportements de subsistance et modifications osseuses à l’aube de l’Acheuléen à Konso, Éthiopie. Anthropologie 116, 291–320. doi: 10.1016/j.anthro.2012.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enloe, J. (2003). Food sharing past and present. Before Farm. 2003, 1–23. doi: 10.3828/bfarm.2003.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Estalrrich, A., El Zaatari, S., and Rosas, A. (2017). Dietary reconstruction of the El Sidrón Neandertal familial group (Spain) in the context of other Neandertal and modern hunter-gatherer groups. A molar microwear texture analysis. J. Hum. Evol. 104, 13–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fellows Yates, J. A., Velsko, I. M., Aron, F., Posth, C., Hofman, C. A., Austin, R. M., et al. (2021). The evolution and changing ecology of the African hominid oral microbiome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118:e2021655118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2021655118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Jalvo, Y., Andrews, P., and Denys, C. (1999). Cut marks on small mammals at Olduvai Gorge Bed-I. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 587–589. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1999.0292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraro, J. V., Plummer, T. W., Pobiner, B. L., Oliver, J. S., Bishop, L. C., Braun, D. R., et al. (2013). Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS One 8:e62174. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiorenza, L., Benazzi, S., Henry, A. G., Salazar-García, D. C., Blasco, R., Picin, A., et al. (2015). To meat or not to meat? New perspectives on neanderthal ecology. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 156, 43–71. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forrest, F. L., Stynder, D. D., Bishop, L. C., Levin, N. E., Lehmann, S. B., Patterson, D. B., et al. (2018). Zooarchaeological reconstruction of newly excavated Middle Pleistocene deposits from Elandsfontein, South Africa. J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 17, 19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2017.10.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallay, A. (1980). Réflexion sur le concept d’ethnoarchéologie. Les Nouv. Archéol. 4, 34–42. doi: 10.3406/nda.1980.1969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudzinski, S., and Roebroeks, W. (2000). Adults only. Reindeer hunting at the Middle Palaeolithic site Salzgitter Lebenstedt, northern Germany. J. Hum. Evol. 38, 497–521. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1999.0359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudzinski, S., Turner, E., Anzidei, A. P., Àlvarez-Fernández, E., Arroyo-Cabrales, J., Cinq-Mars, J., et al. (2005). The use of Proboscidean remains in every-day Palaeolithic life. Quat. Int. 126–128, 179–194. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2004.04.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudzinski-Windheuser, S., Noack, E. S., Pop, E., Herbst, C., Pfleging, J., Buchli, J., et al. (2018). Evidence for close-range hunting by last interglacial Neanderthals. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 1087–1092. doi: 10.1038/s41559-018-0596-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez-Robles, A., Smaers, J. B., Holloway, R. L., Polly, P. D., and Wood, B. A. (2017). Brain enlargement and dental reduction were not linked in hominin evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 468–473. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608798114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grine, F. E., Sponheimer, M., Ungar, P. S., Lee-Thorp, J., and Teaford, M. F. (2012). Dental microwear and stable isotopes inform the paleoecology of extinct hominins. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 148, 285–317. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grove, M., Pearce, E., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Fission-fusion and the evolution of hominin social systems. J. Hum. Evol. 62, 191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guil-Guerrero, J. L., Rodríguez-García, I., Kirillova, I., Shidlovskiy, F., Ramos-Bueno, R. P., Savvinov, G., et al. (2015). The PUFA-enriched fatty acid profiles of some frozen bison from the early holocene found in the Siberian permafrost. Sci. Rep. 5:7926. doi: 10.1038/srep07926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guil-Guerrero, J. L., Tikhonov, A., Rodríguez-García, I., Protopopov, A., Grigoriev, S., and Ramos-Bueno, R. P. (2014). The fat from frozen mammals reveals sources of essential fatty acids suitable for Palaeolithic and Neolithic humans. PLoS One 9:e84480. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guillaud, E., Béarez, P., Daujeard, C., Defleur, A. R., Desclaux, E., Roselló-Izquierdo, E., et al. (2021). Neanderthal foraging in freshwater ecosystems: a reappraisal of the Middle Paleolithic archaeological fish record from continental Western Europe. Quat. Sci. Rev. 252:106731. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2020.106731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurven, M., and Jaeggi, A. V. (2015). “Food sharing,” in Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Science, eds R. S. Scott and S. Kosslyn (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications), 1–12. doi: 10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., and Gutierrez, M. (2006). How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life span. J. Hum. Evol. 51, 454–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, A. S., Mavuso, S. S., Biernat, M., Braun, D. R., Jinnah, Z., Kuo, S., et al. (2021). New hominin remains and revised context from the earliest Homo erectus locality in East Turkana, Kenya. Nat. Commun. 12:1939. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-22208-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, B. L., and Moncel, M. H. (2011). Neanderthal use of fish, mammals, birds, starchy plants and wood 125-250,000 years ago. PLoS One 6:e23768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, B. L., Moncel, M. H., Daujeard, C., Fernandes, P., Béarez, P., Desclaux, E., et al. (2013). Impossible Neanderthals? Making string, throwing projectiles and catching small game during Marine Isotope Stage 4 (Abri du Maras, France). Quat. Sci. Rev. 82, 23–40. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2013.09.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, K., Bocherens, H., Miller, J. B., and Copeland, L. (2022). Reconstructing Neanderthal diet: the case for carbohydrates. J. Hum. Evol. 162:103105. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, K., Brand-Miller, J., Brown, K. D., Thomas, M. G., and Copeland, L. (2015). The importance of dietary carbohydrate in human evolution. Q. Rev. Biol. 90, 251–268. doi: 10.1086/682587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, K., Buckley, S., Collins, M. J., Estalrrich, A., Brothwell, D., Copeland, L., et al. (2012). Neanderthal medics? Evidence for food, cooking, and medicinal plants entrapped in dental calculus. Naturwissenschaften 99, 617–626. doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0942-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., et al. (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521, 310–315. doi: 10.1038/nature14464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off. Tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethol. Sociobiol. 12, 29–54. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(91)90011-e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., and Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001). Hadza meat sharing. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 113–142. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00066-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., and Blurton-Jones, N. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the Hadza-Big game, common goods, foraging goals and the evolution of the human diet. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 334, 243–251. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1991.0113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynes, G., and Krasinski, K. (2021). Butchering marks on bones of Loxodonta africana (African savanna elephant): implications for interpreting marks on fossil proboscidean bones. J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 37:102957. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2021.102957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, A. G., Brooks, A. S., and Piperno, D. R. (2011). Microfossils in calculus demonstrate consumption of plants and cooked foods in Neanderthal diets (Shanidar III, Iraq; Spy I and II, Belgium). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 486–491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016868108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, A. G., Brooks, A. S., and Piperno, D. R. (2014). Plant foods and the dietary ecology of Neanderthals and early modern humans. J. Hum. Evol. 69, 44–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, A. G., Büdel, T., and Bazin, P. L. (2018). Towards an understanding of the costs of fire. Quat. Int. 493, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2018.06.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hlubik, S., Berna, F., Feibel, C., Braun, D., and Harris, J. W. K. (2017). Researching the nature of fire at 1.5 mya on the Site of FxJj20 AB, Koobi Fora, Kenya, using high-resolution spatial analysis and FTIR spectrometry. Curr. Anthropol. 58, S243–S257. doi: 10.1086/692530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Isaac, G. (1978). The food-sharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Sci. Am. 238, 90–108. doi: 10.1038/SCIENTIFICAMERICAN0478-90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isler, K., and van Schaik, C. P. (2012). How our ancestors broke through the gray ceiling: comparative evidence for cooperative breeding in early Homo. Curr. Anthropol. 53, 453–465. doi: 10.1086/667623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isler, K., and Van Schaik, C. P. (2014). How humans evolved large brains: comparative evidence. Evol. Anthropol. 23, 65–75. doi: 10.1002/evan.21403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaouen, K., Beasley, M., Schoeninger, M., Hublin, J. J., and Richards, M. P. (2016). Zinc isotope ratios of bones and teeth as new dietary indicators: results from a modern food web (Koobi Fora, Kenya). Sci. Rep. 6:26281. doi: 10.1038/srep26281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaouen, K., Richards, M. P., Cabec, A. L., Welker, F., Rendu, W., Hublin, J. J., et al. (2019). Exceptionally high δ15N values in collagen single amino acids confirm Neandertals as high-trophic level carnivores. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 4928–4933. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1814087116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Cadeliña, R. V., Hayden, B., Hyndman, D. C., Preston, R. J., et al. (1985). Food sharing among ache foragers: tests of explanatory hypotheses [and comments and reply]. Curr. Anthropol. 26, 223–246. doi: 10.1086/203251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karavanić, I., Richards, M. P., Pettitt, P. B., Trinkaus, E., and Smith, F. H. (2000). Neanderthal diet at Vindija and Neanderthal predation: the evidence from stable isotopes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 7663–7666. doi: 10.1073/pnas.120178997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keene, A. S. (1985). “Nutrition and economy: models for the study of prehistoric diet,” in The Analysis of Prehistoric Diets, eds R. I. Gilbert and J. H. Mileke (Orlando, FL: Academic Press), 155–190.

