Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Dev. Psychol., 07 January 2025
Sec. Adolescent Psychological Development
This article is part of the Research Topic Promoting a Kinder and More Just World: The Development of Prosocial, Moral, and Social Justice Behaviors in Adolescence View all 7 articles

Editorial: Promoting a kinder and more just world: the development of prosocial, moral, and social justice behaviors in adolescence

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, United States
  • 2School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, United States
  • 3Family and Child Studies, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
  • 4Family and Human Development, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, United States

Recent sociopolitical movements around the world are advancing scapegoating, divisiveness, hatred, and violence toward minoritized and vulnerable populations. Although not new to human history, recent mass movements are promoting extraordinary global injustices and inequities. How to counter these injustices with better understanding of the development of prosociality is an urgent challenge for researchers.

Such scholarship entails an integrative, relational developmental system (RDS) metatheoretical approach and the broader process-relational paradigm to which it belongs (Lerner, 2006; Overton, 2013; Witherington et al., 2018). The relational paradigm asserts the interplay of indissociable, co-acting, and interpenetrating sources of developmental influence that move over time in the direction of systemic organized complexity. Indeed, RDS themes are increasingly evident in the study of character development (Lerner, 2018; Nucci, 2018), character education (Lapsley and Narvaez, 2006), positive youth development (Callina et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2015), and prosocial moral development (Carlo et al., 2022; Davis et al., 2021). This Research Topic is a timely set of papers based on RDS-inspired approaches that approximate efforts toward Promoting a Kinder and More Just World.

Maiya et al., for example, on the basis of an ecocultural strength-based model of prosocial moral development (Davis et al., 2021), showed that mothers' social rewards were indirectly related (via familism values and ethnic identity) to higher levels of everyday helping behavior in a sample of recent immigrant U.S. Latino/a adolescents. The lesson here is that parents can influence internalization of cultural strengths that influence prosocial behavior.

In an innovative cross-national study of six countries, Cirimele et al. also invoke the bioecological model in their investigation of whether the processes that drive prosocial development in adolescence are universal or whether other-oriented helping behavior varies in accord with broad cultural factors associated with a country's Human Development Index (HDI: life expectancy at birth, expected years of schooling, mean years of schooling, standard of living). They showed that prosocial behavior generally shows a linear increase as children move into adolescence, but this trend is moderated by HDI. Prosocial behavior tended to increase in contexts with lower HDI, but remain stable in high HDI contexts.

The notion of “altruism-born-of-suffering” refers to persons who are motivated to address social injustice even in the presence of adversity (Staub, 2005). Previous evidence has typically relied on retrospective interviews or cross-sectional study designs. Carlo et al. showed that U.S. Mexican girls who reported early-life trauma exhibited increases in altruistic behavior across adolescence and into young adulthood, but only in girls who reported high levels of familism values in mid-adolescence. These findings also provide evidence on the importance of cultural values as triggers for prosocial moral development even after reported early-life trauma. The findings also counter common negative stereotypes of U.S. ethnic/racial minoritized youth.

Family socialization effects were also investigated with respect to racial bias (Agalar et al.), the development of non-prejudicial attitudes toward sexual minorities (Padilla-Walker et al.), and deviant behavior (Chi et al.). The latter project showed how that supportive parenting that meets adolescent interpersonal needs reduces the tendency toward deviance. Padilla-Walker et al. showed that prejudice and non-prejudice follows different developmental lines, and that mothers' teaching that emphasizes rejection or correction of biased attitudes is particularly important for encouraging non-prejudiced value orientations in adolescents. Finally, Agalar et al. showed that color-conscious racial socialization counters adolescent racial prejudice, particularly when socialization practices support adolescent autonomy.

These papers underscore the fact that prosocial moral development not only interfaces dynamically with broad ecological systems, but is also bound inextricably with other normative challenges whose co-development rely upon a suite of “adaptive developmental regulations” (Lerner and Overton, 2008, p. 249) commonly found in asset promoting families and schools. If so there is as much ordinary magic in the promotion of social justice as there is in building resilience processes in development (cf. Masten, 2014).

Author contributions

DLap: Writing – original draft. GC: Writing – original draft. AD: Writing – review & editing. RF: Writing – review & editing. DLai: Writing – review & editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Callina, K. S., Mueller, M. K., Napolitano, C. M., Lerner, J. V., and Lerner, R. M. (2016). “Positive youth development: Relational systems approaches to thriving from childhood to early adulthood,” The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, C. R. Snyder (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 207–223.

Google Scholar

Carlo, G., Padilla-Walker, L. M., and Hastings, P. D. (2022). “Prosocial behaviors and development,” in Handbook of Moral Development (3rd Ed.), M. Killen & J. Smetana (Oxfordshire: Taylor & Francis).

Google Scholar

Davis, A. N., Carlo, G., and Maiya, S. (2021). Towards a multisystem, strength-based model of social inequities in US Latinx youth. Hum. Dev. 65, 204–216. doi: 10.1159/000517920

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapsley, D. K., and Narvaez, D. (2006). “Character education,” in Handbook of Child Psychology A. Renninger and I. Siegel (New York: Wiley), 248–296.

Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M. (2006). “Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, 6th Ed., Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development, W. Damon and R. M. Lerner (New York: John Wiley), 1–17.

Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M. (2018). Character development among youth: linking lives in time and place. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 42, 267–277. doi: 10.1177/0165025417711057

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M., and Overton, W. F. (2008). Exemplifying the integrations of the relational developmental system: Synthesizing theory, research, and application to promote development and social justice. J. Adolesc. Res. 23, 245–255. doi: 10.1177/0743558408314385

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. (2014). Ordinary Magic: Resilience Processes in Development. New York: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Nucci, L. (2018). “Character: A developmental system,” in Child Development Perspectives. 13, 73–78.

Google Scholar

Overton, W. F. (2013). A new paradigm for developmental science: relationism and relational developmental systems. Appl. Dev. Sci. 17, 94–107. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2013.778717

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Staub, E. (2005). “The roots of goodness,” in Moral Motivation Through the Lifespan, eds. G. Carlo, and C. P. Edwards (Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press), 33–72.

Google Scholar

Wang, J., Ferris, K. A., Hershberg, R. M., and Lerner, R. M. (2015). Developmental trajectories of youth character: a five-wave longitudinal study of cub scouts and non-scout boys. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 2359–2373. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0340-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Witherington, D. C., Overton, W. F., Lickliter, R., Marshall., and Narvaez, D. (2018). Metatheory and the primacy of conceptual analysis in developmental science. Hum. Dev. 61, 182–198. doi: 10.1159/000490160

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: prosocial development, moral development, social justice, adolescence, ethnicity, minority adolescents, relational developmental systems

Citation: Lapsley D, Carlo G, Davis A, Fabes R and Laible D (2025) Editorial: Promoting a kinder and more just world: the development of prosocial, moral, and social justice behaviors in adolescence. Front. Dev. Psychol. 2:1536381. doi: 10.3389/fdpys.2024.1536381

Received: 28 November 2024; Accepted: 12 December 2024;
Published: 07 January 2025.

Edited and reviewed by: May Ling Halim, California State University, Long Beach, United States

Copyright © 2025 Lapsley, Carlo, Davis, Fabes and Laible. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniel Lapsley, ZGFubGFwc2xleUBuZC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.