Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Cognit. , 24 February 2025

Sec. Perception

Volume 4 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2025.1533913

This article is part of the Research Topic Music and Pro-sociality View all 6 articles

Music as a social instrument: a brief historical and conceptual perspective

  • 1Conservatorium of Music, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 2School of Human Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 3Perron Institute for Neurological and Translational Science, Perth, WA, Australia

This article addresses the origins and purpose of communal music-making, including dance, and its role in human sociality. It accords special significance to the adapted nature of human vocalization, and the sensorimotor discrimination that allows the prediction and then generation of musically relevant, coordinated and simultaneous movements. Commencing with a historical survey of the development of ideas about the evolutionary importance of music in human social behavior, this mini-review then sets out to define and explore key issues involved in an evolutionary explanation. These include: acquisition and control of parameters required for vocal production (synchronization of pitch, timbre, duration and loudness); the exchange and transmission of pitched utterances in unison as well as in harmony; the roles of natural and sexual selection in shaping human musical abilities; the nature of cooperative behavior, and the consequences for social bonding of such interaction throughout life; transmission of such behaviors across generations, and the interaction between genes and culture that drives the evolution of complex social behavior in Homo sapiens. The article concludes with a brief review of current research that deals with contributory features of this field, especially in neuroscience which continues to provide important psychophysiological data that reinforces the long-held proposal that music has a key role in promoting cooperative, prosocial interactions leading to health and wellbeing over the human lifespan.

Historical perspectives

Several recent publications concerned with the evolved, universal nature of human musicality have explored the relationship between the adaptive roles of music and social interaction and cooperation throughout life (e.g. Greenberg et al., 2021; MacDonald, 2021; Mehr et al., 2021; Savage et al., 2020, 2021; Leongómez et al., 2022; Stupacher et al., 2022; Wagner and Hoeschele, 2022). Each makes an important contribution to the field, yet speculation about the mystery of human musicality and its close links to human social behavior has a long history. This article sets out to summarize the traditions in philosophy, musicology and the social sciences that view the development of human musical behavior and associated collective expression as being inextricably linked to social culture. It goes on to summarize the influence of these perspectives on current research in anthropology, ethnomusicology and neurology that is contributing to building a more complete picture of the nature of human musicality and its role in social behavior and culture.

Rabinowitch (2020) provides a historical account of the capacity of music to effect social change, tracing this principally within the European philosophical tradition from the Ancient Greeks onwards. Plato attributed to specific musical phenomena (modes; instruments; Enea, 2022) the capacity for regulating moods and for establishing this response in childhood, with a moral purpose arising from the experience that distinguishes humans from animals (Bourgault, 2012). He also wrote that dancing makes humans more social: “For the whole life of man needs a good rhythm and a spirit of harmonious accommodation” [from Protagoras, quoted in Howes (1948)]. Chinese music theory from the 3rd century CE also attributed to musical stimuli the capacity to affect emotions (Kaufmann, 1976). The sixth century Roman philosopher Boethius attributed significance to musical quanta—rhythms and intervallic relationships—as affecting the relationship between the mind and the body (Enea, 2022). Within this tradition, Descartes (1649, cited in Jorgensen, 2012) enumerated the “passions of the soul” (wonder, love, hate, desire, joy, and sadness) that could influence and respond to music, a theory fleshed out in detail by the composer Mattheson (Mattheson and Harriss, 1739/1981) as the “doctrine of the affections” governing specific characteristics in musical form and expression.

Evolutionary insights into the role of music appeared prior to Darwin in the scientific speculation on music and language that arose during the French Enlightenment (Thomas, 1995). Rousseau (1763/2009), both philosopher and composer, considered music to be a species-specific trait, and explored how both music and language might have arisen. The Humboldt brothers, naturalist and explorer Alexander and philosopher-linguist Wilhelm provided reflections on the origins of vocal communication that proved influential for Darwin. Wilhelm wrote: “For man, as a species, is a singing creature, though the notes, in his case, are also coupled with thought” (von Humboldt, 1836/1999). Darwin's (1871) principal influence on the study of the origins of human vocalization is found in The Descent of Man. In his sexual selection theory, Darwin considered the musical nature of language origins and their social significance, the song-like origins of human vocalization important as females attracted mates and retained their affection (Piilonen, 2024). Spencer (1890) derived a theory of the origin of music through natural selection, but his model, which views music as an emotional variant of the existing capacity for speech, is in many ways the reverse of Darwin's.

A brief chronological presentation of publications since Darwin that reflect varied perspectives on the social nature of human musicality illustrates the accelerating recognition of this field as essential to an informed understanding of the human condition. Nietzsche accorded fundamental significance to music in human life, referencing evolutionary theory in his writing about the emotional basis of vocalization (Nietzsche, 1871), and the linguist Jespersen (1922) was a notable representative of his discipline in accepting a song-like origin for speech. Tylor (1871), the leading exponent of the new sub-discipline of cultural anthropology, drew on Darwin's own journal entries from the Beagle voyage as evidence of the role of music in social interaction in the South Sea Islands. Durkheim (1912/1995), who established the first Department of Sociology in Europe, assigned special significance to the role of ritual and its dependence on dance and music in achieving the state of social bonding that he termed “collective effervescence.” Indeed, the explorer Shackleton apparently chose people for his Trans-Antarctic expedition of 1914–1917 who could sing not only to facilitate coordinated work, but also because their ability to do so might well contribute to survival in the appalling conditions they would encounter (Philpott and Leane, 2016).

Seeking, on natural selection lines, to find features of music that are common to most or all musical cultures and identify the survival value that music lends, Roederer (1984) focused on three aspects: communication and attachment between mother and newborn (see also below); extraction of musical components of speech; and how music “equalizes the emotional states of groups of people.” In an important and influential book, Blacking (1976) also emphasized the vital role of music in defining, as well as separating, social groups. Storr (1992) explored his own passion for music in a wide-ranging historical and conceptual survey of sources illustrative of the significance of musical experience to human mental wellbeing, social interaction and spiritual conviction. Others have focussed on the roles of dance and physical drill as defining features of human social and collective engagement (McNeill, 1995), the importance of music in the achievement of altruism and cooperation (Ridley, 1996), and the vocal response to collective affiliation in the form of synchronous chorusing (Merker, 1999). More recently, Jordania (2022) has brought a new interpretation to bear on the synchronous chorusing model, suggesting a combination of predator-avoidance and male-bonding strategies that may have shaped group vocalization.