Google Scholar

Kelly, R. L. (1995). The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways. Laramie, WY: Smithsonian Institution Press.

Google Scholar

Kraft, T. S., Venkataraman, V. V., Wallace, I. J., Crittenden, A. N., Holowka, N. B., Stieglitz, J., et al. (2021). The energetics of uniquely human subsistence strategies. Science 374:eabf0130. doi: 10.1126/science.abf0130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, K. L., Otarola-Castillo, E., and Ot Arola-Castillo, E. (2015). When mothers need others: the impact of hominin life history evolution on cooperative breeding. J. Hum. Evol. 84, 16–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krief, S., Daujeard, C., Moncel, M.-H., Lamon, N., and Reynolds, V. (2015). Flavouring food: the contribution of chimpanzee behaviour to the understanding of Neanderthal calculus composition and plant use in Neanderthal diets. Antiquity 89, 464–471. doi: 10.15184/aqy.2014.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laroulandie, V., Faivre, J. P., Gerbe, M., and Mourre, V. (2016). Who brought the bird remains to the Middle Palaeolithic site of Les Fieux (Southwestern, France)? Direct evidence of a complex taphonomic story. Quat. Int. 421, 116–133. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.06.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. B., and Daly, R. (1999). “Introduction: foragers and the others,” in The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Hunters and Gatherers, eds R. B. Lee and R. Daly (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1–19. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511752964.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, W. R., and Robertson, M. L. (1997). Comparative primate energetics and hominid evolution. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 102, 265–281. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199702)102:2<265::AID-AJPA8>3.0.CO;2-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, W. R., Snodgrass, J. J., and Robertson, M. L. (2011). Handbook of Behavior, Food and Nutrition. New York, NY: Springer. 2–15. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-92271-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., Harmand, S., Quinn, R. L., Brugal, J. P., et al. (2011). An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature 477, 82–85. doi: 10.1038/nature10372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leroi-Gourhan, A., and Brézillon, M. (1972). Fouilles de Pincevent. Essai D’analyse Ethnographique d’un Habitat Magdalénien. Paris: CNRS.

Google Scholar

Lévi-Strauss, C. (1952). Race et Histoire. Paris: UNESCO.

Google Scholar

Lévi-Strauss, C. (1964). Le Cru et le Cuit. Paris: Plon.

