Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Digit. Health, 27 June 2023
Sec. Digital Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Digital tools for relaxation and stress management: use, effectiveness and implementation View all 6 articles

Moderators and mediators of change of an internet-based mindfulness intervention for college students: secondary analysis from a randomized controlled trial

  • 1Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
  • 2Department for Sport and Health Sciences, Professorship for Psychology & Digital Mental Health Care, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany

Background: Existing evidence suggests internet- and mobile-based interventions (IMIs) improve depressive symptoms in college students effectively. However, there is far less knowledge about the potential mechanisms of change of mindfulness-based IMIs, which could contribute to optimizing target groups and interventions. Hence, within this secondary analysis of data from a randomized controlled trial (RCT), potential moderators and mediators of the effectiveness of the IMI StudiCare Mindfulness were investigated.

Methods: Moderation and mediation analyses were based on secondary data from a RCT that examined the effectiveness of the 7-module IMI StudiCare Mindfulness in a sample of college students (intervention group: n = 217; waitlist control group: n = 127). Assessments were collected before (t0; baseline), 4 weeks after (t1; during intervention), and 8 weeks after (t2; post-intervention) randomization. Longitudinal mediation analyses using structural equation modeling were employed, with depressive symptom severity as the dependent variable. For moderation analyses, bilinear interaction models were calculated with depressive symptom severity and mindfulness at t2 as dependent variables. All data-analyses were performed on an intention-to-treat basis.

Results: Mediation analyses showed a significant, full mediation of the intervention effect on depressive symptom severity through mindfulness (indirect effect, a*b = 0.153, p < 0.01). Only the number of semesters (interaction: β = 0.24, p = 0.035) was found to moderate the intervention's effectiveness on depressive symptom severity at t2, and only baseline mindfulness (interaction: β = −0.20, p = 0.047) and baseline self-efficacy (interaction: β = −0.27, p = 0.012) were found to be significant moderators of the intervention effect on mindfulness at t2.

Conclusion: Our results suggest a mediating role of mindfulness. Moderation analyses demonstrated that the intervention improved depressive symptom severity and mindfulness independent of most examined baseline characteristics. Future confirmatory trials will need to support these findings.

Clinical Trial Registration: The trial was registered a priori at the WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform via the German Clinical Studies Trial Register (TRN: DRKS00014774; registration date: 18 May 2018).

1. Introduction

Numerous studies have shown that college students are at risk of developing mental health problems (14). Given the multiple stressors young people face persuing their academic careers (5), it is not surprising that about one-third of them develop psychological disorders such as depression (21%) (6). During the COVID-19 pandemic college students have been confronted with additional challenges, resulting in an increase of mental health problems. For example, the prevalence of depression was estimated to be between 34% and 39% (79). On the other hand, utilization of support services has been reported to be as low as 20% (1012) due to barriers such as the preference to handle problems alone, embarrassment, and the lack of professional help offers. Internet- and mobile-based interventions (IMIs) have been suggested as a potential solution to this problem (13, 14). IMIs can be used anonymously at a time and place suitable to the user and are scalable, enabling the provision of mental health services to a large number of students (15). Evidence suggests that mindfulness-based interventions, such as Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) (16) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) (17), can effectively reduce mental health problems in college students (18) and can be successfully delivered via IMIs (19).

Existing research demonstrates that IMIs can be as effective as therapeutically guided face-to-face therapy (20). Recent meta-analytic results show mindfulness-based IMIs effectively improve mental health outcomes such as depressive symptoms (g = 0.34, 95% CI 0.18–0.50) (19). However, knowledge about how and for whom these interventions work is far less extensive (21, 22). Both the mechanisms of change, through which they work (mediators) and the circumstances, under which psychological interventions are effective (moderators), should be studied as they provide information for optimizing interventions and better adapting them to target groups (23, 24). Knowledge about mediators can identify the most effective treatment components, whereas knowledge about moderators can support tailoring interventions to specific subpopulations (23). Therefore, the current study aimed to identify potential moderators and mediators of change by analyzing secondary data from a randomized controlled trial (RCT) evaluating the effectiveness of the mindfulness-based IMI StudiCare Mindfulness in a college student population (N = 387) (25), as described a priori in the study protocol (26). The intervention consists of 7 modules based on elements of MBSR and ACT. Two different versions of StudiCare Mindfulness, an unguided one (UG) and a minimally guided one (“guidance on demand”, GoD), were demonstrated to be effective compared to a waitlist control group (WLC). Analyses revealed moderate to large effects on the primary outcome mindfulness after 4 weeks (d = 0.75–0.76, 95% CI 0.05–1.02), 8 weeks (d = 0.91–1.06, 95% CI 0.66–1.32), and 6 months (d = 0.87–1.03, 95% CI 0.61–1.29), as well as small effects on the secondary outcome depression after 4 weeks (d = −0.25–−0.22, 95% CI −0.49–0.02), 8 weeks (d = −0.44–−0.33, 95% CI −0.68–−0.08), and 6 months (d = −0.39–−0.31, 95% CI −0.64–−0.07).

Potential moderators and mediators to be investigated within RCTs should be carefully selected based on a strong theoretical foundation (23). When examining mechanisms in mindfulness-based IMIs, both variables specific to the theoretical framework that the mindfulness intervention is built upon (specific factors) and to the IMI setting in general (common factors associated with digital delivery) should be considered (27).

First, the development of mindfulness skills, resulting in a non-judgmental and non-reactive acceptance of all experiences, is a central goal of MBSR and is assumed to be a key mechanism leading to the improvement of mental health outcomes (16). In their systematic review and meta-analysis investigating mediators of change of MBSR and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) (28), Gu and colleagues found moderate, consistent evidence for mindfulness as a mediator of the intervention's effectiveness on mental health outcomes such as depression (21). However, the majority of the included studies were unable to investigate the temporal precedence of changes in mindfulness, which is a crucial criterion for the establishment of mediation (23) and can only be tested with longitudinal designs with at least three assessments. It is met if the intervention initially leads to a change in the mediator, which then leads to a change in the outcome variable. Hence, such studies are needed to confirm mindfulness as a mechanism of change.

Second, as StudiCare Mindfulness integrates elements of ACT (29), another potential mediator is cognitive defusion (CD). It refers to the ability to perceive cognitions and emotions as objective events occurring in our minds rather than to identify with these experiences (29). Thus, CD alleviates psychological well-being by distancing oneself from negative internal events. A systematic review confirmed the mediating role of CD on the effectiveness of ACT and established temporal precedence (22). However, only four studies were included in the analysis. Additionally, to our knowledge, CD as a mediator in the IMI setting has only been studied and confirmed in two RCTs by one research group (30, 31). Thus, results need to be replicated across studies, samples, conditions, and settings to establish mediation (23).