Freeman (2000) proposed how social bonding in response to such musical experiences is open to neurobiological explanation, while Brown (2000) proposed the term “musilanguage” as the precursor to a communicative split that saw speech and song arise from a common origin out of which they acquired independent characteristics. Mithen (2005) also depicted a form of musilanguage as the vocal basis for human enculturation, devising the term Hmmmm to capture its nature (holistic, multimodal, manipulative and musical) and attributing its acquisition to singing Neanderthals. In an important contribution, Levitin (2007) presented a synthesis of his and other researchers' work on how and why the brain has evolved to permit the wide range of operations that musical perception and production involve, discussing the likely role of human musical activity in enhancing social bonding and cooperation (see also Chanda and Levitin, 2013). Patel (2010, 2018) viewed music as a “transformative technology of the mind” (Patel, 2010) and in subsequent work suggested that the “invention of musical behavior triggered processes of gene-culture coevolution” (Patel, 2018).

Morley (2013) placed the latest developments in acoustic and psychological understanding within a chronological framework derived from archaeological and anthropological evidence that explored music's evolutionary origins. Dunbar (2012, 2023) also related archaeological and anthropological evidence to psychological investigations intended to reveal survival value. How might music increase pain tolerance, or promote bonding identification with the group? Tarr et al. (2015, 2017) investigated these issues through experimental work with dancers. Harvey's (2017) Music, Evolution, and the Harmony of Souls develops further the motivation of Storr (1992) to account for the peak experiences and collective cognitive and emotional states that music can evoke, emphasizing a key role of communal music-related activities in enhancing trust, empathy and altruism. Such interactions require an ability to recognize and understand the likely mental state and intentions of others (Theory of Mind), a social-cognitive task that may have been closely linked to the emergence of music (Livingstone and Thompson, 2009; Harvey, 2018).

A fruitful complementary field of study regarding the purpose and development of human vocalization arose in the study of musical response from before birth onwards (Roederer, 1984; Woodward, 2019), and especially the vocal, tactile and gestural interaction of mothers and infants. Kessen et al. (1979) analyzed the matching of pitch to their mother's singing of sustained tones in the voices of infants aged between 3 and 6 months as a window that closes as the child begins to explore vocal production more widely and individually. Trevarthen (1979) similarly examined the vocalizations of infants as contributory to the eventual acquisition of speech. Trainor and Trehub (1993) considered the listening strategies of infants as foundational to music as well as speech development. Dissanayake (2008) considered the survival advantages of the behaviors involved in such mother-infant interactions, and their subsequent adult role in reproductive success, suggesting that they form a link to lifelong expression of affection that places multimodal sensory exchanges at the heart of the human capacity for intimacy and its role in trust and mutual affection.

Evolutionary theory and human social behavior

As part of this brief review into music as a social instrument, it is pertinent to comment on the origin of complex and rewarding prosocial interactions in humans. Perhaps the most widespread phenomenon that plays a part in group synchronization is collective movement. Inter-individual synchronization of movement, and sometimes vocalization, is not unique to Homo sapiens and has been reported in other species (e.g. Ravignani and Norton, 2017; Ravignani, 2019; Bouwer et al., 2021; Seki, 2021), but there may be specific adaptations to auditory-motor circuitry that have evolved in humans (Patel, 2008). Stewart (1998) presented mathematically-derived models for how coordinated movement is achieved, from which one might conclude that, while marching and dancing may be seen as equivalent in humans to the shoaling of fish and the flocking of birds, the complex properties of group singing and dancing that we will discuss represent a further and more elaborate correspondence. Indeed, recent genetic studies provide evidence for inheritance of, and even positive selection for, some elements of inter-individual synchronization, music-related ability and creativity (Liu et al., 2016; Oikkonen et al., 2016a,b; Mariath et al., 2017; Beccacece et al., 2021; Niarchou et al., 2022; Yeom et al., 2022). Furthermore, a few genes related to beat synchronization are located in so-called human accelerated regions (HARs). HARs are human-specific and affect genes involved in, for example, neural development and expansion of some cortical areas (Guardiola-Ripoll and Fatjó-Vilas, 2023). Of the almost seventy gene loci associated with rhythm perception and beat synchronization (many enriched in fetal and adult brain tissue), two were reported to contribute 2.26 times more to observed heritability than would be expected if there was a uniform distribution of variants (Niarchou et al., 2022).

Stewart's (1998) analysis of the coordinated locomotion of widely different species in the media of water, air and on land observed the patterns of limb-movement that give rise to the multi-sensory exchange on which synchrony depends. A parallel investigation by Hockett (1960) focussed on the design features of acoustic communication in a representative range of species from which the specific requirements for the human capacity for language could be assembled. His model illustrates that, while significant individual aspects of human communication are shared with many other species, both through convergent and divergent evolution, human speech is the only example of a communicative ability that ticks every box. While song and speech overlap in relation to Hockett's criteria, the element that lends itself most both to the link between the two and the distinct achievement in vocal perception and production of our species is our adaptive aural response to harmonicity (Bannan et al., 2024a,b).

Singing in harmony is rare in other species, but universal and essential in humans if fathers are to sing in unison with female partners and children. More elaborately, the vowel system on which all languages depend, albeit significantly diverse in quantity and kind (Ladefoged and Maddieson, 1990), employs timbral variants of the fundamental pitch produced by the larynx, termed source-filter production (Fant, 1970). Nikolsky (2020), comparing the relationship between formant frequencies in language and musical timbres in song and jaw-harp performance, concluded that they have influenced one another in their dependence on response to the harmonic series. Bannan (2022) commenced a systematic review that included analysis of the relationship between the perception of harmony in music and the distinct timbral quality of vowel sounds. The critical aspect of human vocalization that permits both speech and song demands investigation in all its dimensions: acoustic, anatomical, semiotic and psychological.

Melis (2018) considered comparative studies with closely related primate species to be crucial to understanding the origins of human prosociality, focusing particularly on the developing behavioral responses of human and chimpanzee infants. Collaborative behavior emergent from “underlying proximate mechanisms” (Duguid and Melis, 2020) can be characterized in four sequenced categories that can be observed in primates: by-product collaboration (in which individuals respond similarly to the same stimulus); socially influenced collaboration (individuals can be influenced by observation of others in their response); actively coordinated collaboration (a learnt collective response arising from previous experience, possibly arising from a bonded relationship); and collaboration based on shared intentionality (O'Madagain and Tomasello, 2022). This sequence could plausibly be applied to how the development of vocalization related to human social and cultural coordination. Marching in-step remains a universal expression of shared physical commitment (McNeill, 1995), the synchrony of the lower limbs leaving the arms free to carry (especially in transporting infants yet unable to walk) or wield tools and weapons, providing the metric underpinning for vocal synchrony and chorusing, suggested to be a distinct stage in the development of vocal communication prior to language (Merker, 1999, see also Patel, 2024).