Google Scholar

Liem, K. F. (1980). Adaptive significance of intra- and interspecific differences in the feeding repertoires of cichlid fishes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 20, 295–314. doi: 10.1093/icb/20.1.295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Louail, M., Gilissen, E., Prat, S., Garcia, C., and Bouret, S. (2019). Refining the ecological brain: strong relation between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and feeding ecology in five primate species. Cortex 118, 262–274. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupo, K. D. (2001). Archaeological skeletal part profiles and differential transport: an ethnoarchaeological example from Hadza bone assemblages. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 20, 361–378. doi: 10.1006/jaar.2000.0378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupo, K. D., and Schmitt, D. N. (2016). When bigger is not better: the economics of hunting megafauna and its implications for Plio-Pleistocene hunter-gatherers. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 44, 185–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2016.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, K., Scherjon, F., van Veen, E., Vaesen, K., and Roebroeks, W. (2021). Middle Pleistocene fire use: the first signal of widespread cultural diffusion in human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118:e2101108118. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.2101108118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magargal, K. (2022). The cost of cooking for foragers. J. Hum. Evol. 162:103091. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marean, C. W. (1989). Sabertooth cats and their relevance for early hominid diet and evolution. J. Hum. Evol. 18, 559–582. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(89)90018-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, J. E., Tacail, T., Braga, J., Cerling, T. E., and Balter, V. (2020). Calcium isotopic ecology of Turkana Basin hominins. Nat. Commun. 11:3587. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17427-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masset, C., Costamagno, S., Cochard, D., and Laroulandie, V. (2016). La fracturation osseuse: du fait technique à l’essai d’interprétation sociétale. Bull. Soc. Prehist. Fr. 113, 691–712. doi: 10.3406/bspf.2016.14684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherron, S. P., Alemseged, Z., Marean, C., Wynn, J. G., Reed, D., Geraads, D., et al. (2011). Tool-marked bones from before the Oldowan change the paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108:E116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101298108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherron, S. P., Alemseged, Z., Marean, C. W., Wynn, J. G., Reed, D., Geraads, D., et al. (2010). Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 466, 857–860. doi: 10.1038/nature09248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherron, S. P., Archer, W., Otárola-Castillo, E. R., Torquato, M. G., and Keevil, T. L. (2021). Machine learning, bootstrapping, null models, and why we are still not 100% sure which bone surface modifications were made by crocodiles. J. Hum. Evol. [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moncel, M., Chacón, M. G., Vettese, D., Courty, M., Daujeard, C., Eixea, A., et al. (2021). Late Neanderthal short-term and specialized occupations at the Abri du Maras (South-East France, level 4.1, MIS 3). Archaeol. Anthropol. Sci. 13:45. doi: 10.1007/s12520-021-01285-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, E. (2020). Rethinking the emergence of bone grease procurement. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 59:101178. doi: 10.1016/j.jaa.2020.101178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, E., and Laroulandie, V. (2012). Presumed symbolic use of diurnal raptors by Neanderthals. PLoS One 7:e32856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, E., Meier, J., El Guennouni, K., Moigne, A. M., Lebreton, L., Rusch, L., et al. (2019). New evidence of broader diets for archaic Homo populations in the northwestern Mediterranean. Sci. Adv. 5:eaav9106. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav9106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosquera, M., Saladié, P., Ollé, A., Cáceres, I., Huguet, R., Villalaín, J. J., et al. (2015). Barranc de la Boella (Catalonia, Spain): an Acheulean elephant butchering site from the European late Early Pleistocene. J. Quat. Sci. 30, 651–666. doi: 10.1002/jqs.2800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naito, Y. I., Chikaraishi, Y., Drucker, D. G., Ohkouchi, N., Semal, P., Wißing, C., et al. (2016). Ecological niche of Neanderthals from Spy Cave revealed by nitrogen isotopes of individual amino acids in collagen. J. Hum. Evol. 93, 82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, A., Van Schaik, C. P., and Isler, K. (2011). Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature 480, 91–93. doi: 10.1038/nature10629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niven, L., Steele, T. E., Rendu, W., Mallye, J. B., McPherron, S. P., Soressi, M., et al. (2012). Neandertal mobility and large-game hunting: the exploitation of reindeer during the Quina Mousterian at Chez-Pinaud Jonzac (Charente-Maritime, France). J. Hum. Evol. 63, 624–635. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Jones, N. B. (1988a). Hadza hunting, butchering, and bone transport and their archaeological implications. J. Anthropol. Res. 44, 113–161.

Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Jones, N. B. (1988b). Hadza scavenging: implications for Plio/Pleistocene Hominid subsistence. Curr. Anthropol. 29, 356–363. doi: 10.1086/203648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Jones, N. B. (1990). Reanalysis of large mammal body part transport among the Hadza. J. Archaeol. Sci. 17, 301–316. doi: 10.1016/0305-4403(90)90025-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., and Jones, N. G. B. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 461–485. doi: 10.1006/JHEV.1998.0285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Lupo, K. D., and Blurton Jones, N. G. (2002). Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. J. Hum. Evol. 43, 831–872. doi: 10.1006/jhev.2002.0604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, J. F., and Marshall, B. (1989). Analysis of kangaroo body part transport among the Alyawara of Central Australia. J. Archaeol. Sci. 16, 393–405. doi: 10.1016/0305-4403(89)90014-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Organ, C., Nunn, C. L., Machanda, Z., and Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Phylogenetic rate shifts in feeding time during the evolution of Homo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 14555–14559. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.1107806108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pante, M. C. (2013). The larger mammal fossil assemblage from JK2, Bed III, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania: implications for the feeding behavior of Homo erectus. J. Hum. Evol. 64, 68–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelletier, M., Desclaux, E., Brugal, J.-P., and Texier, P.-J. (2019). The exploitation of rabbits for food and pelts by last interglacial Neandertals. Quat. Sci. Rev. 224, 105972. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2019.105972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peresani, M., Fiore, I., Gala, M., Romandini, M., and Tagliacozzo, A. (2011). Late Neandertals and the intentional removal of feathers as evidenced from bird bone taphonomy at Fumane Cave 44 ky B.P., Italy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3888–3893. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016212108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontzer, H. (2012). Ecological energetics in early Homo. Curr. Anthropol. 53, 346–358. doi: 10.1086/667402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontzer, H. (2017). The crown joules: energetics, ecology, and evolution in humans and other primates. Evol. Anthropol. 26, 12–24. doi: 10.1002/evan.21513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontzer, H., Raichlen, D. A., Wood, B. M., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Racette, S. B., and Marlowe, F. W. (2012). Hunter-gatherer energetics and human obesity. PLoS One 7:e40503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontzer, H., and Wood, B. M. (2021). Effects of evolution, ecology, and economy on human diet: insights from hunter-gatherers and other small-scale societies. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 41, 363–385. doi: 10.1146/annurev-nutr-111120-105520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potts, R., and Shipman, P. (1981). Cutmarks made by stone tools on bones from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Nature 291, 577–580.