Third, theoretical considerations (32) and existing evidence (33, 34) have suggested emotion regulation (ER) as a potential mediator of mindfulness-based interventions. ER can be defined as the process of influencing emotions in terms of when and how a person experiences and expresses emotions (35). Two such strategies are cognitive reappraisal, an antecedent-focused, experience-modifying strategy, and expressive suppression, a response-focused, expression-modifying strategy (36). Reappraisal has been hypothesized to be a process that occurs during mindful meditation practice through reinterpreting stressful events as helpful or meaningful (32). For example, a large survey study (N = 1,151) found reappraisal and supression to mediate the relationship between mindfulness and depression (37). A RCT found that changes in ER difficulties mediate the relationship between mindfulness and psychological distress in participants of a mindfulness-based IMI (38). This was replicated in another study that found partial mediation of the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based IMI on mental health improvement by ER (39). However, evidence is still scarce, and more research is needed to confirm these results.

Fourth, mindfulness interventions might work by enhancing clarity about one's internal experiences, which might contribute to managing negative emotions (40). An essential construct in this context is alexithymia, a trait that entails difficulties identifying and communicating emotions (41). Alexithymia is associated with various mental health problems and its reduction has been shown to be beneficial to physical and mental health (42). A systematic review examining the effect of mindfulness-based interventions on the outcome of alexithymia (4 RCTs) found significant improvement compared to active and passive control groups (42). Additionally, evidence suggests that improvements in emotional clarity mediate the relationship between mindfulness and mental health (40). However, to our knowledge, no study has tested a potential mediating role of alexithymia reduction on mental health outcomes in mindfulness-based interventions.

Fifth, self-efficacy (SE) has been proposed as a potential mediator of IMI effectiveness (43). Perceived SE refers to a person's belief that they can successfully perform a behavior to produce a specific outcome (44). Thus, SE will influence the initiation and persistence of coping efforts, e.g., whether and how someone will perform particular stress management strategies. Because it is in the nature of self-help interventions to empower participants to help themselves, SE could be a specifically relevant mediator in the context of IMIs. Evidence suggests SE as a mediator of face-to-face psychotherapy (45, 46), but it has not been systematically studied in the IMI setting. To our knowledge, only one study has examined and confirmed the mediating role of mental health SE on mental health outcomes (47). Another study found the closely related construct of “perceived control” to mediate the effects of a CBT-based IMI on depressive symptoms (48). So far, no trial has studied perceived SE in mindfulness-based IMI.

To investigate the potential differential effectiveness of mindfulness-based IMI, moderation analyses should be conducted. To date, different moderators of change have been studied in mindfulness-based interventions, e.g., sociodemographic variables (49), pre-treatment mindfulness (50, 51), and baseline symptomatology (49, 52). Additional moderators of IMI effectiveness that can be derived from other treatment approaches are expectations regarding the intervention's effectiveness (53) and pre-treatment SE (47). Overall, the evidence regarding moderators of effectiveness in mindfulness-based IMIs is scarce, inconclusive, and often exhibits methodological limitations (e.g., insufficient power). Therefore, further research is urgently needed.

In summary, intervention research should routinely include exploratory analyses of moderators and mediators of change (24). However, with regard to mindfulness-based IMIs, such research is still in its infancy (49), warranting further investigation as such factors might be specific to therapeutic approaches (54) or settings (27). Therefore, in the presented secondary analysis of data gathered in a RCT evaluating the effectiveness of the IMI StudiCare Mindfulness, we investigated (1) the potential moderating effect of various variables on the intervention's effectiveness, and (2) several potential mediators as outlined above, considering three assessment time points, thus enabling the establishment of temporal precedence. An overview of the examined moderator and mediator variables can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of potential moderator and mediator variables examined in this study.

2. Methods

2.1. Study design

This secondary, time-lagged mediation and moderation study was based on a three-armed randomized controlled trial (RCT), which was originally designed to evaluate the effectiveness of two versions of the Internet- and mobile-based intervention (IMI) StudiCare Mindfulness in comparison to a waitlist control group (WL) as well as to each other (see Figure 1 for flowchart). One intervention group (UG) received no additional guidance (unguided) to the intervention, the other (GoD) received additional so-called “guidance on demand”, a form of guidance where participants can ask their e-coach (a trained psychologist) for support whenever they need it. For the scope of this secondary study, the two intervention groups were combined (see Section 2.9). Additionally, all groups had unrestricted access to usual treatment options (TAU). The trial was conducted within the StudiCare project (www.studicare.com), which investigates and promotes college student mental health by providing IMIs for various psychological and behavioral subjects. The trial was conducted according to the CONSORT 2010 (55) statement and registered a priori at the WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform via the German Clinical Studies Trial Register (TRN: DRKS00014774; registration date: 05/18/2018). Details on the study design (26) of the main study can be obtained from the study protocol and the publication on the main analysis (25).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram (see Küchler et al. (25); modified).

2.2. Eligibility criteria

Written informed consent was obligatory to participate in the trial. Further, participants had to meet the following inclusion criteria: (a) aged 18 or older, (b) enrolled in university or college, (c) sufficient German language skills, (d) internet access, (e) moderate to low mindfulness (Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory FMI < 37, corresponding to a FMI mean based on the general population (56). Participants were excluded if they currently underwent psychotherapy or any kind of mindfulness intervention at the time of the screening. To be included in the secondary moderation and mediation analyses, participants additionally had to (f) have completed a minimal intervention dose of at least one module post-intervention (t2; 8 weeks after randomization).

2.3. Setting/recruitment

Participants were recruited at 18 cooperating colleges in Germany, Austria and Switzerland via circular e-mails, flyers and posters, social media, student unions, and student counseling. All recruitment channels led to the StudiCare homepage, where students found detailed information about StudiCare Mindfulness and registration. If students successfully completed an online screening, they were either assigned to a partner trial (students of Ulm University) (57) or the current trial (students of all other colleges). After the provision of informed consent, they completed the baseline assessment and were then randomized into one of the three groups. UG and GoD participants received immediate access to StudiCare Mindfulness, whereas WL participants received access after completing the follow-up assessment (6 months after randomization).

2.4. Randomization

Randomized allocation was conducted by an independent researcher not otherwise involved in the study. Permuted block randomization was performed using an automated, online-based randomization program (58) with an allocation ratio of 1:1:1 and randomly arranged variable block sizes of 6, 9 and 12.