We have already briefly mentioned suggested links between music and Theory of Mind (Livingstone and Thompson, 2009; see also Harvey, 2017). In humans, trust and reciprocal altruistic behavior between non-relatives can be beneficial to individuals but can also prove favorable to the group as a whole (Trivers, 1971; Kurzban et al., 2015; Silk and House, 2016), facilitating the sharing of information and diverse physical resources. In humans, the proposed evolutionary mechanism of group selection (Hamilton, 1964a,b) is associated with inclusive fitness and greater social and cultural adaptability (Richerson et al., 2016), a condition in which “natural selection favored genes that gave rise to new, more pro-social motives” (Boyd and Richerson, 2009). There remains some controversy about cultural group selection theory [see for example commentaries in Boyd and Richerson (2009)], although evidence strongly supports the importance of cooperation, norm-abiding behavior and positive feedback in promoting emotional alignment and social interaction (Korn et al., 2012; Tomasello and Vaish, 2013; Prehn et al., 2015). “Humans are the only multilevel species in which mere tolerance between interbreeding groups evolved into multigroup cooperative networks and the coordination of whole social groups, giving rise to many derived features of human sociality such as intense cooperation, prosociality, and cultural transmission” (Greuter et al., 2012). Of course, altruism can sometimes prove costly to an individual, but in times of stress or conflict, interactions that promote reliable collective action (e.g. hunting, food sharing, fighting) likely improve the survival potential of group members and their kin (Bowles, 2006). The potential impact that musical activities can have in such circumstances is discussed further below.

Musicality and social interaction

In humans, and in contrast to the music universal, language is the cognitive vehicle used to express thoughts and creative ideas, it is the primary means of communicating knowledge and learned skills within and between generations. It contains past, present and future tenses that allow “mental time travel” and a shared appreciation of the past, present and future. So why music? Why does this expensive endeavor in time and energy remain a universal, why does it remain “a permanent part of our mental furniture” (Storr, 1992)? Paraphrasing this author, music is not usually propositional, does not present testable hypotheses or set down precepts. Several proposals have been put forward that almost certainly have common foundations, at least at the neurological level. In addition to the Darwinian idea of courtship and mate selection, music has also been suggested to derive from the need to develop parent-infant attachment before babies learn to talk and use language (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Trehub, 2000; Falk, 2004; Levitin, 2007; Dissanayake, 2008; Trevarthen, 2002). This early communication, important in driving the behavioral, perceptual, cognitive, emotional and social development of preverbal infants, is generally known as motherese, and already employs the meaningful production and perception of variation in the four parameters of pitch, timbre, amplitude and duration that represent musical thinking and feeling (Bannan, 2008). Lastly, and the primary subject of this brief review: in childhood, adolescence and beyond, music yokes physiological arousal, stimulates cooperative group behavior and survival strategies, enhances trust and prosocial interactions, and helps to define social identity. In essence, it “intersects with cultural boundaries, facilitating our ‘social self' by linking our shared experiences and intentions” (Schulkin and Raglan, 2014).

Dissanayke's persuasive model (2008) for the relationship between musical interaction and sexual selection provides the basis for tracing a timeline that represents the nature of its potential effect over the human life cycle. Behaviors enacted initially in the mother-infant dyad to provide security and affection (touching, hugging, kissing, imitative facial expression, vocal duetting: see also Trevarthen, 1979, 2002) reappear as the multi-sensory repertoire through which adults convey and sustain affection. This sexual selection role that Darwin (1871) identified as the basis of human vocal communication has informed several recent experimental studies, including Marin and Gingras (2024), Bamford et al. (2024), and Keller et al. (2023), that illustrate how music can affect sexual attraction both in shared listening and in performance. In even later life, these behaviors remain available as means by which continued active participation in singing and dancing, as well as nostalgia arising from musical memories, can contribute to prolonged emotional wellbeing.

Between the mother-infant dyad and the adult romantic phase there is the development of peer-interactive musical play behaviors (Romet, 1980; Kartomi, 1999; Opie, 1993). When studying children in Western Java, Romet (1992) found that, over time, infants cease to engage in the pattern of behavior represented by interaction with their mothers and prefer the new enticement of songs and movement games learnt from their peers. The rebellious nature of this departure represents a new context for creativity, built upon during later childhood and adolescence, that has a vital role in the transmission of culture and the social engagement in play that prepares for adult activities. Of course, while musical participation often leads to positive outcomes—therapeutic, informative, happy, inspiring, enriching—music, rather than being neutral or beneficial, is sometimes seen as a threat and can be exploited and used with malicious and unpleasant intent. As Jordania (2022) argues, it can prepare for conflict and lower pain thresholds. But as much as these may aid predator avoidance and induce the mindset on which successful hunting depends, they may also be deployed in warfare (see also Bowles, 2006). One nation's stirring anthem may be the death knell of its victims, while music has on occasion been employed as a means of torture (Peters, 2019). That the power of music is open to such ill-use is an inevitable consequence of its shared adaptive history with the complex behavioral repertoire of our species.

Music as a driver of social behavior—Recent neuroscience findings

Building on the historical philosophical, anthropological and ethnomusicological perspectives that view the development of human musical behavior as being inextricably linked to social culture, to what extent has modern psychophysiological research reinforced this position? Is there a neurobiological basis that helps understand how, and perhaps clues as to why, humans continue to maintain two related yet distinct universal communication systems—language/speech and music? Bannan (2022) addressed this issue as representing “two servants, one master,” discussing the similarities in acoustic processing of these two communication streams. There are indeed common components of auditory analysis such as the processing of pitch, timbre and rhythmicity (Rogalsky et al., 2011; Perrachione et al., 2013), and while the distinction between music and language is not always as clearcut in all cultures (Cross, 2021; Ozaki et al., 2024), there are differences in left vs. right hemisphere functionality (Albouy et al., 2020). Recent functional imaging has identified separate regions in cerebral cortex that process only music, or only language (Angulo-Perkins et al., 2014; Norman-Haignere et al., 2015; see also Sihvonen et al., 2024). There is even a report, based on intracranial recording and functional imaging, of a neuronal population in human auditory cortex that is selective for song (Norman-Haignere et al., 2022).

As sophisticated imaging equipment for investigating brain function has become increasingly available, further insights into the neural organization of complex musical processing in humans have been revealed. Peretz and Zatorre (2005) were among the first to systematically review research on the workings of aural perception, as well as the processing of not only pitch and timing, but also musical memory and diverse emotional responses. They also discussed early neuropsychological research on singing, playing an instrument, reading music notation, and the effects of musical training on neural networks. Beyond primary and secondary auditory cortex in the superior temporal gyrus, numerous “higher level” cortical areas have been found to be activated by listening to music that elicits strong emotional responses (either positive or negative). These include several regions within prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal cortex and the supplementary motor area. Singing or playing music obviously involves activity in other regions including motor cortex, cerebellum and parts of the basal ganglia. In addition, music also activates components of the limbic system (hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala and cingulate cortex), critically important in a number of functions including learning, memory, motivation and emotional responsiveness. Music is often arousing and rewarding, and induces activation in the nucleus accumbens in ventral striatum, an anticipatory and reward center (e.g. Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Chanda and Levitin, 2013; Zatorre and Salimpoor, 2013; Ferreri et al., 2019). Furthermore, the social sharing of musical experiences is especially rewarding and enhances pleasure (and memory of the event) even further (Curzel et al., 2024).