Google Scholar

Power, R. C., Salazar-García, D. C., Rubini, M., Darlas, A., Havarti, K., Walker, M., et al. (2018). Dental calculus indicates widespread plant use within the stable Neanderthal dietary niche. J. Hum. Evol. 119, 27–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2018.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prat, S., Péan, S. C., Crépin, L., Drucker, D. G., Puaud, S. J., Valladas, H., et al. (2011). The oldest anatomically modern humans from far southeast europe: direct dating, culture and behavior. PLoS One 6:e20834. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruetz, J. D., Bertolani, P., Ontl, K. B., Lindshield, S., Shelley, M., and Wessling, E. G. (2015). New evidence on the tool-assisted hunting exhibited by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in a savannah habitat at Fongoli, Sénégal. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2:140507. doi: 10.1098/rsos.140507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyke, G. H., and Stephens, D. W. (2019). Optimal Foraging Theory: Application and Inspiration in Human Endeavors Outside Biology, 2nd Edn. Amsterdam: Elsevier. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-809633-8.90161-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinovich, R., Gaudzinski-Windheuser, S., and Goren-Inbar, N. (2008). Systematic butchering of fallow deer (Dama) at the early Middle Pleistocene Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov (Israel). J. Hum. Evol. 54, 134–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rendu, W., Costamagno, S., Meignen, L., and Soulier, M. C. (2012). Monospecific faunal spectra in Mousterian contexts: implications for social behavior. Quat. Int. 247, 50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.01.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, D., Grün, R., Joannes-Boyau, R., Steele, T. E., Amani, F., Rué, M., et al. (2017). The age of the hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, and the origins of the Middle Stone Age. Nature 546, 293–296. doi: 10.1038/nature22335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, J., Rodríguez-Gómez, G., Martín-González, J. A., Goikoetxea, I., and Mateos, A. (2012). Predator-prey relationships and the role of Homo in Early Pleistocene food webs in Southern Europe. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 365–366, 99–114. doi: 10.1016/j.palaeo.2012.09.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A., Morales, J. I., Cebrià, A., Courtenay, L. A., Fernández-Marchena, J. L., García-Argudo, G., et al. (2019). The châtelperronian neanderthals of cova foradada (Calafell, Spain) used imperial eagle phalanges for symbolic purposes. Sci. Adv. 5:eaax1984. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aax1984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebroeks, W., and Villa, P. (2011). On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 5209–5214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018116108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rufà, A., Blasco, R., Rosell, J., and Vaquero, M. (2018). What is going on at the Molí del Salt site? A zooarchaeological approach to the last hunter-gatherers from South Catalonia. Hist. Biol. 30, 786–806. doi: 10.1080/08912963.2017.1315685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rufa Bonache, A. (2017). El Papel de las Pequeñas Presas en la Dieta Humana del Paleolítico Medio y Superior. Una Visión Diacrónica Desde el Valle del Ródano al Arco Mediterráneo. Tarragona: Universitat Rovira i Virgili.

Google Scholar

Sahle, Y., El Zaatari, S., and White, T. D. (2017). Hominid butchers and biting crocodiles in the African Plio–Pleistocene. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 13164–13169. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1716317114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shahack-Gross, R., Berna, F., Karkanas, P., Lemorini, C., Gopher, A., and Barkai, R. (2014). Evidence for the repeated use of a central hearth at Middle Pleistocene (300ky ago) Qesem Cave, Israel. J. Archaeol. Sci. 44, 12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2013.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipman, P., and Walker, A. (1989). The costs of becoming a predator. J. Hum. Evol. 18, 373–392. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(89)90037-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmen, B., Morino, L., Blanc, S., and Garcia, C. (2021). The energy allocation trade-offs underlying life history traits in hypometabolic strepsirhines and other primates. Sci. Rep. 11:14196. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93764-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sistiaga, A., Husain, F., Uribelarrea, D., Martín-Perea, D. M., Ferland, T., Freeman, K. H., et al. (2020). Microbial biomarkers reveal a hydrothermally active landscape at Olduvai Gorge at the dawn of the Acheulean, 1.7 Ma. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 24720–24728. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2004532117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. R., Carmody, R. N., Dutton, R. J., and Wrangham, R. W. (2015). The significance of cooking for early hominin scavenging. J. Hum. Evol. 84, 62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. A. (1979). Human adaptation and energetic efficiency. Hum. Ecol. 7, 53–74. doi: 10.1007/bf00889352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. A. (1992). Human behavioral ecology. Evol. Anthropol. 1, 20–25.

Google Scholar

Smith, E. A., Bettinger, R. L., Bishop, C. A., Blundell, V., Cashdan, E., Casimir, M. J., et al. (1983). Anthropological applications of optimal foraging theory: a critical review. Curr. Anthropol. 24, 625–651.