2.5. Intervention

StudiCare Mindfulness comprises seven core modules and two booster sessions (4 and 12 weeks after the last core module). Modules were unlocked sequentially and weekly module completion was recommended, with each module taking about 40–60 min to work through. Module content is based on elements of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) (17), Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) (16) and general stress management techniques (59), and is diversely and interactively designed (e.g., including psychoeducational texts, case-examples, self-reflection exercises, meditation audio files). As homework assignments, participants are practicing mindfulness exercises between modules, which are reflected at the beginning of each module. For a detailed description of the intervention, see Table 2 as well as the study protocol (26). To participants of the GoD group, e-coach guidance was provided via the Minddistrict platform's message function whenever they actively requested it. E-coaches were supervised psychologists and gave semi-standardized feedbacks to participants' module input that was focused on motivation and encouragement. In contrast, UG participants only received short standardized messages that were automatically sent by the platform after completion of each module. Both groups were sent automatic reminders if they had not logged in for more than 7 days. Additionally, they could sign up for an SMS-coach to receive motivational text messages every other day. Participants could access StudiCare Mindfulness via the Minddistrict platform (www.minddistrict.com, accessed 18 February 2023) on a 24/7 basis using a personal username and password. All transferred data was secured based on ISO27001 and guidelines NEN7510.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intervention content [see (25)].

2.6. Control condition

At the beginning of the study, WL participants received a leaflet about alternative support options (such as helplines, university counselling services) at the beginning of the study and had unrestricted access to usual treatment options (TAU) throughout their participation. They were able to access an unguided version of StudiCare Mindfulness six months after randomization.

2.7. Assessments and outcomes

Assessments were conducted via the online survey platform “Unipark” (www.unipark.com, accessed 18 February 2023) before (t0; baseline), 4 weeks (t1; intermediate), and 8 weeks (t2; post-treatment) after randomization (see Figure 1) to enable establishment of temporal precedence. Here, only a subset of instruments that are relevant to the proposed mediation and moderation analyses is presented. For an overview of all outcomes of the RCT, see Küchler et al. (26).

2.7.1. Dependent variable

2.7.1.1. Depressive symptom severity

Depressive symptom severity was selected as dependent variable in accordance with the majority of the existing mediation studies on mindfulness-based interventions (21). It was assessed with the depression module of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) (60). Within the PHQ-9, 9 items are rated on a 4-point scale (0 = “not at all” to 3 = “nearly every day”). The PHQ-9 is a widely used depression screening instrument and was shown to have good diagnostic properties and a high internal consistency of α = 0.89 (61).

2.7.2. Mediator variables

2.7.2.1. Mindfulness

Mindfulness was assessed via the 14-item short scale of the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) (56), which used a 4-point scale ranging from 1 = “rarely” to 4 = “almost always”. The FMI has demonstrated high internal consistency (α = 0.84) (62) and sensitivity to change (56) in previous studies.

2.7.2.2. Self-efficacy

To measure perceived general self-efficacy, the Self-efficacy Scale (SES) (63) was employed. 10 items are rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 = “not at all true” to 4 = “very true”. The SES has demonstrated good internal consistency of 0.75–0.91 in previous research (64).

2.7.2.3. Cognitive fusion

Cognitive fusion was assessed with the 7-item Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire (CFQ-D) (65) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = “never true” to 7 = “always true”. Previous research has found internal consistency to be high with a Cronbach's α = 0.95 (65).

2.7.2.4. Emotion regulation

Habitual use of the two emotion regulation strategies “cognitive reappraisal” (6 items) and “expressive suppression” (4 items) was assessed with the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (36) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”. The ERQ has previously demonstrated good internal consistency (reappraisal: α = 0.74; suppression: α = 0.76) (66).

2.7.2.5. Alexithymia

Using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (41, 67), alexithymia was assessed with 20 items which are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”; 5 = “strongly agree”). The TAS-20 has three subscales: “difficulty identifying feelings” (DIF), “difficulty describing feelings” (DDF) and “externally oriented thinking” (EOF) and was found to have good internal consistency (α = 0.85–0.86) (68).

2.7.3. Moderator variables

Several variables were assessed that potentially moderate the dependent variables mindfulness (FMI) and depressive symptom severity (PHQ-9) at t2: age, gender, nationality, marital status, and number of semesters, previous experience with mindfulness (assessed retrospectively at t2), previous help-seeking (mindfulness and psychotherapy), and use of alternative support services. In addition, participants rated the six items of the Credibility Expectancy Questionnaire (CEQ) (69). The CEQ consists of three items for the credibility subscale (“how believable, convincing, and logical the treatment is” and three for the expectancy subscale (“improvements that clients believe will be achieved” (70), each measured on a 9-point Likert Scale to assess positive expectations and credibility of StudiCare Mindfulness. The CEQ's internal consistency was found to be high in previous research, Cronbach's α = 0.84–0.85 (69). Finally, mindfulness (FMI), depression (PHQ-9), and self-efficacy (SES) at baseline (t0) were examined as potential moderators.

2.8. Statistical analyses

2.8.1. Mediation analysis

Mediation effects for each potential mediator were investigated in separate structural equation models (SEM), following the principles of time-lagged mediation analyses by Cole and Maxwell (71). For the independent variable “group”, the two intervention groups (UG and GoD) were combined (1 = intervention group, IG) and compared to the waitlist control group (0 = WLC). This decision was based on findings of the effectiveness analyses (72), which revealed no substantial differences in depressive symptom severity and pre-defined moderator and mediator variables between UG and GoD at all assessment times. Additionally, guidance utilization in the GoD group was very low (15%), resulting in high comparability of the intervention actually received by both groups. Depression severity (PHQ-9) was set as dependent variable. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was applied to estimate model parameters, as this approach allows for accurate estimation even in the presence of missing data (73). Model fit was evaluated with root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) as the χ2-test has a tendency to reject mis-specified models too sensitively (7476). Standard modeling criteria were used to establish cut-off values for an acceptable goodness of fit: RMSEA < 0.06, SRMR < 0.08 (77). Nested models were compared via model deviance tests using χ2-tests.

Following Cole and Maxwell (71), we conducted five steps for testing mediational effects with longitudinal data using SEM. These steps aimed to find the most parsimonious model with a good fit to the data (71). First, the measurement model (“full model”) was tested allowing “group” and all latent variables to correlate with each other. This model constituted the basis for all model comparisons in the following steps. Second, the equality of various parameters across waves (i.e., assessment times) was evaluated by testing for equilibrium and factorial invariance. Third, it was tested whether variables not included in the model may account for the covariation between latent variables. Fourth, we investigated which additional causal paths should be added to the longitudinal full mediation model. Finally, in the fifth step, direct effects (c'), indirect effects (a*b) and total effects (c = c' + a × b) were estimated using the most parsimonious model with a good model fit derived from the previous steps. Autoregressive effects between waves were set equal to substantiate the stationarity assumption. As the direct effect (c'-path) was only significant in the SEM of one potential mediator (expressive suppression), mediation analyses for the other four mediator variables were also calculated with SEM that did not include the c'-path as a sensitivity analysis.