Communal music-making and the increased empathy and social bonding that results (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010; Rabinowitch et al., 2013; Pearce et al., 2015; Savage et al., 2020), requires sensorimotor matching of vocal parameters such as pitch, loudness, timing and duration (Bannan et al., 2024b). It is also expressed multi-modally through the tendency of humans “to temporally coordinate when engaging with each other” (Hoehl et al., 2021), relating vocal production also to gesture and facial expression as well as gait. Synchronized chorusing is the prime sensorimotor component of the specific human capacity for communal music-making, requiring the mutual recognition and production of pitch in unison and in harmony and operating within the parameters of rhythm, including meter and tempo, as well as pitch. This complex group behavior requires sophisticated sensorimotor processing, involving coordination between breathing, vocal cords in the larynx, and muscles controlling the pharynx, jaw, lips, tongue and so on. In this regard it may be relevant that in humans there are specific projections from motor cortex to the motor neurons in the brainstem that control vocalization (Zarate, 2013) and enhanced connectivity between regions interconnecting laryngeal motor cortex with sensory processing regions, presumably increasing sensorimotor integration (Kumar et al., 2016).

Social collaboration is greater during music improvisation (Keeler et al., 2015; Walton et al., 2018; Hadar and Rabinowitch, 2023) and silent-disco dancing in time together enhances social memory of others in the group, likely involving gaze and enhanced visual attention (Woolhouse et al., 2016; Behrens et al., 2020) although under some conditions facial cues may not be necessary for developing synchronous interactions in young children (Rabinowitch et al., 2024). Links between sound and movement are well-established in many species; in human imaging studies, music and movement are closely linked (Sievers et al., 2013; Karpati et al., 2016; Laland et al., 2016; Levitin et al., 2018), and the term “groove” has been coined to describe the pleasure derived from movement in time to music (e.g. Witek et al., 2014; Matthews et al., 2020). Successful coordination in a group context requires real-time adjustments and is also associated with activation of reward centers within the brain (Menon and Levitin, 2005; Salimpoor et al., 2011; Koelsch, 2018). Prediction of action is also needed, and it has been reported that there is increased activity in premotor cortex, basal ganglia and cerebellum when silently anticipating a known musical sequence (Leaver et al., 2009). Neural entrainment (the temporal relationship between an external stimulus and an internal rhythmic response) to music in individuals is a well-established phenomenon (Trost et al., 2014). This is clearly an important attribute when coordinating interactive, presumably prosocial behaviors. Brain networks associated with interpersonal coordination of rhythmic behavior have been identified (Harry et al., 2023), and it is also of interest that recent research has found there is synchronization of brain wave activity (Schirmer et al., 2016; Müller et al., 2021; Cheng et al., 2024) in groups playing musical instruments (Shahal et al., 2020), when singing in choirs (Delius and Müller, 2023), and when moving/dancing to a meter or beat in coordinated groups (Dalla Bella et al., 2015; Hu et al., 2022). Such synchronization and “brain-to-brain coupling” may help promote interpersonal coordination (Hu et al., 2022; Delius and Müller, 2023).

Finally, with regard to what is known about the neurochemistry of music-induced prosocial behaviors, functional imaging studies have revealed similarities in the cortical regions that are active when performing non-selfish tasks requiring mutual cooperation (Rilling et al., 2002), and when listening to music that is familiar and rewarding (Harvey, 2017, 2018). While co-activation of brain regions does not necessarily indicate shared neural circuitry (Peretz et al., 2015), these regions also overlap with areas involved in oxytoninergic processing and contain oxytocin receptors (Harvey, 2020; Arnold et al., 2024). Both oxytocin and endorphin have been linked to musical activities, including dance (Dunbar, 2012; Kreutz, 2014; Keeler et al., 2015; Tarr et al., 2015, 2017; Yuhi et al., 2017; Good and Russo, 2022), and both—along with dopamine—influence important aspects of human sociality (Chanda and Levitin, 2013; Baskaran et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2017, 2018; Harvey, 2020). These neuromodulators are also critically important in maternal-infant attachment behavior (Izaki et al., 2024), reinforcing the impact they have across the human life-cycle and their continued role in human social behaviors, with music a major driver. Note that the exact contribution of these neuromodulators to the impact that musical activities have on human sociality may require further study because there are potentially complex interactions of all three of these systems at the receptor and synaptic level (Arnold et al., 2024; Izaki et al., 2024; Borland, 2025; Yao and Kendrick, 2025).

Conclusions

We began by citing recent, influential publications that have emphasized key links between the evolution of human musicality and prosocial behaviors. Here we remind readers that the publication of ideas relating to the nexus between music and prosociality has a long history in the scholarly fields of philosophy, musicology and social science, and emphasize how modern research and new technologies have only served to reinforce and interpret this view. But this is not merely an academic exercise since in the 21st century the impact of music on human sociality, empathy and overall wellbeing across the lifespan remains just as important as it was for our ancestors. Music has beneficial effects on general health and a range of cognitive functions, and its value in therapy is increasingly accepted (e.g. Brancatisano et al., 2020). Its use in aiding in the treatment of age-related motor (e.g. Fan et al., 2023; Huang et al., 2024) and memory disorders (e.g. Bannan and Montgomery-Smith, 2008; Jiménez-Palomares et al., 2024; Sun et al., 2024) is well-known, as its use in reducing the experience of pain (Arnold et al., 2024). At the level of brain circuitry, musical expertise and music-making including choral singing consistently enhances the structural integrity (and it is assumed functionality) of some white matter tracts (Leipold et al., 2021), even in aged populations (Moisseinen et al., 2024). But in continuing to improve our understanding as to the psychophysiological basis that links musical activity with prosociality we will, to suggest just a few examples: (i) significantly improve social engagement in young people with autism spectrum disorders (Shi et al., 2024); (ii) further improve the efficacy of music therapy in awakening positive, social interactions between persons with dementia and their caregivers (where language may already have been impaired) or in choirs (Ridder et al., 2024); (iii) facilitate relationship completion and improved quality of life for patients in palliative care and their families (Ramesh, 2024). Such health benefits as these therapeutic procedures may achieve clearly build upon the specific evolved characteristics that shaped our species in prehistoric times.