Google Scholar

Smith, G. M. (2015). Neanderthal megafaunal exploitation in Western Europe and its dietary implications: a contextual reassessment of La Cotte de St Brelade (Jersey). J. Hum. Evol. 78, 181–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. E., Swanson, E. M., Reed, D., and Holekamp, K. E. (2012). Evolution of cooperation among mammalian carnivores and its relevance to hominin evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 53, 436–452. doi: 10.1086/667653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snodgrass, J. J., Leonard, W. R., and Robertson, M. L. (2009). “The energetics of encephalization in early hominids,” in The Evolution of Hominin Diets. Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology, eds J. J. Hublin and M. P. Richards (Dordrecht: Springer), 15–29. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-9699-0_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J. D. (1987). Early hominid subsistence strategies in seasonal habitats. J. Archaeol. Sci. 14, 13–29. doi: 10.1016/S0305-4403(87)80003-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J. D. (1989). Early hominid hunting and scavenging: the role of meat as an energy source. J. Hum. Evol. 18, 329–343. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(89)90035-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J. D. (2010). The Paleoanthropology and Archaeology of Big-Game Hunting. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Speth, J. D. (2015). When did humans learn to boil? Paleoanthropology 2015, 54–67. doi: 10.4207/PA.2015.ART96

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J. D. (2017). Putrid meat and fish in the eurasian Middle and upper paleolithic: are we missing a key part of neanderthal and modern human diet? Paleoantrhopology 2017, 44–72. doi: 10.4207/PA.2017.ART105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J. D., and Spielmann, K. A. (1983). Energy source, protein metabolism, and hunter-gatherer subsistence strategies. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 2, 1–31.

Google Scholar

Stephens, D. W., and Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Attenborough, R. D., Mabulla, I. A., and Marlowe, F. W. (2020). To the hunter go the spoils? No evidence of nutritional benefit to being or marrying a well-reputed Hadza hunter. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 173, 61–79. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.24027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stiner, M. C., Barkai, R., and Gopher, A. (2009). Cooperative hunting and meat sharing 400-200 kya at Qesem Cave, Israel. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 13207–13212. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900564106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stiner, M. C., Munro, N. D., and Surovell, T. A. (2000). The tortoise and the hare. Small-game use, the broad spectrum revolution, and paleoltihic demography. Curr. Anthropol. 41, 39–60. doi: 10.2307/3596428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strassmann, B. I., and Kurapati, N. T. (2010). Are humans cooperative breeders?: most studies of natural fertility populations do not support the grandmother hypothesis. Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 35–39. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09991749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svoboda, J., Péan, S., and Wojtal, P. (2005). Mammoth bone deposits and subsistence practices during Mid-Upper Palaeolithic in Central Europe: three cases from Moravia and Poland. Quat. Int. 126–128, 209–221. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2004.04.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teaford, M. F., and Ungar, P. S. (2000). Diet and the evolution of the earliest human ancestors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 13506–13511. doi: 10.1073/pnas.260368897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teaford, M. F., Ungar, P. S., and Grine, F. E. (2002). “Paleontological evidence for the diets of African Plio-Pleistocene hominins with special reference to early Homo,” in Human Diet: Its Origin and Evolution, eds P. S. Ungar and M. F. Teaford (Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey), 143–166.

Google Scholar

Thompson, J. C., McPherron, S. P., Bobe, R., Reed, D., Barr, W. A., Wynn, J. G., et al. (2015). Taphonomy of fossils from the hominin-bearing deposits at Dikika, Ethiopia. J. Hum. Evol. 86, 112–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toth, N. (2017). Revised taphonomic perspective on African Plio–Pleistocene fauna. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 13066–13067. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1718815114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, A. (1992). Large carnivores and earliest European hominids: changing determinants of resource availability during the Lower and Middle Pleistocene. J. Hum. Evol. 22, 109–126. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(92)90033-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ungar, P. S. (2004). Dental topography and diets of Australopithecus afarensis and early Homo. J. Hum. Evol. 46, 605–622. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ungar, P. S. (2012). Dental evidence for the reconstruction of diet in African early Homo. Curr. Anthropol. 53, S318–S329. doi: 10.1086/666700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ungar, P. S., and Sponheimer, M. (2011). The diets of early hominins. Science 334, 190–193. doi: 10.1126/science.1207701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Schaik, C. P., and Burkart, J. M. (2010). “Mind the gap: cooperative breeding and the evolution of our unique features,” in Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals, eds P. Kappeler and J. Silk (Berlin: Springer), 477–496. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vettese, D., Daujeard, C., Blasco, R., Borel, A., Caceres, I., and Moncel, M. H. (2017). Neandertal long bone breakage process?: standardized or random patterns?? The example of Abri du Maras (Southeastern France, MIS 3). J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 13, 151–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2017.03.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, D. P. (2020). Meat eating by nonhuman primates: a review and synthesis. J. Hum. Evol. 149:102882. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2020.102882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weyrich, L. S., Duchene, S., Soubrier, J., Arriola, L., Llamas, B., Breen, J., et al. (2017). Neanderthal behaviour, diet, and disease inferred from ancient DNA in dental calculus. Nature 544, 357–361. doi: 10.1038/nature21674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willems, E. P., and van Schaik, C. P. (2017). The social organization of Homo ergaster: inferences from anti-predator responses in extant primates. J. Hum. Evol. 109, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2017.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B. (1981). Optimal Foraging Strategies and Hunter-Gatherer Research in Anthropology: Theory and Models. Davis, CA: University of California.

Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B. (1983). “Boreal foraging strategies,” in Boreal Forest Adaptations: the Northern Algonkians, ed. A. T. J. Steegman (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 201–241.

Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B. (1996). Social foraging and the behavioral ecology of intragroup resource transfers. Evol. Anthropol. 5, 46–57. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6505(1996)5:2<46::aid-evan4>3.0.co;2-u

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B. (1997). Gifts given, gifts taken: the behavioral ecology of nonmarket, intragroup exchange. J. Archaeol. Res. 5, 121–168. doi: 10.1007/BF02229109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B. (2001). “Intragroup resource transfers: comparative evidence, models and implication for human evolution,” in Meat-Eating and Human Evolution, eds C. B. Stanford and H. T. Bunn (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 279–301.

Google Scholar

Winterhalder, B., and Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evol. Anthropol. 9, 51–72. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6505(2000)9:2<51::aid-evan1>3.0.co;2-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wißing, C., Rougier, H., Crevecoeur, I., Germonpré, M., Naito, Y. I., Semal, P., et al. (2016). Isotopic evidence for dietary ecology of late Neandertals in North-Western Europe. Quat. Int. 411, 327–345. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2015.09.091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrangham, R., and Conklin-Brittain, N. (2003). Cooking as a biological trait. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 136, 35–46. doi: 10.1016/S1095-6433(03)00020-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrangham, R. W., Jones, J. H., Laden, G., Pilbeam, D., and Conklin-Brittain, N. (1999). The Raw and the Stolen. Curr. Anthropol. 40, 567–594. doi: 10.1086/300083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yellen, J. E. (1977). Ethnoarchaeological Approaches to the Present: Models for Reconstructing the Past. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Yravedra, J., Rubio-Jara, S., Courtenay, L. A., and Martos, J. A. (2020). Mammal butchery by Homo erectus at the Lower Pleistocene acheulean site of Juma’s korongo 2 (JK2), bed III, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Quat. Sci. Rev. 249:106612. doi: 10.1016/j.quascirev.2020.106612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zink, K. D., and Lieberman, D. E. (2016). Impact of meat and Lower Palaeolithic food processing techniques on chewing in humans. Nature 531, 500–503. doi: 10.1038/nature16990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zink, K. D., Lieberman, D. E., and Lucas, P. W. (2014). Food material properties and early hominin processing techniques. J. Hum. Evol. 77, 155–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Pleistocene, energetics, extinct hominins, behavioral ecology, meat and fat consumption

Citation: Daujeard C and Prat S (2022) What Are the “Costs and Benefits” of Meat-Eating in Human Evolution? The Challenging Contribution of Behavioral Ecology to Archeology. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10:834638. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.834638

Received: 13 December 2021; Accepted: 28 January 2022;
Published: 14 March 2022.

Edited by:

Florent Rivals, Catalan Institution for Research and Advanced Studies (ICREA), Spain

Reviewed by:

Karen Lupo, Southern Methodist University, United States
Ran Barkai, Tel Aviv University, Israel

Copyright © 2022 Daujeard and Prat. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Camille Daujeard, Y2FtaWxsZS5kYXVqZWFyZEBtbmhuLmZy

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.