2.8.2. Moderation analysis

For the moderation analyses, dichotomous variables were dummy-coded, multicategorical variables were dichotomized (e.g., civil status into “single” vs. “in partnership”), and continuous variables were z-standardized. Bilinear interaction models were employed, with depression at t2 (post-intervention) serving as the dependent variable. Additionally, the same analyses were conducted with mindfulness at t2 as dependent variable, as it was the primary outcome variable in the main study and this was specified in the study protocol (26). In both cases, “group” was set as independent variable. If significant interactions between group and moderator were detected, moderation effects were subsequently examined using simple slopes analyses. This involved testing high (+1 SD), average (M), and low (−1 SD) levels of the moderation variable.

All analyses were performed using the software R (78). The R package “lavaan” (version 0.6–8) (79) was used for SEM. All statistical analyses were conducted following the intention-to-treat (ITT) principle. Missing data was presumed to be at random (73). To account for this, the full information maximum likelihood estimator was utilized in the mediation analyses to handle missing values. For the moderation analysis, ITT was operationalized via multiple imputation using chained equations (80). Predictive mean matching (81) was employed to impute N = 20 datasets. ITT analyses were conducted for each imputed dataset, and results were subsequently pooled according to Rubin's rule (82). Only participants who completed a minimal dose of treatment (at least one module of StudiCare Mindfulness) were included in the analyses (N = 344).

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Of the 1,526 college students that registered for the trial, 405 fulfilled eligibility criteria and 386 were included and randomized into the three study groups (see Figure 1 for flowchart). The resulting sample sizes were n = 129 for the unguided group (UG), n = 130 for the “guidance on demand” group (GoD), and n = 127 for the waiting list control group (WL). After excluding participants that had not received a minimal dose of at least one module of StudiCare Mindfulness and combining the two intervention groups (IG), this resulted in n = 217 (IG) and n = 127 (WL). Participants were mainly female (74%) and on average 25.7 years old (SD = 5.28) (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Baseline characteristics.

3.2. Mediation analysis

Tests of model fit and model comparisons within Cole and Maxwell's (71) steps 1–3 (described above) revealed a good fit of the measurement models and the presence of equilibrium and factorial invariance. This was modeled accordingly in all six final SEM. Factor loadings were set equal across waves and the residuals of the endogenous latent variables were allowed to correlate. Autoregressive effects between waves were set equal. Additional causal paths that were found to be significant in step 4 were included in the final SEM. Model fits of the final SEM of each mediator were good overall (RMSEA: 0.038–0.046; SRMR: 0.066–0.083) and can be obtained from Table 4. The only significant mediation was found for mindfulness (see Table 4 and Figure 2), whereas no other potential mediator variable demonstrated a mediating effect (see Table 2 and Supplementary Figures S1–S5 in the Supplementary Material for the respective results and path models). For mindfulness, we found a significant a-path (effect of StudiCare Mindfulness on mindfulness: β = 0.832, p < 0.001), a significant b-path (effect of mindfulness on depressive symptom severity: β = −0.184, p = 0.005), and a significant a*b-path (indirect effect of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity mediated through mindfulness: β = −0.153, p = 0.007). Both the c-path (total effect of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity: β = −0.196, p = 0.055) and c'-path (direct effect of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity: β = −0.043, p = 0.709) did not reach significance, implying full mediation of the effect of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity at t2 (post-intervention) through mindfulness at t1 (intermediate assessment). Substantiating the causal direction of the mediation effect, all backwards effects tested within step 4 did not reach significance, nor did inclusion of these paths significantly improve model fit. These included paths from depressive symptom severity at t0 to mindfulness at t1 (β = 0.041, p = 0.497), from depressive symptom severity at t1 to mindfulness at t2 (β = −0.007, p = 0.915), from depressive symptom severity at t0 to mindfulness at t2 (β = 0.092, p = 0.125) and from mindfulness at t0 to depressive symptom severity t2 (β = −0.039, p = 0.600). Sensitivity analyses with SEM excluding the c'-paths (direct effect) yielded comparable results (see Supplementary Table S1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Path model for the mediator mindfulness. Ellipses are latent variables, rectangles are manifest variables. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of a longitudinal mediation analysis with three measurement time points.

3.3. Moderation analysis

See Supplementary Tables S2, S3 for the detailed results of the moderation analyses. Regarding the dependent variable depressive symptom severity at t2, only “number of semesters” was found to be a significant moderator of the intervention's effectiveness (interaction: β = 0.24, 95% CI: 0.02–0.47, p = 0.035). Simple slope analyses revealed that the intervention's effectiveness on depressive symptom severity at t2 was most pronounced for IG participants with a low number of semesters (−1 SD; β = −0.65, 95% CI: −0.95–−0.18), compared to WL. Individuals with a high number of semesters (+1 SD; β = −0.18, 95% CI: −0.47–0.17) experienced the smallest benefits, with depressive symptom severity no longer being significantly reduced compared to WL. For the dependent variable mindfulness at t2, two variables were found to significantly moderate the intervention's effectiveness, baseline mindfulness (interaction: β = −0.20, 95% CI: −0.41–−0.00; p = 0.047) and baseline self-efficacy (SE) (interaction: β = −0.27, 95% CI: −0.48–−0.06; p = 0.012) compared to WL. In both cases, the intervention was more effective for participants with low baseline levels of these variables (baseline mindfulness: −1 SD; β = 1.15, 95% CI: 0.91–1.40; baseline SE: −1 SD; β = 1.21, 95% CI: 0.95–1.46) and less effective for participants with high baseline levels (baseline mindfulness: +1 SD; β = 0.75, 95% CI: 0.50–0.99; baseline SE: +1 SD; β = 0.70, 95% CI: 0.41–0.93).

4. Discussion

Within this study, we analyzed secondary data from a randomized controlled trial (RCT) evaluating the Internet- and mobile-based mindfulness intervention (IMI) for college students, StudiCare Mindfulness, to gain insights into potential moderating and mediating variables of the intervention's effectiveness. Mindfulness was found to fully mediate the intervention's effectiveness on depressive symptoms in the sense that the more mindfulness increased during the intervention, the more depressive symptoms decreased by the end of the intervention. All other examined mediators, i.e., self-efficacy, cognitive defusion, emotion regulation, and alexithymia, could not be confirmed as mediators. Only one of the examined variables (i.e., number of semesters) moderated the intervention's effectiveness on depressive symptom severity, whereas two variables (i.e., baseline mindfulness, baseline SE) were found to moderate the intervention effect on mindfulness.