Author contributions

NB: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AH: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Albouy, P., Benjamin, L., Morillon, B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2020). Distinct sensitivity to spectrotemporal modulation supports brain asymmetry for speech and melody. Science 367, 1043–1047. doi: 10.1126/science.aaz3468

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Angulo-Perkins, A., Aubé, W., Peretz, I., Barios, F. A., Armony, J. L., Concha, L., et al. (2014). Music listening engages specific cortical regions within the temporal lobes: differences between musicians and non-musicians. Cortex 59, 126–137. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, C. A., Bagg, M. K., and Harvey, A. R. (2024). The psychophysiology of music-based interventions and the experience of pain. Front. Psychol. 15:1361857. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1361857

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamford, J. S., Vigl, J., Hämäläinen, M., and Saarikallio, S. H. (2024). Love songs and serenades: a theoretical review of music and romantic relationships. Front. Psychol. 15:1302548. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1302548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannan, N. (2008). Language out of music: the four dimensions of vocal learning. Aust. J. Anthropol. 19, 272–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1835-9310.2008.tb00354.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannan, N. (2022). Two servants, one master: the common acoustic origins of the divergent communicative media of music and speech. Evol. Studies Imagin. Cult. 6, 21–42. doi: 10.26613/esic.6.2.297

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannan, N., Dunbar, R. I. M., and Bamford, J. S. (2024a). The evolution of gender dimorphism in the human voice: the role of octave equivalence. Curr. Anthropol. 65, 503–527. doi: 10.1086/730147

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannan, N., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harvey, A. R., and Podlipniak, P. (2024b). Acoustic processing and the origin of human vocal communication. Evol. Studies Imagin. Cult. 8, 1006–1039. doi: 10.70082/esiculture.vi.982

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannan, N., and Montgomery-Smith, C. (2008). ‘Singing for the Brain': reflections on the human capacity for music arising from a pilot study of group singing with Alzheimer's patients. J. Roy. Soc. Promot. Health 128, 73–78. doi: 10.1177/1466424007087807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskaran, C., Plessow, F., Silva, L., Azanza, E., Marengi, D., Eddy, K. T., et al. (2017). Oxytocin secretion is pulsatile in men and is related to social-emotional functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology 85, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.07.486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beccacece, L., Abondio, P., Cilli, E., Restani, D., and Luiselli, D. (2021). Human genomics and the biocultural oriigin of music. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:5397. doi: 10.3390/ijms22105397

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, F., Snijdewint, J. A., Moulder, R. G., Prochazkova, E., Sjak-Shie, E. E., Boker, S. M., et al. (2020). Physiological synchrony is associated with cooperative success in real-life interactions. Sci. Rep. 10:19609. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76539-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blacking, J. (1976). How Musical is Man? London: Faber and Faber.

Google Scholar

Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. L. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borland, J. M. (2025). A review of the effects of different types of social behaviors on the recruitment of neuropeptides and neurotransmitters in the nucleus accumbens. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 77:101175. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2025.101175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourgault, S. (2012). Music and pedagogy in the platonic city. J. Aesth. Educ. 46, 59–72. doi: 10.5406/jaesteduc.46.1.0059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouwer, F. L., Nityananda, V., Rouse, A. A., and ten Cate, C. (2021). Rhythmic abilities in humans and non-human animals: a review and recommendations from a methodological perspective. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 376:20200335. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0335

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowles, S. (2006). Group competition, reproductive leveling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314, 1569–1572. doi: 10.1126/science.1134829

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 364, 3281–3288. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brancatisano, O., Baird, A., and Thompson, W. F. (2020). Why is music therapeutic for neurological disorders? The therapeutic music capacities model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 600–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S. (2000). “The “musilanguage” model of music evolution,” The Origin of Music, eds. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Boston, MA: MIT Press), 271–291. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/5190.003.0022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chanda, M. L., and Levitin, D. J. (2013). The neurochemistry of music. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 179–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S., Wang, J., Luo, R., and Hao, N. (2024). Brain to brain musical interaction: a systematic review of neural synchrony in musical activities. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 164:105812. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105812

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, I. (2021). Music, attachment, and uncertainty: music as communicative interaction. Behav. Brain Sci. 44:e66. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X20001028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curzel, F., Osiurak, F., Trân, E., Tillmann, B., Ripollés, P., Ferreri, L., et al. (2024). Enhancing musical pleasure through shared musical experience. iScience 27:109964. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.109964

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Berkowska, M., and Sowiński, J. (2015). Moving to the beat and singing are linked in humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:663. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00663

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray. doi: 10.5962/bhl.title.2092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DeCasper, A. J., and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science 208, 1174–1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delius, J. A., and Müller, V. (2023). Interpersonal synchrony when singing in a choir. Front. Psychol. 13:1087517. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1087517

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissanayake, E. (2008). If music is the food of love, what about survival and reproductive success? Mus. Sci. 12:169–195. doi: 10.1177/1029864908012001081

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duguid, S., and Melis, A. P. (2020). How animals collaborate: underlying proximate mechanisms. WIREs Cog. Sci. 11:e1529. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). “On the evolutionary function of song and dance,” in Music, Language, and Human Evolution, ed. N. Bannan (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 201–214. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199227341.003.0008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2023). The origins and function of musical performance. Front. Psychol. Perf. Sci. 14:1257390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1257390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Durkheim, E. (1912/1995). The Elementary Forms of Religious Life (Ed. and Transl. by K. E. Fields). New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Enea, F. (2022). Music and emotion: from ancient greece to contemporaneity. Ann. Univ. Bucharest Philos. Ser. LXXI, 57–78.

Google Scholar

Falk, D. (2004). Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 491–503. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x04000111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, L., Hu, E. Y., Hey, G. E., and Hu, W. (2023). Music therapy for gait and speech deficits in Parkinson's Disease: a mini-review. Brain Sci. 13:993. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13070993

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fant, G. (1970). Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110873429

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreri, L., Mas-Herrero, E., Zatorre, R. J., Ripollés, P., Gomez-Andres, A., Alicart, H., et al. (2019). Dopamine modulates the reward experiences elicited by music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 3793–3798. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811878116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (2000). “A neurobiological role of music in social bonding,” in The Origins of Music, eds. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Boston, MA: MIT Press), 411–424. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/5190.003.0028

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, A., and Russo, F. A. (2022). Changes in mood, oxytocin, and cortisol following group and individual singing: a pilot study. Psychol. Mus. 50, 1340–1347. doi: 10.1177/03057356211042668

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, D. M., Decety, J., and Gordon, I. (2021). The social neuroscience of music: understanding the social brain through human song. Am. Psychol. 76, 1172–1185. doi: 10.1037/amp0000819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greuter, C. C., Chapais, B., and Zinner, D. (2012). Evolution of multilevel social systems in nonhuman primates and humans. Int. J. Primatol. 33, 1002–1037. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9618-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guardiola-Ripoll, M., and Fatjó-Vilas, M. (2023). A systematic review of the human accelerated regions in Schizophrenia and related disorders: where the evolutionary and neurodevelopmental hypotheses converge. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 24:3597. doi: 10.3390/ijms24043597

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadar, T., and Rabinowitch, T.-C. (2023). The varying social dynamics in orally transmitted and notated vs. improvised musical performance. Front. Psychol. 14:1106092. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1106092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetic evolution of social behaviour. I. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetic evolution of social behaviour. 2. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 17–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90039-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harry, B. B., Margulies, D. S., Falkiewicz, M., and Keller, P. E. (2023). Brain networks for temporal adaptation, anticipation, and sensory-motor integration in rhythmic human behavior. Neuropsychologia 183:108524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, A. R. (2017). Music, Evolution, and the Harmony of Souls. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198786856.001.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, A. R. (2018). Music and the meeting of human minds. Front. Psychol. 9:762. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00762

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, A. R. (2020). Links between the neurobiology of oxytocin and human musicality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:350. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hockett, C. (1960). The origin of speech. Sci. Am. 203, 88–97. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0960-88

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoehl, S., Fairhurst, M., and Schirmer, A. (2021). Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits. Soc. Cog. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 5–18. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howes, F. (1948). Man, Mind and Music. London: Secker and Warburg. doi: 10.2307/933855

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Zhu, M., Liu, Y., Wang, Z., Cheng, X., Pan, Y., et al. (2022). Musical meter induces interbrain synchronization during interpersonal coordination. eNeuro 9:0504-21.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0504-21.2022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Dong, K., Gan, C., Xu, Z., Lei, D., Dong, X., et al. (2024). Effect of rhythmically cued exercise interventions on functions in patients with Parkinson Disease: a meta-analysis. Phys. Ther. 104:pzad158. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzad158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Izaki, A., Verbeke, W. J. M. I., Vrticka, P., and Ein-Dor, T. (2024). A narrative on the neurobiological roots of attachment-system functioning. Comm. Psychol. 2:96. doi: 10.1038/s44271-024-00147-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jespersen, O. (1922). Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin. New York, NY: Norton.

Google Scholar

Jiménez-Palomares, M., Garrido-Ardila, E. M., Chávez-Bravo, E., Torres-Piles, S. T., González-Sánchez, B., Rodríguez-Mansilla, M. J., et al. (2024). Benefits of music therapy in the cognitive impairments of Alzheimer's-type dementia: a systematic review. J. Clin. Med. 13:2042. doi: 10.3390/jcm13072042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordania, J. (2022). “Music, homosexuality, and cannibalism as predator avoidance evolutionary strategies in early humans,” in Music in Human Experience: Perspectives on a Musical Species, ed. J. Friedman (Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing), 139–159.

Google Scholar

Jorgensen, L. M. (2012). Descartes on music: between the ancients and the aestheticians. Br. J. Aesth. 52, 407–424. doi: 10.1093/aesthj/ays041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karpati, F. J., Giacosa, C., Foster, N. E., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2016). Sensorimotor integration is enhanced in dancers and musicians. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 893–903. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4524-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kartomi, M. J. (1999). Play songs by children and their educational implications. Aborig. Hist. 23, 61–71. doi: 10.22459/AH.23.2011.07

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, W. (1976). Musical References in the Chinese Classics. Sterling Heights, MI: Harmonie Park Press.

Google Scholar

Keeler, J. R., Roth, E. A., Neuser, B. L., Spitsbergen, J. M., Waters, D. J., Vianney, J.-M., et al. (2015). The neurochemistry and social flow of singing: bonding and oxytocin. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:518. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, P. E., Lee, J., König, R., and Novembre, G. (2023). Sex-related communicative functions of voice spectral energy in human chorusing. Biol. Lett. 19:20230326. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2023.0326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessen, W., Levine, J., and Wendrich, K. A. (1979). The imitation of pitch in infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 2, 93–99. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(79)80014-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Human Behav. 31, 354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2018). Investigating the neural encoding of emotion with music. Neuron 98, 1075–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korn, C. W., Prehn, K., Park, S. Q., Walter, H., and Heekeren, H. R. (2012). Positively based processing of self-relevant social feedback. J. Neurosci. 32:16832–16844. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3016-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreutz, G. (2014). Singing and social bonding. Music Med. 6, 51–60. doi: 10.47513/mmd.v6i2.180

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, V., Croxson, P. L., and Simonyan, K. (2016). Structural organization of the laryngeal motor cortical network and its implication for evolution of speech production. J. Neurosci. 36, 4170–4418. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3914-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R., Burton-Chellew, M. N., and West, S. A. (2015). The evolution of altruism in humans. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 575–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladefoged, P., and Maddieson, I. (1990). Vowels of the world's languages. J. Phon. 18, 93–122. doi: 10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30396-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laland, K., Wilkins, C., and Clayton, N. (2016). The evolution of dance. Curr. Biol. 26, R5–R9. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leaver, A. M., Van Lare, J., Zielinski, B., Halpern, A. R., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2009). Brain activation during anticipation of sound sequences. J. Neurosci. 29, 2477–2485. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4921-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leipold, S., Klein, C., and Jänke, L. (2021). Musical expertise shapes functional and structural brain networks independent of absolute pitch ability. J. Neurosci. 41, 2496–2511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1985-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leongómez, J. D., Havlíček, J., and Roberts, S. C. (2022). Musicality in human vocal communication: an evolutionary perspective. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 377:20200391. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0391

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. (2007). This Is Your Brain on Music: The Science of a Human Obsession. New York, NY: Plume (Penguin).

Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., Grahn, J. A., and London, J. (2018). The psychology of music: rhythm and movement. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69, 51–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011740

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Kanduri, C., Oikkonen, J., Karma, K., Raijas, P., Ukkola-Vuoti, L., et al. (2016). Detecting signatures of positive selection associated with musical aptitude in the human genome. Sci. Rep. 6:21198. doi: 10.1038/srep21198

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingstone, S., and Thompson, W. F. (2009). The emergence of music from the theory of mind. Mus. Sci. Spec. Issue 13, 83–115. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002061

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, R. (2021). “The social functions of music: Communication, wellbeing, art, ritual, identity and social networks (C-WARIS),” in Routledge International Handbook of Music Psychology in Education and the Community, eds. A. Creech, D. A. Hodges, and S. Hallam (London: Routledge), 5–20. doi: 10.4324/9780429295362-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariath, L. M., da Silva, A. M., Kowalski, T. W., Gattino, G. A., Araujo, G. A., Figueiredo, F. G., et al. (2017). Music genetics research: association with musicality of a polymorphism in the AVPRA gene. Genet. Mol. Biol. 40, 421–429. doi: 10.1590/1678-4685-gmb-2016-0021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marin, M. M., and Gingras, B. (2024). How music-induced emotions affect sexual attraction: evolutionary implications. Front. Psychol. 15:1269820. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1269820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattheson, J., and Harriss, E. C. (1739/1981). Johann Mattheson's der Vollkommene Capellmeister: A Revised Translation with Critical Commentary. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Research Press.