4.1. Mediation analysis

The finding that mindfulness mediated the intervention's effectiveness on depressive symptom severity is in line with existing evidence. In a previous RCT evaluating a guided version of StudiCare Mindfulness, mindfulness was found to mediate improvements in depressive symptoms in a cross-sectional mediation analysis (72). Additionally, in their systematic review and meta-analysis, Gu and colleagues (21) found consistent moderate evidence for mindfulness as a mediator of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) (16) and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) (28). Mindfulness as a mediator was further substantiated by a more recent systematic review on mechanisms of change of IMIs for depression (43). The current study extends these existing findings by employing multiple assessment times and the method of time-lagged mediation analysis (71). This enabled the establishment of temporal precedence, an essential criterion for mediation (23) and a shortcoming of many preexisting studies. Finally, our results align with the theoretical assumption of mindfulness-based approaches that developing mindfulness is a key mechanism for improving mental health outcomes (16). Taken together, the promotion of mindfulness appears to be an “active ingredient” in the improvement of (subthreshold) depressive symptoms and might be routinely incorporated in mental health promotion IMIs. However, recent evidence on potential negative effects of mindfulness under certain circumstances (e.g., derealization, anxiety) should be considered (25, 83, 84).

None of the other mediator variables could be confirmed within our analyses. Alexithymia was found to significantly decrease in the intervention groups of StudiCare Mindfulness in the main analysis of this RCT (25), which is in line with previous meta-analytic evidence (42). However, we were unable to demonstrate a mediating role of this construct in the improvement of depressive symptoms. No other trial so far has studied alexithymia as a mediator of mindfulness-based interventions. As of today, it is still unclear whether alexithymia predisposes to depression, is a reaction to depression or whether these two constructs simply co-occur (85). Prospective epidemiological research and finegrained mediation studies may shed some light on these questions.

We could also not replicate previous findings of a mediating role of emotion regulation (ER) on the effectiveness of mindfulness-based IMIs by Ma and colleagues (38) and Sanilevici and colleagues (39). This might be explained by the fact that their study only employed cross-sectional mediation analysis, which does not allow for definite conclusions about the timeline of change and the establishment of mediation (71). Thus theoretically, the relationship between ER and depression could have been correlational or backwards. Additionally, both Ma and colleagues (38) and Sanilevici and colleagues (39) employed the “Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale” (DERS) (86) instead of the “Emotion Regulation Questionnaire” (ERQ) (36) we used. The DERS captures different facets of emotion regulation, such as “lack of emotional awareness”, “lack of emotional clarity”, or “non-acceptance of emotional responses”. These facets might be more sensitive to change through mindfulness-based interventions and therefore lead to significant effects on depressive symptoms. More research employing longitudinal mediation designs and various measures of ER are needed to unravel the potential mediating role of ER in the context of mindfulness-based IMIs.

Similarly to ER, evidence for self-efficacy (SE) is very scarce but suggests this construct as a potential mediator of IMIs' effectiveness on depressive symptoms (47). Our study was unable to replicate these findings. However, the study conducted by Clarke and colleagues (47) also only allowed for cross-sectional mediation analyses. Additionally, the construct of mental health SE examined in their study is not directly comparable to the construct of perceived SE (63) analyzed in the current trial. Just as in the case of ER, more studies employing longitudinal research designs and various instruments for measuring SE are needed to evaluate this construct's role as a potential mediator of IMI effectiveness on depression.

The non-significant findings concerning the potential mediational role of cognitive fusion (CF) are somewhat surprising, as previous meta-analytic evidence demonstrated mediation of the effectiveness of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) (29) in the face-to-face setting by CF (22). One explanation for this discrepancy could be that StudiCare Mindfulness is not exclusively based on ACT, but rather includes elements of ACT alongside elements from MBSR (25) and general stress management techniques (59). Compared to ACT, more emphasis was placed on mindful meditation practice (every intervention module), whereas cognitive defusion was only explicitly addressed in two modules. Although cognitive fusion was significantly reduced in both intervention groups (d = −0.52–−0.49, 95% CI −0.77–−0.25) (25), it appears not to be a driving force of the effectiveness of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptoms.

As a final remark, in the sensitivity analysis conducted within the current study, the indirect effects of both SE and CF approximated statistical significance (p = 0.6). This might indicate that these variables play a mediational role for the effectiveness of (mindfulness-based) IMIs that we were unable to detect in our study, e.g., due to insufficient power. Consequently, it might be worthwhile to further examine these constructs in adequately powered, meticulously planned mediation studies.

4.2. Moderation analysis

Concerning the moderation analyses, most of the examined variables could not be confirmed as moderators of the effectiveness of StudiCare Mindfulness on both depressive symptom severity and mindfulness. These findings are in line with a previous study examining the effectiveness of a guided version of StudiCare Mindfulness. In this study, we did not find sociodemographic variables, baseline mindfulness, baseline symptomatology, psychotherapy experience and attitudes towards IMIs to moderate the intervention's effectiveness on mindfulness (72). Somewhat surprisingly, we found the number of semesters to have a moderating role in the effectiveness of StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity in the current trial. College students with a higher number of semesters benefited less from the intervention. As this is an exploratory finding and, to our knowledge, no other studies have explored the potential moderating effect of this variable so far, confirmative studies will have to show if these results can be validated.

Concerning the outcome mindfulness, results of this study demonstrated that the intervention's effectiveness is moderated by baseline mindfulness and baseline SE. We found lower baseline levels are associated with larger intervention effects, whereas the moderation found by Shapiro and colleagues (51) was in the other direction, i.e., higher baseline mindfulness was associated with greater improvements (51). Potential explanations are that StudiCare Mindfulness was designed for “mindfulness beginners” and lower baseline levels might leave more room for improvement.

Concerning baseline SE, we found this variable to moderate the intervention's effectiveness on mindfulness, with lower baseline SE being associated with larger intervention effectiveness on mindfulness. This aligns with existing evidence by Clark and colleagues (47), which found participants with low mental health efficacy to benefit more from an IMI for depression. Insofar, results of the moderation analyses might indicate that the intervention is particularly beneficial for college students low on mindfulness and SE. However, our findings are exploratory, existing evidence is still scarce, and more research is needed to substantiate the results of this secondary analyses.

Overall, StudiCare Mindfulness was found to effectively increase mindfulness and reduce depressive symptoms regardless of most baseline variables in two different studies (25, 72) examining different guidance formats. This suggests that the IMI might be a universally applicable intervention for improving college students' psychological well-being, which is an important prerequisite for large-scale implementation.

4.3. Strengths, limitations and future directions

The presented mediation study has several strengths. First, the study is of high methodological quality, featuring elements such as a randomized controlled design, theoretically derived mediator variables, multiple assessment time points (with one assessment occurring during the intervention), and the employment of a rigorous statistical method of analyses, i.e., time-lagged mediation analysis (71), which enabled the establishment of temporal precedence (23). Consequently, many of the criteria Kadzin (23) proposed to establish mediation are met. Further, the current study examined potential mediator variables of mindfulness-based IMI derived from theory that have not been investigated systematically so far, i.e., SE, ER, and alexithymia.

However, there are also multiple limitations and directions for future research. First, the sample size was larger than in many other mediation studies in the field of mindfulness-based IMIs (21). Nonetheless, moderation and mediation analyses might still have been underpowered, as such analyses generally necessitate very large sample sizes (87). A related matter concerns that this RCT was designed and powered to evaluate the effectiveness of the primary outcome mindfulness rather than to identify mechanisms of change. Although we did consider secondary mediation analyses in the trial design a priori (26), future confirmatory mediation studies should take further aspects of rigorous meditation study planning into account (23), such as more than one assessment point during the intervention, in the best case after every module. This would enable a more detailed mapping of change processes, e.g., the identification of mechanisms of change that are relevant for specific intervention modules.

Further, it cannot be ruled out that a third variable not assessed within our study design caused the change in both the outcome and the mediator (88). Although this possibility can never be ruled out completely, the evidence for the causal role of a mediator such as mindfulness might be further strengthened by experimental manipulation. For example, a future RCT could compare two intervention versions with different doses of mindfulness exercises. Another limitation relates to the very low utilization of guidance in the GoD group, thus both groups (unguided, “guidance on demand”) were combined into one intervention group. As a consequence, we were unable to examine mediator and moderator variables that might be unique to these specific guidance formats. Future trials comparing full guidance to no guidance could examine such potentially unique effects or even guidance itself as a mechanism of change (43).

Concerning the moderation analyses, due to the investigated sample's nature, there was a range restriction for several potential moderator variables, which limits the generalizability of results. For example, this regards age (college students), gender (primarily female participants), baseline mindfulness (high mindfulness was an exclusion criterium), and credibility/expectancy (positive due to self-selection, no incentives). Non-significant moderator variables might still be significant if examined in samples which are not subject to the mentioned range restrictions. Another limitation is that we employed a waitlist control group instead of an active control group, which does not allow for the examination of the specificity of mediators (23). Future studies could investigate the specificity of mediators of mindfulness-based IMIs by choosing active control conditions such as Cognitive Behavioral Therapy. This would allow conclusions about whether mindfulness is indeed specific to MBSR/ACT rather than a common mechanism of change of psychotherapy.

Finally, our analyses only included participants who received a minimal intervention dose (i.e., one module). This decision was made to increase the probability of detecting existing mediators or moderators within the exploratory context of this study. However, this approach might also have introduced bias, as completely non-adherent participants were excluded (23). Future studies could examine the influence of intervention adherence on the mediating effect of different variables via moderated mediation analyses.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the results of this study indicate that mindfulness is a causal mediator of the effectiveness of the Internet- and mobile-based intervention (IMI) StudiCare Mindfulness on depressive symptom severity. In addition, moderation analyses demonstrated that the intervention's effectiveness is independent of most baseline characteristics, suggesting the effectiveness of the intervention in college students with a wide array of baseline characteristics. These findings support mindfulness' beneficial role in improving psychological health, the capacity of IMIs to successfully teach these skills and the universal applicability of mindfulness-based IMI for college student mental health promotion. Future confirmative studies will have to substantiate these findings.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: External researches may get access to the final trial dataset from HB on request, depending on to be specified data security and data exchange regulation agreements. To ensure confidentiality, data dispersed to any investigator or researcher will be blinded of any identifying participant information. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to harald.baumeister@uni-ulm.de.

Ethics statement

All study procedures have been approved by the ethics committee of Ulm University (application no. 47/18, 7 March 2018). Participants received written information on study conditions, data security, voluntariness of participation and the right to leave the study at all times. To confirm understanding of the above, written consent was obtained from all participants prior to study entry. Data collection was pseudonymized and data was only accessed by authorized study personnel obliged to secrecy. After completion of data collection, personalized information was deleted and all data was completely anonymized.

Author contributions

DDE and HB: obtained funding for this study. A-MK, DDE, and HB: initiated this study. A-MK, FK, and HB: contributed to the design of this study. A-MK: revised and developed intervention content. A-MK: obtained ethics approval and coordinated trial management. A-MK: conducted data curation. A-MK and LB: conducted data analyses. YT: provided consultation for data analyses. A-MK: wrote the draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the further writing of the manuscript and interpretation of data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The project was funded by BARMER, a major statutory health care insurance company in Germany. BARMER had no role in study design, decision to publish or preparation of this manuscript. BARMER was/is not involved in data collection, analyses, decision to publish, or preparation of future papers regarding the StudiCare project.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mathias Harrer for taking care of the StudiCare website. Moreover, we would like to thank Danielle Vollbrecht and Katharina Peip for their contributions to the development of the original version of StudiCare Mindfulness and Morten Moshagen for his consultation concerning data analyses. Furthermore, we would like to thank or study assistants for their support in the development of the intervention, the assessment procedures, and the study administration processes. Special thanks to all cooperating colleges in Germany, Austria, and Switzerland for regularly informing their students about the StudiCare interventions and all participants for taking part in this trial.