Google Scholar

Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A., Lund, T., Vuust, P, and Penhune, V. B. (2020). The sensation of groove engages motor and reward networks. NeuroImage 214:116768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116768

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. doi: 10.4159/9780674040878

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehr, S. A., Krasnow, M. M., Bryant, G. A., and Hagen, E. H. (2021). Origins of music in credible signaling. Behav. Brain Sci. 44:e60. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X20000345

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Melis, A. P. (2018). The evolutionary roots of prosociality: the case of instrumental helping. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 82–86. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Levitin, D. J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage 28, 175–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merker, B. (1999). Synchronous chorusing and the origins of music. Mus. Sci. 3, 59–73. doi: 10.1177/10298649000030S105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mithen, S. (2005). The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

Google Scholar

Moisseinen, N., Ahveninen, L., Martínez-Molina, N., Sairanen, V., Melkas, S., Kleber, B., et al. (2024). Choir singing is associated with enhanced structural connectivity across the adult lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 45:e26705. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26705

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morley, I. (2013). The Prehistory of Music: Human Evolution, Archaeology, and the Origins of Musicality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199234080.001.0001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., Perdikis, D., Mende, M. A., and Lindenberger, U. (2021). Interacting brains coming in sync through their minds: an interbrain neurofeedback study. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1500, 48–68. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14605

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niarchou, M., Gustavson, D. E., Sathirapongsasuti, J. F., Angklada-Tort, M., Eising, E., Bell, E., et al. (2022). Genome-wide association study of musical beat synchronization demonstrates high polygenicity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 1292–1309. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01359-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nietzsche, F. (1871). “On music and words,” in Between Romanticism and Modernism (Transl. by W. Kauffman, Reprinted in C. Dahlhaus, 1980) (Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press), 103–120.

Google Scholar

Nikolsky, A. (2020). “Talking jew's harp” and its relation to vowel harmony as a paradigm of formative influence of music on language,” in The Origins of Language Revisited: Differentiation from Music and the Emergence of Neurodiversity and Autism, ed. N. Masataka (Cham: Springer Nature), 217–322. doi: 10.1007/978-981-15-4250-3_8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman-Haignere, S., Kanwisher, N. G., and McDermott, J. H. (2015). Distinct cortical pathways for music and speech revealed by hypothesis-free voxel decomposition. Neuron 88, 1281–1296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman-Haignere, S. V., Feather, J., Boebinger, D., Brunner, P., Ritaccio, A., McDermott, J. H., et al. (2022). A neuronal population selective for song in human auditory cortex. Cur. Biol. 32, 1470–1484.e12. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.01.069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oikkonen, J., Kuusi, T., Peltonen, P., Raijas, P., Ukkola-Vuoti, L., Karma, K., et al. (2016a). Creative activities in music - a genome-wide linkage analysis. PLoS ONE 11:e0148679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148679

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oikkonen, J., Onkamo, P., Järvelä, I., and Kanduri, C. (2016b). Convergent evidence for the molecular basis of musical traits. Sci. Rep. 6:39707. doi: 10.1038/srep39707

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Madagain, C., and Tomasello, M. (2022). Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of human culture. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 377:20200320. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0320

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Opie, I. (1993). The People in the Playground. Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ozaki, Y., Tierney, A., Pfordresher, P. Q., McBride, J. M., Benetos, E., Proutskova, P., et al. (2024). Globally, songs and instrumental melodies are slower and higher and use more stable pitches than speech: a registered report. Sci. Adv. 10:eadm9797. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.adm9797

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2010). “Music, biological evolution, and the brain,” in Emerging Disciplines, eds. C. Levander and C. Henry (Houston, TX: Rice University Press), 91–144.

Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2018). “Music as a transformative technology of the mind: an update,” in The Origins of Musicality, ed. H. Honing (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 113–126. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/10636.003.0009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2024). Beat-based dancing to music has evolutionary foundations in advanced vocal learning. BMC Neurosci. 25:65. doi: 10.1186/s12868-024-00843-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Launay, J., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). The ice-breaker effect: singing mediates fast social bonding. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2:150221. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2017). Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 114, 5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: a replication study. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 4, 400–422. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0101-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Vuvan, D., Lagrois, M.-E., and Armony, J. L. (2015) Neural overlap in processing music speech. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370:20140090. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., and Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annu. Rev. Psych. 6, 89–114. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070225

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrachione, T. K., Fedorenko, E. G., Vinke, L., Gibson, E., and Dilley, L. C. (2013). Evidence for shared cognitive processing of pitch in music and language. PLoS ONE 8:e73372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073372

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, E. (2019). The Use and Abuse of Music: Criminal Records. Leeds: Emerald Publishing Limited. doi: 10.1108/9781787569997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Philpott, C., and Leane, E. (2016). Making music on the march: sledging songs of the ‘heroic age'of Antarctic exploration. Polar Rec. 52, 698–716. doi: 10.1017/S0032247416000255

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piilonen, M. (2024). Theorizing Music Evolution: Darwin, Spencer, and the Limits of the Human. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780197695289.001.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prehn, K., Korn, C. W., Bajbouj, M., Klann-Delius, G., Menninghaus, W., Jacobs, A. M., et al. (2015). The neural correlates of emotion alignment in social interaction. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 10, 435–443. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinowitch, T.-C. (2020). The potential of music to effect social change. Music Sci. 3. doi: 10.1177/2059204320939772

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinowitch, T.-C., Brooks, R. Z., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2024). Children in sync: exploring how interpersonal synchrony experience induces cooperation between child peers. Sci. Rep. 14:28130. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-78810-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinowitch, T.-C., Cross, I., and Burnard, P. (2013). Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children. Psychol. Mus. 41, 484–498. doi: 10.1177/0305735612440609

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramesh, B. (2024). Role of music therapy in palliative care – methods and techniques. J. Palliat. Care 5:8258597241235110. doi: 10.1177/08258597241235110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravignani, A. (2019). Rhythm and synchrony in animal movement and communication. Corr. Zool. 65, 77–81. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravignani, A., and Norton, P. (2017). Measuring rhythmic complexity: a primer to quantify and compare temporal structure in speech, movement, and animal vocalizations. J. Lang. Evol. 2, 4–19. doi: 10.1093/jole/lzx002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., et al. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: a sketch of the evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e30. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1400106X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridder, H. M., Anderson-Ingstrup, J., and Ridder, L. O. (2024). The effect of choir-singing on self-reported embodied cognition in persons with dementia. A pilot study. Dementia 12:14713012241272910. doi: 10.1177/14713012241272910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridley, M. (1996). The Origins of Virtue: Human Instincts and the Evolution of Cooperation. New York, NY: Viking.

Google Scholar

Rilling, J., Gutman, D., Zeh, T., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G., Kilts, C., et al. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 35, 395–405. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00755-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roederer, J. G. (1984). The search for a survival value of music. Music Percept. 1, 350–356. doi: 10.2307/40285265

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogalsky, C., Rong, F., Saberi, K., and Hickok, G. (2011). Functional anatomy of language and music perception: temporal and structural factors investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 31, 3843–3852. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4515-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Romet, C. (1980). The play rhymes of children-a cross cultural source of natural learning materials for music educators. Aust. J. Mus. Ed. 27–31.