Conflict of interest

A-MK, HB were involved in the development of StudiCare Mindfulness or its predecessor versions. A-MK has received fees for lectures/workshops from chambers of psychotherapists and health insurance companies. HB reports having received consultancy fees and fees for lectures/workshops from chambers of psychotherapists and training institutes for psychotherapists in the e-mental-health context. DDE reports having received consultancy fees from, and served on the scientific advisory boards of, several companies such as Minddistrict, Lantern, Schoen Kliniken, and German health insurance companies. He is a stakeholder of the Institute for health training online (GETON), which aims to implement scientific findings related to digital health interventions into routine care.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fdgth.2023.1179216/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Blanco C, Okuda M, Wright C, Hasin DS, Grant BF, Liu S-M, et al. Mental health of college students and their non-college-attending peers: results from the national epidemiologic study on alcohol and related conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2008) 65(12):1429–37. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ibrahim AK, Kelly SJ, Adams CE, Glazebrook C. A systematic review of studies of depression prevalence in university students. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47(3):391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sheldon E, Simmonds-Buckley M, Bone C, Mascarenhas T, Chan N, Wincott M, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for mental health problems in university undergraduate students: a systematic review with meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2021) 287:282–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Zivin K, Eisenberg D, Gollust SE, Golberstein E. Persistence of mental health problems and needs in a college student population. J Affect Disord. (2009) 117(3):180–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am Psychol. (2000) 55(5):469–80. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Auerbach RP, Mortier P, Bruffaerts R, Alonso J, Benjet C, Cuijpers P, et al. Who world mental health surveys international college student project: prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. (2018) 127(7):623–38. doi: 10.1037/abn0000362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chang J-J, Ji Y, Li Y-H, Pan H-F, Su P-Y. Prevalence of anxiety symptom and depressive symptom among college students during COVID-19 pandemic: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2021) 292:242–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Deng J, Zhou F, Hou W, Silver Z, Wong CY, Chang O, et al. The prevalence of depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms and sleep disturbance in higher education students during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2021) 301:113863. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Li Y, Wang A, Wu Y, Han N, Huang H. Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the mental health of college students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:669119. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.669119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bruffaerts R, Mortier P, Auerbach RP, Alonso J, La Hermosillo De Torre AE, Cuijpers P, et al. Lifetime and 12-month treatment for mental disorders and suicidal thoughts and behaviors among first year college students. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2019) 28(2):e1764. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ebert DD, Mortier P, Kählke F, Bruffaerts R, Baumeister H, Auerbach RP, et al. Barriers of mental health treatment utilization among first-year college students: first cross-national results from the WHO world mental health international college student initiative. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2019) 28(2):e1782. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hunt JB, Eisenberg D. Mental health problems and help-seeking behavior among college students. J Adolesc Health. (2010) 46(1):3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Harrer M, Adam SH, Fleischmann RJ, Baumeister H, Auerbach RP, Bruffaerts R, et al. Effectiveness of an internet- and app-based intervention for college students with elevated stress: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. (2018) 20(4):e136. doi: 10.2196/jmir.9293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pedrelli P, Nyer M, Yeung AS, Zulauf C, Wilens T. College students: mental health problems and treatment considerations. Acad Psychiatry. (2015) 39(5):503–11. doi: 10.1007/s40596-014-0205-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ebert DD, van Daele T, Nordgreen T, Karekla M, Compare A, Zarbo C, et al. Internet- and mobile-based psychological interventions: applications, efficacy, and potential for improving mental health. Eur Psychol. (2018) 23(2):167–87. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000318

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (1982) 4(1):33–47. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York City: Guilford Press (1999).

18. Dawson AF, Brown WW, Anderson J, Datta B, Donald JN, Hong K, et al. Mindfulness-based interventions for university students: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. (2020) 12(2):384–410. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sommers-Spijkerman M, Austin J, Bohlmeijer ET, Pots WTM. New evidence in the booming field of online mindfulness: an updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. JMIR Ment Health. (2021) 8(7):e28168. doi: 10.2196/28168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Carlbring P, Andersson G, Cuijpers P, Riper H, Hedman-Lagerlöf E. Internet-based vs. Face-to-face cognitive behavior therapy for psychiatric and somatic disorders: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Cogn Behav Ther. (2018) 47(1):1–18. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2017.1401115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, Cavanagh K. How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 37:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Stockton D, Kellett S, Berrios R, Sirois F, Wilkinson N, Miles G. Identifying the underlying mechanisms of change during acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT): a systematic review of contemporary mediation studies. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2019) 47(3):332–62. doi: 10.1017/S1352465818000553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kazdin AE. Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2007) 3:1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kraemer HC, Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, Agras WS. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2002) 59(10):877–83. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Küchler A-M, Schultchen D, Dretzler T, Moshagen M, Ebert DD, Baumeister H. A three-armed randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness, acceptance, and negative effects of StudiCare mindfulness, an internet- and Mobile-based intervention for college students with No and “on demand” guidance. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2023) 20(4):3208. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20043208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Küchler A-M, Schultchen D, Pollatos O, Moshagen M, Ebert DD, Baumeister H. Studicare mindfulness-study protocol of a randomized controlled trial evaluating an internet- and mobile-based intervention for college students with no and “on demand” guidance. Trials. (2020) 21(1):975. doi: 10.1186/s13063-020-04868-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Domhardt M, Cuijpers P, Ebert DD, Baumeister H. More light? Opportunities and pitfalls in digitalized psychotherapy process research. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:544129. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.544129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Teasdale JD, Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: a new approach to preventing relapse. New York City: The Guilford Press (2002).

29. Hayes SC. Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies—republished article. Behav Ther. (2016) 47(6):869–85. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Viskovich S, Pakenham KI. Pilot evaluation of a web-based acceptance and commitment therapy program to promote mental health skills in university students. J Clin Psychol. (2018) 74(12):2047–69. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Viskovich S, Pakenham KI. Randomized controlled trial of a web-based acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) program to promote mental health in university students. J Clin Psychol. (2020) 76(6):929–51. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2011) 6(6):537–59. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Goldin PR, Gross JJ. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion. (2010) 10(1):83–91. doi: 10.1037/a0018441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gratz KL, Tull MT. Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in acceptance- and mindfulness-based treatments. In: Baer RA, editors. Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: illuminating the theory and practice of change. Oakland, California: New Harbinger Publications (2010):107–33.

35. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. (1998) 2(3):271–99. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 85(2):348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Parmentier FBR, García-Toro M, García-Campayo J, Yañez AM, Andrés P, Gili M. Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population: the mediating roles of worry, rumination, reappraisal and suppression. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:506. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ma Y, She Z, Siu AF-Y, Zeng X, Liu X. Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions on psychological distress and the mediating role of emotion regulation. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:2090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Sanilevici M, Reuveni O, Lev-Ari S, Golland Y, Levit-Binnun N. Mindfulness-based stress reduction increases mental wellbeing and emotion regulation during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic: a synchronous online intervention study. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:720965. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.720965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Coffey KA, Hartman M, Fredrickson BL. Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness. (2010) 1(4):235–53. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0033-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item toronto alexithymia scale–I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. (1994) 38(1):23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Norman H, Marzano L, Coulson M, Oskis A. Effects of mindfulness-based interventions on alexithymia: a systematic review. Evid Based Ment Health. (2019) 22(1):36–43. doi: 10.1136/ebmental-2018-300029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Domhardt M, Steubl L, Boettcher J, Buntrock C, Karyotaki E, Ebert DD, et al. Mediators and mechanisms of change in internet- and mobile-based interventions for depression: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2021) 83:101953. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. (1977) 84(2):191–215. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Breuninger C, Tuschen-Caffier B, Svaldi J. Dysfunctional cognition and self-efficacy as mediators of symptom change in exposure therapy for agoraphobia—systematic review and meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. (2019) 120:103443. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Fentz HN, Arendt M, O'Toole MS, Hoffart A, Hougaard E. The mediational role of panic self-efficacy in cognitive behavioral therapy for panic disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. (2014) 60:23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Clarke J, Proudfoot J, Birch M-R, Whitton AE, Parker G, Manicavasagar V, et al. Effects of mental health self-efficacy on outcomes of a mobile phone and web intervention for mild-to-moderate depression, anxiety and stress: secondary analysis of a randomised controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2014) 14:272. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0272-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Warmerdam L, van Straten A, Jongsma J, Twisk J, Cuijpers P. Online cognitive behavioral therapy and problem-solving therapy for depressive symptoms: exploring mechanisms of change. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2010) 41(1):64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Pots WTM, Trompetter HR, Schreurs KMG, Bohlmeijer ET. How and for whom does web-based acceptance and commitment therapy work? Mediation and moderation analyses of web-based ACT for depressive symptoms. BMC Psychiatry. (2016) 16:158. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0841-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nyklíček I, van Son J, Pop VJ, Denollet J, Pouwer F. Does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy benefit all people with diabetes and comorbid emotional complaints equally? Moderators in the DiaMind trial. J Psychosom Res. (2016) 91:40–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Thoresen C, Plante TG. The moderation of mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: results from a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol. (2011) 67(3):267–77. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kuyken W, Warren FC, Taylor RS, Whalley B, Crane C, Bondolfi G, et al. Efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in prevention of depressive relapse: an individual patient data meta-analysis from randomized trials. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73(6):565–74. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hedman-Lagerlöf E, Andersson E, Ljótsson B, Andersson G, Schalling M, Lindefors N, et al. Clinical and genetic outcome determinants of internet- and group-based cognitive behavior therapy for social anxiety disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2012) 126(2):126–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01834.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mulder R, Murray G, Rucklidge J. Common versus specific factors in psychotherapy: opening the black box. Lancet Psychiatry. (2017) 4(12):953–62. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30100-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Mohr DC. Consort 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMC Med. (2010) 8:18. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Walach H, Buchheld N, Buttenmüller V, Kleinknecht N, Schmidt S. Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI). Pers Individ Dif. (2006) 40(8):1543–55. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Schultchen D, Küchler A-M, Schillings C, Weineck F, Karabatsiakis A, Ebert DD, et al. Effectiveness of a guided online mindfulness-focused intervention in a student population: study protocol for a randomised control trial. BMJ Open. (2020) 10(3):e032775. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Sealed Envelope Ltd. (2019). Create a Blocked Randomisation List. Available at: https://www.sealedenvelope.com/.

59. Kaluza G. (2015). Stressbewältigung. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44016-2

60. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL. The PHQ-9: a new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatr Ann. (2002) 32(9):509–15. doi: 10.3928/0048-5713-20020901-06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. (2001) 16(9):606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Heidenreich T, Ströhle G, Michalak J. Achtsamkeit: konzeptuelle aspekte und ergebnisse zum freiburger achtsamkeitsfragebogen. Verhaltenstherapie. (2006) 16(1):33–40. doi: 10.1159/000091521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M. Generalized self-efficacy scale. In: Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M, editors. Measures in health psychology: a User's Portfolio causal and control beliefs. London, UK: NFER-Nelson (1995):35–7.

64. Schwarzer R, Mueller J, Greenglass E. Assessment of perceived general self-efficacy on the internet: data collection in cyberspace. Anxiety Stress Coping. (1999) 12(2):145–61. doi: 10.1080/10615809908248327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. China C, Hansen LB, Gillanders DT, Benninghoven D. Concept and validation of the German version of the cognitive fusion questionnaire (CFQ-D). J Contextual Behav Sci. (2018) 9:30–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Abler B, Kessler H. Emotion regulation questionnaire—eine deutschsprachige fassung des ERQ von gross und john. Diagnostica. (2009) 55(3):144–52. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.55.3.144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kupfer J, Brosig B, Brähler E. Toronto-Alexithymie-Skala-20 (TAS-20). Göttingen: Hogrefe (2001).

68. Parker JD, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. The 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale. Iii. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population. J Psychosom Res. (2003) 55(3):269–75. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00578-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Devilly GJ, Borkovec TD. Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2000) 31(2):73–86. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(00)00012-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kazdin AE. Therapy outcome questions requiring control of credibility and treatment-generated expectancies. Behav Ther. (1979) 10(1):81–93. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(79)80011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol. (2003) 112(4):558–77. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Küchler A-M, Kählke F, Vollbrecht D, Peip K, Ebert DD, Baumeister H. Effectiveness, acceptability, and mechanisms of change of the internet-based intervention StudiCare mindfulness for college students: a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness (N Y). (2022) 13(9):2140–54. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01949-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. New York City: Guilford Press (2010).

74. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol Methods Res. (1992) 21(2):230–58. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Moshagen M. The model size effect in SEM: inflated goodness-of-fit statistics are due to the size of the covariance matrix. Struct Equ Modeling. (2012) 19(1):86–98. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2012.634724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Moshagen M, Erdfelder E. A new strategy for testing structural equation models. Struct Equ Modeling. (2016) 23(1):54–60. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.950896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. (1999) 6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. R Development Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2021). https://www.R-project.org/.

79. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. (2012) 48(2):1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. (2011) 45(3):1–67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Little RJA. Missing-data adjustments in large surveys. J Bus Econ Stat. (1988) 6(3):287. doi: 10.2307/1391878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Rubin DB. Multiple imputation after 18+ years. J Am Stat Assoc. (1996) 91(434):473. doi: 10.2307/2291635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Britton WB, Lindahl JR, Cooper DJ, Canby NK, Palitsky R. Defining and measuring meditation-related adverse effects in mindfulness-based programs. Clin Psychol Sci. (2021) 9(6):1185–204. doi: 10.1177/2167702621996340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. van Dam NT, van Vugt MK, Vago DR, Schmalzl L, Saron CD, Olendzki A, et al. Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2018) 13(1):36–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691617709589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Hemming L, Haddock G, Shaw J, Pratt D. Alexithymia and its associations with depression, suicidality, and aggression: an overview of the literature. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:203. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2004) 26(1):41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Fritz MS, Mackinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18(3):233–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Cuijpers P, Reijnders M, Huibers MJH. The role of common factors in psychotherapy outcomes. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2019) 15:207–31. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: college, university, mindfulness, internet- and mobile-based intervention, moderation, longitudinal mediation

Citation: Küchler A-M, Kählke F, Bantleon L, Terhorst Y, Ebert DD and Baumeister H (2023) Moderators and mediators of change of an internet-based mindfulness intervention for college students: secondary analysis from a randomized controlled trial. Front. Digit. Health 5:1179216. doi: 10.3389/fdgth.2023.1179216

Received: 3 March 2023; Accepted: 7 June 2023;
Published: 27 June 2023.

Edited by:

Philip Lindner, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Elsa Vitale, Bari Local Health Authority, Italy
Paul Alan Arkin Alvarado Garcia, Cesar Vallejo University, Peru

© 2023 Küchler, Kählke, Bantleon, Terhorst, Ebert and Baumeister. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ann-Marie Küchler ann-marie.kuechler@uni-ulm.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.