Google Scholar

Romet, C. (1992). “Song acquisition in culture: a West Javanese study in children's song development,” in Music Education: Sharing Musics of the World. Proceedings of the 20th World Conference of the International Society for Music Education, Seoul, Korea, eds. H. Lees (Christchurch, NZ: ISME/University of Canterbury), 164–173.

Google Scholar

Rousseau, J. J. (1763/2009). Essay on the Origin of Languages and Writings Related to Music (Transl. by J. T. Scott). UPNE. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.

Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion in music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–264. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Savage, P. E., Loui, P., Tarr, B., Schachner, A., Glowacki, L., Mithen, S., et al. (2020). Music as a coevolved system for social bonding. Behav. Brain Sci. e59. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/qp3st

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Savage, P. E., Loui, P., Tarr, B., Schachner, A., Glowacki, L., Mithen, S., et al. (2021). Toward inclusive theories of the evolution of musicality. Behav. Brain Sci. 44, 1–42. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X21000042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schirmer, A., Meck, W. H., and Penney, T. B. (2016). The socio-temporal brain: connecting people in time. Trends Cog. Sci. 20, 760–772. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulkin, J., and Raglan, G. B. (2014). The evolution of music and social capability. Front. Neurosci. 8:292. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00292

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seki, Y. (2021). Cockatiels sing human music in synchrony with a playback of the melody. PLoS ONE 16:e0256613. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256613

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shahal, S., Wurzberg, A., Sibony, I., Duadi, H., Shniderman, E., Weymouth, D., et al. (2020). Synchronization of complex human networks. Nat. Comm. 11:3854. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17540-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Z., Wang, S., Chen, M., HU, A., Long, Q., and Lee, Y. (2024). The effect of music therapy on language communication and social skills in children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 15:1336421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1336421

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sievers, B., Polansky, L., Casey, M., and Wheatley, T. (2013). Music and movement share a dynamic structure that supports universal expressions of emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 70–75. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1209023110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sihvonen, A. J., Ferguson, M. A., Chen, V., Soinila, S., Särkämö, T., Joutsa, J., et al. (2024). Focal brain lesions causing acquired amusia map to a common brain network. J. Neurosci. 44:e1922232024. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1922-23.2024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silk, J. B., and House, B. R. (2016). The evolution of altruistic social preferences in human groups. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150097. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, H. (1890). The origin of music. Mind 15, 449–468. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-XV.60.449

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, I. (1998). Life's Other Secret: The New Mathematics of the Living World. New York, NY: John Wiley.

Google Scholar

Storr, A. (1992). Music and the Mind. London: HarperCollins.

Google Scholar

Stupacher, J., Mikkelsen, J., and Vuust, P. (2022). Higher empathy is associated with stronger social bonding when moving together with music. Psych. Music 50, 1511–1526. doi: 10.1177/03057356211050681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, L., Wang, Q., and Ai, J. (2024). The underlying roles and neurobiological mechanisms of music-based intervention in Alzheimer's disease: a comprehensive review. Ageing Res. Rev. 96:102265. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2024.102265

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Benson, C., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2017). Naltrexone blocks endorphins released when dancing in synchrony. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 3, 241–254. doi: 10.1007/s40750-017-0067-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., and Dunbar, R. I. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 11:20150767. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, D. A. (1995). Music and the Origins of Language: Theories from the French Enlightenment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511582608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., and Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 231–255. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trainor, L. J., and Trehub, S. E. (1993). Musical context effects in infants and adults: Key distance. J. Exp. Psych. Hum. Percept. Perf. 19, 615–626. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trehub, S. (2000). “Human processing predispositions and musical universals,” in The Origins of Music, eds. N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (The MIT Press), 427–448.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1979). “Communication and cooperation in early infancy,” in Before Speech, ed. M. Bullowa (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 321–348.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2002). “Origins of musical identity: evidence from infancy for musical social awareness,” in Musical Identities, eds. R. A. MacDonald, D. J. Hargreaves, and D. Miell (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 21–38. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198509325.003.0002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–57. doi: 10.1086/406755

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trost, W., Frühholz, S., Schön, D., Labbé, C., Pichon, S., Grandjean, D., et al. (2014). Getting the beat: entrainment of brain activity by musical rhythm and pleasantness. NeuroImage 103, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive Culture, Vol. 1. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

von Humboldt, W. (1836/1999). On Language, On the Diversity of Human Language Construction and its Influence on the Mental Development of the Human Species. (Ed. M. Losonsky). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wagner, B., and Hoeschele, M. (2022). The links between pitch, timbre, musicality and social bonding from cross-species research. Comp. Cog. Behav. Rev. 17, 13–32. doi: 10.3819/CCBR.2022.170002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, A. E., Langland-Hassan, P., Chemero, A., Kloos, H., and Richardson, M. J. (2018). Creating time: social collaboration in music improvisation. Top. Cogn. Sci. 10, 95–119. doi: 10.1111/tops.12306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Witek, M. A. G., Clarke, E. F., Wallentin, M., Kringelbach, M. L., and Vuust, P. (2014). Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS ONE 9:e94446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, S. C. (2019). “Fetal, neonatal, and early infant experiences of maternal singing,” in The Oxford Handbook of Singing, eds. G. F. Welch, D. M. Howard, and J. Nix (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 431–454.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Woolhouse, M. H., Tidhar, D., and Cross, I. (2016). Effects of inter-personal memory dancing in time with others. Front. Psychol. 7:167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, S., and Kendrick, K. M. (2025). How does oxytocin modulate human behavior? Mol. Psychiatry. 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41380-025-02898-1. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeom, D., Tan, Y. T., Haslam, N., Mosing, M. A., Yap, V. M. Z., Fraser, T., et al. (2022). Genetic factors and shared environment contribute equally to objective singing ability. iScience 25:104360. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104360

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuhi, T., Kyuta, H., Mori, H. A., Murakami, C., Furuhara, K., Okuno, M., et al. (2017). Salivary oxytocin concentration changes during a group drumming intervention for maltreated school children. Brain Sci. 7:E152. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7110152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarate, J. M. (2013). The neural control of singing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:237. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00237

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., and Salimpoor, V. N. (2013). From perception to pleasure: music and its neural substrates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 10430–10437. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301228110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: evolution, music, cooperation, sociality, reward, synchronization, dance

Citation: Bannan N and Harvey AR (2025) Music as a social instrument: a brief historical and conceptual perspective. Front. Cognit. 4:1533913. doi: 10.3389/fcogn.2025.1533913

Received: 10 December 2024; Accepted: 10 February 2025;
Published: 24 February 2025.

Edited by:

Kumiko Toyoshima, Osaka Shoin Women's University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Bruno Gingras, University of Vienna, Austria
Susana Carnero-Sierra, University of Oviedo, Spain
Felix Haiduk, University of Vienna, Austria

Copyright © 2025 Bannan and Harvey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alan R. Harvey, YWxhbi5oYXJ2ZXlAdXdhLmVkdS5hdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more